Conference PaperPDF Available

Effects of Argumentative Knowledge Construction on Attitude Change Processes in SNS-blended seminars Paper

Authors:

Abstract

150 words) (Arial, 10 pt) New self-organized and large-scale forms of communication, like SNS (social networking sites), bring new possibilities for supporting argumentative learning, that is learning through argumentation. Because of the social character of the interactions in SNS, SNS may extend our knowledge on group processes and outcomes of argumentative learning like attitude change. This article presents a comparison of different supports for argumentative learning in a university teacher-trainee course on communication theory that included weekly SNS discussions: Group Awareness Tools (GATs), to increase attitude conflict awareness, vs. argumentation scripts, as a cognitive guidance to help learners capitalize on this awareness. We use Social Network Analysis (SNA) to analyze conversational data and data from a communication attitude questionnaire on group-level processes relevant to attitude change during argumentative SNS discussions: number of interactions, information flow control, influence distribution, and attitude similarity. Both GATs and argumentation scripts influence argumentative processes, but scripts influence more processes. Extended summary (paper presentation: 1500 words) a) Group Awareness Tools and Argumentation Scripts for Attitude Change through Argumentative Learning in SNS Communication competence can help teachers communicate well with students, parents, colleagues, and school administrative staff. Teacher trainees have pronounced negative attitudes towards the need for communication skills (Ihmeideh & Al-omari, 2010) and communication theory seminars are offered to change their attitudes and improve their skills. However, attitudes tend to be stable (Erber, Hodges, & Wilson, 1995) and attitude change presupposes long-term deep learning and conflict awareness (Erber, Hodges, & Wilson, 1995). Deep learning can be obtained through argumentation which supports Argumentative Knowledge Construction (AKC). AKC is the deliberate practice of elaborating learning material by constructing formally and semantically sound arguments with the goal of gaining knowledge (Weinberger & Fischer, 2006), but also co-construction of opinions and attitudes (Andriessen,). The social character of SNS may leverage attitude differences and lead to socio-cognitive conflict and attitude change beyond what is possible in purpose-specific collaborative learning tools. On the other hand, public discussions encourage the focus on attitudes and may rather reinforce private beliefs Lampert, Rittenhouse, & Crumbaugh (1996). To seize the social affordances for attitude change, GATs can visualize covert information about the group processes (Buder & Bodemer, 2008), and make attitude differences salient, which is a prerequisite of dissonance and attitude change (Festinger, 1957). In the collaborative setting of SNS this may lead to a higher number of interactions in an attempt to collaboratively understand and resolve differences, less centralized information flow control, distributed influence, and maybe more
Effects of Argumentative Knowledge Construction on Attitude Change
Processes in SNS-blended seminars
Paper
Abstract (150 words) (Arial, 10 pt)
New self-organized and large-scale forms of communication, like SNS (social networking sites), bring new
possibilities for supporting argumentative learning, that is learning through argumentation. Because of the
social character of the interactions in SNS, SNS may extend our knowledge on group processes and
outcomes of argumentative learning like attitude change. This article presents a comparison of different
supports for argumentative learning in a university teacher-trainee course on communication theory that
included weekly SNS discussions: Group Awareness Tools (GATs), to increase attitude conflict awareness,
vs. argumentation scripts, as a cognitive guidance to help learners capitalize on this awareness. We use
Social Network Analysis (SNA) to analyze conversational data and data from a communication attitude
questionnaire on group-level processes relevant to attitude change during argumentative SNS discussions:
number of interactions, information flow control, influence distribution, and attitude similarity. Both GATs
and argumentation scripts influence argumentative processes, but scripts influence more processes.
Extended summary (paper presentation: 1500 words)
a) Group Awareness Tools and Argumentation Scripts for Attitude Change through Argumentative Learning in
SNS
Communication competence can help teachers communicate well with students, parents, colleagues, and school
administrative staff. Teacher trainees have pronounced negative attitudes towards the need for communication skills
(Ihmeideh & Al-omari, 2010) and communication theory seminars are offered to change their attitudes and improve
their skills. However, attitudes tend to be stable (Erber, Hodges, & Wilson, 1995) and attitude change presupposes
long-term deep learning and conflict awareness (Erber, Hodges, & Wilson, 1995). Deep learning can be obtained
through argumentation which supports Argumentative Knowledge Construction (AKC). AKC is the deliberate
practice of elaborating learning material by constructing formally and semantically sound arguments with the goal
of gaining knowledge (Weinberger & Fischer, 2006), but also co-construction of opinions and attitudes (Andriessen,
2006; Asterhan, Schwarz, 2007; Baker, 2003; Felton & Kuhn, 2001, Sassenberg & Boos, 2003, Tsovaltzi, Puhl,
Judele, & Weinberger, 2014; Wenger, McDermott, & Snyder, 2002). The social character of SNS may leverage
attitude differences and lead to socio-cognitive conflict and attitude change beyond what is possible in purpose-
specific collaborative learning tools. On the other hand, public discussions encourage the focus on attitudes and may
rather reinforce private beliefs Lampert, Rittenhouse, & Crumbaugh (1996). To seize the social affordances for
attitude change, GATs can visualize covert information about the group processes (Buder & Bodemer, 2008), and
make attitude differences salient, which is a prerequisite of dissonance and attitude change (Festinger, 1957). In the
collaborative setting of SNS this may lead to a higher number of interactions in an attempt to collaboratively
understand and resolve differences, less centralized information flow control, distributed influence, and maybe more
similar attitudes. Scripts can help analyze lines of ongoing argumentation and model adequate argumentative
processes and have been shown to improve processes and outcomes of argumentative knowledge co-construction
(Weinberger, Stegmann & Fischer, 2010). They may, thus, increase interactions by helping discussants elaborate on
their differences, externalize their attitudes and put them under scrutiny (Larson & Nussbaum, 2008), and thus lead
to more distributed reciprocal influence and more similar attitudes. Therefore, attitude change may occur (Erber,
Hodges, & Wilson, 1995). Their combination may promote socio-cognitive conflict and information exchange that
can lead to a higher level of influence between discussants and attitude change.
Results from inferential statistics have indicated that socio-cognitive conflict and argumentation quality can
be supported through SNS discussions with incorporated GATs and argumentation scripts, and their combination
even more, to influence their attitude towards communication skills over longer-stretches of time (semester long)
(Authors, 2015). Can similar effects be observed through the interactions and the network of relations that attitude
differences represent when students argue in SNS?
b) Method
In two long-term 2×2 field-studies, with factors GAT and argumentation script, we used Facebook, a prominent
SNS, to complement face-to-face teacher training seminars on communication theory with online argumentative
discussions over 9 weeks. German teacher trainees (N=148) filled out a weekly case-based questionnaire with cases
from every-day social interaction scenarios in the school, to capture their communication attitudes. Scenarios
consisted of four Likert-scaled answers on how a teacher may assess the situation. The questions covered two
communication attitude dimensions: multi-perspective / flexible attitudes vs. goal-oriented / structured attitudes,
following Buder & Bodemer (2008) and Jermann & Dillenbourg (1999, 2002), rated on a scale from 0 to 6. Each of
six seminars was accompanied by one closed Facebook group, where students held argumentative discussions on
problem cases using communication theories, to foster a balance between multi-perspective / flexible vs. goal
oriented / structured attitudes.
Students in the GAT conditions saw a graphical visualization of the result of the attitude questionnaire
presented in a Facebook application. It depicted their communication attitudes of their group (Figure 1), to make
conflicts salient and increase socio-cognitive conflict (Jermann & Dillenbourg, 1999), leveraging the high
interactivity in Facebook and foster attitude change. Students in the argumentation script conditions had to “like”
the best argument. They then received weekly feedback that evaluated the epistemic (theoretical concepts and
relations) and the formal (reasoning and evidence) argumentation quality for the most “liked”, and for the
instructors’ favorite argument. Students in the control condition merely discussed in their Facebook group.
Figure 1. Group Awareness Tool in Facebook.
SNA Analysis and Results
To measure interactions and information flow, we recorded the number of outgoing posts by each member and the
number of incoming replies to every post. Attitude data comes from the communication questionnaire. We
operationalize our dependent variables in SNA based on measures of graph theory and network analysis (Freeman,
1978; Stephenson & Zelen, 1989; Wasserman & Faust, 1994; Ester, Kriegel, Sander & Xu, 1996), as depicted in
Table 1:
Table. 1
Dependent Variable
SNA Operationalisation
Number of interactions
The sum of incoming and outgoing interactions for each member of a group measured across
time (8 weeks)
Information flow control
(information centrality)
The proportion of total information flow that is controlled by each node (group member) in a
network. It uses the distance between two nodes, traversing the attitude values (Euclidean
distance over time) of all nodes that mediate these two nodes
Influence distribution of
individual attitudes on
the group’s attitude
(out degree centrality)
The collective amount of attitude change of a group across time that can be attributed to the
attitude of an individual member in a weighted network. The weights are assigned based on
the attitude change after each discussion using the Euclidean distance of the two dimensions of
communication attitude questionnaire.
Influence distribution of
the group on individual
attitudes
(in degree centrality)
The collective amount of attitude change of an individual across time that can be attributed to
the attitude of the group in a weighted network. The weights are assigned based on the
attitude change after each discussion using the Euclidean distance of the two dimensions of
communication attitude questionnaire.
Attitude similarity
(density based
clustering)
The magnitude of attitude similarity inside a group across the two dimensions of
communication attitude questionnaire over time: the Euclidean distance of the two dimensions
is used to measure density.
We hypothesize that, participant groups using the GAT, the argumentation script, and their combination
will engage in a higher number of interactions, as they will become aware of differences and externalise more
information about their attitudes (H1); they will exhibit less centralized control on the total information flow, due to
new communication paths in the group (H2); their individual influence on the magnitude of attitude change of the
group will be lower, due to the increased interaction and reflection thereof among members (H3); equivalently, the
group’s influence on the magnitude of attitude change of individuals will be lower, due to an even distribution of
people central to the network (H4); and finally, they will develop higher attitude similarity (H5), because the above
changes in group processes will lead to co-construction of attitudes.
d) Results
We found significant differences regarding the number of interactions (H1), F(5, 142) =192.28, p = .00. As
expected, the argumentation script, F(1,144) = 123.44, p =.00, ηp² = .46, and the combination, F(1,144) = 63.64, p =
.00, ηp² = .31, increased the number of interactions with strong effects. However, the GAT had no effect, F(1,144) =
1.43, p =.23, ηp² = .01.
There were significant differences regarding information flow control (H2), F(5, 35.35) = 172.86, p = .00.
Following our hypothesis, we found strong effects for the GAT, F(1, 144) = 21.69, p = .00, ηp² = .13, the
argumentation script, F(1,144) = 131.02, p =.00, ηp² = .48 and their combination, F(1,144) = 90.02, p = .00, ηp² =
.39. They lead to evenly distributed information flow.
There were significant differences regarding influence distribution of individual attitudes on the group’s
attitude (out degree centrality, H3), F(5, 60.42) = 14.38, p = .00. We found a weak significant effect for GAT, F(1,
144) = 5.56, p = .02, ηp² = .04, a strong effect for the argumentation script, F(1,144) = 33.57, p =.00, ηp² = .19, and
for the interaction, F(1,144) = 23.07, p = .00, ηp² = .14. They forged evenly distributed influence of individual
attitudes.
There were significant differences regarding the centralized influence of the group’s attitude on the
individual attitudes (H4), F(5, 78.03) = 144.47, p = .00. As expected, we found a significant strong effect for the
GAT, F(1, 144) = 20.26, p = .00, ηp² = .12, the argumentation script, F(1,144) = 122.39, p =.00, ηp² = .46, and their
interaction, F(1,144) = 84.09, p = .00, ηp² = .37. They lead to evenly distributed influence of the group’s attitude on
individual attitudes.
There was a significant difference regarding attitude similarity (H5), F(5, 21.9) = 9.33, p = .00. As expected, we
found a strong significant effect for the argumentation script, F(1,50) = 9.40, p = .004 ηp² = .16. On the contrary,
neither the GAT, F(1,50) = 0.11, p = .74 ηp² = .002 nor its combination with the script, F(1,50) = 1.88, p = .18 ηp² =
.036 fostered similarity.
e) Discussion
The results revealed that GAT and argumentation scripts, and their combination can cater for a more equal
distribution of information flow, of individual attitude influence on the group’s attitude and vice versa during
argumentative discussions in SNS. Interactivity was increased either by the script alone or combined with the GAT,
but not by the GAT alone; a possible indication that GAT takes over the role of externalization promoted by the
script. Moreover, the argumentation script helped learners change their attitude and attain similarity, which did not
occur with the GAT or their combination. This might indicate that members resolved conflicts together when they
received cognitive guidance by the script, or it might be a sign of increased group bias (Sassenberg & Boos, 2003)
without explicit awareness of attitude differences (Festinger, 1954).
The results of this study compliment previous results on attitude change and show that argumentative SNS
discussions as part of university courses can be leveraged through GATs and argumentation scripts to support
argumentative group processes that are relevant to attitude change. More research is necessary to reveal the
intricacies of how group bias is affected through GATs and argumentation scripts, and to investigate the usefulness
of such tools in the wild, which may have a large impact not only on university course learning, but on societal
development.
f) References
Andriessen, J. E. B. (2006). Arguing to learn. In K. Sawyer (Ed.), (Tran.), Handbook of the Learning Sciences (443-
459). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Asterhan, C. S. C., Schwarz, B. B. (2007). The effects of monological and dialogical argumentation on concept
learning in evolutionary theory. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99(3), 626639.
Baker, M. (2003). Computer-mediated argumentative interactions for the co-elaboration of scientific notions. In
Arguing to learn (pp. 47-78). Springer Netherlands.
Buder, J., & Bodemer, D. (2008). Supporting controversial CSCL discussions with augmented group awareness
tools. International Journal of Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning, 3(2), 123139.
http://doi.org/10.1007/s11412-008-9037-5
Erber, M. W., Hodges, S. D., & Wilson, T. D. (1995). Attitude strength, attitude stability , and the effects of
analyzing reasons. In Petty, R. E. & J. A. Krosnich (Eds.), Attitude Strength: Antecedents and consequences
(pp. 433454).
Ester, M., Kriegel, H. P., Sander, J., Xu, X. (1996). A density-based algorithm for discovering clusters in large
spatial databases with noise. Proceedings of the Second International Conference on Knowledge Discovery
and Data Mining (KDD-96). AAAI Press. 226231.
Felton, M., & Kuhn, D. (2001). The Development of Argumentive Discourse Skill. Discourse Processes, 32(2),
135153. http://doi.org/10.1207/S15326950DP3202&3_03
Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 117140.
Fischer, F., Kollar, I., Stegmann, K. & Wecker, C. (2013). Toward a script theory of guidance in computer-
supported collaborative learning. Educational psychologist, 48(1), 56-66.
Freeman, L. C. (1978). Centrality in social networks conceptual clarification. Social Networks, 1(3), 215-239.
Jermann, P., & Dillenbourg, P. (1999). An analysis of learner arguments in a collective learning environment.
Proceedings of the 1999 Conference on Computer Support for Collaborative Learning - CSCL ’99, 33–es.
http://doi.org/10.3115/1150240.1150273
Jermann, P., & Dillenbourg, P. (2002). Elaborating new arguments through a cscl script. In J. Andriessen, M. Baker,
& D. Suthers (Eds.), Arguing to Learn: Confronting Cognitions in Computer-Supported Collaborative
Learning environments (pp. 16). Kluwer Academic Publishers. Printed in the Netherlands.
Lampert, M. L., Rittenhouse, P., & Crumbaugh, C. (1996). Agreeing to disagree: Developing sociable mathematical
discourse. In D. R. Olson & N. Torrance (Eds.), Handbook of human development in education (pp. 731
764). Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.
Larson, Nussbaum, E. M. (2008). Collaborative discourse, argumentation, and learning: Preface and literature
review. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 33(3), 345359.
Puhl, T., Tsovaltzi, D., & Weinberger, A. (2015). Blending Facebook into seminars for practicing argumentation.
Computers in Human Behavior 53, 605-616. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.04.006
Sassenberg, K., & Boos, M. (2003). Attitude Change in Computer-Mediated Communication: Effects of Anonymity
and Category Norms. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 6(4), 405422.
http://doi.org/10.1177/13684302030064006
Stephenson, K., & Zelen, M. (1989). Rethinking centrality: Methods and examples. Social Networks, 11(1), 1-37.
Tsovaltzi, D., Puhl, T., Judele, R. & Weinberger, A. (2014). Group awareness support and argumentation scripts for
individual preparation of arguments in Facebook. Computers & Education, 76, 108-118,
doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2014.03.012
Wasserman, S., and Faust, K. (1994). Social Network Analysis: Methods and Applications. Cambridge, ENG and
New York: Cambridge University Press.
Weinberger, A. & Fischer, F. (2006). A framework to analyse argumentative knowledge construction in computer-
supported collaborative learning. Computers & Education, 46, 71-95.
Weinberger, A., Stegmann, K. & Fischer, F. (2010). Learning to argue online: Scripted groups surpass individuals
(unscripted groups do not). Computers in Human Behavior, 26(4), 506515.
Wenger, E, McDermott, R & Snyder, W.M., Cultivating Communities of Practice, HBS press 2002
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Chapter
Full-text available
Many people think that arguing interferes with learning, and that’s true for a certain type of oppositional argument that is increasingly prevalent in our media culture. Tannen (1998) analyzed the aggressive types of argument that are frequently seen on talk shows and in the political sphere, where representatives of two opposed viewpoints spout talking points at each other. In these forms of argument, the goal is not to work together toward a common position, but simply to score points. All teachers and parents have seen children engaged in this type of argumentation, and most would probably agree that it has little to contribute to education. The learning sciences is studying a different kind of argumentation, which we call collaborative argumentation. For example, collaborative argumentation plays a central role in science; science advances not by the accumulation of facts, but by debate and argumentation (Osborne, 2010). Even when two scientists disagree, they still share the common values of science and both of them are interested in achieving the same goals (determining what claim should be upheld). Argumentation in science should not be primarily oppositional and aggressive; it is a form of collaborative discussion in which both parties are working together to resolve an issue, and in which both scientists aim to reach agreement. Engagement in collaborative argumentation can help students learn to think critically and independently about important issues and contested values. Before students can successfully engage in collaborative argumentation, they must overcome the traditional and deep-seated opposition between reason and emotion (Baker, Andriessen, & Järvelä, 2013; Picard et al., 2004), stop being aggressively opposed to others, and instead orient their positive motivations and emotions toward the question being discussed and others’ views on it.
Article
Full-text available
The CSCL community faces two main challenges with respect to learning and argumentation. The scientific challenge is to understand how argumentation produces learning, that is to discover which cognitive mechanisms, triggered by argumentative interactions, generate new knowledge and in which conditions. The engineering challenge is to determine how to trigger productive argumentation among students. These two challenges are often investigated in parallel, but this contribution focuses on the latter. There are two ways to favour the emergence of argumentation, either pro-actively, by structuring collaboration, or retroactively, by regulating interactions (e.g. a tutor monitors the pair dialogues). Structuring collaboration either means scripting collaborative activities or designing a dedicated communication tool. The features of such argumentation tools constitute a central concern of this book. This contribution addresses both forms of structuring. (http://edu.fss.uu.nl/medewerkers/ja/Confrontations/Ch8.pdf)
Article
Full-text available
It is now well recognised that argumentative interactions can be vehicles of collaborative learning, especially on a conceptual plane (see e.g. Andriessen & Coiner, 1999). Information and communication technologies such as Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning (“CSCL”) environments can play an important role in such learning to the extent that they enable task sequences and interpersonal communication media to be structured in ways that favour the co-elaboration1 of knowledge (e.g. Baker, 1996, 1999; Baker, de Vries, Lund & Quignard, 2001).
Article
Full-text available
Although communities of practice develop organically, a carefully crafted design can drive their evolution. In this excerpt from a new book, the authors detail seven design principles. The payoff? Knowledge management that works. Seven principles for cultivating communities of practice In Silicon Valley, a community of circuit designers meets for a lively debate about the merits of two different designs developed by one of the participants. Huddling together over the circuit diagrams, they analyze possible faults, discuss issues of efficiency, propose alternatives, tease out each other's assumptions, and make the case for their view. In Boston, a group of social workers who staff a help line meet to discuss knotty client problems, express sympathy as they discuss difficulties, probe to understand each other's feelings, and gently offer suggestions. Their meetings are often deeply challenging and sometimes highly emotional. The fact-driven, sometimes argumentative, meetings of the Silicon Valley circuit designers are extremely different from the compassionate meetings of the social workers in Boston. But despite their differences, the circuit designers' and social workers' communities are both vibrant and full of life. Their energy is palpable to both the regular participants and visitors. Because communities of practice are voluntary, what makes them successful over time is their ability to generate enough excitement, relevance, and value to attract and engage members. Although many factors, such as management support or an urgent problem, can inspire a community, nothing can substitute for this sense of aliveness. How do you design for aliveness? Certainly you cannot contrive or dictate it. You cannot design it in the traditional sense of specifying a structure or process and then implementing it. Still, aliveness does not always happen automatically. Many natural communities never grow beyond a network of friends because they fail to attract enough participants. Many intentional communities fall apart soon after their initial launch because they don't have enough energy to sustain themselves. Communities, unlike teams and other structures, need to invite the interaction that makes them alive. For example, a park is more appealing to use if its location provides a short cut between destinations. It invites people to sit for lunch or chat if it has benches set slightly off the main path, visible, but just out of earshot, next to something interesting like a flower bed or a patch of sunlight. 1 The structure of organizational relationships and events also invite a kind of interaction. Meetings that contain some open time during a break or lunch, with enough space for people to mingle or confer privately, invite one-on-one discussion and relationship building. Just as a good park has varied spaces for neighborhood baseball games, quiet chats, or The goal of community design is to bring out the community's own internal direction, character, and energy.
Article
Full-text available
This study investigates how group awareness support and argumentation scripts influence learning in social networking sites like Facebook, which may be conducive to informal learning, but often lacks argumentative quality. Supporting participants’ group awareness about the visibility of the arguments they construct and about prospective future debate with peers in order to promote argument quality may be particularly suited for learning in Social Networking Sites. Additional argumentation scripts may directly foster argumentative knowledge construction. In a 2 × 2 study (N = 81), we isolated and investigated the effects of group awareness support and argumentation scripts during individual preparation in a Facebook app on domain and argumentative knowledge. Our results reveal that group awareness support of upcoming argumentative processes can be counterproductive for learning in Social Networking Sites. Argumentation scripts in Facebook may remedy possible negative effects of such awareness. Process analysis showed that group awareness support promotes individual argument elaboration but reduces broad analysis of the domain.
Article
Full-text available
The skills involved in argument as a social discourse activity presumably develop during the childhood and adolescent years, but little is known about the course of that development. As an initial step in examining this development, a coding system was developed for the purpose of analyzing multiple dialogues between peers on the topic of capital punishment. A comparison of the dialogues of young adolescents and those of young adults showed the teens to be more preoccupied with producing the dialogue and less able to behave strategically with respect to the goals of argumentive discourse. Teens also did not exhibit the strategic skill that adults did of adapting discourse to the requirements of particular argumentive contexts (agreeing vs. disagreeing dialogues).
Article
Full-text available
This article presents an outline of a script theory of guidance for computer-supported collaborative learning (CSCL). With its 4 types of components of internal and external scripts (play, scene, role, and scriptlet) and 7 principles, this theory addresses the question of how CSCL practices are shaped by dynamically reconfigured internal collaboration scripts of the participating learners. Furthermore, it explains how internal collaboration scripts develop through participation in CSCL practices. It emphasizes the importance of active application of subject matter knowledge in CSCL practices, and it prioritizes transactive over nontransactive forms of knowledge application in order to facilitate learning. Further, the theory explains how external collaboration scripts modify CSCL practices and how they influence the development of internal collaboration scripts. The principles specify an optimal scaffolding level for external collaboration scripts and allow for the formulation of hypotheses about the fading of external collaboration scripts. Finally, the article points toward conceptual challenges and future research questions.
Chapter
All students have beliefs about what to do in school in order to learn. These beliefs-their "folk learning theories"-are an expression of what our culture believes about school knowledge and how it is acquired.
Article
The intuitive background for measures of structural centrality in social networks is reviewed and existing measures are evaluated in terms of their consistency with intuitions and their interpretability.
Article
Social media like Facebook can blend classroom work with collaborative online learning. Different instructional approaches may support such online learning phases. Group awareness tools provide collaborating learners with additional information about the processes and the knowledge in the group and how these are distributed within a group to support the regulation of learning. Scripts are a form of external regulation of collaborative learning processes. Scripts may complement group awareness tools and promote active processing of the additional group information. In a 1 3 (group awareness tool with script vs. without script, and control group) quasi-experimental field study (N = 63) we observed long-term effects of a group awareness tool and its combination with a script on processes and outcomes of argumentative learning in a seminar accompanied by Facebook discussions. In addition to domain specific learning outcomes, this study explores attitude change as a potential outcome of prolonged periods of argumentative knowledge construction. Results showed a main effect of group awareness tools on declarative knowledge, but no significant effects on argumentative knowledge. Participants with group awareness tool (with script vs. without) attained an attitude which aligned with the learning goals. This attitude change partly correlates with the significantly higher learning outcomes on declarative knowledge.