PosterPDF Available

ARTIFICIAL AROUSAL IN BABIES AS STRATEGY TO RESUME PROLONGED APNEAS WITHOUT DISRUPTING SLEEP

Authors:
  • Grupo Hospitalario Quiron

Abstract

SIDS happens when babies are asleep and newborn babies spend 15 hours per day sleeping. In fact, the term SIDS refer to infants who die in their sleep with no evidence of accidental asphysia, inflicted injury or organic disease .This study has the objective to demonstrate that there are artificial mechanisms precise enough to induce an arousal similar to the expected naturally in current risky situations (like a prolonged apnea) in healthy babies.
SIDS happens when babies are asleep [1] and newborn babies
spend 15 hours per day sleeping. In fact, the term SIDS refer to
infants who die in their sleep with no evidence of accidental
asphysia, inflicted injury or organic disease [2]. SIDS is a
multifactorial syndrome mainly related to overheating, prolonged
apnea, gastroesophageal reflux or inadequate bedding system and
posture. According to the Triple Risk Model [3], SIDS results when
three factors simultaneously influence the infant:
· An underlying vulnerability in the infant.
· A critical developmental periodn
· An exogenous stressor, e.g. hyperthermia
PRECISE ARTIFICIAL AROUSALS IN BABIES
AS A STRATEGY TO RESUME PROLONGED
APNEAS WITHOUT DISRUPTING SLEEP
INTRODUCTION
M. Valero
M. Morell / G. Pin
T. Zamora
J. Diez
Instituto de Biomecánica de Valencia.
Unidad de Sueño Hospital Quirón Valencia.
European Sleep Care Institute (ESCI).
ASEIP, Valencia, Spain.
Authors Affiliation
In this model, such exogenous stressors are postulated to induce
asphyxia, hypercapnia and hypoxia.
A failure to arouse is also one of the causes of SIDS, if babies stop
breathing during sleep; as a defense mechanism, they usually
arouse and start breathing again. In eect, they revive themselves.
However, if the baby does not arouse in time -fail to wake up and
take a deep breath to end a prolonged apnea-, there is a second line
of defense, gasping [6]. The infant’s brain stimulates slow, deep,
labored breaths that temporarily restore his oxygen supply. If this
mechanism also fails, the infant will die from a lack of oxygen.
References
[1] T. Hoppenbrouwers and J. Hodgman, SIDS, Calabasas, CA:
Monte Nido Press (ISBN: 0-9742663-0-2), 2004.
[2] R. Byard and H. F. Krous, “Sudden Infant Death Syndrome:
Overview and Update,” Pediatric and Developmental Pathology,
no. 6, pp. 112-127, 2003.
[3] H. Kinney, H. Richerson, S. Dymecki, R. Darnall and E.
Nattie, “The Brainstem and Serotonin in the Sudden Infant
Death Syndrome,” The Annual Review of Pathology:
Mechanisms of Disease, vol. 4, pp. 517-50, 2009.
[4] J. Filiano and H. Kinney, “A perspective on
neuropathologic findings in victims of the sudden infant death
syndrome: the triple-risk model,” Biol. Neonat., vol. 65, p.
194–97, 1994.
[5] F. McNamara, F. Issa and C. Sullivan, “Arousal pattern
following central and obstructive breathing abnormalities in
infants and children,” Journal of Applied Physiology, vol. 81,
pp. 2651-2657, 1996.
[6] F. McNamara, H. Wulbrand and B. Thach, “Characteristics
of the infant arousal response,” Journal of Applied Physiology,
vol. 87, pp. 2314-2321, 1998.
The importance of arousal mechanisms related to SIDS is postulated
by several reports ([5], [6], [7], [8], [9], [10], [11]). In fact two studies
have provided evidence of decreased spontaneous arousals during
sleep in SIDS compared with control infants ([12], [13]).
This study has the objective to demonstrate that there are artificial
mechanisms precise enough to induce an arousal similar to the
expected naturally in current risky situations (like a prolonged
apnea) in healthy babies.
Figure 3. Evidence of an
arousal registered during one
of the pilot experiments. Left:
Vibratory signal registered by
the accelerometer in blue.
Alfha, beta, delta and theta
waves. Right: FFT of the
screen-shoot period. Figure 5. The system set up considered 6 vibratory motors and 1 accelerometer
to measure the stimulus.
Figure 4. Interface
programmed in Labview to
control de experiment and
capture the results.
Table 1. Design of
experiments for
trials with 36
babies (T2.6)
Table 2. Summary of the arousals resulted.
Figure 6. Left: Image of a baby sleeping in lateral position during the experiment.
Right: Temperature sensor used inside the mattress.
MATERIAL AND METHODS
The stimulus selected by the study researchers to induce an artificial
arousal was a vibratory surface. This mechanism was inspired on the
evidence that a baby nurse usually produce a positive reaction when
touching softly the baby body during a prolonged apnea. During a
pilot study with 6 babies, it was explored dierent designs to
produce the stimulus and validate the hypothesis. Results of pilot
study validated that with fullterm babies, it was possible to induce a
cortical arousal with 6 vibratory motors (figure 1, figure 2) but the
sleep stage and the posture of the baby was presented as key
factors to control the vibratory energy and don’t wake up the baby.
To control the vibratory stimulus produced by the 6 motors during
the experiment, it was designed a labview interface which was
programmed to switch on the motors by the researched producing
an intermittent and random vibration calculated automatically. The
vibration energy could also be controlled using 3 force ranges. The
interface capture simultaneously the accelerometer signal and
allows the researcher to introduce the feedback of the EEG signal
observed and described by the Neurophysiologist that took part in
the experiment.
To do the experiment, it was considered 12 baby groups according
to the criterial of 8 experts (pediatricians) that collaborated during
the study: 6 age periods and 2 gestational groups (full-term,
preterm). Table 2 presents the number of babies recluted for the
experiment. All babies were recluted by pediatricians involved in the
study and were all health and without any arousal nor respiratory
problems detected by doctors.
RESULTS
Table X presents a summary of the arousals resulted during the 28
experiments according to the sleep stage (N1, N2, N3) and the
vibratory energy (LEVEL) used each time. The experiment got 222
cortical arousals.
Finally it was done a fuzzy logic analysis to simulate graphically
an algorithm to produce the correct vibratory stimulus
according to the significant factors (Figure 8).
The ANOVA analysis detected as significant factors to produce an
arousal (Figure 7) following parameters:
· Vibratory LEVEL
· Gestational Age
· Baby sleep Posture
Figure 1. The
motor was
allocated inside a
plastic capsule
which was
inserted inside a
baby mattress.
Figure 2.
Microvibe motor
used during the
experiment.
The experiment consisted in producing dierent stimulus trying to
induce an arousal in all sleep stages (REM and NonREM: N1, N2,
N3-N4) without waking up the baby. All the experiments were done
during the early night sleep in the same hospital room by the same
research team (1 engineer and 1 neurophysiologist). Posture of the
THE FOLLOWING SIGNALS WERE INSTRUMENTED:
· EEG. 1: SaO2, 2: T2-O2, 3: Fp1-T3, 4: T3-O1, 5: Fp4-C4,
6:C4-O8, 7: Fp1-C3, (Figure 6-Left); C3-O1.
· Temperature (sensor to be added in the mattress surface)
(Figure 6 Right).
· Respiration.
· Mattress acceleration and input signal for the vibrators.
baby was selected voluntary by the baby career to do not influence
the baby current sleep habits. Only prone posture was not allowed.
Dierent analysis (ANOVA, Correlations, Fuzzy logic) were
performed to understand how factors controlled during the
experiment could influence the results.
Dierent analysis (ANOVA, Correlations, Fuzzy logic) were
performed to understand how factors controlled during the
experiment could influence the results.
The system, presented as an artificial stimulator under the baby
body, has been able to induce a cortical arousal in the 12 baby
groups included into the study. The fuzzy logic analysis indicates
that the designed system works properly only in supine posture.
This could be explained because in supine posture the baby rest
the whole shoulder over the vibratory mattress.
None of the groups gave significant results with the minimum
vibratory level selected at the experiment, future experiment
should contemplate higher vibratory levels.
Younger-full term babies needed the highest vibratory levels to
produce an arousal, maybe this evidence indicates that during
this life-period the babies sleep deeper.
The system presented in this study could be trained for each baby
to produce an arousal in case of risky situations that could be
automatically detected by a respiratory monitoring system at
home or in a hospital. If the vibratory Level is performed properly,
the baby will arouse without disrupting the sleep stage avoiding
an emergency alarm every risky situation or every false positive
signal could shoot an emergency alarm.
This study has been financed by EU FP7 under the project Baby
Care Sleep.
Project coordinator: Colchones Delax
Project Partners: Cogent, VDS, Pin Arboledas, IBV, ISRI,
Centexbel and ASEIP.
DISCUSSION
AND CONCLUSION
[7] A. Lijowska, N. Reed, B. Mertins Chiodini and B. Thach,
“Sequential arousal and airway defensive behaviour of
infants in asphixial sleep environments,” Journal of Applied
Physiology, vol. 83, pp. 219-228, 1997.
[8] M. Page and H. Jeffery, “Airway protection in sleeping
infants in response to pharyngeal fluid stimulation in the
supine position,” Pediatric Research, vol. 44, pp. 691-698,
1998.
[9] H. Wulbrand, F. McNamara and B. Thach, “Suppression
of sigma spindle EEG activity as a measure of transient
arousal following spontaneous and occlusion sighs and
startles,” Pediatric
Research, vol. 44, pp. 767-773, 1998.
[10] F. McNamara, A. Lijowska and B. Thach, “Spontaneous
arousal activity in infants during NREM and REM sleep,”
Journal of Physiology, vol. 538, no. 1, pp. 263-269, 2002.
[11] B. Galland, G. Reeves, B. Taylor and D. Bolton, “Sleep
position, autonomic function and arousal”, Archives of
Diseases of Children, Fetal Neonatal Edition, vol. 78, pp.
189-194, 1998.
[12] A. Kahn, J. Groswasser, E. Rebuffat, M. Sottiaux, D.
Blum, M. Foerster, P. Franco, A. Bochner, M. Alexander, A.
Bachy, P. Richard, M. Verghote, D. Le Polain and J.
Wayenberg, “Sleep and cardiorespiratory characteristics of
infant victims of sudden infant death: a prospective
casecontrol study,” Sleep, vol. 15, pp. 287-292, 1992.
[13] V. Schechtmann, R. Harper, A. Wilson and D. Southall,
“Sleep state organization in normal infants and victims of
sudden infant death syndrome,” Pediatrics, vol. 89, pp.
865-870, 1992.
Tomás Zamora
innovation@escinstitute.com
(+34) 670 372 937
www.escinstitute.com
Figure 8. Decision tree resulted at the fuzzy logic analysis for training the
system according to main significant factors resulted in the experiment.
Figure 7. Influence of the main factors
(baby posture, vibration level and
gestational age) over the AROUSAL
produced by the experiment. Vibration
level 3, Preterm babies and supine
position favors a higher arousal
registration..
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
We analyzed the polysomnographic records of 15 children and 20 infants with obstructive sleep apnea (OSA) to examine the interaction between central and obstructive breathing abnormalities and arousal from sleep. Each patient was matched for age with an infant or child who had no OSA. We found that the majority of respiratory events in infants and children was not terminated with arousal. In children, arousals terminated 39.3 +/- 7.2% of respiratory events during quiet sleep and 37.8 +/- 7.2% of events during active (rapid-eye-movement) sleep. In infants, arousals terminated 7.9 +/- 1.0% of events during quiet sleep and 7.9 +/- 1.2% of events during active sleep. In both infants and children, however, respiratory-related arousals occurred more frequently after obstructive apneas and hypopneas than after central events. Spontaneous arousals occurred in all patients with OSA during quiet and active sleep. The frequency of spontaneous arousals was not different between children with OSA and their matched controls. During active sleep, however, infants with OSA had significantly fewer spontaneous arousals than did control infants. We conclude that arousals is not an important mechanism in the termination of respiratory events in infants and children and that electroencephalographic criteria are not essential to determine the clinical severity of OSA in the pediatric population.
Article
Full-text available
Arousal is considered to be an important response to a life-threatening stimulus. Recently, it has been shown that the infant arousal response to an elevated inspired CO2 level occurs as a sequence of events involving presumptive brain stem responses before awakening (A. Lijowska, N. Reed, B. Chiodini, and B. T. Thach. Am. J. Respir. Crit. Care Med. 151: A151, 1995; A. S. Lijowska, N. W. Reed, B. A. Mertins Chiodini, and B. T. Thach. J. Appl. Physiol. 83: 219-228, 1997). We wanted to further evaluate the relationship of subcortical reflexes to cortical arousal in infants. We used a nonrespiratory (tactile) stimulus to elicit arousal in infants during non-rapid-eye-movement (NREM) and rapid-eye-movement (REM) sleep. We found that a tactile stimulus elicited an arousal sequence that commenced with a spinal withdrawal reflex, was followed by brain stem responses (respiratory and startle responses), and ended in a cortical arousal. The entire pathway or part of it in the order of spinal to cortical responses could be elicited. REM and NREM responses were similar except for significant differences in the latencies of spinal and subcortical reflexes. These observations suggest that the infant arousal response to a tactile stimulus involves a progression of central nervous system activation from the spinal to cortical levels. The different components of the arousal pathway may be important for an infant to respond appropriately to stimuli during sleep without necessarily disturbing sleep.
Article
The sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS) is the sudden death of an infant under one year of age that is typically associated with sleep and that remains unexplained after a complete autopsy and death scene investigation. A leading hypothesis about its pathogenesis is that many cases result from defects in brainstem-mediated protective responses to homeostatic stressors occurring during sleep in a critical developmental period. Here we review the evidence for the brainstem hypothesis in SIDS with a focus upon abnormalities related to the neurotransmitter serotonin in the medulla oblongata, as these are the most robust pathologic findings to date. In this context, we synthesize the human autopsy data with genetic, whole-animal, and cellular data concerning the function and development of the medullary serotonergic system. These emerging data suggest an important underlying mechanism in SIDS that may help lead to identification of infants at risk and specific interventions to prevent death.
Article
Neuropathologic studies in SIDS victims support the concept that they are not entirely 'normal' prior to death, but rather possess underlying vulnerabilities which put them at risk for sudden death. This concept forms a key link in a triple-risk model for the pathogenesis of SIDS proposed by us. According to this model, sudden death in SIDS results from the intersection of three overlapping factors: (1) a vulnerable infant; (2) a critical developmental period in homeostatic control, and (3) an exogenous stressor(s). An infant will die of SIDS only if he/she possesses all three factors; the infant's vulnerability lies latent until he/she enters the critical period and is subject to an exogenous stressor. According to this model, heterogeneous disorders may make the infant vulnerable to sudden death during the critical period, as potentially exemplified by two previously reported lesions in SIDS brains (arcuate nucleus hypoplasia and subtle hypomyelination). Nevertheless, the triple-risk model does not preclude the possibility that the majority of SIDS deaths will be explained by a single common pathway upon which multiple stressors impinge to produce sudden death during the critical period.
Article
The past decade and a half has seen marked changes in the epidemiology of sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS). The avoidance of certain risk factors such as sleeping prone and cigarette smoke exposure has resulted in the death rate falling dramatically. Careful evaluation of environmental factors and endogenous characteristics has led to a greater understanding of the complexities of the syndrome. The development and implementation of death scene and autopsy protocols has led to standardization in approaches to unexpected infant deaths with increasing diagnoses of accidental asphyxia. Despite these advances, there is still confusion surrounding the diagnosis, with deaths being attributed to SIDS in many communities and countries where death scene investigations and autopsies have not been conducted. The following review provides a brief overview of the historical background, epidemiology, pathology, and pathogenesis of SIDS. Contentious issues concerning the diagnosis and current problems are discussed. Despite calls to abandon the designation, SIDS remains a viable term for infants who die in their sleep with no evidence of accident, inflicted injury, or organic disease after a full investigation has been conducted according to standard guidelines.