ArticlePDF Available

Avoiding Seeking Help in the Classroom: Who and Why?

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Why do some students avoid seeking help in the classroom when they need it? When students do not seek the help they need, they put themselves at a disadvantage for learning. We discuss how students' personal motivational characteristics relate to their avoidance of help seeking. In particular, we discuss our work regarding perceived academic and social competence and achievement- and social-goal orientations. We also discuss how various dimensions of the classroom context relate to help avoidance. Specifically, we discuss rules and norms of classrooms as well as our work examining the achievement goal structure and social climate of classrooms. We conclude by discussing how new developments in achievement goal theory distinguishing between approach- and avoidance-goal orientations might add to psychologists' understanding of help avoidance. We also consider how students' strivings for autonomy and different aspects of the social climate might be incorporated into theories of students' help avoidance behavior. Peer Reviewed http://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/44450/1/10648_2004_Article_292341.pdf
Content may be subject to copyright.
P1: Vendor/GCQ/GCQ P2: GCV/FZN/LOV QC: FZN
Educational Psychology Review [jepr] PP043-292341 January 15, 2001 16:52 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
Educational Psychology Review, Vol. 13, No. 2, 2001
Avoiding Seeking Help in the Classroom:
Who and Why?
Allison M. Ryan,1,3 Paul R. Pintrich,2and Carol Midgley2
Why do some students avoid seeking help in the classroom when they need it?
When students do not seek the help they need, they put themselves at a disad-
vantage for learning. We discuss how students’ personal motivational charac-
teristics relate to their avoidance of help seeking. In particular, we discuss our
work regarding perceived academic and social competence and achievement-
and social-goal orientations. We also discuss how various dimensions of the
classroom context relate to help avoidance. Specifically, we discuss rules and
norms of classrooms as well as our work examining the achievement goal
structure and social climate of classrooms. We conclude by discussing how new
developments in achievement goal theory distinguishing between approach-
and avoidance-goal orientations might add to psychologists’ understanding
of help avoidance. We also consider how students’ strivings for autonomy and
different aspects of the social climate might be incorporated into theories of
students’ help avoidance behavior.
KEY WORDS: help seeking; motivation; achievement goals; autonomy; classroom context.
INTRODUCTION
Help seeking is an important self-regulatory strategy that contri-
butes to student learning (Karabenick and Sharma, 1994; Newman, 1994;
Zimmerman and Martinez-Pons, 1988). Inevitably, students encounter
ambiguity or difficulty in their schoolwork and need assistance. In such a
situation, it is adaptive for students to use others as a resource to secure the
1University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign.
2University of Michigan, Ann Arbor.
3Correspondence should be addressed to Allison M. Ryan, 230 Education Building, 1310 South
Sixth Street, Champaign, Illinois 61820-6990; e-mail: ryan2@uiuc.edu.
93
1040-726X/01/0600-0093$19.50/0 C
°2001 Plenum Publishing Corporation
P1: Vendor/GCQ/GCQ P2: GCV/FZN/LOV QC: FZN
Educational Psychology Review [jepr] PP043-292341 January 15, 2001 16:52 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
94 Ryan, Pintrich, and Midgley
necessary help and to continue the learning process (McCaslin and Good,
1996). As children develop into adolescents, their metacognitive skills im-
prove (Keating, 1990), and they are better able to monitor and reflect on
their performance and to determine their need for help in academic situa-
tions (Myers and Paris, 1978; Nelson Le-Gall, 1981, 1985; Newman, 1991,
1994; Paris and Newman, 1990). However, many adolescents do not seek
help with their academic work when needed (Good et al., 1987; Newman,
1990; Newman and Goldin, 1990; Ryan et al., 1998; Ryan and Pintrich, 1997).
In fact, help avoidance has been found to increase during early adolescence
(Ryan and Midgley, 1998).
This apparent contradiction between young adolescents’ increasing
cognitive capabilities and their help-seeking behavior highlights the impor-
tance of considering the motivational and social factors that influence
help seeking in the classroom (Newman, 1994, 1998; Ryan and Pintrich,
1997). In our work, we have investigated how students’ motivational char-
acteristics, as well as classroom contextual characteristics, relate to
adolescents’ decision to avoid seeking help with their academic work. We
use the term “avoidance of help seeking” to refer to instances when stu-
dents know that they need help but do not seek it. For example, a stu-
dent might skip a problem altogether or put down any answer rather than
ask for help. The decision by students to not get help represents an in-
tentional goal-directed act to cease or avoid engagement. We are inter-
ested in this avoidance behavior because when students don’t garner help
when it is needed they put themselves at a disadvantage for learning and
performance.
In this article, we review research concerning students’ avoidance of
help seeking in the classroom. After describing the avoidance of help seek-
ing, we discuss how students’ personal motivational characteristics relate
to the avoidance of help seeking. Specifically, we discuss our work regard-
ing perceptions of academic and social competence and achievement and
social-goal orientations. Next, we discuss how various dimensions of the
classroom context relate to students’ avoidance of help seeking. Specifically,
we discuss rules and norms of classrooms as well as our work examining the
achievement goal structure and social climate of classrooms. We conclude
with some ideas for future research on the avoidance of help seeking. We
discuss how new developments in achievement goal theory distinguishing
between approach and avoidance-goal orientations might add to our un-
derstanding of help avoidance. We also consider how students’ strivings for
autonomy and different aspects of the social climate might be further an-
alyzed and incorporated into current theories of students’ help avoidance
behavior.
P1: Vendor/GCQ/GCQ P2: GCV/FZN/LOV QC: FZN
Educational Psychology Review [jepr] PP043-292341 January 15, 2001 16:52 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
Avoidance of Help Seeking 95
THE AVOIDANCE OF HELP SEEKING
The help-seeking process begins with the awareness of the need for help.
Van der Meij (1994) and Dillon (1988, 1990) have described this stage as per-
plexity. Perplexity refers to a state of puzzlement or uncertainty that arises
when there is a discrepancy between personal knowledge and new informa-
tion or expectations. For example, students may solve a math problem only
to find that the solution in the back of the book is different. Or, students may
think of an example that contradicts a statement that the teacher presents
in class. These two examples highlight that awareness of the need for help
may be externally or internally generated. External cues are abundant in
the classroom. For example, verbal responses by the teacher and graded as-
signments are external indications to students of how well they understand
a given concept. Students’ ability to internally monitor their comprehension
and determine their need for help increases as they age (Nelson-Le Gall,
1981, 1985; Newman, 1991, 1994).
When students become aware that they need help, they must decide
whether to actually seek help. Considerable research has identified this stage
as a critical juncture in the overall help-seeking process. Dillon (1988, p. 20)
wrote, “95 percent of the questions that we have in mind to ask we never go
on to utter ...we may think the better of it and follow one of the numerous
other paths available. These include keeping quiet and giving off that we
know and understand.” It is at this juncture in the help-seeking process that
our research has focused: the decision to avoid asking for help when students
know they need help.
Research has identified several reasons why students avoid asking for
help in the classroom. First, it may not be practical or feasible to ask for help
in a given situation. There may be explicit rules or norms against help seeking
and, thus, students may refrain from asking for help because they do not want
to get in trouble. Or students may judge that asking for help is not going to
be effective because (a) there is not a competent, willing helper who can
provide assistance or (b) it will take too long to get help. In addition to such
practical and expedient concerns, research has identified two psychosocial
concerns that underlie the avoidance of help seeking: desire for autonomy
and threat to competence (Butler, 1998; Newman, 1990; Van der Meij, 1988).
Our own research has focused on students’ reluctance to seek help
because of competence concerns, but we discuss the role of the desire for
autonomy in the last section of this article. The need for help may be per-
ceived by individuals as evidence that they lack ability and is likely to engen-
der negative reactions of judgments from others (Butler and Neuman, 1995;
Butler, 1998; Karabenick and Knapp, 1991; Newman, 1990; Ryan, Hicks, and
P1: Vendor/GCQ/GCQ P2: GCV/FZN/LOV QC: FZN
Educational Psychology Review [jepr] PP043-292341 January 15, 2001 16:52 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
96 Ryan, Pintrich, and Midgley
Midgley, 1997; Ryan and Pintrich, 1997). For example, Ryan and Pintrich
(1997) found that many students worried about negative judgments from
both their teacher and classmates regarding their abilities (e.g., “I think the
teacher/other kids might think I am dumb when I ask a question in math
class”). These competence concerns are positively related to the avoidance
of help seeking. Thus, the need for help is often construed as an indicator
that one lacks competence and, in that way, it explains the avoidance of help
seeking. An important question is what personal and contextual factors are
related to competence concerns about help seeking and to avoidant help-
seeking behavior? In the next section, we consider how students’ personal
academic and social characteristics relate to competence concerns and to
the avoidance of help seeking.
STUDENTS’ PERSONAL CHARACTERISTICS AND THE
AVOIDANCE OF HELP SEEKING
Perceptions of Cognitive Competence
Personal characteristics of students such as performance level, self-
esteem, and perceptions of competence have been linked with the decision
to avoid seeking help. In academic settings, research has shown that the
need for help is most threatening to low-achieving students. This is in line
with findings that the need for help is most threatening to students with low
self-esteem or perceptions of cognitive competence (Karabenick and Knapp,
1991; Newman, 1990; Newman and Goldin, 1990; Ryan et al., 1997; Ryan and
Pintrich, 1997). Students who are low achievers or who perceive that they
are not especially competent are concerned that others will think that their
need for help indicates that they are incapable and, therefore, they will be less
likely to seek help. In contrast, when students who are high achievers or who
have perceptions of high cognitive competence encounter difficulty, they are
less likely to worry that others will attribute it to their lack of ability, and
they are more likely to secure the necessary help (Butler and Neuman, 1995;
Karabenick and Knapp, 1991; Newman, 1990; Newman and Goldin, 1990;
Ryan et al., 1997; Ryan and Pintrich, 1997).
Perceptions of Social Competence
A similar relation exists between perceptions of social competence and
help-seeking behavior (Ryan and Pintrich, 1997). Feeling comfortable and
skillful in relating to others lessens the perception that there will be nega-
tive fallout (e.g., negative reaction or judgment from the helper) from help
P1: Vendor/GCQ/GCQ P2: GCV/FZN/LOV QC: FZN
Educational Psychology Review [jepr] PP043-292341 January 15, 2001 16:52 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
Avoidance of Help Seeking 97
seeking. Ryan and Pintrich (1997) found that adolescents who perceived
themselves as socially competent were less likely to feel threatened by help
seeking and were more likely to ask for help. This relation was independent
of the relation between perceived cognitive competence and help seeking,
suggesting an independent role of perceptions of social competence. It ap-
pears that if individuals feel comfortable and skillful in relating to others
socially, then they feel more confident in their ability to ask for help with-
out incurring a negative reaction from the helper. Accordingly, it is not just
perceptions of cognitive competence that can influence the help-seeking
process, but also perceptions of social competence. This pattern of results
highlights the importance of (a) considering help seeking, not just as an aca-
demic self-regulatory strategy, but also as a social interaction with others,
and (b) recognizing that social competence can play a role in the dynamics
of help seeking.
Achievement-Goal Orientations4
In addition to perceptions of competence, students’ achievement-goal
orientations are also related to the avoidance of help seeking. Theory and
research on goal orientations indicates that two general goal orientations are
important precursors to various motivational, cognitive, and behavioral out-
comes (see Ames, 1992; Dweck, 1986; Harackiewicz et al., 1998; Maehr and
Midgley, 1991; Nicholls, 1990; Pintrich, 2000b). Although there are a num-
ber of different labels attached to these two goal orientations, in this article
we use the terms mastery- and performance-goal orientations. When ori-
ented to mastery goals, students are focused on learning, self-improvement,
progress, and mastery of the task. In contrast, when oriented to performance
goals, students are focused on trying to better others, receiving public recog-
nition for superior performance, and trying to attain positive judgments of
their ability. These two achievement-goal orientations represent disparate
reasons for involvement and different conceptions of success regarding aca-
demic tasks, and they have been linked to different help-seeking beliefs and
behaviors.
A mastery-goal orientation focuses students on the task at hand, and
effort is seen as a way to master the task. As such, it decreases competence
concerns regarding help seeking and decreases the avoidance of help seeking
(Butler and Neuman, 1995; Newman 1991, 1994; Ryan et al., 1997; Ryan and
Pintrich, 1997). In contrast, a performance-goal orientation focuses students
4Researchers have now distinguished between approach and avoidance achievement-goal ori-
entations. The contribution of this distinction to research on help avoidance will be discussed
later in this paper. The discussion here focuses on the approach component of goal orientations.
P1: Vendor/GCQ/GCQ P2: GCV/FZN/LOV QC: FZN
Educational Psychology Review [jepr] PP043-292341 January 15, 2001 16:52 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
98 Ryan, Pintrich, and Midgley
on themselves and their competency relative to others. A sense of accom-
plishment is derived from demonstrating that one is smart, regardless of the
learning involved. When students are oriented to performance goals, they
are concerned about how others will evaluate them because external evalu-
ation will determine whether they are smart. This goal orientation increases
competence concerns regarding help seeking and increases the avoidance
of help seeking (Butler and Neuman, 1995; Newman 1991, 1994; Ryan et al.,
1997; Ryan and Pintrich, 1997).
Social-Goal Orientations
Goal orientations are salient regarding social achievement as well as
academic achievement in the classroom (Dweck, 1996; Hicks, 1997; Urdan
and Maehr, 1995; Wentzel, 1989). A social-goal orientation concerns the
purpose and meaning that students ascribe to their social behavior in the
classroom. Drawing on a large body of literature that demonstrates that
young adolescents value both the opinions and the companionship of their
peers (e.g., Berndt, 1982; Brown, 1990; Hartup, 1989; Parker and Asher,
1987), Hicks postulated two social-goal orientations: a social intimacy-goal
orientation and a social status-goal orientation (Hicks, 1997). These goal ori-
entations concern two types of alliances adolescents have with their peers. An
intimacy-goal orientation concerns the desire for both general acceptance
and for closer and more interpersonal relationships. In contrast, a social
status-goal orientation concerns the desire for social visibility and prestige
within the larger peer group. A social status-goal orientation is related to
the emergence of an elite social crowd in early adolescence, typically labeled
“popular,” which various researchers have documented (e.g., Brown et al.,
1994; Eder, 1985; Kinney, 1993). Group membership, in these terms, does not
necessarily equate with students’ friendship selections or with their actual
interaction patterns, but is related more to their status within the larger peer
group (Brown, 1990; Eder, 1985).
Social status- and intimacy-goal orientations also have been related
to the avoidance of help seeking. A status-goal orientation is similar to a
performance-goal orientation in that both concern maintaining a certain
image: A performance-goal orientation concerns a student’s image of aca-
demic ability, whereas a status-goal orientation concerns a student’s social
image or reputation. In both cases, there is a heightened awareness of the
self relative to others and a potential need to protect self-worth. Because
help seeking is a public behavior that has the potential to garner attention
and evaluation from one’s peers, students who have a status-goal orienta-
tion perceive help seeking as threatening to their self-worth and are more
likely to avoid seeking help when they need it (Ryan et al., 1997). In contrast,
P1: Vendor/GCQ/GCQ P2: GCV/FZN/LOV QC: FZN
Educational Psychology Review [jepr] PP043-292341 January 15, 2001 16:52 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
Avoidance of Help Seeking 99
an intimacy-goal orientation represents the desire to interact with and form
relationships with peers. Seeking help within the classroom provides an op-
portunity for legitimate peer interaction. The goal to form and maintain
positive peer relationships is negatively related to avoidance of help seek-
ing (Ryan et al., 1997). These findings parallel the findings for perceptions
of social competence and reinforce the proposition that social motivational
constructs are linked to help seeking and that help seeking needs to be con-
sidered both a self-regulatory strategy and a social interaction.
Achievement Level and Goal Orientations
The discussion to this point has focused on main effects of goal orien-
tations, but research suggests that it is important to consider interactive
effects of goal orientations and achievement level on avoidance of help
seeking. Ryan et al. (1997) found that the relation between adolescents’
performance-goal orientation and their help-seeking behavior depended
in part on their actual level of achievement. As grade point average de-
creased, the endorsement of a performance-goal orientation was related
to increased competence concerns regarding help seeking and increased
avoidance of help seeking. Thus, when students adopt a goal to outper-
form others but receive information that they are not achieving that goal,
they are particularly vulnerable to negative perceptions about help seek-
ing and are more likely to avoid it. These findings are in line with the re-
sults of an experimental study by Butler and Neuman (1995), which found
a similar interaction between a performance-goal orientation and initial
competence at solving puzzles. These findings are troubling but provide
insight as to which students in the classroom do not seek help when they
need it. First, lower achieving students feel more threatened and report
more avoidance of help seeking when needed than do higher achieving
students. Furthermore, when students who are lower achievers endorse a
performance-goal orientation, the discrepancies become even greater. The
very children who need help the most, seek it the least, and a performance-
goal orientation exacerbates the situation.
The finding that actual academic achievement moderates the impact of
performance-goal orientation on help-seeking attitudes and behaviors sug-
gests that a student’s social situation might moderate the impact of a social
status-goal orientation on help seeking. The desire to be popular might im-
pact help-seeking attitudes and behavior differently, depending on whether
students actually are liked and respected by their peers. An interesting ques-
tion for future research is whether students’ sociometric status would mod-
erate the impact of a social status-goal orientation on competence concerns
regarding help seeking and the avoidance of help seeking.
P1: Vendor/GCQ/GCQ P2: GCV/FZN/LOV QC: FZN
Educational Psychology Review [jepr] PP043-292341 January 15, 2001 16:52 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
100 Ryan, Pintrich, and Midgley
CLASSROOM CONTEXT, STUDENT MOTIVATION,
AND AVOIDANCE OF HELP SEEKING
It is also important to consider how the classroom context may influence
help seeking (Nelson-Le Gall, 1985; Newman, 1994, 1998; Ryan et al., 1998).
Teachers may set up environments that vary greatly in regards to fostering
or discouraging students’ help seeking. We discuss three dimensions of class-
rooms that are likely to affect the avoidance of help seeking: (1) classroom
rules and norms, (2) classroom goal structure, and (3) social/interpersonal
climate of the classroom.
Classroom Rules and Norms
The rules and norms teachers establish in the classroom are likely to
influence the opportunity structure for student help seeking. For example,
Karabenick and Sharma (1994) found that college students’ perceptions of
the amount of support the instructor provided in the class were linked to
students’ asking of questions, even when controlling for the students’ level
of confusion. In their model, teacher support included providing opportuni-
ties for question asking, specific instructions to the students on how to ask
questions, and reward–punishment contingencies (aspects of rules), as well
as teachers’ informational or procedural responses to questions, emotional
responses to questions, and the value placed on questions by the teacher
(aspects of norms).
In interviews, we conducted with seventh- and eighth-grade students,
they often referred to such rules and norms when asked about help seeking in
the classroom (Ryan, 1996). For example, students told us “Like if the teacher
is giving a lecture she doesn’t want you to raise your hand and start talking”;
“Usually there is a time (for questions) when, after she gives instructions”;
“Most of the time at the end of class if you really don’t understand something
you can go up and ask the teacher.” Such procedures may or may not be
explicitly stated, but students’ perceptions of such rules and norms are likely
to influence the timing and manner in which help is sought, as Karabenick
and Sharma (1994) showed for the college classroom.
In addition to rules and norms specific to help seeking, rules and norms
for completing tasks, participating in class, and interacting with students are
also likely to impact help seeking (Karabenick and Sharma, 1994; McCaslin
and Good, 1996). Students’ might be aware that they need help, but the
rules and norms of the environment will be important determinants of how
the student responds to this awareness. For example, some teachers allow
students to talk to others during seatwork to receive help. In such a class-
room, help seeking should be facilitated. In other classrooms, talking to other
P1: Vendor/GCQ/GCQ P2: GCV/FZN/LOV QC: FZN
Educational Psychology Review [jepr] PP043-292341 January 15, 2001 16:52 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
Avoidance of Help Seeking 101
students during seatwork may be prohibited by the classroom rules because
it is construed as cheating or as not doing your own work, and should con-
strain help-seeking behavior. Thus, help avoidance must be considered in
the context of such rules and norms. Classroom rules and norms interact
with students’ personal motivational characteristics to influence help seek-
ing. Rules and norms can be conceptualized in their own right and taken at
face value (i.e., “No talking during seatwork” means no talking during seat-
work and influences help seeking in an obvious way). However, the norms
and rules of classrooms also reflect other characteristics or dimensions of
classrooms that have been found to be important influences on students’
help-seeking beliefs and behaviors: the achievement goal structure and the
social/interpersonal climate.
Achievement Goal Structure in the Classroom
The classroom goal structure is communicated to students in many ways,
including the types of academic tasks they are given, how they are recog-
nized and evaluated, and how they are encouraged to do their work (Ames,
1992; Midgley, 1993). A mastery-goal structure communicates to students
that understanding, improvement, and the intrinsic value of learning are the
primary reasons for involvement in schoolwork. A performance-goal struc-
ture communicates to students that demonstrating their ability relative to
others is of primary importance in the classroom.
In a recent study (Ryan et al., 1998), we used multilevel analyses to inves-
tigate how the classroom goal structure related to avoidance of help seeking.
We examined differential patterns of help avoidance across 63 seventh-grade
math classrooms. First, students’ reported levels of avoidance of help seek-
ing did vary across classrooms. Thus, in some classrooms students reported
avoiding seeking help significantly more than in other classrooms. We then
explored whether this variablility in levels of help avoidance could be ex-
plained by the classroom goal structure. Students’ perception of a mastery
classroom goal structure was associated with a lower level of help avoid-
ance, whereas their perception of a performance classroom goal structure
was associated with a higher level of help avoidance. In classrooms where
students perceived that the focus was on understanding, mastery, and the
intrinsic value of learning—compared to classrooms where the focus was
on competition and proving one’s ability—students were less likely to avoid
seeking help with their work when they needed it. This finding was in line
with previous studies that have found that students’ personal goal orien-
tations are related to their help-seeking behavior (e.g., Ryan and Pintrich,
1997). However, this study provided evidence for contextual effects on the
avoidance of help seeking, which previous studies had not addressed.
P1: Vendor/GCQ/GCQ P2: GCV/FZN/LOV QC: FZN
Educational Psychology Review [jepr] PP043-292341 January 15, 2001 16:52 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
102 Ryan, Pintrich, and Midgley
Although the study did not examine help seeking in the classroom con-
text, Newman (1998) designed a microgenetic experimental study that ma-
nipulated mastery and performance goals to investigate how these manipu-
lated contextual goals interacted with elementary students’ personal mastery
and performance goals to predict help seeking and performance over time.
He found that there was an interaction between the contextual goals and
personal goals that related to help seeking. When personal and contextual
goals were in congruence and focused on performance concerns, students
were more likely to avoid seeking help. When the goals were incongruent, in
particular, when students who were personally more performance oriented
were placed in a mastery context, the context moderated their tendency to
avoid help seeking, and they sought more help. This study provides insights
about how personal goals and contextual-goal stresses can interact to in-
fluence help seeking. In addition, it highlights the need for more research
that examines how different contextual factors, including goal structures and
classroom rules and climate, might interact with personal characteristics of
students to influence help-seeking behavior.
Social/Interpersonal Climate of the Classroom
Help seeking combines aspects of social and cognitive engagement, in
that it is both a learning strategy and a social interaction with others (Nelson-
Le Gall, 1981, 1985). Previously, we discussed how various social constructs,
such as perceptions of social competence and social-goal orientations, are
related to help avoidance in the classroom. Familiarity as well as positive
relations increase the likelihood that a student will ask another person for
help (Nelson-Le Gall and Gumerman, 1984; Newman and Schwager, 1993;
Wintre et al., 1988). Specifically, Nelson-Le Gall and Gumerman (1984)
found that when children were presented with vignettes of problems they re-
ported that friendship was an important consideration when deciding whom
they would ask for help. Newman and Schwager (1993) found that students’
perceptions that that the teacher likes the student and that she likes the
teacher was related to students’ reports of help seeking in the classroom.
Additionally, when students perceive themselves as comfortable and skillful
in relating to others, they are more likely to seek help (Ryan and Pintrich,
1997). When they are concerned with their social image or status among
their peers, they are less likely to ask for help (Ryan et al., 1997).
Given the social nature of help seeking, it is likely that the social cli-
mate of the classroom influences students’ help-seeking behavior. Class-
rooms characterized as caring, supportive, and friendly are likely to make
students feel more comfortable interacting with the teacher and other stu-
dents. In an environment in which students feel that others know and relate
P1: Vendor/GCQ/GCQ P2: GCV/FZN/LOV QC: FZN
Educational Psychology Review [jepr] PP043-292341 January 15, 2001 16:52 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
Avoidance of Help Seeking 103
to them beyond their academic abilities, they may be less likely to feel that
asking for help will incur negative judgments. In a study investigating one di-
mension of the social climate in students’ help avoidance, Ryan et al. (1998)
found that the teacher’s concern about the social and emotional well-being
of their students was related to less help avoidance, particularly for students
who doubted their ability to successfully do their academic work. As we
discussed previously, prior research has found that students who have low
self-efficacy for their academic work are more likely to feel threatened by
their need for help and to avoid help seeking than are students who have
high self-efficacy for their academic work. However, Ryan et al. (1998) found
that this relation between academic self-efficacy and help avoidance varied
across classrooms. In some classrooms, there was a more equitable distribu-
tion of help seeking by students with varying levels of efficacy. Importantly,
the teacher’s concern about social–emotional nurturing lessened the relation
between efficacy and help avoidance. Thus, the troubling situation where
low-efficacy adolescents avoid seeking help more than do their high efficacy
peers was ameliorated when teachers were concerned with their students’
social–emotional needs.
DIRECTIONS FOR FUTURE RESEARCH
Our research has explored help avoidance among young adolescent stu-
dents. Developmental characteristics of young adolescent students include
increased self-consciousness and sensitivity to social comparison, increased
desire for autonomy, and increased need for positive and supportive relation-
ships with both peers and teachers (Eccles et al., 1993; Eccles and Midgley,
1989; Nicholls, 1990). Our findings are in line with a consideration of young
adolescents’ developmental needs. Regarding sensitivity to social compar-
ison, we discussed how a goal orientation toward performing the best and
demonstrating ability, both at the individual and the classroom level, exacer-
bates competence concerns and help avoidance. Regarding increased need
for positive social relationships, we discussed how students’ social status-goal
orientations as well as the perception that one lacks social competence in-
crease competence concerns and help avoidance. Further, the social climate
of the classroom is important for understanding students’ help avoidance.
However, consideration of the developmental needs of young ado-
lescents also suggests important avenues for future research. In this sec-
tion, we discuss three potentially fruitful areas for future research into
young adolescents’ avoidance of help seeking. First, regarding young ado-
lescents’ increased sensitivity regarding social comparison, new develop-
ments in achievement goal theory that distinguishes between approach
and avoidance-goal orientations might add to our understanding of goal
P1: Vendor/GCQ/GCQ P2: GCV/FZN/LOV QC: FZN
Educational Psychology Review [jepr] PP043-292341 January 15, 2001 16:52 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
104 Ryan, Pintrich, and Midgley
orientations and help avoidance. Second, regarding young adolescents’ in-
creased desire for autonomy, further exploration of autonomy concerns that
adolescents have about asking for help might further our understanding
of their avoidance of help seeking. Third, regarding young adolescents’ in-
creased need for positive relationships with peers and teachers, further anal-
ysis of the social climate of classrooms might build on our finding that teach-
ers’ concern with the social–emotional well-being of their students relates
to help avoidance in the classroom.
The Role of Avoidance-Goal Orientations
Research investigating help avoidance has predominantly examined
the role of approach-goal orientations. However, as described in this special
issue, recently there has been theory and research to suggest that mastery-
and performance-goal orientations are not sufficient to explain the diver-
sity of findings in the goal orientation literature. Elliot and his colleagues
(Elliot, 1997; Elliot and Church, 1997; Elliot and Harackiewicz, 1996;
Harackiewicz et al., 1997; Harackiewicz et al., 1998) as well as others
(Middleton and Midgley, 1997; Skaalvik, 1997) suggested that a distinction
between approach and avoidance performance-goal orientations is impor-
tant. With an approach performance-goal orientation, students are con-
cerned about demonstrating their ability, looking smart, or outperforming
others. In contrast, with an avoidance performance-goal orientation, stu-
dents are concerned with avoiding negative judgments of their ability or
competence, in effect, trying to avoid looking dumb or stupid. This work has
suggested that although an approach performance orientation is sometimes
associated with maladaptive patterns of learning, it may also be associated
with some positive outcomes. In contrast, an avoidance performance orien-
tation is associated with maladaptive outcomes with no evidence of positive
effects (Midgley et al., in press; Pintrich, 2000b).
In terms of help seeking, an avoidance performance-goal orientation
is likely to be important to avoidant behaviors like the avoidance of help
seeking (Middleton and Midgley, 1997). Indeed, the goal to avoid negative
judgments about ability resonates with competence concerns about help
seeking (i.e., “I worry about asking for help because others will think I
am dumb”). However, students who are concerned about avoiding looking
dumb may be willing to seek “covert” help, that is help from others (such
as a teacher) as long as other students are not able to see the help seeking
and giving. In contrast, students who are focused on approach performance
goals and who are trying to be the best students in the class may not seek
any kind of help at all, covert or other, given that they do not want anyone
to think they are less able. Middleton and Midgley (1997) included both the
P1: Vendor/GCQ/GCQ P2: GCV/FZN/LOV QC: FZN
Educational Psychology Review [jepr] PP043-292341 January 15, 2001 16:52 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
Avoidance of Help Seeking 105
approach and the avoidance components of performance goal orientations in
a study examining several student outcomes, including avoiding help seeking.
The avoidance component of performance-goal orientations emerged as a
predictor of avoiding help seeking, whereas the approach component did
not, when both were included in the same model. Additional research on
approach and avoidance performance-goal orientations and different help-
seeking behaviors and attitudes would add to our understanding.
There have been proposals to extend the approach–avoidance distinc-
tion to mastery goals (cf., Elliot, 1999; Pintrich, 2000a,b). In an approach
mastery-goal orientation, students are focused on mastery, self-improve-
ment, and progress; in effect, approaching or going toward mastery. Pintrich
(2000a,b) has suggested that there may be an avoid mastery-goal orientation
where students are trying to avoid not mastering the task or to avoid mis-
understanding the task. There is no empirical support for an avoid mastery
goal yet, but it seems reasonable that students could be focused on avoiding
misunderstanding or on not mastering the task, without concern for how
others are doing. For example, there may be “perfectionistic” students who
are not concerned with how others are doing or with looking stupid or dumb,
but who are only concerned with not doing the task incorrectly.
This distinction might be useful in predicting differential help avoid-
ance behaviors. Whereas an approach mastery-goal orientation ameliorates
concerns about competence and decreases avoidance of help seeking, a fo-
cus on avoiding misunderstanding—and a determination to not get some-
thing wrong because of high standards for oneself—might engender con-
cerns about competence that would lead students to avoid help seeking. On
the other hand, because an avoidance mastery goal does not reflect a con-
cern about others, these students might be willing to ask for “dependent”
help to avoid doing the task or work improperly or incorrectly. Again, fu-
ture research needs to examine the potential existence of an avoid mastery
goal as well as the possibility of differential relations between approach and
avoidance mastery goal orientations and the avoidance of help seeking.
The approach–avoidance distinction between achievement-goal orien-
tations might also be interesting to explore with social-goal orientations.
We have found that students who are concerned with garnering social pres-
tige and a “popular” reputation are more threatened by help seeking and
are more likely to avoid help seeking. However, there might very well be
students who are most concerned with avoiding any damage to their social
reputation (e.g., those who don’t want to be seen with unpopular students).
In more colloquial terms, this is the distinction between the desire to be
socially successful (i.e., “cool” or “popular”) and the dread of being socially
awkward (i.e., “dork” or “geek”). During adolescence, a time character-
ized by increased self-consciousness and egocentrism that leads individuals
P1: Vendor/GCQ/GCQ P2: GCV/FZN/LOV QC: FZN
Educational Psychology Review [jepr] PP043-292341 January 15, 2001 16:52 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
106 Ryan, Pintrich, and Midgley
to believe their behavior is the focus of everyone else’s attention, such an
avoidance-goal orientation might foster avoidant behaviors in the classroom.
Future research should address these potential relations with help seeking
that might be fostered by different approach and avoidance orientations to
social goals.
The Role of Autonomy Concerns
Predominantly, our research has focused on students’ reluctance to seek
help because of competence concerns, that is, the perception that help seek-
ing conveys to others that one lacks ability. This approach has been fruitful
in understanding help avoidance. Some young adolescent students worry
about “looking dumb” when asking for help, and this is a strong predictor
of reported help avoidance (Ryan and Pintrich, 1997). However, in addition
to competence concerns, autonomy concerns underlie the decision to avoid
seeking help (Butler, 1998). The perception of help seeking as a dependent
behavior competes with the desire for autonomy and impedes students’ ask-
ing for help when they need it. In a sample of young adolescents, Butler
(1998) found that autonomy reasons for help avoidance were more strongly
endorsed than competence concerns did.
In addition to investigating different reasons for help avoidance, Butler
(1998) also put students into experimental situations and observed their
help-seeking behavior. Interestingly, students who had endorsed autonomy
reasons for avoiding help seeking were more likely to request adaptive type
help when solving problems. This suggests that if help avoidance is born
out of a desire to be autonomous, then when students ultimately do seek
help, they are going to ask for the type of help that will support their own
autonomy in the future. In contrast, students who had endorsed competence
reasons for avoiding help seeking were more likely to avoid help seeking in
solving problems (Butler, 1998).
Current explanations of the links between autonomy and competence
concerns regarding help seeking and goal orientations are incomplete. Speci-
fically, competence concerns have been conceptualized as judgments about
ability (relative to others) (Butler and Neuman, 1995; Butler, 1998; Ryan
et al., 1997; Ryan and Pintrich, 1997) and autonomy concerns have been
conceptualized as (reflecting a) striving for independent mastery (Butler
and Neuman, 1995; Butler, 1998; Newman, 1990). Given these conceptual-
izations, it is not surprising that researchers have linked a performance-goal
orientation with competence concerns (Ryan et al., 1997; Ryan and Pintrich,
1997) and a mastery-goal orientation with autonomy concerns (Butler and
Neuman, 1995). We suggest that the reasons for help avoidance (concerns
about autonomy and concerns about competence) may be conceptualized
P1: Vendor/GCQ/GCQ P2: GCV/FZN/LOV QC: FZN
Educational Psychology Review [jepr] PP043-292341 January 15, 2001 16:52 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
Avoidance of Help Seeking 107
Fig. 1. Reasons for help avoidance.
along the lines of both a mastery-goal orientation and a performance-goal
orientation (see Fig. 1). That is, concerns for autonomy and competence
should not be considered one dimension but rather distinguished as be-
ing driven by a mastery-goal orientation or driven by a performance-goal
orientation.
Avoiding help seeking for reasons related to a desire for autonomy
could be quite different depending on students’ goal orientation. Students
might desire autonomy because they believe they will learn more if they
persist on the task independently. This is congruent with a mastery-goal
orientation, which concerns a focus on developing competence with self-
referential standards and defining success as when something new is learned
(the top left cell in Fig. 1). Students might also desire autonomy because
they want to be self-reliant and they believe completing a task independently
means they are more capable. This is congruent with a performance approach
goal orientation, which focuses on demonstrating competence and defines
success as when one garners positive judgments about one’s ability relative
to others (the bottom left cell in Fig. 1). Thus, the difference in these cells is
illustrated by a student thinking “I wouldn’t ask for help because I’ll learn
more if I do it alone” (mastery-goal orientation/autonomy concerns) vs.
“I wouldn’t ask for help because I want to show that I can do it myself”
(performance-goal orientation/autonomy concerns).
Similarly, avoiding help seeking for reasons related to threat to compe-
tence could be quite different in a way that relates to achievement-goal ori-
entation. Students might construe help seeking as evidence that they didn’t
learn what they were supposed to, that they didn’t master the task. This is
P1: Vendor/GCQ/GCQ P2: GCV/FZN/LOV QC: FZN
Educational Psychology Review [jepr] PP043-292341 January 15, 2001 16:52 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
108 Ryan, Pintrich, and Midgley
congruent with a mastery-goal orientation, when a student is focused on
gaining understanding and mastery and avoiding misunderstanding or on
not mastering the task (top right cell in Fig. 1). This is quite different than
students who are threatened by help seeking because they see it as evidence
that they lack ability and that others will think they are dumb or stupid if
they ask for help. Such concerns resonate with a performance-goal orienta-
tion to demonstrate competence relative to other students (bottom right cell
in Fig. 1). The difference in these cells is a student thinking “I wouldn’t ask
for help because it would mean that I don’t understand the task” (mastery-
goal orientation/competence concerns) vs. “I wouldn’t ask for help because
it would make me look stupid” (performance-goal orientation/competence
concerns).
The proposed framework needs to be tested as extant research on rea-
sons for help avoidance and goal orientations has either examined only one
dimension of competence concerns (top right cell in Fig. 1) or grouped both
of the proposed dimensions of autonomy concerns together (top and bot-
tom cells in left column in Fig. 1) and discussed these autonomy concerns as
striving for independent mastery. For example, our research indicates that
students do experience competency concerns regarding seeking help (Ryan
and Pintrich, 1997). However, the items in the scale only asked about con-
cerns about relative ability (e.g., “I think other kids will think I am dumb
if I ask a question in math class”), and thus our knowledge is incomplete.
Further, distinctions in concerns about autonomy regarding help seeking
might resolve previous inconsistent findings regarding goal orientation and
autonomy concerns. In an experimental study comparing student explana-
tions for help avoidance in mastery and performance goal conditions, Butler
and Neuman (1995) found no differences in the provided reasons for help
avoidance when students were asked about their own behavior after the ex-
periment. However, when asked about another, hypothetical, student’s help
avoidance, students in the mastery condition were more likely to mention
autonomy concerns than were students in the performance condition. The
example of student statements about autonomy concerns—“I wanted to be
independent”—did not distinguish whether the student wanted to be inde-
pendent, to gain mastery, or to demonstrate self-reliance. Perhaps further
distinction between aspects of autonomy would show clearer relations to
goal orientations.
The proposed framework may be useful for understanding different
help-seeking behaviors in the classroom. As discussed previously, Butler’s
research on students’ reasons for help avoidance and students’ help-seeking
behaviors (Butler, 1998) has indicated that autonomy concerns lead to adap-
tive help-seeking behavior, and competence concerns lead to avoidance
of help seeking. Research on students’ goal orientations and help-seeking
P1: Vendor/GCQ/GCQ P2: GCV/FZN/LOV QC: FZN
Educational Psychology Review [jepr] PP043-292341 January 15, 2001 16:52 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
Avoidance of Help Seeking 109
behaviors has indicated that a mastery-goal orientation leads to adaptive
help-seeking behavior and a performance-goal orientation leads to the avoid-
ance of help-seeking behavior. Thus, predictions stemming from Butler’s
work on reasons for help avoidance (Butler, 1998) and the studies on goal
orientation (e.g., Ryan and Pintrich, 1997) converge regarding two of the cat-
egories. Students in the autonomous-mastery category (top left cell in Fig. 1)
would be more likely to ask for adaptive type help both to achieve mas-
tery goals and to ultimately achieve autonomy. Students in the competence-
performance category (bottom right cell in Fig. 1) would be more likely
to avoid help seeking both to achieve performance goals and to address
competence concerns. However, extant research yields different hypothe-
ses about what we might expect from two of the categories. Students in the
competence-mastery category (top right cell in Fig. 1) may ask for adap-
tive type help to achieve mastery goals or avoid seeking help to address
competence concerns. Would mastery goals or competence concerns pre-
vail? Students in the autonomous-performance category (bottom left cell in
Fig. 1) may ask for adaptive type help to ultimately achieve autonomy or
avoid seeking help to achieve performance goals. Would performance goals
or autonomy concerns prevail?
Recent developments in achievement goal theory distinguishing be-
tween approach and avoidance orientations may help clarify the differential
predictions for these two categories. In particular, an approach performance-
goal orientation (focus on demonstrating competence) may be particularly
salient to students in the autonomy-performance category, whereas an avoid-
ance performance-goal orientation (focus on not demonstrating incompe-
tence) may be particularly salient to students in the competence-perform-
ance group (bottom right cell, Fig. 1).
The Role of the Social Environment
Research has clearly shown that at the individual level, students’ so-
cial motivation (perceptions of competence and social-goal orientations)
and relationships (familiarity and positive relationships) impact students’
help-seeking behavior (Nelson-Le Gall and Gumerman, 1984; Newman and
Schwager, 1993; Ryan and Pintrich, 1997; Ryan et al., 1997; Wintre et al.,
1988). There is also some evidence that the social climate plays a role in
the avoidance of help seeking (Ryan et al., 1998). We need to know more
about how the social aspects of the classroom environment can support or
undermine students’ help-seeking behavior.
Teachers vary in the values that they communicate to students about
relating to and respecting peers (Ryan and Patrick, 1999). An observational
study of 19 classrooms found considerable variability in interactions among
P1: Vendor/GCQ/GCQ P2: GCV/FZN/LOV QC: FZN
Educational Psychology Review [jepr] PP043-292341 January 15, 2001 16:52 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
110 Ryan, Pintrich, and Midgley
students and between the teacher and students (Anderson et al., 1988). Some
classrooms were characterized by positive and comfortable relationships and
frequent prosocial and cooperative interactions, whereas other classrooms
had an unpleasant affective tone and frequent negative student interactions
such as bickering, criticism, and insults. Thus, important questions include (1)
What messages does the teacher send to students about their relationships
with other students in the classroom? (2) What are the norms and values
regarding student relationships in the classroom? (3) How do these norms
and values impact the avoidance of help seeking?
Norms and values that teachers might differentially promote concern
interaction and mutual respect with peers around tasks (Ryan and Patrick,
1999). Is it acceptable and encouraged for students to interact with their class-
mates on academic work? Is learning something one does independently or
collaboratively? What is acceptable behavior in responding to classmates’
ideas and efforts? Is there an awareness and valuing of positive, respectful
interactions between classmates that do not exclude some students or gen-
erate negative feelings toward any students? When students are encouraged
to interact and exchange ideas with each other during academic tasks they
should feel more efficacious because they have a greater array of resources
on which to draw than if they were only working individually. An emphasis
on mutual respect in the classroom should contribute to students’ feelings
of psychological safety and comfort, including low anxiety and low threat
regarding making mistakes. Thus, the promotion by teachers, and percep-
tion by students, that interaction with peers is integral to learning and that
mutual respect is valued are likely to decrease help avoidance.
CONCLUSION
When students do not ask for help when they need it, they run the risk
of undermining their learning and achievement. In this article, we have dis-
cussed a number of reasons why students avoid help seeking in the classroom.
Individual as well as contextual characteristics influence this avoidance be-
havior. Regarding individual characteristics, we discussed our findings re-
garding students’ perceptions of competence and goal orientations in the
academic as well as the social domain. Regarding contextual characteris-
tics, we discussed classroom rules as well as the achievement goal structure
and social or interpersonal climate of classrooms. Taken together, research
has accumulated significant information about which type of students avoid
help seeking and what circumstances enhance or diminish this behavior.
Students who feel insecure about their abilities—academically or relating
socially to other students—are more likely to avoid help seeking. Students
P1: Vendor/GCQ/GCQ P2: GCV/FZN/LOV QC: FZN
Educational Psychology Review [jepr] PP043-292341 January 15, 2001 16:52 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
Avoidance of Help Seeking 111
who are focused on their reputation—academic or social—are more likely
to avoid help seeking. In classrooms where teachers emphasize personal im-
provement and promote positive social relationships, concerns about help
seeking decrease and concerns about help avoidance decrease. In contrast,
in classrooms where teachers highlight ability comparisons among students,
concerns about help seeking increase and concerns about help avoidance
increase. However, there is still much psychologists do not know about the
avoidance of help seeking. We discussed three areas that future research
might address to make knowledge more complete: avoidance-goal orienta-
tions, autonomy concerns, and the social environment. By more fully ex-
plicating and integrating avoidance-goal orientations, autonomy issues, and
characteristics of the social climate of classrooms into current theories, re-
search will further enhance the understanding of help avoidance. Such re-
search has the potential to help teachers design classroom environments in
which students who need help can seek help when they need it.
REFERENCES
Ames, C. (1992). Classrooms: Goals, structures, and student motivation. J. Educ. Psychol. 84:
261–271.
Anderson, L. M., Stevens, D. D., Prawat, R. S., and Nickerson, J. (1988). Classroom task envi-
ronments and students’ task-related beliefs. Elementary School J. 88: 281–295.
Berndt, T. J. (1982). The features and effects of friendship in early adolescence. Child Devel.
53: 147–1460.
Brown, B. B. (1990). Peer groups and peer culture. In Feldman, S. S., and Elliott, G. R. (eds.),
At the Threshold: The Developing Adolescent, Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.
pp. 171–196.
Brown, B. B., Mory, M. S., and Kinney, D. (1994). Casting adolescent crowds in an relational
perspective: Caricature, channel, and context. In Montemayor, R., Adams, G. R., and
Gullotta, T. P. (eds.), Personal Relationships During Adolescence: Vol. 6. Advances in Ado-
lescent Development, Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA, pp. 123–167.
Butler, R. (1998). Determinants of help seeking: Relations between perceived reasons for
classroom help-avoidance and help-seeking behaviors in an experimental context. J. Educ.
Psychol. 90: 630–644.
Butler, R., and Neuman, O. (1995). Effects of task and ego achievement goals on help-seeking
behaviors and attitudes. J. Educ. Psychol. 87: 261–267.
Dillon, J. T. (1988). Questioning and Teaching: A Manual of Practice, New York Teachers
College.
Dillon, J. T. (1990). The Practice of Questioning, Routledge, New York.
Dweck, C. S. (1986). Motivational processing affecting learning. Am. Psychol. 41: 1040–1048.
Dweck, C. S. (1996). Social motivation: Goals and social-cognitive processes. A comment. In
Juvonen, J., and Wentzel, K. (eds.), Social Motivation: Understanding Children’s School
Adjustment, Cambridge University Press, New York, pp. 181–198.
Eccles, J., and Midgley, C. (1989). Stage/environment fit: Developmentally appropriate class-
rooms for early adolescents. In Ames, R. E., and Ames, C. (eds.), Research on Motivation
in Education, Academic, New York, Vol. 3, pp. 139–186.
Eccles, J., Midgley,C., Wigfield, A., Buchanan, C. M., Reuman, D.,Flanagan, C., and Maclver, D.
(1993). Development during adolescence: The impact of stage-environment fit on young
adolescents’ experience in schools and families. Am. Psychol. 48: 90–101.
P1: Vendor/GCQ/GCQ P2: GCV/FZN/LOV QC: FZN
Educational Psychology Review [jepr] PP043-292341 January 15, 2001 16:52 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
112 Ryan, Pintrich, and Midgley
Eder, D. (1985). The cycle of popularity: Interpersonal relations among female adolescents.
Sociol. Educ. 58: 154–165.
Elliot, A. J. (1997). Integrating the “classic” and “contemporary” approaches to achievement
motivation: A hierarchical model of approach and avoidance achievement motivation. In
Maehr, M. L., and Pintrich, P. R. (eds.), Advances in Motivation and Achievement,JAI
Press, Greenwich, CT, Vol. 10, pp. 143–179.
Elliot, A. J. (1999). Approach and avoidance motivation and achievement goals. Educational
Psychologist, 34: 169–189.
Elliot, A. J., and Church, M. (1997). A hierarchical model of approach and avoidance achieve-
ment motivation. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 72: 218–232.
Elliot, A. J., and Harackiewicz, J. M. (1996). Approach and avoidance achievement goals and
intrinsic motivation: A mediational analysis. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 70: 461–475.
Good, T. L., Slavings, R. L., Harel, K. H., and Emerson, H. (1987). Student passivity: A study
of question-asking in K-12 classrooms. Sociol. Educ. 60: 181–199.
Harackiewicz, J.M., Barron, K. E., Carter, S. M., Lehto, A. T., and Elliot, A. J. (1997). Predictors
and consequences of achievement goals in the college classroom: Maintaining interest and
making the grade. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 73: 1284–1295.
Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., and Elliot, A. J. (1998). Rethinking achievement goals:
When are they adaptive for college students and why? Educ. Psychol. 33: 1–21.
Hartup, W. W. (1989). Social relationships and their development significance. Am. Psychol.
44: 120–126.
Hicks, L. (1997). Academic motivation and peer relationships-how do they mix in an adoles-
cent’s world? Middle School J. 28: 18–22.
Karabenick, S. A., and Knapp, J. R. (1991). Relationship of academic help seeking to the use of
learning strategies and other achievement behavior in college students. J. Educ. Psychol.
83: 221–230.
Karabenick, S. A., and Sharma. R. (1994). Seeking academic assistance as a strategic learning
resource. In Pintrich, P. R., Brown, D. R., and Weinstein, C. E. (eds.), Student Motivation,
Cognition, and Learning: Essays in Honor of Wilbert J. McKeachie, Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 189–211.
Keating, D. P.(1990). Adolescent thinking. In Feldman, S.S., and Elliot, G.R. (eds.), At threshold:
The Developing Adolescent, Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 54–89.
Kinney, D. A. (1993). From “nerds” to “normals”: Adolescent identity recovery within a chang-
ing social system. Sociol. Educ. 66: 21–40.
Maehr, M. L., and Midgley, C. (1996). Transforming School Culture, Westview Press, Boulder,
CO.
Middleton, M. and Midgley, C. (1997). Avoiding the demonstration of the lack of ability: An
underexplored aspect of goal theory. J. Educ. Psychol. 89: 710–718.
Midgley, C. (1993). Motivation and Middle Level Schools. In Pintrich, P. R., and Maehr, M. L.
(eds.), Advances in Motivation and Achievement: Motivation and Adolescent Development,
JAI Press, Greenwich, CT, Vol. 8, pp. 191–217.
Midgley, C., Kaplan, A., and Middleton, M. (in press). Performance-approach goals: Good
for what, for whom, under what circumstances, and at what cost? Journal of Educational
Psychology.
McCaslin, M., and Good, T. L. (1996). The informal curriculum. In Berliner, D., and Calfee, R.
(eds.), Handbook of Educational Psychology, Macmillan, New York, pp. 622–670.
Myers, M., and Paris, S. G. (1978). Children’s metacognitive knowledge about reading. J. Educ.
Psychol. 70: 680–690.
Nelson-Le Gall, S. (1981). Help seeking: An understudied problem solving skill in children.
Dev. Rev. 1: 224–246.
Nelson-Le Gall, S. (1985). Help seeking behavior in learning. In Gordon, E. W. (ed.), Review
of Research in Education, American Educational Research Association, Washington, DC,
Vol. 12. pp. 55–90.
Nelson-Le Gall, S., and Gumerman, R. A. (1984). Children’s perceptions of helpers and helper
motivation. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 5: 1–12.
P1: Vendor/GCQ/GCQ P2: GCV/FZN/LOV QC: FZN
Educational Psychology Review [jepr] PP043-292341 January 15, 2001 16:52 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
Avoidance of Help Seeking 113
Newman, R. S. (1990). Children’shelp seeking in the classroom: The role of motivational factors
and attitudes. J. Educ. Psychol. 82: 71–80.
Newman, R. S. (1991). Goals and self-regulated learning: What motivates children to seek
academic help? In Maehr, M. L., and Pintrich, P. R. (eds.), Advances in Motivation and
Achievement: Vol. 7. Goals and Self-Regulatory Processes, JAI, Greenwich, CT, pp. 151–
183.
Newman, R. S. (1994). Adaptive help seeking: A strategy of self-regulated learning. In Schunk,
D., and Zimmerman, B. (eds.), Self-Regulation of Learning and Performance: Issues
and Educational Applications, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 283–
301.
Newman, R. S. (1998). Students’ help seeking during problem solving: Influences of personal
and contextual achievement goals. J. Educ. Psychol. 90: 644–658.
Newman, R. S., and Goldin, L. (1990). Children’s reluctance to seek help with schoolwork. J.
Educ. Psychol. 82: 92–100.
Newman, R. S., and Schwager, M. T. (1993). Student perceptions of the teacher and classmates
in relation to reported help seeking in math class. Elementary School J. 94: 3–17.
Nicholls, J. (1990). What is ability and why are we mindful of it? A developmental perspective.
In Sternberg, R., and Kolligian, J. (eds.), Competence Considered, Yale University Press,
New Haven, pp. 11–40.
Paris, S. G., and Newman, R. S. (1990). Developmental aspects of self-regulated learning. Educ.
Psychol. 25: 87–102.
Parker, J. G., and Asher, S. R. (1987). Peer relations and later personal adjustment: Are low-
accepted children “at risk”? Psychological Bulletin 102: 357–389.
Pintrich, P. R. (2000a). An achievement goal theory perspective on issues in motivation termi-
nology, theory, and research. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 25: 92–104.
Pintrich, P. R. (2000b). The role of goal orientation in self-regulated learning. In Boekaerts,
M., Pintrich, P. R., and Zeidner, M. (eds.), Handbook of Self-Regulation, Academic Press,
San Diego, CA, pp. 451–502.
Pintrich, P. R., and Schunk, D. H. (1996). Motivation in Education: Theory, Research, and
Applications, Merrill Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ.
Ryan, A. M. (1996). Interviews with adolescent students. Unpublished data.
Ryan, A. M., Gheen, M., and Midgley, C. (1998). Why do some students avoid asking for
help? An examination of the interplay among students’ academic efficacy, teacher’s social–
emotional role and classroom goal structure. J. Educ. Psychol. 90: 528–535.
Ryan, A. M., Hicks, L., and Midgley, C. (1997). Social goals, academic goals, and avoiding
seeking help in the classroom. J. Early Adolesc. 17: 152–171.
Ryan, A. M., and Midgley, C. (1998). Changes in students’ help seeking across the transition
to middle school. Paper presented at American Educational Research Association annual
meeting in San Diego.
Ryan, A. M., and Patrick, H. (in press). The classroom social environment and young adoles-
cents’ motivation and engagement in school. American Educational Research Journal.
Ryan, A. M., and Pintrich, P. R. (1997). Should I ask for help?: The role of motivation and
attitudes in adolescents’ help seeking in math class. J. Educ. Psychol. 89: 329–341.
Skaalvik, E. M. (1997). Self-enhancing and self-defeating ego orientation: Relations with task
and avoidance orientation, achievement, self-perceptions, and anxiety. Journal of Educa-
tional Psychology 89: 71–81.
Skinner, E. A., and Belmont, M. J. (1993). Motivation in the classroom: Reciprocal effects of
teacher behavior and student engagement across the school year. J. Educ. Psychol. 85:
571–581.
Trickett, E., and Moos, R. H. (1974). Personal correlates of contrasting environments: Student
satisfaction in high school classrooms. Am. J. Commun. Psychol. 2: 1–12.
Urdan, T.C., and Maehr, M. L. (1995). Beyond a two goal theory of motivation and achievement:
A case for social goals. Rev. Educ. Res. 65: 213–243.
Van der Meij, H. (1988). Constraints on question-asking in classrooms. J. Educ. Psychol. 80:
401–405.
P1: Vendor/GCQ/GCQ P2: GCV/FZN/LOV QC: FZN
Educational Psychology Review [jepr] PP043-292341 January 15, 2001 16:52 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
114 Ryan, Pintrich, and Midgley
Van der Meij, H. (1994). Student questioning: A componential analysis. Learn. Individual Diff.
6: 137–161.
Wentzel, K. R. (1989). Adolescent classroom goals, standards for performance, and academic
achievement: An interactionist perspective. J. Educ. Psychol. 81: 131–142.
Wintre, M. G., Hicks, R., McVey, G., and Fox, J. (1988). Age and sex differences in choice of
consultant for various types of problems. Child Dev. 59: 1046–1055.
Zimmerman, B. J., and Martinez-Pons, M. (1988). Construct validation of a strategy model of
student self-regulated learning. J. Educ. Psychol. 80: 284–290.
... There is extensive literature on when and how parents and teachers provide help (e.g., Cooper et al. 2000;Ng et al. 2004;Park et al. 2016;Pomerantz et al. 2007;van de Pol et al. 2010;Vygotsky and Cole 1978) and when and how children seek help (Butler and Neuman 1995;Cluver et al. 2013;De Cooke and Brownell 1995;Nelson-Le Gall 1987;Ryan et al. 2001;Selmeczy et al. 2023). A smaller literature has examined the psychological consequences of receiving help. ...
... Receiving help is a common experience for children (Cooper et al. 2000). In many cases, receiving help benefits children's academic and social development (Ryan et al. 2001;Zimmerman 2002). Often, however, children receive unsolicited help, which could have unintended consequences. ...
Article
Full-text available
What are the psychological consequences of receiving direct and indirect help in childhood? We conducted three preregistered experiments (N = 619, 7–9 years, 80% Dutch, 51% girls, 49% boys, mostly higher socioeconomic status) in the Netherlands (July 2020–July 2022). Children received direct help (correct answer), indirect help (hint), or no help. An internal meta‐analysis showed that children who received help felt less competent, liked the task less, and felt more in need of help. Children who received help also sought fewer challenges (Study 3). Effect sizes were modest. Direct and indirect help had largely similar effects, except that children disliked and misreported receiving direct help more. Thus, despite being well‐intentioned, direct and indirect help can be discouraging.
... Self-directed learning empowers learners to take initiative, set learning goals, and assess outcomes independently-enhancing learning quality (Knowles, INSECTA 2722-8495 1983). Help-seeking behavior supports teacher trainees in overcoming academic challenges (Karabenick & Knapp, 1991;Ryan, Pintrich, & Midgley, 2001). Together, these four aspects provide a framework for understanding how learners actively engage with and sustain their learning efforts. ...
... Finally, positive behavioral learning attitudes also emphasizes help-seeking behavior, which refers to teacher trainees' desire and ability to seek assistance when necessary (Newman, 1994). Proactive learning is an important social interaction in education (Vygotsky, 1978), and seeking help and self-regulation strengthen each other, leading to academic success (Karabenick & Knapp, 1991), while seeking help as a strategy helps overcome learning challenges and promotes deeper understanding (Ryan, Pintrich, & Midgley, 2001). ...
Article
Full-text available
Self-directed learning is significant to empower learners to take ownership of their learning, particularly in fostering positive behavioral learning attitudes, enhancing autonomy, and improving academic performance. This study examines its impact on teacher trainees' behavioral learning attitudes by: (1) analyzing changes after self-directed learning intervention, (2) assessing its overall impact, and (3) exploring trainees' perceptions of their experiences. In the research, the study involved 83 first-year teacher trainees. Data were collected through pre-and post-tests, a questionnaire, and focus group discussions. Quantitative data were analyzed using a Paired-Sample T-Test and Multiple Regression, while qualitative data underwent Thematic Analysis. Findings shown a significant improvement in behavioral learning attitudes following self-directed learning with Self-directed learning was found to be a significant predictor (B = .544, p < .001), indicating that higher levels of self-directed learning led to higher post-test scores in behavioral learning attitudes. The approach strongly influenced overall behavioral learning attitudes, as teacher trainees perceived it as enhancing their self-regulation, learning engagement, persistence, proactive learning, and help-seeking behavior. The study concludes that self-directed learning is an effective instructional approach for fostering positive behavioral attitudes. These findings suggest integrating self-directed learning model into teacher training programs to promote autonomy and engagement. Future research should examine its long-term effects on professional development.
... Whereas instrumental help seeking refers to a situation where learners seek the minimum amount of information needed to perform the task on their own. For example, students ask for hints, but find the solution to the problem by themselves (Karabenick, 2003;Ryan et al., 2001). This help-seeking strategy is adaptive for learning because the students are actively involved in the problem solving and the help just serves as extra input for deep processing (Mart ın-Arb os et al., 2021; Ryan et al., 1997). ...
... The finding revealed that students having enhanced social goals, specifically having a higher level of social intimacy goals in their peer interactions, have better engaged in help seeking behavior in the classroom. This is a possibility that students' social goals endorsed are associated with their interaction and willingness of seeking help from peers in the classroom (Altermatt, 2007;Roussel et al., 2011;Ryan et al., 2001). Given the social nature of help seeking behavior, social goal orientations are more likely to clarify students help seeking behavior in the classroom (Kiefer & Shim, 2016;Ryan & Shin, 2011). ...
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this study was to examine the role of students' social goal orientation and academic help seeking behavior on their mathematics achievement. Data were collected from 930 Grade 8 primary school students in Ethiopia. The data were analyzed using the Smart-PLS software, specifically the partial least squares structural equation modeling (PLS-SEM) technique. Confirmatory factor analyses confirmed the expected 2-factor structure of social goal orientation and the 3-factor structure of academic help seeking behavior. The results of structural equation modeling analyses indicated that (a) social goal orientation positively predicted both academic help-seeking behavior and mathematics achievement, and (b) academic help seeking behavior had a significant positive impact on mathematics achievement. The research highlights the importance of social goal orientations and academic help seeking behavior in order to understand students' behavior and achievement in school. Moreover, the result confirmed that students’ academic help seeking behavior is more academically related behavior, which boosts learning and achievement. Therefore, the results provide evidence to suggest that researchers and practitioners should focus on improving students' social goal orientation and academic help-seeking behavior, which, in turn, would enhance their academic achievement.
... On the other hand, Caemmerer and Keith 16 & DiPerna and Elliott 17 suggested that social skills indirectly influence academic achievement by allowing students to more successfully work with peers, ask questions, listen, and attend to the classroom environment. Additionally, some studies have also evaluated the importance of social skills in order to explain students' help seeking behavior 18,19 . However, still less research has established the nature of the relation between these constructs to enhance students' academic achievement. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study aimed to examine the mediating role of students’ academic help-seeking behavior in explaining the relationship between students’ social skills as motivational orientation and mathematics achievement among primary schools of Grade 8 students. Data were collected from 930 students among nine primary schools at the Dessie city administration in Ethiopia. The participant students were selected using systematic random sampling technique among selected schools. In line with the research objective, the research used correlational design in order to explain the relationship among variables. SPSS 24.0 and Smart PLS 4 were used for data analysis. The result of this study shows a significant direct positive effect of social skill on academic help-seeking behavior and mathematics achievement. Moreover, academic help-seeking behavior demonstrates a direct positive effect on mathematics achievement and plays a partial mediating role in the relationship between social skill and mathematics achievement. Thus, the findings indicated that the students’ academic help-seeking behaviors are part of the mechanism through which social skill affects students’ mathematics achievement. Accordingly, the research suggests the importance of comprehensive interventions to enhance students’ academic achievement.
... Numerosos estudiantes desaprovechan oportunidades de preguntar porque lo consideran inapropiado o sienten vergüenza de hacerlo, cometiendo errores que podrían salvar. La necesidad de ayuda puede percibirse como una evidencia de incompetencia y generar reacciones negativas basadas en los juicios que otros podrían tener (Karabenick, 2003;Ryan, Pintrich, & Midgley, 2001). ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction.Instrumental adaptation is essential to ensure that items are understandable, relevant, and culturally appropriate in the context in which they are applied.Objective.To linguistically and culturally adapt the Spanish version of the Test Anxiety Inventory (TAI) and the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES) into a virtual format. Additionally, to examine their psychometric properties through reliability analysis, and exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis.Materials and Methods.A non-experimental survey design was used with a sample of 359 students aged 17 to 44 from the National University of Asunción, enrolled in the Psychology, Communication Sciences, Literature, and History programs. Of the participants, 66,4% were female; the mean age ± standard deviation was 22 ± 5,26 years. The linguistic comprehension test was conducted with 18 students from the UNA Medicine program, and a preliminary evaluation was performed by an expert committee consisting of two local professors in Spanish language and literature, who verified semantic, idiomatic, and conceptual equivalence to Paraguayan Spanish.Results.The TAI obtained a Cronbach’s alpha of 0,895 and showed a multidimensional structure with four factors. The Self-Esteem Scale obtained a Cronbach’s alpha of 0,884 and a two-factor structure. The sample showed a high level of test anxiety, while the self-esteem level was moderate. According to Pearson’s coefficient, the correlation between the two variables was extremely weak.Conclusion.Both instruments demonstrated sufficient empirical evidence; their use is recommended in Paraguayan university populations.
... Butler (1998) noted that self-regulated learning does not exclude support and assistance from outside, nor does each student necessarily accomplish their task independently. It is important to seek advice from others, but still stay abreast of being autonomous (Ryan et al., 2001). Continuous feedback from the teachers and peers is necessary. ...
Article
Full-text available
Reading comprehension is a crucial component of academic life, especially for students majoring in English. However, EFL students face various challenges in their learning, including, for example, the interpretation of reading materials and a lack of motivation. In this respect, students could utilize self-regulated learning (SRL) strategies to increase their learning autonomy and motivation, which are believed to contribute to academic success. Given that Vietnamese EFL learners also apply SRL strategies, this study aims to investigate the degree to which English majors employ SRL strategies in reading. The study also attempts to examine the most and least frequently used strategies and the differences, if any, between the two genders regarding SRL strategy use. 130 English-majored students from a university in the Mekong Delta were involved in the study. The quantitative data were gathered using a closed-ended questionnaire adapted from Duong and Ta (2022), and a follow-up interview for qualitative data was conducted with five students randomly chosen from the participants. The findings revealed that students generally employed SRL strategies to enhance reading comprehension, with different levels across the eight subcategories of strategy. In addition, a statistical test showed no significant difference between the male and female students regarding their implementation of SRL strategies. These results suggest pedagogical implications for teachers and learners, emphasizing the importance of SRL strategies in developing reading skills. Thus, teachers are expected to raise students' awareness and practical employment of SRL to achieve their academic goals.
... Recent work in multi-agent AIF (Maisto et al., 2023) formalizes communication as reciprocal belief alignment: teachers and students synchronize their generative models (internal representations of each other's knowledge states) through iterative dialogue, mirroring longitudinal co-adaptation in classrooms. This approach could model how misconceptions persist despite corrective feedback (e.g., belief inertia due to strong priors (Nyhan and Reifler, 2010)) and provide a principled account of social inhibition (Ryan et al., 2001), where students avoid questions to minimize interpersonal prediction errors. ...
Preprint
Human education transcends mere knowledge transfer, it relies on co-adaptation dynamics -- the mutual adjustment of teaching and learning strategies between agents. Despite its centrality, computational models of co-adaptive teacher-student interactions (T-SI) remain underdeveloped. We argue that this gap impedes Educational Science in testing and scaling contextual insights across diverse settings, and limits the potential of Machine Learning systems, which struggle to emulate and adaptively support human learning processes. To address this, we present a computational T-SI model that integrates contextual insights on human education into a testable framework. We use the model to evaluate diverse T-SI strategies in a realistic synthetic classroom setting, simulating student groups with unequal access to sensory information. Results show that strategies incorporating co-adaptation principles (e.g., bidirectional agency) outperform unilateral approaches (i.e., where only the teacher or the student is active), improving the learning outcomes for all learning types. Beyond the testing and scaling of context-dependent educational insights, our model enables hypothesis generation in controlled yet adaptable environments. This work bridges non-computational theories of human education with scalable, inclusive AI in Education systems, providing a foundation for equitable technologies that dynamically adapt to learner needs.
Article
Due to the increasing academic pressure, it is almost impossible for college students to flourish without seeking any help from others. Yet, how individuals’ beliefs about stress impact help-seeking and its outcomes lacks adequate attention. Despite a growing recognition of help-seeking, less is known about whether help-seeking always benefits students under pressure, and how different stress mindsets motivate help-seeking in different ways. We adopt the two-dimensional theory of help-seeking to highlight how stress mindsets influence academic behaviors via help-seeking. A three-wave survey of 488 students in China revealed that: (1) Autonomous help-seeking (AHS) enhances academic engagement and inhibits academic burnout, whereas dependent help-seeking (DHS) exacerbates academic burnout and weakens academic engagement. (2) Stress-is-enhancing mindset (SIEM) drives AHS, and stress-is-debilitating mindset (SIDM) drives DHS. (3) SIEM contributes to academic engagement via AHS, and SIDM contributes to academic burnout via DHS. By examining the distinctive drivers of two-dimensional help-seeking and the differential impact of help-seeking on academic behaviors, our study seeks to build on previous research by using help-seeking to develop a new theoretical model of stress mindsets and academic behaviors, theorizing and testing multiple antecedents and consequences associated with the bright and dark sides of help-seeking. In doing so, our research reveals the paradoxical mediation effects of help-seeking between stress mindsets and academic behaviors, providing a more in-depth depiction of the nature of help-seeking that advances the understanding of the existing literature.
Article
This study investigates the dynamics of peer-to-peer support exchange (P2P SE) in non-monetized sharing economies. Utilizing a scenario-based within-subjects experiment with 93 participants across nine conditions, we examined how support request visibility and recognition for making requests affect perceived social costs of seeking help. We also explored the interplay between these social costs, community belonging, and confidence in giving and getting support in shaping future participation intentions. Our findings revealed that participants often leveraged different strategies to mitigate social costs of seeking support, such as justifying needs to ask for help, using apologetic language, and offering direct reciprocation or compensation, diverging from the ideals of non-monetized P2P SE. However, we found that receiving recognition for making requests, particularly from helpers, effectively reduces social cost concerns. We also discovered that interventions to strengthen a sense of community belonging and confidence in giving support should be prioritized to enhance future participation intentions. Our study offers insights for designing platforms to enhance participation and achieve critical mass in non-monetized sharing economies, addressing key psychological barriers and leveraging facilitators of P2P SE.
Article
Sociologists analyse ongoing disparities in college enrolment between students from disadvantaged and advantaged backgrounds in the United States. They emphasise the need for access to college information and support for effective navigation such as academic help-seeking. However, discussions rarely address how social-emotional competencies could influence academic help-seeking behaviours, especially across different school environments. Scholars in social-emotional learning (SEL) explore how these competencies impact academic outcomes, potentially reducing the achievement gap, especially with widespread SEL programs. Yet, literature lacks extensive examination of discussed internal processes driving students’ academic help-seeking behaviours for college information during their college transition. This study examines the relationship between students’ social-emotional competence in their 10th grade and academic help-seeking behaviours for college information in school in their senior year (N = 9,093). It then reveals the disproportionate effects in different school contexts. Students’ social and emotional competencies (e.g. self-efficacy; engagement; sociopolitical commitment; peer relationships; teacher–student relationships) predict their academic help-seeking for college information in school. The association differs by school sector and location, which lends insight into how educators can contextualise school-based SEL programs. The paper discusses the role of schools in facilitating SEL programs in college transition, particularly for applicants from disadvantaged backgrounds.
Article
Full-text available
Although most individuals pass through adolescence without excessively high levels of "storm and stress," many do experience difficulty. Why? Is there something unique about this developmental period that puts adolescents at risk for difficulty? This article focuses on this question and advances the hypothesis that some of the negative psychological changes associated with adolescent development result from a mismatch between the needs of developing adolescents and the opportunities afforded them by their social environments. It provides examples of how this mismatch develops in the school and in the home and how it is linked to negative age-related changes in early adolescents' motivation and self-perceptions. Ways in which more developmentally appropriate social environments can be created are discussed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
On the basis of a new model of motivation, we examined the effects of 3 dimensions of teacher (n = 14) behavior (involvement, structure, and autonomy support) on 144 children's (Grades 3-5) behavioral and emotional engagement across a school year. Correlational and path analyses revealed that teacher involvement was central to children's experiences in the classroom and that teacher provision of both autonomy support and optimal structure predicted children's motivation across the school year. Reciprocal effects of student motivation on teacher behavior were also found. Students who showed higher initial behavioral engagement received subsequently more of all 3 teacher behaviors. These findings suggest that students who are behaviorally disengaged receive teacher responses that should further undermine their motivation. The importance of the student-teacher relationship, especially interpersonal involvement, in optimizing student motivation is highlighted.
Article
Full-text available
The present study investigated motivational influences on help-seeking behavior in math classrooms, focusing on early adolescents' perceptions of the benefits and threats associated with such behavior. Seventh and 8th graders (N = 203) responded to a questionnaire on perceptions of social and cognitive competence, achievement goals, attitudes, and avoidance of and adaptive help-seeking behavior in math class. Both threats and benefits were important influences on avoidance of help-seeking behavior, whereas only benefits predicted adaptive help seeking. Findings indicated that perceived threats and benefits partially mediated the effects of relative ability goals, task-focused goals, extrinsic goals, and perceptions of cognitive competence on avoidance of help seeking. Perceived benefits partially mediated the effects of task-focused goals on adaptive help seeking. Social competence had an indirect effect on avoidance of help seeking. Results illustrate the importance of linking cognitive, motivational, and social characteristics of students to provide a fuller understanding of adolescent help seeking in math.
Article
Full-text available
The authors investigated personality predictors of achievement goals in an introductory psychology class, as well as the consequences of these goals for the motivation and performance of 311 undergraduates. Two dimensions of achievement motivation (workmastery and competitive orientations; J. T. Spence & R. L. Helmreich, 1983) predicted the goals endorsed. Individuals high in workmastery were more likely to adopt mastery goals and less likely to adopt work avoidance goals, whereas competitive individuals were more likely to endorse performance and work avoidance goals. Students adopting mastery goals were more interested in the class, but students adopting performance goals achieved higher levels of performance. These results suggest that both mastery and performance goals can lead to important positive outcomes in college classes.
Article
Full-text available
The types of goals that students adopt in educational settings and the consequences of those goals on 2 important educational outcomes - performance and intrinsic motivation - are discussed. In the case of performance, we briefly review and evaluate a large body of theory and research conducted by other investigators. In particular, we consider the possibility that some commonly accepted conclusions about the effects of achievement goals are premature. In the case of intrinsic motivation, we describe a theoretical model that has guided our own work on this topic and provide some recent experimental results. We believe that this model and our experimental results can contribute to a more comprehensive understanding of goals and optimal motivation. Finally, we return to the college classroom environment and examine the consequences of goals for both performance and intrinsic motivation, offering a broader analysis of success in college courses.
Chapter
How are children's social lives at school related to their motivation to achieve and how do motivational and social processes interact to explain children's adjustment at school? This volume, first published in 1990, features work by leading researchers in educational and developmental psychology and provides perspectives on how and why children tend to thrive or fail at school. The individual chapters examine the unique roles of peers and teachers in communicating and reinforcing school-related attitudes, expectations, and definitions of self. Relations of children's school adjustment to school motivation, interpersonal functioning, and social skillfulness are also explored. The developmental and social perspectives on motivation and achievement presented in this volume provide new insights into the complex processes contributing to school success.