ArticlePDF Available

Evidence that photos promote rosiness for claims about the future

Authors:

Abstract

When people rapidly judge the truth of claims about the present or the past, a related but nonprobative photo can produce “truthiness,” an increase in the perceived truth of those claims (Newman, Garry, Bernstein, Kantner, & Lindsay, 2012). What we do not know is the extent to which nonprobative photos cause truthiness for the future. We addressed this issue in four experiments. In each experiment, people judged the truth of claims that the price of certain commodities (such as manganese) would increase (or decrease). Half of the time, subjects saw a photo of the commodity paired with the claim. Experiments 1A and 1B produced a “rosiness” bias: Photos led people to believe positive claims about the future but had very little effect on people’s belief in negative claims. In Experiment 2, rosiness occurred for both close and distant future claims. In Experiments 3A and 3B, we tested whether rosiness was tied to the perceived positivity of a claim. Finally, in Experiments 4A and 4B, we tested the rosiness hypothesis and found that rosiness was unique to claims about the future: When people made the same judgments about the past, photos produced the usual truthiness pattern for both positive and negative claims. Considered all together, our data fit with the idea that photos may operate as hypothesis-confirming evidence for people’s tendency to anticipate rosy future outcomes.
Evidence that photos promote rosiness for claims about the future
Eryn J. Newman
1,2
&Tanj e e m A z a d
3
&D. Stephen Lindsay
3
&Maryanne Garry
1
#Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2016
Abstract When people rapidly judge the truth of claims
about the present or the past, a related but nonprobative photo
can produce Btruthiness,^an increase in the perceived truth of
those claims (Newman, Garry, Bernstein, Kantner, & Lindsay,
2012). What we do not know is the extent to which
nonprobative photos cause truthiness for the future. We ad-
dressed this issue in four experiments. In each experiment,
people judged the truth of claims that the price of certain
commodities (such as manganese) would increase (or de-
crease). Half of the time, subjects saw a photo of the commod-
ity paired with the claim. Experiments 1Aand1Bproduceda
Brosiness^bias: Photos led people to believe positive claims
about the future but had very little effect on peoplesbeliefin
negative claims. In Experiment 2, rosiness occurred for both
close and distant future claims. In Experiments 3Aand3B, we
tested whether rosiness was tied to the perceived positivity of
a claim. Finally, in Experiments 4A and 4B, we tested the
rosiness hypothesis and found that rosiness was unique to
claims about the future: When people made the same judg-
ments about the past, photos produced the usual truthiness
pattern for both positive and negative claims. Considered all
together, our data fit with the idea that photos may operate as
hypothesis-confirming evidence for peoplestendencytoan-
ticipate rosy future outcomes.
Keywords Truthiness .Photographs .Fluency .Future
thinking
Peoples predictions about the future are colored by cognitive
biases. In the face of uncertainty, we often ignore base rates,
overvalue resemblances, and overrely on our emotions.
Perhaps the best known of these pervasive biases hinges on
cognitive availability: Future scenarios seem more likely if,
for example, they are easy to imagine (Jacoby, Kelley, &
Dywan, 1989; Tversky & Kahneman, 1974; see also Alter
& Oppenheimer, 2009, for the similar notion of Bretrieval
fluency^). The contents of the imagined scenario are likely
nonprobative: They probably do not reliably reveal the likeli-
hood that the scenario will actually occur. But people do
sometimes use the ease with which they can imagine or think
about a scenario as a gauge to its likelihood (Szpunar &
Schacter, 2013; see also Unkelbach, 2006,2007). In one
study, people tended to think they were more likely to contract
a disease with easy-to-imagine symptoms (headache or low
energy) than one with difficult-to-imagine symptoms
(inflamed liver or a malfunctioning nervous system;
Sherman, Cialdini, Schwartzman, & Reynolds, 1985). In an-
other study, people on average expressed more interest in vis-
iting travel destinations that were easier to imagine (Petrova &
Cialdini, 2005). When people can easily imagine
nonprobative information about the future, they often mistak-
enly construe that information and the ease of simulating that
future event as probative. And so, manipulations that help
people imagine a future scenario more easily should make that
future seem more likely. Photographs are especially powerful
tools that help people imagine. To what extent can
*Eryn J. Newman
erynnewm@usc.edu
1
Victoria University of Wellington, Wellington, New Zealand
2
Mind and Society Center, Department of Psychology, University of
Southern California, Los Angeles, CA 90089-1061, USA
3
University of Victoria, Victoria, British Columbia, Canada
DOI 10.3758/s13421-016-0652-5
Published online: 19 September 2016
(2018) 46:12231233
Mem Cogn
photographs make future scenarios seem true? That is the
question we addressed here.
Photographs promote ease of imagination
We know that photographseven nonprobative ones, which
do not provide any evidence that an event has happenedcan
lead people to falsely remember their past. In one study, sub-
jects heard three descriptions of childhood events, two true
and one false (Lindsay, Hagen, Read, Wade, & Garry, 2004).
The false description was about the subject and a friend sneak-
ing Slimethe gooey, sticky childrenstoyinto their grade
school teachers desk and later getting in trouble. The sub-
jectstask was to recall as much as possible about each event.
Over a week, one group received a verbal suggestions, but
another group received the suggestions plus a copy of that
school-years class photo (supplied to the researchers by the
subjectsparents). By the end of the week, 45 % of the
Bsuggestion^people reported something about the Slime ep-
isode, as compared with 78 % of the Bsuggestion plus photo^
people. Moreover, two-thirds of those who also saw the photo
were judged by raters to have reported false memories that
were marked by high confidence (see BlandónGitlin,
Pezdek, Lindsay, & Hagen, 2009, for a content analysis of
true and suggested events).
We can consider these findings in the language of the
source-monitoring framework (Johnson, Hashtroudi, &
Lindsay, 1993; Lindsay, 2008,2014). Source monitoring
helps us understand how people decide whether a particular
mental event really happened, and from what source it came:
Is it something one simply imagined, or something one really
experienced? When mental products such as thoughts, im-
ages, and feelings come to mind easily, are full of sensory
detailsuch as vivid, concrete imagesand leave little re-
cord of cognitive operations that might otherwise point to an
internal origin, people typically attribute those mental prod-
ucts to real experience. The results from Lindsay and col-
leagues(2004) findings fit with the idea thatthe nonprobative
class photo made it easier for subjects to generate thoughts and
images about themselves, their friends, their teacher, and the
event itself. Coupled with a bias to seek confirming informa-
tion (Nickerson, 1998) and a gradual loss of source memory
over the week-long procedure (see Lindsay, 2008), subjects
may have construed these mental products as evidence that the
suggested event really happened.
We also know from recent work that nonprobative photos
can more rapidly nudge people to find associated claims true
(Fenn, Newman, Pezdek, & Garry, 2013; Newman, Garry,
Bernstein, Kantner, & Lindsay, 2012). In this paradigm, peo-
ple quickly judge a series of trivia claims (The liquid metal
inside a thermometer is magnesium)astrueorfalse.
Sometimes the claims appear with a nonprobative photo (such
as a thermometer), but other times the claims appear alone.
The general finding is that when people make rapid judgments
about the truth of a claim, nonprobative but relevant photos
nudge them toward believing that claiman effect known as
Btruthiness.^This Btruthiness^research, too, fits with the idea
that nonprobative photos help people generate thoughts and
images related to a claim. In turn, a bias to confirm a claim
may encourage people to construe these mental products and
the ease of generating them as evidence that the claim is true
(Newman et al., 2012;Newmanetal.,2015). Truthiness re-
search also suggests that nonprobative photos bias judgments
about the past and present by what seem to be similar process-
esprocesses like those we know cause future scenarios to
seem true.
To investigate the degree to which nonprobative photo-
graphs produce truthiness for future scenarios, we asked the
people in our present experiments to make judgments about
each of a large set of commodities, such as gabardine, ben-
zene, and thyme. For each commodity, one group quickly
responded Btrue^or Bfalse^to the scenario BThis commodity
is likely to have increased in price three months from today.^
To address the possible counterexplanation that people might
think that commodities with photos are more valuable, and
thus would increase in price, we asked another group to re-
spond to the scenario BThis commodity is likely to have de-
creased in price three months from today.^In both conditions,
half of the scenarios appeared with a photo of the commodity.
On the basis of our review of the memory and future judg-
ments literatures, we might expect that nonprobative photos
would produce truthiness for the future. Indeed, the source-
monitoringframework accommodates the idea that people use
similar approaches when judging past and present outcomes to
decide the likelihood that a particular future event will really
happen. That is, when thoughts, images, and feelings about
future events come to mind easily, are full of sensory details,
and are low on markers of cognitive operations, people typi-
cally decide that the future event is more likely to really hap-
pen (see Johnson & Sherman, 1990, and Schacter, Addis, &
Buckner, 2007, for related views). Thus, we might expect that
nonprobative photos would operate in similar ways for judg-
ments about the past, present, and future, making related
thoughts, feelings, and images spring to mind easily and serv-
ing as evidence for claims regardless of the direction of time.
On the other hand, we might expect that photos would not
influence peoples judgments about the future. After all,
photos record the past, not the future. Although it makes sense
that people may gather evidence from a photo to make judg-
ments about a past event, it makes much less sense to gather
evidence from a photo to make judgments about a future
event.
Below we report how we determined our sample sizes, all
data exclusions, all manipulations, and all measures used in
our experiments. Decisions about sample size and measures
(2018) 46:12231233
Mem Cogn
1224
were made prior to the data analysis. The data (both the initial
raw data and the more processed data files) from the studies
reported here are posted on the first authors ResearchGate
profile, as are copies of the materials and of the programs used
to present them.
Experiment 1A and 1B
Method
Subjects In Experiment 1A, 81 students from Victoria
University of Wellington participated in the study for course
credit. In Experiment 1B, 76 students from the University of
Victoria participated in the study for optional bonus points. If
the effect sizes of photos are comparable for past and future
judgments, then on the basis of our prior research, the effects
should have similar precisions when Ns were in the mid 70s,
so that is what we set out to test; the final Ns varied depending
on no-show rates (Newman et al., 2012).
Design We used a 2 (Photo: yes, no) × 2 (Claim: increase,
decrease) mixed design, manipulating the presence of a photo
within subjects.
Materials and procedure We told subjects that they would
seeaseriesofcommodities(Braw materials or goods that have
some value and are often used to make other products^). Their
task was to decide, for each commodity, whether the claim
BThis commodity is likely to have increased in price three
months from today^was true or false. Half of the subjects
made the same decision for the claim BThis commodity is
likely to have decreased in price three months from today.^
To reduce a potential tendency to assume that prices typically
increase over time, we told subjects that commodity prices
often increase and decrease week to week and month to
month.
Subjects saw 30 commodities, presented one at a time
using the Superlab software. We developed this list by
selecting nouns from the MRC Psycholinguistic word data-
base (http://websites.psychology.uwa.edu.
au/school/MRCDatabase/uwa_mrc.htm) that, given our
broad definition, are plausible commodities. We selected
nouns that fell at the lower end of the familiarity scale and
the upper end of the imagery scale (see the supplementary
materials for our list of nouns), so although the nouns were
unfamiliar, they were relatively easy to picture if you knew
what they were, and were easily represented in a photo
(familiarity: M= 343.37, SD =71.01,range=199442;
imagery: M=492.57,SD = 72.62, range = 402627;
database range = 100700).
We told subjects that sometimes they would see a photo
with the commodity and sometimes they would not, and we
showed them gold as an example in both the photo and no-
photo versions. The photos always depicted the commodity,
and all of the photos used in the experiments reported here can
be found on the first authors ResearchGate profile. We did not
provide any instructions about how subjects should use the
photos; instead, we said Bdont try to analyze and puzzle
things out, just go with your gut feel or hunch,^and that they
should decide the truth of the claim as quickly as possible,
within a couple of seconds. The names of the commodities
appeared one at a time, in large black font against a white
background. Half of these names appeared with a photo
depicting the commodity (see Fig. 1). The order of commod-
ities was randomized anew for each subject and
counterbalanced so that each commodity appeared equally
often with and without a photo.
In Experiment 1B we replicated Experiment 1A, except
that we increased the number of commodities to 68 and broad-
ened the range of familiarity and imagery (familiarity: M=
330.79, SD = 127.79, range = 103518; imagery: M= 430.49,
SD = 145.79, range = 162649; database range = 100700).
Results and discussion
Our primary aim was to determine the extent to which
nonprobative photographs can make future scenarios seem
true. To answer this question, we calculated the proportion
of times each subject said claims were true, grouped those
responses according to whether those scenarios had appeared
with or without a nonprobative photo; the results are displayed
in Fig. 2. The black bars show the responses to commodities
that appeared with photos; the gray bars show the responses
when commodities appeared alone. In addition, the left bars
show the pattern of responses for the scenario in which com-
modities would increase in price, and the right bars show the
pattern for the scenario in which commodities would decrease
in price.
As the figure shows, nonprobative photos produced
truthiness for the future. That is, relative to the no-photo con-
dition, subjects responded Btrue^more often to future scenar-
ios that appeared with a photo, F(1, 79) = 4.61, p= .03, mean
difference = .05 (i.e., a 5 % raw effect size), 95 % confidence
interval (CI) [.004, .09]. The photo effect in Experiment 1A
was small (and the lower bound of the CI very close to 0), so it
is reassuring that a photo effect was also obtained in
Experiment 1B, F(1, 74) = 16.23, p< .01, mean difference =
.07, 95 % CI [.03, .10]. In Experiment 1B, we also found a
marginal tendency toward a main effect of claim, suggesting
that people more often agreed with the Bincrease^claim than
with the Bdecrease^claim, F(1, 74) = 4.03, p= .05, mean
difference = .06, 95 % CI [.004, .12]. This trend suggests a
bias toward assuming that prices tend to increase. No signifi-
cant Photo × Claim interaction emerged in either Experiment
1Aor1B, both Fs < 1.32, but the pattern suggests that photos
(2018) 46:12231233
Mem Cogn 1225
exerted more of an influence on the Bincrease^than on the
Bdecrease^claim (a pattern that we replicated in subsequent
experiments). Taken together, our findings fit with recent re-
search showing that nonprobative photos can promote the
truthiness of associated claims (Fenn et al., 2013; Newman
et al., 2012;Newmanetal.,2015)
Did subjects have any insight into the way that photos
biased them? When we asked subjects at the end of
Experiment 1A how the photographs influenced their deci-
sions,
1
52 % of the people said the photographs helped them
understand what the commodity was, 10 % told us that the
photo helped them imagine the commodity, and only 10 %
reported that the photo added credibility to the claim (another
28 % said that the photo did nothing or gave a different ex-
planation, such as that the photo made them respond more
slowly). Thus, the majority of subjects reported that the photos
boosted their understanding or helped them picture the com-
moditya belief that fits with a fluency account in which
subjects may have misattributed the ease of understanding or
imagining the commodity as a sign that the claim was true
(Jacoby, Woloshyn, & Kelley, 1989;Schwarz,2010).
Indeed, we know that other manipulations that add context
information, such as words that help describe a target, can lead
people to conclude that an associated claim is true. For in-
stance, a claim about Nick Cave seems more true when his
name is accompanied by words such as Bwhite male, musi-
cian, microphone^perhaps because the words (although
nonprobative) help people understand who he is, and this pro-
duces a feeling of conceptual fluency (Newman et al., 2012;
Whittlesea, 1993).
Although we found that nonprobative photos produced
truthiness for the future, there are at least two reasons to expect
that photos would not influence judgments about a more im-
mediate future. First, events that are closer in time are more
detailed and concrete, and thus easier for people to bring to
mind and imagine (Alter & Oppenheimer, 2008;Hansen&
Wänke, 2010; Trope & Liberman, 2003,2010). Perhaps when
claims are close in time an accompanying photo would not
add anything because people would find the claim easy to
imagine and comprehend (Schwarz, 2010; Sherman et al.,
1985;Unkelbach,2007). Second, in the context of the com-
modity market, people may have an implicit belief that prices
do not vary within a matter of weeks. If so, then we might
predict that photos would not influence peoples beliefs about
claims concerning near future relative to distant future events.
Experiment 2
Method
Subjects In Experiment 2, we used the Amazon Mechanical
Turk (MTurk; www.mturk.com)torecruit200subjects.
2
Because of a quirk in the way MTurk assigns subject slots,
184 subjects completed the experiment (similar deviations
follow in the other experiments we report here). These
people received a $0.60 Amazon credit.
Design We used a 2 (Photo: yes, no) × 2 (Temporal distance:
near, far) × 2 (Claim; increase, decrease) mixed design, ma-
nipulating the presence of a photo within subjects.
Procedure We used the same method and materials as in
Experiment 1B, except that we altered the claims so that our
subjects responded to a claim set in the near or the distant
future. People either responded to the distant claim that
BThis commodity is likely to have increased [decreased] in
price three months from today^or a temporally close claim
that BThis commodity is likely to have increased [decreased]
in price three weeks from today.^This time manipulation is
within the range commonly used in the mental-construal liter-
ature (for reviews, see Trope & Liberman, 2003,2010). We
counterbalanced so that people responded to the three months/
three weeks and increase/decrease claims equally often, and
each commodity equally often appeared with and without a
photo.
1
We had 68 responses to this follow-up question.
2
We determined the Non the basis of previous research with photos and
past judgments on Mturk (e.g., Newman et al., 2015).
Fig. 1 Example of commodity names appearing either with or without a photo. Photo credit: http://notanminerals.com/Pr-Manganese.html#sthash.
ozPLy1of.dpuf
(2018) 46:12231233
Mem Cogn
1226
Results and discussion
As Fig. 3shows, photos produced truthiness for both near and
far future claims, F(1, 180) = 13.88, p< .01, mean difference =
.04, 95 % CI [.02, .07]. Moreover, people thought claims
about the far future were more likely to be true than near future
claims; indeed, people tended to disbelieve claims set in the
near future (the bars on the left side of the graph all fall below
50 %), a finding that fits with the idea that people expect less
fluctuation over short intervals, F(1, 180) = 12.99, p<.01,
mean difference = .09, 95 % CI [.14, .04,]. As in the
marginal tendency observed in Experiment 1B, we found that
people agreed more with the increase than with the Bdecrease^
claim, F(1, 180) = 5.08, p= .03, mean difference = .06, 95 %
CI [.01, .11]. Finally, the pattern in which the photos exerted a
directionally stronger influence on the Bincrease^claim in
Experiment 1A emerged as a significant interaction in this
experiment: We found significant truthiness for the Bincrease^
claim, F(1, 180) = 15.03, p< .01, mean difference
Incr
=.08,
95 % CI [.05, .11], but not for the Bdecrease^claim, mean
difference
Decr
=.002,95%CI[.03, .03].
Taken together, these data suggest that nonprobative photos
can promote the truthiness of claims about a future price in-
crease, even when people judge claims about the more imme-
diate future and when people have a low belief in the claims
they are judging (Fig. 3shows that people in the three-weeks
condition had lower belief in all of the claims). Put another
way, photos can also shift peoples belief in claims they tend to
think are false.
The finding that photos encouraged more truthiness for the
Bincrease^claim fits with two lines of research. First, it fits
with research showing that people tend to see the future
through Brose-colored^lenses: That is, they generate positive
future scenarios more quickly and remember imagined posi-
tive future scenarios better than negative ones (Newby-Clark
& Ross, 2003; Szpunar, Addis, & Schacter, 2012). Second, it
fits with research showing that people often interpret an expe-
rience of easy processing in a positive wayand so, a feeling
of fluency increases judgments of liking but not of disliking
and boosts evaluations of beauty but not of ugliness (Reber,
Schwarz, & Winkielman, 2004; Winkielman, Schwarz, Reber,
& Fazendeiro, 2003; see also Harmon-Jones & Allen, 2001,
and cf. Westerman, Lanska, & Olds, 2015). These fluent ex-
periences are sometimes accompanied by physiological
markers of positive affect (Reber, Winkielman, & Schwarz,
1998; Seamon, McKenna, & Binder, 1998; Topolinski,
Likowski, Weyers, & Strack, 2008;Winkielman&
Cacioppo, 2001).
These ideas suggest that photos of commodities may have
given rise to a positive experience of processing fluency that
in turn enhanced peoples tendency to view the future as rosy.
It may be that subjects made their judgments from the per-
spective of a buyer, rather than a seller, and hence tended to
evaluate future price increases as positive. This explanation
could account for the tendency in Experiment 1B and both
conditions of Experiment 2for the effect of photos to be larger
for Bincrease^than for Bdecrease^judgments. In Experiments
3A and B, we examined this positivity explanation more di-
rectly. In Experiment 3A, we simply asked people whether
they thought the Bincrease^and Bdecrease^claims felt positive
or negative. In Experiment 3B, we took a different approach
and altered the claims so that the interpretation of the
Fig. 3 Experiment 2: Proportions of Btrue^responses for claims that
commodities would increase or decrease in price in three weeks or three
months, presented with or without photos. Error bars represent 95 %
within-subjects confidence intervals.
Fig. 2 Experiments 1A (top) and 1B (bottom): Proportions of Btrue^
responses for claims that commodities would increase or decrease in
price, presented with or without photos. Error bars represent 95 %
within-subjects confidence intervals.
(2018) 46:12231233
Mem Cogn 1227
Bincrease^or Bdecrease^claims was clearly positive (making
a profit) or negative (making a loss) for the subject. If we
found that photos influence judgments for Bprofit^but not
for Bloss^claims, this would be better evidence that the influ-
ence of photos was tied to positive/rosy future claims.
Experiment 3A
Method
Subjects Again, we used MTurk to recruit 253 subjects.
3
Each
subject received $0.10 Amazon credit for participating in this
shorter experiment.
Design We used a one-way between-subjects design (Claim:
increase, decrease).
Procedure The procedure was similar to that of Experiment
1Bsubjects saw the Bincrease^or the Bdecrease^claim, and
then made judgments about whether that claim was true or
false for each commodity that appeared on the screenexcept
that subjects only evaluated ten commodities, selected at ran-
dom from the full set of 68. The commodities task served to
give subjects a flavor of the judgment task. After subjects had
completed the ten true/false judgments, they were asked to
rate the affective valence of the claim they had just judged
from 1 = negative to 6 = positive.
Results and discussion
We classified peoples responses according to whether they
responded to the Bincrease^or Bdecrease^claim. We found
that subjects rated the Bincrease^claim as more positive (M=
4.56, 95 % CI [4.36, 4.76]) than the Bdecrease^claim (M=
3.39, 95 % CI [3.18, 3.59]), t(250) = 7.99, p< .001, mean
difference = 1.17, 95 % CI of the difference [0.89, 1.46], a
pattern consistent with the idea that photos promote truth for
rosy future claims.
Experiment 3B
Method
Subjects We used MTurk to recruit 220 subjects. Each sub-
jects received $0.60 Amazon credit for participating.
Design We used a 2 (Photo: yes, no) × 2 (Relevance: self-
relevant, not self-relevant) × 2 (Price: up, down) mixed de-
sign, manipulating the presence of a photo within subjects.
Procedure We used the same method and materials as in
Experiment 2, except that we altered the claims so that half
the subjects evaluated a claim that was personally relevant.
That is, people either responded to the standard claim, BThis
commodity is likely to have increased [decreased] in price
three months from today^or to a self-relevant claim, BIf I
buy this commodity today, then three months from now I will
have made a profit [loss].^We counterbalanced so that people
responded to the profit/loss and increase/decrease claims
equally often, and each commodity appeared equally often
with and without a photo.
Results and discussion
As Fig. 4shows, we replicated the finding that photos signif-
icantly promoted truthiness for positive but not for negative
claims. We also found that this pattern held for self-relevant
claims, which is a stronger test of our positivity hypothesis.
Although people responded Btrue^equally often to the
Bdecrease^and Bloss^claims, they responded Btrue^more
to the Bincrease^than to the Bprofit^claim. That is, we ob-
served a Photo × Price interaction, F(1, 216) = 51.10, p<.01
(photo vs. no photo: mean difference
Pos
= .11, 95 % CI [.08,
.13]; mean difference
Neg
=.03, 95 % CI [.06, .01]), and a
significant Relevance × Price interaction, F(1, 216) = 7.47, p=
.01 (truth ratings for self-relevant vs. not self-relevant: mean
difference
Pos
=.08, 95 % CI [.13, .02]; mean difference
Neg
=.04,95%CI[.03, .10]). We found no other interactions, all
Fs<1.
The data from Experiments 3Aand3B support the idea
that nonprobative photos promote rosiness.And
Experiment 3B shows that photos increased the truth of
positive (but not of negative) future outcomes. This may
be due to a mechanism whereby photos facilitate peoples
bias to think of positive future events, but these data also
fit with a general effect in the fluency literature, in which
fluency tends to increase judgments about positive but not
about negative attributes (e.g., Reber et al., 2004;
Winkielman et al., 2003). So it is possible that photos
make positive future claims seem true because of the gen-
eral tendency to experience fluency as positive, rather
than because of a rose-colored bias in future thinking. If
it is the case that photos enhance positive claims about the
future largely because people interpret fluency in a posi-
tive way, then we should see a similar pattern for claims
about the past. To address this hypothesis, in Experiment
4we asked people to make judgments about both the
future and the past.
3
We posted 200 subject slots, but because they were in multiple batches,
more subjects participated than anticipated.
(2018) 46:12231233
Mem Cogn
1228
Experiments 4A and 4B
Method
Subjects We usedMTurk to recruit 218 subjects. Each subject
received $0.60 Amazon credit for participating.
Design We used a 2 (Photo: yes, no) × 2 (Time: past, future) ×
2 (Price: worth more, worth less) mixed design, manipulating
the presence of a photo within subjects.
Procedure We used the same method and materials as in
Experiment 2, except that we altered the claims slightly so
that subjects either responded to claims about the future
(e.g., BThree months from now this commodity will be worth
more [less] than it is today^) or about the past (e.g., BThree
months ago this commodity was worth more [less] than it is
today^). We counterbalanced so that people responded to the
past/future and Bworth more^/Bworth less^claims equally of-
ten, and each commodity appeared equally often with and
without a photo.
Results and discussion
As Fig. 5shows, we replicated the finding that for claims
about the future, nonprobative photos promoted rosiness.
But for claims about the past, truthiness was unrelated to va-
lence: That is, photos promoted the truthiness for both positive
and negative claims.
We observed a Photo × Time × Price interaction, F(1, 214)
=5.66,p= .02 (future claims: mean difference
More
=.08,95%
CI [.03, .12], mean difference
Less
= .01, 95 % CI [.03, .05];
past claims: mean difference
More
= .06, 95 % CI [.02, .10],
mean difference
Less
= .09, 95 % CI [.05, .13]). We found no
other interactions, all Fs < 1. Considered together, these data
suggest that something isindeed special about futureclaims
that photos produce rosiness for judgments about the future,
but general truthiness for claims about the past.
Because this pattern with the past claims was novel and
different from our findings in Experiments 13,wereplicated
Experiment 4A. We used the same materials and increased our
MTurk sample size by a factor of 3. In Experiment 4B, we
found the same pattern of resultsincluding, most notably,
the key Photo × Time × Price interaction, F(1, 668) = 8.34, p<
.01 (future claims: mean difference
More
= .11, 95 % CI [.08,
.13], mean difference
Less
= .01, 95 % CI [.03, .04]; past
claims: mean difference
More
= .07, 95 % CI [.05, .09], mean
difference
Less
= .04, 95 % CI [.02, .07]).
We then used the data from Experiments 4A and 4Bto
arrive at a more precise estimate of the truthiness effect sizes
for both the Bworth more^and Bworth less^claims when set in
the future or the past. We conducted two random-effects
Fig. 4 Experiment 3B: Proportions of Btrue^responses for positive and negative claims about commodities that were either self-relevant or not self-
relevant, presented with or without photos. Error bars represent 95 % within-subjects confidence intervals.
Fig. 5 Experiment 4A: Proportions of Btrue^responses for positive and
negative commodity claims set in the future or the past, presented with or
without photos. Error bars represent 95 % within-subjects confidence
intervals.
(2018) 46:12231233
Mem Cogn 1229
model mini-meta-analyses (Cumming, 2012), and display
those data in Fig. 6. The top part of the figure shows the first
analysis, which focused on the Bworth more^claims. We com-
pared the effects for future and past truthiness, and calculated
estimated raw effect sizes of 0.10, 95 % CI [0.08, 0.12], for the
future and 0.07, 95 % CI [0.05, 0.09], for the past. As Fig. 6
shows, the truthiness for the future was trivially larger: esti-
mated raw difference 0.03, 95 % CI [0.06, 0.005].
The bottom part of the figure shows the second analysis,
which focused on the Bworth less^claim. We again compared
the truthiness effect sizes for future and past claims, and cal-
culated estimated raw effect sizes of 0.01, 95 % CI [0.03,
0.05], for the future, and 0.06, 95 % CI [0.03, 0.10], for the
past. As Fig. 6shows, the estimated raw effect sizes for future
truthiness plausibly include the possibility that photos do not
promote truthiness, whereas the estimate for the past condition
does not. In addition, the overall difference between the sizes
of the photo effects across the future and past conditions
shows that there is plausibly no difference, 0.05, 95 % CI [
0.01, 0.10].
General discussion
Photos, like memory, are a record of the past. But just as recent
work has shown that we draw on the past to imagine the future
(Addis, Wong, & Schacter, 2007; Suddendorf & Corballis,
2007; Szpunar, 2010; Szpunar, Watson, & McDermott,
2007; cf. Johnson & Sherman, 1990), we found that people
drew on photos to predict the future. Across seven experi-
ments, our data tell a consistent story: Photos promoted rosi-
ness for future events. This finding fits with a growing body of
work showing that people tend to see future events through
rose-colored lenses (Sharot, Riccardi, Raio, & Phelps, 2007;
Szpunar et al., 2012;Taylor&Brown,1988; Weinstein, 1980;
cf. Szpunar & Schacter, 2013). If people are biased to antici-
pate positive future outcomes, it makes sense that they would
use photos as hypothesis-confirming evidence for positive but
not for negative claims (see the confirmation bias; e.g.,
Nickerson, 1998). Indeed, in each experiment, the confidence
intervals of the effect sizes for the negative future claims in-
cluded zero as a plausible value (with the exception of Exp.
3B, in which a negative or close-to-zero effect was plausible).
Although our results are consistent with the literature on
cognitive fluency, mental construal, and the source-
monitoring framework (Alter & Oppenheimer, 2009;
Hansen & Wänke, 2010; Johnson et al., 1993;Lindsay,
2008), there are alternative explanations for our findings.
One possibility is that photos did not boost fluency nor foster
the ability to imagine a claim, but that subjects used a rule in
which they assumed that photos lent credibility to the claims.
But our results do not square with this account: If people
suspected that photos lent credibility to a claim, we should
have seen truthiness for both the Bincrease^and Bdecrease^
claims. We did not see that pattern; photos had the most pro-
nounced effect on positive claims, and a negligible effect on
negative claims. Only when we asked people to make judg-
ments about the past did photos produce truthiness for both
types of claims. Moreover, when we asked people at the end
of Experiment 1A about how the photos may have influenced
their answers, the large majority of our subjects reported that
the photo helped them understand what each commodity
wasa finding that is consistent with the idea that photos
made it easier for people to imagine the claim at hand.
A second alternative account of our effects is the possibility
that the photos somehow made the commodities seem more
valuable. Such a mechanism might explain why we saw an
increase in perceived truth for the positive and not for the
negative claims. There are several reasons why we do not
think the value mechanism can explain our findings. First,
our photos simply depicted the commodity on its own, with-
out packaging or advertising (e.g., a hunk of raw manganese),
and thus should not have conveyed information about its val-
ue. Second, if photos were a cue to value, we should have seen
Fig. 6 Forest plot of the photo effect sizes between conditions and across
experiments (Derzon & Alford, 2013). The top panel of the plot displays
the effects for photos inthe Bworth more^condition. The bottom panel of
the plot displays the effects for photos in the Bworth less^condition. The
location of each shape on the horizontal axis represents the raw effect
sizethe difference between peoplesBtrue^responses when they saw
claims paired with a photo versus when they saw claims without photos.
The lines extending on either side of a shape represent 95 % confidence
intervals(CIs; notethat a 95 % CI for a mean difference that excludes zero
will be significantly different from zero using null hypothesis significance
testing). Finally, within each panel on the plot, data that fall to the right of
the zero line show that people were inclined to say Btrue^more often
when they saw a photoBrosiness^for future claims, and Btruthiness^
for past claims.
(2018) 46:12231233
Mem Cogn
1230
that photos increased belief in the positive claims and de-
creased belief in the negative claims. That is, people should
have taken the photos as evidence against claims that the
commodity would decrease in price. Instead, we found that
photos increased belief in positive claims and that they did
very little to influence peoples judgments about negative
claims. Finally, we found that when people made judgments
about the past, photos increased their belief in both positive
and negative claimsa pattern that is also inconsistent with
the idea that photos signal value.
Our data raise another intriguing question: Why is it
that photos produced similar effects for both positive and
negative past claims? We think there are at least two pos-
sibilities. First, we know that people think about the past
in a flexible way. We recall events as being either better
or worse than they really were, and we can come to re-
member both positive and negative events that never ac-
tually happened (Hyman & Pentland, 1996;Loftus&
Pickrell, 1995; Ross & Wilson, 2003; Schryer & Ross,
2012;Wadeetal.,2002). Second, that we detected
truthiness for the past may be a consequence of the ways
that the claims were worded. That is, it could be that the
photos inflated Byes^responses to questions about both
past increases and past decreases because subjects had
difficulty interpreting the implications of past increases
versus decreases.
A future increase in the price of a commodity represents a
gain, and a future decrease inits price represents a loss, both of
which seems naturally and intuitively positive or negative
(higher price = gain = good; lower price = loss = bad). In
contrast, if a commodity used to be worth less when you
bought it than it is today, that represents a gain, but it seems
less natural and intuitive (lower price = gain = good).
Likewise if it used to be worth more, that represents a loss
(higher price = loss = bad). So, it is possible that the conse-
quences of the past claims (whether you gained or lost money,
whether the outcome was good or bad) were difficult to extract
for subjects while they were making these quick judgments. If
so, that might have made the Bincrease^and Bdecrease^judg-
ments affectively comparable, and that, in turn, might have set
the stage for the truthiness pattern in which photos inflate
Byes^responses to both alternatives.
This pattern for past claims squares with an earlier finding
in which photos inflated Btrue^responses to two alternative
claimseither that Bthis famous person is alive^or that Bthis
famous person is dead^(Newman et al., 2012). In both of
these experiments, the apparent valence of the claim did not
alter the effect of photos. In contrast, yet-to-be published data
that are part of a thesis by Cardwell (2015), show that when
valence is described in a concrete way, past claims look more
like future claims. For instance, when people were asked to
evaluate relatively concrete claims about the past, such as
Bthis wine was rated [high] low in quality,^photos produced
truthiness for positive but not for negative claims (Cardwell,
2015). Further investigation of this intriguing pattern for past
claims will be an interesting avenue for future research.
Might photos influence high-stakes, real-world judgments
about financial investments? That is an empirical question, but
there is evidence that important financial decisions can be
sensitive to tangential factors such as ease of processing. For
example, Alter and Oppenheimer (2006)showedthatstock
names and ticker labels that are easy to pronounce tended to
perform better (especially in the short term) than those that are
difficult to pronounce. So, even when real money is involved,
surface-level features that logically should not bear on peo-
ples judgments sometimes do just that. These findings sug-
gest to us that our photorosiness effect may indeed have real-
world implications.
Across several experiments, nonprobative photos encour-
aged subjects to believe in positive claims about commodity
fluctuations. One of the many questions these findings raise is
whether we would see effects of rosiness for more autobio-
graphical future claims. For instance, would seeing a photo of
scuba diving lead people to predict they were more likely to
go diving in the future? Another interesting possibility is that
some people might show a reverse effect. If rosiness occurs
because people are biased to view the future in a positive light,
perhaps people who suffer from anxiety and depression would
be swayed into believing negative, more than positive, future
claims. These are just some of the future research avenues that
will stem from our findings. We encourage researchers to ex-
plore this new effect and its implications in the domain of
future thinking.
Author note E.J.N. is now at the Mind and Society Center, University
of Southern California, and T.A. is now in the Department of
Psychological Sciences, Kent State University. This research was sup-
ported by the New Zealand Government through the Marsden Fund,
administered by the Royal Society of New Zealand on behalf of the
Marsden Fund Council, and by the Natural Sciences and Engineering
Research Council of Canada.
References
Addis, D. R., Wong, A. T., & Schacter, D. L. (2007). Remembering the
past and imagining the future: Common and distinct neural sub-
strates during event construction and elaboration.
Neuropsychologia, 45, 13631377. doi:10.1016/j.
neuropsychologia.2006.10.016
Alter, A. L., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2006). Predicting short-term stock
fluctuations by using processing fluency. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences, 103, 93699372. doi:10.1073
/pnas.0601071103
Alter, A. L., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2008). Easy onthe mind, easy on the
wallet: The roles of familiarity and processing fluency in valuation
judgments. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 985990.
doi:10.3758/PBR.15.5.985
Alter, A. L., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2009). Suppressing secrecy through
metacognitive ease: Cognitive fluency encourages self-disclosure.
(2018) 46:12231233
Mem Cogn 1231
Psychological Science, 20, 14141420. doi:10.1111/j.1467-
9280.2009.02461.x
BlandónGitlin, I., Pezdek, K., Lindsay, D. S., & Hagen, L. (2009).
Criteriabased content analysis of true and suggested accounts of
events. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23, 901917. doi:10.1002
/acp.1504
Cardwell, B. A. (2015). Non-probative photos promote the truth of pos-
itive claims (Unpublished PhD thesis). Victoria University of
Wellington, New Zealand.
Cumming, G. (2012). Understanding the new statistics: Effect sizes, con-
fidence intervals, and meta-analysis. New York, NY: Routledge.
Derzon, J. H., & Alford, A. A. (2013). Forest plots in Excel: moving
beyond a clump of trees to a forest of visual information. Practical
Assessment, Research & Evaluation, 18(7), 2.
Fenn, E., Newman, E. J., Pezdek, K., & Garry, M. (2013). The effect of
nonprobative photographs on truthiness persists over time. Acta
Psychologica, 144, 207211. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.06.004
Hansen, J., & Wänke, M. (2010). Truth from language and truth from fit:
The impact of linguistic concreteness and level of construal on sub-
jective truth. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 1576
1588. doi:10.1177/0146167210386238
Harmon-Jones, E., & Allen, J. J. B. (2001). The role of affect in the mere
exposure effect: Evidence from psychophysiological and individual
differences approaches. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
27, 889898.
Hyman, I. E., Jr., & Pentland, J. (1996). The role of mental imagery in the
creation of false childhood memories. Journal of Memory and
Language, 35, 101117.
Jacoby, L. L., Kelley, C.M., & Dywan,J. (1989). Memory attributions. In
H. L. Roediger III & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Varieties of memory and
consciousness: Essays in honour of Endel Tulving (pp. 391422).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Jacoby, L. L., Woloshyn, V., & Kelley, C. (1989). Becoming famous
without being recognized: Unconscious influences of memory pro-
duced by dividing attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
General, 118, 115125. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.118.2.115
Johnson, M. K., Hashtroudi, S., & Lindsay, D. S. (1993). Source moni-
toring. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 328. doi:10.1037/0033-
2909.114.1.3
Johnson, M. K., & Sherman, S. J. (1990). Constructing and
reconstructing the past and the future in the present. In T. Higgins
& R. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition:
Foundations of social behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 482526). New York,
NY, US: Guilford Press.
Lindsay, D. S. (2008). Source monitoring. In J. H. Byrne (Ed. in Chief) &
H. L. Roediger III (Vol. Ed.), Learning and memory: A comprehen-
sive reference: Vol. 2. Cognitive psychology of memory (pp. 325
348). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier.
Lindsay, D. S. (2014). Memory source monitoring applied. In T. Perfect
& D. S. Lindsay (Eds.), Sage handbook of applied memory (pp. 59
75). London, UK: Sage.
Lindsay, D. S., Hagen, L., Read, J. D., Wade, K. A., & Garry, M. (2004).
True photographs and false memories. Psychological Science, 15,
149154. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.01503002.x
Loftus, E. F., & Pickrell, J. (1995). The formation of false memories.
Psychiatric Annals, 25, 720724.
Newby-Clark, I., & Ross, M. (2003). Conceiving the past and future.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 807818.
doi:10.1177/0146167203253120
Newman, E. J., Garry, M., Bernstein, D. M., Kantner, J., & Lindsay, D. S.
(2012). Nonprobative photographs (or words) inflate truthiness.
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 969974. doi:10.3758
/s13423-012-0292-0
Newman, E. J., Garry, M., Unkelbach, C., Bernstein, D. M., Lindsay, D.
S., & Nash, R. A. (2015). Truthiness and falsiness of trivia claims
depend on judgmental contexts. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41, 13371348.
doi:10.1037/xlm0000099
Nickerson, R. S. (1998). Confirmation bias: A ubiquitous phenomenon in
many guises. Review of General Psychology, 2, 175220.
doi:10.1037/1089-2680.2.2.175
Petrova, P. K., & Cialdini, R. B. (2005). Fluency of consumption imagery
and the backfire effects of imagery appeals. Journal of Consumer
Research, 32, 442452. doi:10.1086/497556
Reber, R., Schwarz, N., & Winkielman, P. (2004). Processing fluency and
aesthetic pleasure: Is beauty in the perceivers processing experi-
ence? Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 364382.
doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0804_3
Reber, R., Winkielman, P., & Schwarz, N. (1998). Effects of perceptual
fluency on affective judgments. Psychological Science, 9, 4548.
doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00008
Ross, M., & Wilson, A. E. (2003). Autobiographical memory and con-
ceptions of self getting better all the time. Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 12, 6669. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.01228
Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., & Buckner, R. L. (2007). Remembering the
past to imagine the future: the prospective brain. Nature Reviews
Neuroscience, 8, 657661.
Schryer, E., & Ross, M. (2012). Peoples thoughts about their personal
past and futures. In P. Briñol & K. G. DeMarree (Eds.), Social
metacognition (pp. 141158). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
Schwarz, N. (2010). Meaning in context: Metacognitive experiences. In
B. Mesquita, L. F. Barrett, & E. R. Smith (Eds.), The mind in context
(pp. 105125). New York, NY: Guilford.
Seamon, J. G., McKenna, P. A., & Binder, N. (1998). The mere exposure
effect is differentially sensitive to different judgment tasks.
Consciousness and Cognition, 7, 85102.
Sharot, T., Riccardi, A. M., Raio, C. M., & Phelps, E. A. (2007). Neural
mechanisms mediating optimism bias. Nature, 450, 102105.
doi:10.1038/nature06280
Sherman, S. J., Cialdini, R. B., Schwartzman, D. F., & Reynolds, K. D.
(1985). Imagining can heighten or lower the perceived likelihood of
contracting a disease: The mediating effect of ease of imagery.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 11, 118127.
doi:10.1177/0146167285111011
Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M. C. (2007). The evolution of foresight:
What is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? Behavioral
and Brain Sciences, 30, 299313.
Szpunar, K. K. (2010). Episodic future thought an emerging concept.
Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5(2), 142162.
Szpunar, K. K., Addis, D. R., & Schacter, D. L. (2012). Memory for
emotional simulations: Remembering a rosy future. Psychological
Science, 23, 2429. doi:10.1177/0956797611422237
Szpunar, K. K., & Schacter, D. L. (2013). Get real: Effects of repeated
simulation and emotion on the perceived plausibility of future expe-
riences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 323
327. doi:10.1037/a0028877
Szpunar, K. K., Watson, J. M., & McDermott, K. B. (2007). Neural
substrates of envisioning the future. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences, 104, 642647. doi:10.1073/pnas.0610082104
Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social
psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin,
103, 193210. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.103.2.193
Topolinski, S., Likowski, K. U., Weyers, P., & Strack, F. (2008; 2009).
The face of fluency: Semantic coherence automatically elicits a spe-
cific pattern of facial muscle reactions. Cognition and Emotion,23,
260271. doi:10.1080/02699930801994112
Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2003). Temporal construal. Psychological
Review, 110, 403421. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.110.3.403
Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2010). Construal-level theory of psycholog-
ical distance. Psychological Review, 117, 440463. doi:10.1037
/a0018963
(2018) 46:12231233
Mem Cogn
1232
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty:
Heuristics and biases. Science, 185, 11241131. doi:10.1126
/science.185.4157.1124
Unkelbach, C. (2006). The learned interpretation of cognitive fluency.
Psychological Science, 17, 339345.
Unkelbach, C. (2007). Reversing the truth effect: Learning the interpre-
tation of processing fluency in judgments of truth. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33,
219230. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.33.1.219
Wade,K.A.,Garry,M.,Read,J.D.,&Lindsay,D.S.(2002).A
picture is worth a thousand lies: Using false photographs to
create false childhood memories. Psychonomic Bulletin &
Review, 9, 597603.
Weinstein, N. D. (1980). Unrealistic optimism about future life events.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 806820.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.39.5.806
Westerman, D. L., Lanska, M., & Olds, J. M. (2015). The effect of pro-
cessing fluency on impressions of familiarity and liking. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41,
426438. doi:10.1037/a0038356
Whittlesea, B. W. A. (1993). Illusions of familiarity. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19,
12351253. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.19.6.1235
Winkielman, P., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2001). Mind at ease puts a smile on
the face: Psychophysiological evidence that processing facilitation
elicits positive affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
81, 9891000. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.6.989
Winkielman, P., Schwarz, N., Reber, R., & Fazendeiro, T. (2003).
Cognitive and affective consequences of visual fluency: When see-
ing is easy on the mind. In R. Baatra & L. Scott (Eds.), Persuasive
imagery: A consumer response perspective (pp. 7589). Mahwah,
NJ: Erlbaum.
(2018) 46:12231233
Mem Cogn 1233
... There is suggestive evidence of an overlap between the simulation of future events and the constructive processes involved in remembering past events (Schacter, 2012). Several studies have also shown that thinking about the future and remembering the past engage some of the same brain networks (Addis, Sacchetti, Ally, Budson, & Schacter, 2009;Schacter et al., 2012;Szpunar, Watson, & McDermott, 2007), share similar underlying phenomenology (Berntsen & Bohn, 2010;D'Argembeau & Van der Linden, 2004), and are subject to similar biases (Gregg, Upadhyay, Kuntzelman, Sacchi, & Westerman, 2019;Newman, Azad, Lindsay, & Garry, 2018;Westerman, Miller, & Lloyd, 2017). Given such commonalities, there's reason to think that the biases affecting confidence that past events occurred would also influence confidence that future events will occur. ...
... In line with past research, it was thought that the inherent ambiguity of the future would lead to a greater reliance on heuristics, such as the paired photographs. We also sought to extend prior research showing that memory-biasing manipulations can also be used to shape perceptions of the future (Gregg et al., 2019;Newman et al., 2018;Westerman et al., 2017). However, although it was not expected that an effect-size disparity between the quantitative and affective conditions would necessarily be eliminated in Experiment 3, it was expected that the effect size in the affective condition would at least be stronger than in Experiment 2. ...
Article
Do the images we see every day influence how we remember our lives? Research on this matter often concerns how entire memories of events can be created or shaped through the use of doctored photographs of personal (Wade et al., Psychonomic bulletin & review, 9 (3), 597-603, 2002) and public events (Sacchi et al., Applied Cognitive Psychology, 21 (8), 1005-1022, 2007). Although this paradigm has yielded insights into false memory production, it may underestimate the extent to which photographs can bias or distort memory in a subtler manner (i.e., without the use of doctored photographs or suggestion). In Experiments 1 (N = 95) and 2 (N = 186) of the present study, we examined whether the mere presence of generic images, typical of stock photography, could influence aspects of our memories. Given the parallel between autobiographical remembering and forecasting (Berntsen & Bohn, Memory & Cognition, 38(3), 265-278, 2010), we also examined (Experiment 3: N=204) how such images would influence future autobiographical judgments. Specifically, three experiments investigated whether photographs would bias autobiographical judgments for either quantitative (e.g., How many movies have you seen in the past year?) or affective (e.g., How enjoyable do you think your next date will be?) aspects of events in everyday life. We found that photographs reliably influenced judgments related to quantitative aspects of autobiographical events. Moreover, though less robustly, there was an indication that these photos could bias our affective construal of such events as well. Overall, we conclude that the mere presence of generic photographs may exert an influence on the way we think about our lives to an extent previously under-recognized.
... In order to disentangle the illusion-of-evidence account from a f luency account, Zhang et al. (2020) examined truthiness effects in the context of comparative claims. Zhang et al. altered materials from the commodity market prediction task described earlier (Newman, Azad, Lindsay, & Garry, 2018), so that (1) each claim was presented in a comparative format and (2) half of the participants saw comparative claims about easy-to-visualize commodities (e.g., "Shrimp will have increased more in price than rose in three months") and the other half saw claims about difficult-to-visualize commodities (e.g., "Betal will have increased more in price than leghorn in three months"). A third of the time, those claims appeared with a photo of the linguistic subject of the claim (e.g., Shrimp for the high and Betal for the low imageability group), a third of the time those claims appeared with a photo of the linguistic referent in the claim (e.g., Rose for the high and Leghorn for the low imageability group), and a third of the time, those claims appeared with no photo. ...
Book
Full-text available
This open-access book examines the phenomenon of fake news by bringing together leading experts from different fields within psychology and related areas, and explores what has become a prominent feature of public discourse since the first Brexit referendum and the 2016 US election campaign. Thanks to funding from the Swiss National Science Foundation, all chapters can be downloaded free of charge at the publisher's website: https://www.taylorfrancis.com/books/e/9780429295379 There is also an Amazon Kindle edition that's free of charge: https://www.amazon.com/Psychology-Fake-News-Correcting-Misinformation-ebook-dp-B08FF54H53/dp/B08FF54H53/ref=mt_other?_encoding=UTF8&me=&qid=
... In order to disentangle the illusion-of-evidence account from a f luency account, Zhang et al. (2020) examined truthiness effects in the context of comparative claims. Zhang et al. altered materials from the commodity market prediction task described earlier (Newman, Azad, Lindsay, & Garry, 2018), so that (1) each claim was presented in a comparative format and (2) half of the participants saw comparative claims about easy-to-visualize commodities (e.g., "Shrimp will have increased more in price than rose in three months") and the other half saw claims about difficult-to-visualize commodities (e.g., "Betal will have increased more in price than leghorn in three months"). A third of the time, those claims appeared with a photo of the linguistic subject of the claim (e.g., Shrimp for the high and Betal for the low imageability group), a third of the time those claims appeared with a photo of the linguistic referent in the claim (e.g., Rose for the high and Leghorn for the low imageability group), and a third of the time, those claims appeared with no photo. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
... The consistent finding is that when people make rapid judgments about the truth of a claim, nonprobative but related photos nudge them toward believing that claim-an effect known as "truthiness." This influence of photos is robust across materials and contexts; nonprobative photos bias people's beliefs about general trivia facts, claims about products, predictions about the future, and even memories of their own recent actions (e.g., Cardwell et al., 2016;Cardwell, Newman, Garry, Mantonakis, & Beckett, 2017;Newman, Azad, Lindsay, & Garry, 2018;Newman et al., 2012). ...
Article
Ease of processing-cognitive fluency-is a central input in assessments of truth, but little is known about individual differences in susceptibility to fluency-based biases in truth assessment. Focusing on two paradigms-truthiness and the illusory truth effect-we consider the role of Need for Cognition (NFC), an individual difference variable capturing one's preference for elaborative thought. Across five experiments, we replicated basic truthiness and illusory truth effects. We found very little evidence that NFC moderates truthiness. However, we found some evidence that (without an experimental warning), people high on NFC may be more susceptible to the illusory truth effect. This may reflect that elaborative thought increases the fluency with which encoded statements are processed after a delay (thus increasing the illusory truth effect). Future research may fruitfully test whether the influence of NFC and other individual difference measures depends on whether people are making immediate or delayed truth judgments.
... Signal detection analysis revealed a bias to accept statements as true (i.e., lower C scores) when they appeared alongside a picture. Truthiness persists for days (Fenn, Newman, Pezdek, & Garry, 2013) and occurs unconsciously: Most participants (90%) fail to notice that pictures boost perceptions of truth (Newman, Azad, Lindsay, & Garry, 2018). In addition to making claims more believable, irrelevant photos increase people's desire to share both true and false information on social JUDGING TRUTH 7 media (Fenn, Ramsay, Kantner & Pezdek 2019). ...
Article
Full-text available
Deceptive claims surround us, embedded in fake news, advertisements, political propaganda, and rumors. How do people know what to believe? Truth judgments reflect inferences drawn from three types of information: base rates, feelings, and consistency with information retrieved from memory. First, people exhibit a bias to accept incoming information, because most claims in our environments are true. Second, people interpret feelings, like ease of processing, as evidence of truth. And third, people can (but do not always) consider whether assertions match facts and source information stored in memory. This three-part framework predicts specific illusions (e.g., truthiness, illusory truth), offers ways to correct stubborn misconceptions, and suggests the importance of converging cues in a post-truth world in which falsehoods travel further and faster than the truth. Expected final online publication date for the Annual Review of Psychology, Volume 71 is January 4, 2020. Please see http://www.annualreviews.org/page/journal/pubdates for revised estimates.
Article
This research examined the hypothesis that people judge as true those claims aligned with the normative content of their salient social identities. In Experiment 1a, participants’ social identities were manipulated by assigning them to ‘inductive‐thinker’ and ‘intuitive‐thinker’ groups. Participants subsequently made truth judgements about aphorisms randomly associated with ‘science’ and ‘popular wisdom’. Those with salient inductive‐thinker social identities judged science‐based claims as more truthful than popular wisdom‐based claims to a greater extent than those with salient intuitive‐thinker social identities. Experiment 1b was a preregistered replication, with additional conditions eliminating an alternative semantic‐priming explanation. In Experiment 2, American Conservatives and Liberals judged as more true claims associated with the ideological content of their social identities. This difference was attenuated through a manipulation that framed participants as more moderate than they had originally indicated. Overall, these experiments suggest an identity‐truth malleability, such that making salient specific social identities can lead to related perceptions of truth normatively aligned with those identities.
Article
The illusory truth effect refers to the fact that repetition increases perceptions of truth. In these experiments, we examined whether the magnitude of the illusory truth effect varies based upon repetition spacing. In Experiment 1, participants read facts that repeated twice, with a lag of 0, 1, 2, 4, 8, 16, or 32 intervening facts. Participants later rated the truthfulness of these facts in addition to new facts. Here, we observed a spacing effect: Truth ratings were higher when repetitions were spaced (i.e., lags of 1 through 32) rather than massed (i.e., lag of 0). We also observed a lag effect that followed a quadratic pattern: Truth ratings increased up to a lag of 16, after which they decreased. In Experiment 2 we tested whether the spacing effect in truth ratings depends upon the number of repetitions. In Experiment 2a we used a small number of repetitions (2, 3, or 4) and short lag lengths (0, 1, or 2). In Experiment 2b, we increased the number of repetitions (2 or 10) and the lag lengths (0 or 10). In Experiment 2a there was an illusory truth effect, but its magnitude did not depend upon lag length or repetition number. However, in Experiment 2b the magnitude of the illusory truth effect increased as lag length and repetition number increased, but there was no interaction between these two factors. Together, these findings show that repetition most likely to increases perceived truth when repetitions are spaced with a moderate number of intervening items.
Article
Full-text available
Some research suggests people are overconfident because of personality characteristics, lack of insight, or because overconfidence is beneficial in its own right. But other research fits with the possibility that fluent experience in the moment can rapidly drive overconfidence. For example, fluency can push people to become overconfident in their ability to throw a dart, know how rainbows form or predict the future value of a commodity. But surely there are limits to overconfidence. That is, even in the face of fluency manipulations known to increase feelings of confidence, reasonable people would reject the thought that they, for example, might be able to land a plane in an emergency. To address this question, we conducted two experiments comprising a total of 780 people. We asked some people (but not others) to watch a trivially informative video of a pilot landing a plane before they rated their confidence in their own ability to land a plane. We found watching the video inflated people's confidence that they could land a plane. Our findings extend prior work by suggesting that increased semantic context creates illusions not just of prior experience or understanding—but also of the ability to actually do something implausible.
Article
Full-text available
Claims are more likely to be judged true when presented with a related nonprobative photo (Newman, Garry, Bernstein, Kantner, & Lindsay, 2012). According to a processing fluency account, related photos facilitate processing and easy processing fosters acceptance of the claim. Alternatively, according to an illusion-of-evidence account, related photos may increase acceptance of the claim because they are treated as tentative supportive evidence. We disentangle these potential mechanisms by using comparative claims. In forming comparative judgments, people first assess attributes of the linguistic subject of comparison and subsequently compare them to attributes of the referent (Tversky, 1977). Hence, photos of the linguistic subject in a sentence should facilitate, but photos of the linguistic referent impair, fluent processing of this sequence. In contrast, a photo of either the subject or the referent can be perceived as tentative evidence. In two experiments (total N = 1200), photos of the subject increased acceptance of comparative claims relative to a no-photo condition (a truthiness effect), but only when the subject was otherwise difficult to visualize. Photos of the referent decreased acceptance of comparative claims relative to a no-photo condition (a falsiness effect), but only when the subject of comparison was otherwise easy to visualize. All results are consistent with a context-sensitive fluency account: increases in fluency foster, and decreases in fluency impair, acceptance of a claim as true. The results provide no support for an illusion-of-evidence account.
Chapter
Full-text available
This chapter reviews the context sensitivity of people's inferences from subjective experiences, focusing on inferences of ease of recall and processing fluency.
Article
Full-text available
Confirmation bias, as the term is typically used in the psychological literature, connotes the seeking or interpreting of evidence in ways that are partial to existing beliefs, expectations, or a hypothesis in hand. The author reviews evidence of such a bias in a variety of guises and gives examples of its operation in several practical contexts. Possible explanations are considered, and the question of its utility or disutility is discussed.
Article
Full-text available
When people rapidly judge the truth of claims presented with or without related but nonprobative photos, the photos tend to inflate the subjective truth of those claims-a "truthiness" effect (Newman et al., 2012). For example, people more often judged the claim "Macadamia nuts are in the same evolutionary family as peaches" to be true when the claim appeared with a photo of a bowl of macadamia nuts than when it appeared alone. We report several replications of that effect and 3 qualitatively new findings: (a) in a within-subjects design, when people judged claims paired with a mix of related, unrelated, or no photos, related photos produced truthiness but unrelated photos had no significant effect relative to no photos; (b) in a mixed design, when people judged claims paired with related (or unrelated) and no photos, related photos produced truthiness and unrelated photos produced "falseness;" and (c) in a fully between design, when people judged claims paired with either related, unrelated, or no photos, neither truthiness nor falsiness occurred. Our results suggest that photos influence people's judgments when a discrepancy arises in the expected ease of processing, and also support a mechanism in which-against a backdrop of an expected standard-related photos help people generate pseudoevidence to support claims. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2015 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
The affect system, in its position to monitor organismic-environmental transactions, may be sensitive to the internal dynamics of information processing. Hence, the authors predicted that facilitation of stimulus processing should elicit a brief, mild, positive affective response. In 2 studies, participants watched a series of neutral pictures while the processing ease was unobtrusively manipulated. Affective reactions were assessed with facial electromyography (EMG). In both studies, easy-to-process pictures elicited higher activity over the region of zygomaticus major, indicating positive affect. The EMG data were paralleled by self-reports of positive responses to the facilitated stimuli. The findings suggest a close link between processing dynamics and affect and may help understand several preference phenomena, including the mere-exposure effect. The findings also highlight a potential source of affective biases in social judgments.
Article
The source monitoring (SM) framework is an evolving set of ideas regarding the cognitive processes by which thoughts, images, and feelings are attributed to particular origins or sources of experience (e.g., episodic memory vs. inference, or episodic memory of a dream vs. of an actual event). The core idea of the SM framework is that rather than being specified directly in abstract tags or labels, the multiple dimensions of source are inferred from the contents and qualities of the thoughts/images/feelings in question (e.g., a mental event with the characteristics of a memory is likely to be attributed to memory).
Article
Feelings of familiarity are not direct products of memory. Although prior experience of a stimulus can produce a feeling of familiarity, that feeling can also be aroused in the absence of prior experience if perceptual processing of the stimulus is fluent (e.g., Whittlesea, Jacoby, & Girard, 1990). This suggests that feelings of familiarity arise through an unconscious inference about the source of processing fluency. The present experiments extend that conclusion. First, they show that a wide variety of feelings about the past are controlled by a fluency heuristic, including feelings about the meaning, pleasantness, duration, and recency of past events. Second, they demonstrate that the attribution process does not rely only on perceptual fluency, but can be influenced even more by the fluency of conceptual processing. Third, they show that although the fluency heuristic itself is simple, people's use of it is highly sophisticated and makes them robustly sensitive to the actual historical status of current events.