ChapterPDF Available

Perfectionism and Personality

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

This chapter provides a synopsis of research on where multidimensional perfectionism “fits” within the broader framework of contemporary personality theory. Focusing on Hewitt and Flett’s (1991) model of perfectionism--differentiating self-oriented, other-oriented, and socially prescribed perfectionism--the chapter presents a summary and critical discussion of how multidimensional perfectionism relates to the dimensions and facets of two major structural models of personality (the five-factor model and the HEXACO model) and one neuropsychological model of personality (reinforcement sensitivity theory). Implications of the findings for multidimensional theories and models of perfectionism, as well as future perfectionism research, are discussed.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Stoeber, J., Corr, P. J., Smith, M. M., & Saklofske, D. H. (2018). Perfectionism and
personality. In J. Stoeber (Ed.), The psychology of perfectionism: Theory, research,
applications (pp. 68-88). London: Routledge.
Perfectionism and Personality
Joachim Stoeber, Philip J. Corr, Martin M. Smith, & Donald H. Saklofske
Overview
This chapter provides a synopsis of research on where multidimensional perfectionism
fits within the broader framework of contemporary personality theory. Focusing on Hewitt and
Fletts (1991) model of perfectionismdifferentiating self-oriented, other-oriented, and socially
prescribed perfectionismthe chapter presents a summary and critical discussion of how
multidimensional perfectionism relates to the dimensions and facets of two major structural
models of personality (the five-factor model and the HEXACO model) and one
neuropsychological model of personality (reinforcement sensitivity theory). Implications of the
findings for multidimensional theories and models of perfectionism, as well as future
perfectionism research, are discussed.
Introduction
Perfectionism is best conceptualized as a multidimensional personality disposition, which
is important because perfectionisms multiple dimensions show different, sometimes opposite,
relationships with adaptive and maladaptive psychological processes and outcomes (see Chapters
1-3). For a complete understanding of multidimensional perfectionism, however, it is important
to know not only how different perfectionism dimensions are related to processes and outcomes,
but also how they are related to stable personality characteristics. Furthermore, it is important to
know where perfectionism and its different dimensions fit within broader frameworks of
personality
To provide answers to these questions, we reviewed the research literature looking for
studies that have investigated perfectionisms relationships with structural (trait) and
neuropsychological models of personality. In this search, we focused on Hewitt and Fletts
(1991) tripartite model of multidimensional perfectionism which differentiates three forms of
perfectionism: self-oriented, other-oriented, and socially prescribed. Self-oriented perfectionism
reflects beliefs that striving for perfection and being perfect are important. Self-oriented
perfectionists have exceedingly high personal standards, expect to be perfect, and are highly self-
Stoeber, Corr, Smith, & Saklofske 2
critical if they fail to meet these demands. In contrast, other-oriented perfectionism reflects
beliefs that it is important for others to strive for perfection and be perfect. Other-oriented
perfectionists have exceedingly high standards for others, expect others to be perfect, and are
highly critical of others who fail to meet these expectations. Finally, socially prescribed
perfectionism reflects beliefs that striving for perfection and being perfect are important to
others. Socially prescribed perfectionists believe that exceedingly high standards are being
imposed on them. They believe others expect them to be perfect, and think that others will be
highly critical of them if they fail to meet their expectations (Hewitt & Flett, 1991, 2004).
There were a number of reasons why we focused on Hewitt and Fletts (1991) model. First,
the model is one of the most widely used in perfectionism research, and there are many studies
that have investigated how this models dimensions relate to broader personality dimensions.
Second, the model includes the two superordinate dimensions that can be regarded as key
indicators of perfectionistic strivings and perfectionistic concerns. Self-oriented perfectionism is
a key indicator of perfectionistic strivings, and socially prescribed perfectionism a key indicator
of perfectionistic concerns (Frost, Heimberg, Holt, Mattia, & Neubauer, 1993; Stoeber & Otto,
2006). Consequently, examining how self-oriented and socially prescribed perfectionism relate
to personality gives us an indication of how perfectionistic strivings and perfectionistic
concernsand other dimensions that are key indicators of perfectionistic strivings and
perfectionistic concernsrelate to personality. Third, perfectionism is a personality
characteristic that has personal and social aspects (which we will see is important when
examining perfectionisms relationships with personality), and Hewitt and Fletts model clearly
differentiates personal and social aspects. Moreover, the model was the first to suggest that
other-oriented perfectionism is an important dimension of perfectionism, which is recently
seeing a reinvigorated interest from psychological research. Moreover, other-oriented
perfectionism plays a central role in dyadic perfectionism (Stoeber, 2012) and is a defining
component of narcissistic perfectionism (see Chapter 6). Hence, we wanted to make sure that
other-oriented perfectionism played a prominent role in our review of how perfectionism relates
to personality, which should begin with looking at structural models of personality.
Structural Models of Personality
Structural models of personality aim to describe personality in terms of underlying traits,
that is, broad descriptions of individual differences between people that refer to consistent
patterns in the way people behave, feel, and think that are consistent over time and situations
Stoeber, Corr, Smith, & Saklofske 3
(McAdams, 2006; Pervin, Cervone, & John, 2005). These models aim to provide a complete
description of personality, that is, they seek to capture all relevant traits. At the same time, the
models aim to be economical and avoid redundancy, and they try to do so by capturing broad,
non-overlapping traits that are relevant to most people most of the time. To find these traits,
structural models of personality rely on a statistical procedure called factor analysis (Ashton,
2013).
1
The Five-Factor Model (FFM)
The five-factor model (FFM) of personality is a structural model of personality that
evolved from psycholexical analyses of traits (e.g., Allport & Odbert, 1936; Norman, 1963)
followed by factor analyses (Ashton, 2013; Pervin et al., 2005). According to the lexical
hypothesis, the descriptive terms for all traits that are relevant to describe individual differences
are communicated between people (e.g., Sam is organized) and are therefore represented in
their language’s lexicon as adjectives (e.g., organized). Consequently, a list of all adjectives in
a lexicon will contain descriptors of all relevant traits andonce synonyms and rarely used
adjectives are removedcan be administered as self-report questionnaires to large samples of
participants with instructions to rate how accurately each adjective describes them. These ratings
are then subjected to factor analyses with the aim to find the basic dimensions (factors) that
explain individual differences in peoples self-ratings. The resulting factors then represent the
structure of personality.
Beginning with numerous earlier studies of personality trait descriptions following, for
example, Cattell (1943), Norman (1963), and Eysenck (1991; see also Bowden, Saklofske, van
de Vijver, Sudarshan, & Eysenck, in press), converging evidence published by a number of
prominent personality researchers showed a growing agreement on a model (the FFM) according
to which five broad dimensions are sufficient to describe the basic structure of personality:
neuroticism, extraversion, openness to experience (or openness for short), agreeableness, and
conscientiousness (McCrae & Costa, 1999; see John & Srivastava, 1999, for a comprehensive
review of the history of the FFM). These five personality dimensionsalso referred to as the
Big Five”—are sometimes described as bipolar dimensions (e.g., neuroticism vs. emotional
1
For a gentle introduction to factor analysis in personality researchwhat it is, what it
does, and how it worksthe interested reader is referred to Ashton (2013, Chapter 3.2).
Stoeber, Corr, Smith, & Saklofske 4
stability, extraversion vs. introversion) or may appear under different names (e.g., extraversion
may be called surgency, openness may be called intellect), but they all represent essentially the
same five broad dimensions of personality as the FFM. Consequently, our review used the FFM
as a frame of reference.
According to Pervin et al. (2005), the five factors can be described as follows. Neuroticism
captures individual differences in psychological maladjustment versus emotional stability and
identifies individuals who are prone to psychological distress, dysfunctional beliefs, and
maladaptive coping responses. Typical adjectives describing people high in neuroticism are
moody, nervous, anxious, touchy, and emotional (Saucier & Goldberg, 1996). Extraversion
captures individual differences in the quantity and intensity of interpersonal interaction, activity
level, need for stimulation, andimportantlythe capacity for joy. Typical adjectives
describing people high in extraversion are talkative, sociable, assertive, enthusiastic, and
energetic. Openness captures individual differences in the proactive seeking and appreciation of
experience for its own sake and the toleration for and exploration of the unfamiliar. Typical
adjectives describing people high in openness are inquisitive, intellectual, philosophical,
innovative, and unconventional. Agreeableness captures individual differences in the quality of
peoples interpersonal orientation along a continuum from social antagonism to compassion.
(Note the difference to extraversion which captures the quantity of interpersonal interactions.)
Typical adjectives describing people high in agreeableness are kind, warm, considerate, helpful,
and generous. Finally, conscientiousness captures individual differences in the degree of
organization, persistence, and goal-directed behavior. Typical adjectives describing people high
in conscientiousness are organized, responsible, thorough, efficient, and self-disciplined.
Multidimensional Perfectionism and the FFM
FFM dimensions. To gauge how multidimensional perfectionism relates to the broad
dimensions of the FFM, we reviewed the literature for studies published or in press that
examined self-oriented, other-oriented, and/or socially-prescribed perfectionism and reported
bivariate correlations with any or all dimensions of the FFM (Campbell & Di Paula, 2002; Davis,
Karvinen, & McCreary, 2005; Dunkley, Blankstein, & Berg, 2012; Dunkley & Kyparissis, 2008;
Enns & Cox, 1999; Enns, Cox, & Clara, 2005; Hewitt & Flett, 2004; Hewitt, Flett, & Blankstein,
1991; Hill, McIntire, & Bacharach, 1997; Molnar, Sadava, Flett, & Colautti, 2012; Nathanson,
Paulhus, & Williams, 2006; Rice, Ashby, & Slaney, 2007; Sherry, Hewitt, Flett, Lee-Baggley, &
Hall, 2007; Sherry, Hewitt, Sherry, Flett, & Graham, 2010; Smith, Sherry, Rnic, Saklofske, &
Stoeber, Corr, Smith, & Saklofske 5
Gralnick, 2016; Stairs, Smith, Zapolski, Combs, & Zettles, 2012; Stoeber, in press; Stoeber,
Otto, & Dalbert, 2009).
2
When summarizing these findings in the following sections, we adopted
Cohens (1992) guidelines and regarded correlations with absolute values of .10, .30, and .50 as
small, medium-sized, and large. In addition, we referred to medium-sized and large correlations
as substantial.
As regards neuroticism, socially prescribed perfectionism was the only perfectionism
dimension of Hewitt and Fletts (1991) model that consistently showed substantial positive
correlations, suggesting that socially prescribed perfectionism is a neurotic form of perfectionism
(cf. Hamachek, 1978). In comparison, self-oriented perfectionism did not always show positive
correlations with neuroticism. Whereas a number of studies found positive correlations (e.g.,
Enns & Cox, 1999; Molnar et al., 2012; Smith et al., 2016), other studies found nonsignificant
correlations (e.g., Hewitt & Flett, 2004; Hill, McIntire, & Bacharach, 1997; Stoeber et al., 2009).
Moreover, the studies that found positive correlations consistently found these correlations to be
smaller than those for socially prescribed perfectionism (e.g., Rice et al., 2007; Smith et al.,
2016). This indicates thatalthough self-oriented perfectionists may have neurotic tendencies
neuroticism is not characteristic of self-oriented perfectionism to the same degree as it is
characteristic of socially prescribed perfectionism. In contrast, other-oriented perfectionism
usually showed near-zero correlations with neuroticism (e.g., Hewitt & Flett, 2004; Hill,
McIntire, & Bacharach, 1997; Rice et al., 2007).
For extraversion, approximately half of the reviewed studies found socially prescribed
perfectionism to show negative correlations (e.g., Molnar et al., 2012; Sherry et al., 2007;
Stoeber et al., 2009) whereas the other half found nonsignificant correlations (e.g., Dunkley &
Kyparissis, 2008; Hewitt & Flett, 2004; Rice et al., 2007). This indicates that socially prescribed
perfectionism is negatively related to extraversion, but the relationship is much weaker than the
positive relationship that socially prescribed perfectionism shows with neuroticism. Still, the
findings suggest that socially prescribed perfectionists tend to be less talkative, sociable,
assertive, enthusiastic, and energetic, andimportantlymay show a reduced capacity for joy.
In contrast, self-oriented perfectionism and other-oriented perfectionism showed no consistent
2
Stairs et al. (2012) were included because they measured perfectionism toward others
using items from Hewitt and Fletts (1991) measure of other-oriented perfectionism.
Stoeber, Corr, Smith, & Saklofske 6
pattern of relationships with extraversion. Indeed, the majority of studies found nonsignificant
correlations, which suggests that both self-oriented and other-oriented perfectionism are largely
unrelated to extraversion.
The reviewed studies on openness suggest that this FFM dimension does not play a
significant role in multidimensional perfectionism. Whereas there are singular studies reporting
small negative correlations between socially prescribed perfectionism and openness (e.g.,
Stoeber et al., 2009), the vast majority of studies examining the perfectionism dimensions of
Hewitt and Fletts (1991) model failed to find any significant correlations with openness (e.g.,
Hewitt & Flett, 2004; Hill, McIntire, & Bacharach, 1997; Rice et al., 2007). Hence, perfectionists
do not appear to be less open to experience than non-perfectionists.
As regards agreeableness, the case was different. In particular, other-oriented
perfectionism showed substantial negative correlations with agreeableness across studies (e.g.,
Hewitt & Flett, 2004; Hill, McIntire, & Bacharach, 1997; Stoeber, in press) which indicates that
social antagonism (low agreeableness) is highly characteristic of other-oriented perfectionists.
Socially prescribed perfectionism was also negatively correlated with agreeableness, but these
correlations were often considerably smaller than those of other-oriented perfectionism (e.g.,
Hill, McIntire, & Bacharach, 1997) and sometimes nonsignificant (e.g., Hewitt & Flett, 2004). In
contrast, self-oriented perfectionism appeared to be largely unrelated to agreeableness. Except
for one study finding a negative correlation (Stoeber et al., 2009), all other studies found self-
oriented perfectionism to show nonsignificant correlations with agreeableness (e.g., Dunkley &
Kyparissis, 2008; Hewitt & Flett, 2004; Rice et al., 2007).
Turning to conscientiousness, all reviewed studies found self-oriented perfectionism to
show positive and often substantial correlations with this personality factor (e.g., Hewitt & Flett,
2004; Hill, McIntire, & Bacharach, 1997; Rice et al., 2007). This was not the case for the other
two perfectionism dimensions. Other-oriented perfectionism showed only small positive
correlations with conscientiousness that were significant in approximately half of the reviewed
studies (e.g., Hill, McIntire, & Bacharach, 1997; Rice et al., 2007) and nonsignificant in the other
half (e.g., Molnar et al., 2012; Nathanson et al., 2006). In contrast, socially prescribed
perfectionism showed mostly nonsignificant correlations (e.g., Dunkley & Kyparissis, 2008;
Hill, McIntire, & Bacharach, 1997) except for a few studies that found significant negative
correlations (e.g., Hewitt & Flett, 2004; Molnar et al., 2012).
FFM facets. One advantage of the FFM is thatwhile the five dimensions provide a broad
Stoeber, Corr, Smith, & Saklofske 7
framework for an economical analysis of individual differences in personality traitsthe model
also allows for a more fine-grained analysis. The reason is that the FFM is conceptualized as a
hierarchical model in which each of the five broad dimensions (domain level) is composed of a
number of lower-level dimensions that are more specific (facet level). The most widely used
measure to examine the FFM at the facet level is the NEO Personality Inventory-Revised (NEO
PI-R; Costa & McCrae, 1992) which assesses six facets for each of the five dimensions.
3
Table 1
shows the NEO-PI-R dimensions and facets (see also Costa & McCrae, 1995a).
[Insert Table 1 about here]
Three studies have examined how Hewitt and Flett’s perfectionism dimensions relate to the
NEO PI-R facets. Unfortunately, only two of the studies included other-oriented perfectionism
(Hewitt & Flett, 2004 [Table 6.19]; Hill, McIntire, & Bacharach, 1997) whereas the third
examined self-oriented and socially prescribed perfectionism only (Dunkley & Kyparissis,
2008). When we reviewed these studies focusing on the convergent findingsthat is, the
correlations that were significant across the studiesthe following picture emerged.
As regards the neuroticism facets, self-oriented perfectionism showed nonsignificant
correlations across all studies, confirming again that self-oriented perfectionism has no strong
links with neuroticism. The same applied to other-oriented perfectionism with the notable
exception that both studies including other-oriented perfectionism found a positive correlation
with angry hostility, which dovetails with the FFM findings linking other-oriented perfectionism
to social antagonism (low agreeableness). In contrast, socially prescribed perfectionism showed
positive correlations with five of the six neuroticism facetsanxiety, angry hostility, depression,
self-consciousness, and vulnerabilityacross all three studies,
4
and positive correlations with the
remaining neuroticism facetimpulsivenessacross two of the studies (Dunkley & Kyparissis,
2008; Hewitt & Flett, 2004). This again demonstrates that socially prescribed perfectionism
shares the strongest and most consistent links with neuroticism.
3
Successively an improved NEO-PI-R version was developed called the NEO-PI-3
(McCrae, Costa, & Martin, 2005).
4
The minus sign before the correlation of socially prescribed perfectionism and anxiety in
Table 2 of Hill, McIntire, and Bacharach’s (1997) article is a typographical error. The correlation
should be positive (R. W. Hill, personal communication, September 6, 2016).
Stoeber, Corr, Smith, & Saklofske 8
Regarding the extraversion facets, it is noteworthy that self-oriented perfectionism showed
positive correlations with two facetsassertiveness and activityacross all studies. This finding
indicates that self-oriented perfectionists may not be more extraverted than others in general, but
may be more assertive and active. Moreover, it also demonstrates the value of examining FFM
facets in addition to FFM dimensions. Other-oriented perfectionism also showed positive
correlations with activity across the two studies that included other-oriented perfectionism, but
not with assertiveness which was surprising given that other-oriented perfectionists tend to report
high self-esteem (e.g., Flett, Hewitt, Blankstein, & OBrien, 1991). In contrast, socially
prescribed perfectionism showed negative correlations with the positive emotions facets across
all studies, suggesting that socially prescribed perfectionists have a lower capacity to experience
positive emotions. There are studies indicating that low positive emotionality is a risk factor for
depression (Khazanov & Ruscio, 2016), and thus the finding of socially prescribed perfectionism
showing negative correlations with positive emotions dovetails with the finding of socially
prescribed perfectionism showing positive correlations with depression (e.g., Hewitt & Flett,
2004; Smith et al., 2016). Furthermore, the finding suggest that socially prescribed perfectionists
may not be less extraverted than others in general, but may have a lower capacity for joy.
Turning to the openness facets, there was only one correlation significant across studies:
Socially prescribed perfectionism showed a negative correlation with openness to values. This
indicates that socially prescribed perfectionists may not be generally less open to experience than
others, but they may be less open-minded regarding values, ideas, and principles and less willing
to accept that values, ideas, and principles may be relative and open to change and different
interpretations.
The agreeableness facets and self-oriented perfectionism did not show any significant
correlations across studies. This finding is in line with the domain-level findings indicating that
self-oriented perfectionism is largely unrelated to agreeableness. In contrast, other-oriented
perfectionism showed significant negative correlations with five of the six facetstrust,
straightforwardness, altruism, compliance, and modestyacross the two studies including other-
oriented perfectionism, which further corroborates the strong links of other-oriented
perfectionism and low agreeableness. The picture was different for socially prescribed
perfectionism which showed no negative correlations with any agreeableness facet that were
significant across all studies. This again shows that, even though numerous FFM studies found
socially prescribed perfectionism to show negative correlations with agreeableness at the domain
Stoeber, Corr, Smith, & Saklofske 9
level, socially prescribed perfectionism is not as strongly linked to low agreeableness as other-
oriented perfectionism, but shows much stronger links with neuroticism.
Finally, as regards the conscientiousness facets, self-oriented perfectionism showed
significant negative correlations with five of the facetscompetence, order, dutifulness,
achievement striving, and self-discipline (but not deliberation)across all three studies.
Moreover, the correlation with achievement strivings was always larger than the other
correlations, indicating that achievement striving is the conscientiousness facet most closely
related to self-oriented perfectionism. In contrast, neither other-oriented perfectionism nor
socially prescribed perfectionism showed any correlations with the conscientiousness facets that
were significant across studies.
Summary. The finding from the studies examining how the perfectionism dimensions of
Hewitt and Fletts (1991) model relate to the FFM dimensions and facets demonstrate that the
three perfectionism dimensions have a unique personality profile for four of the five FFM
dimensions: neuroticism, extraversion, agreeableness, and conscientiousness (but not openness).
Self-oriented perfectionism is primarily characterized by high conscientiousness. This suggests
self-oriented perfectionists tend to show a high degree of organization, persistence, and goal-
directed behavior, and can be regarded as organized, responsible, thorough, efficient, and self-
disciplined. Furthermore, self-oriented perfectionists may show higher levels of extraversion
regarding assertiveness and activity. Other-oriented perfectionism is primarily characterized by
low agreeableness. This suggests that other-oriented perfectionists show a high degree of social
antagonism (i.e., the opposite of agreeableness) and may be unsympathetic, uncooperative,
egotistical, cold, and impersonal (cf. Saucier & Goldberg, 1996). Furthermore, other-oriented
perfectionists may show higher levels of neuroticism regarding angry hostility which is in line
with other-oriented being a socially antagonistic form of perfectionism (Hewitt & Flett, 1991;
Stoeber, 2014a, 2014b). Socially prescribed perfectionism is primarily characterized by high
levels of neuroticism. This suggests that socially prescribed perfectionists tend to be moody,
nervous, anxious, touchy, and emotional. Furthermore, they are prone to psychological distress,
dysfunctional beliefs, and maladaptive coping responses, which corresponds to findings that
socially prescribed perfectionism is a decidedly maladaptive form of perfectionism associated
with emotional distress and psychological maladjustment (e.g., Hewitt & Flett, 1991, 2004). In
addition, socially prescribed perfectionism showed negative relationships with extraversion and
agreeableness indicating that socially prescribed perfectionists may be introverted and socially
Stoeber, Corr, Smith, & Saklofske 10
antagonistic. We should note, however, that the negative relationships with extraversion tended
to be small and were often nonsignificant; and the negative relationships with agreeableness
tended to be weaker than those found for other-oriented perfectionism. Consequently, low levels
of extraversion and agreeableness seem to characterize socially prescribed perfectionism to a
lesser extent than high levels of neuroticism. Furthermore, low levels of agreeableness seem to
be more characteristic of other-oriented perfectionism than socially prescribed perfectionism.
The HEXACO Model
Another important structural model of personality based on psycholexical analyses is the
HEXACO model (Ashton & Lee, 2007; Ashton et al., 2004). The main difference to the FFM is
that the HEXACO model suggests that the FFM is missing an important dimension of
personality labelled honesty-humility. Honesty-humility differentiates people who are sincere,
honest, faithful, loyal, modest, unassuming, and fair-minded from those who are sly, greedy,
pretentious, hypocritical, boastful, and pompous. Consequently, the HEXACO model comprises
six broad personality dimensions: honesty-humility (H), emotionality (E), extraversion (X),
agreeableness (A), conscientiousness (C), and openness (O). Emotionality, conscientiousness,
and openness are supposed to correspond to FFM neuroticism, conscientiousness, and openness,
but agreeableness has different characteristics than FFM agreeableness: HEXACO agreeableness
differentiates people who are patient, tolerant, peaceful, mild, agreeable, lenient, and gentle from
those who are ill-tempered, quarrelsome, stubborn, and choleric (Ashton & Lee, 2007).
Like the FFM, the HEXACO is conceptualized as a hierarchical model because each of the
six broad dimensions (domain level) is comprised of a number of lower-level dimensions (facet
level). To assess these facets, Ashton and Lee developed a 100-item version of the HEXACO
Personality Inventory-Revised (HEXACO-PI-R) assessing four facets for each of the six
dimensions (Lee & Ashton, n.d.). Table 2 shows the HEXACO-PI-R dimensions and facets.
(Note that Lee and Ashton consider perfectionism to be a unidimensional facet of
conscientiousness.)
[Insert Table 2 about here]
Multidimensional Perfectionism and the HEXACO Model
Unfortunately, so far only one study (Stoeber, 2014a) employed the HEXACO-PI-R to
examine how the three perfectionism dimensions of Hewitt and Fletts model relate to the
dimensions and facets of the HEXACO model. However, due to space restrictions, Stoeber only
reported the correlations with the domain scores. Consequently, correlations from Stoeber
Stoeber, Corr, Smith, & Saklofske 11
(2014a) are reproduced here with facet scores included (see Table 2). In addition, Table 2
presents partial correlations controlling for the overlap between the three perfectionism
dimensions to examine the dimensions unique relationships with the HEXACO dimensions and
facets (cf. Stoeber & Gaudreau, 2017).
Honesty-humility and self-oriented perfectionism were not significantly correlated.
However, a unique positive relationship was observed of self-oriented perfectionism with
fairness and a unique negative relationship with greed-avoidance, suggesting that self-oriented
perfectionists value fairness but may be greedy. In contrast, other-oriented perfectionism showed
a unique negative relationship with the domain score, sincerity, greed-avoidance, and modesty.
This suggests that other-oriented perfectionists may not only be greedy (like self-oriented
perfectionists), but generally manifest a deficit in honesty/sincerity and humility/modesty, which
dovetails with studies linking other-oriented perfectionism to callousness and narcissistic
grandiosity (Smith et al., in press; Stoeber, 2015; Stoeber, Sherry, & Nealis, 2015). Also, socially
prescribed perfectionism showed a unique negative relationship with the domain score and
modesty, butdifferently from the other perfectionism dimensionsalso showed a unique
negative relationship with fairness. It appears that socially prescribed perfectionists do not value
fairness, and that socially prescribed perfectionists are perfectionists who “dont play nicely with
others (Sherry, Mackinnon, & Gautreau, 2016).
As regards emotionality, the pattern of correlations was unexpected because self-oriented
perfectionism showed a unique positive relationship with the domain score whereas socially
prescribed perfectionism did not. This stands in stark contrast to the FFM studies in which
socially prescribed perfectionism showed consistent positive correlations with neuroticism
whereas self-oriented perfectionism did not. Also, as regards the emotionality facets, the pattern
of correlations was unexpected. Self-oriented perfectionism had unique positive relationships
with anxiety and sentimentality, and other-oriented perfectionism had unique positive
relationships with fearfulness and dependence. In contrast, socially prescribed perfectionism was
not significantly correlated with any emotionality facets once the overlap with the other two
perfectionism dimensions was partialled out. Whereas the correlations that other-oriented
perfectionism showed are odd and not in line with previous findings that other-oriented
perfectionism is unrelated to neuroticism, there are findings linking self-oriented perfectionism
to anxiety (e.g., Hewitt & Flett, 2004; Klibert, Langhinrichsen-Rohling, & Saito, 2005).
Moreover, Ashton and Lee (2007) suggest that HEXACO emotionality is linked to empathy and
Stoeber, Corr, Smith, & Saklofske 12
attachment, and self-oriented perfectionism shares positive relationships with nurturance and
intimacy (Stoeber, 2014a). Nevertheless, the present findings do not align with Ashton and Lees
(2007) assertion that emotionality is comparable to neuroticism. However, further research on
multidimensional perfectionism and emotionality is needed before firm conclusions can be
drawn.
The pattern of correlations for extraversion showed close correspondence with the
findings from the FFM studies including analyses at the facet level. Self-oriented perfectionism
had unique positive relationships with the domain score, social self-esteem, sociability, and
liveliness, which corresponds to the finding that self-oriented perfectionism showed positive
correlations with the FFM extraversion facets of assertiveness and activity. Other-oriented
perfectionism showed unique positive relationships with the domain score, social self-esteem,
and social boldness whereas socially prescribed perfectionism showed unique negative
relationships with the domain score, social self-esteem, social boldness. Also, these findings
highlight the close correspondence to the findings with the FFM extraversion facets.
Furthermore, the negative correlation with social self-esteem replicates previous research
indicating that socially prescribed perfectionists have low social self-esteem (Flett, Hewitt, & De
Rosa, 1996).
Regarding agreeableness, self-oriented perfectionism did not show a unique relationship
with the domain score but showed a unique negative relationship with flexibility, and a unique
positive relationship with patience. Whereas this finding dovetails with the FFM findings that
self-oriented perfectionism shows no consistent relationships with agreeableness, it suggests that
self-oriented perfectionists may lack flexibility in social relations, but show patience when
interacting with other. In contrast, other-oriented perfectionism had unique negative relationships
with both the domain score and all facetsforgiveness, gentleness, flexibility, and patience
which is in line with the FFM findings that other-oriented perfectionism shows consistent
negative relationships with agreeableness. Conversely, socially prescribed perfectionism showed
no significant unique relationshipsneither with the domain score nor with any of the facets
which again demonstrates that socially prescribed perfectionism is less strongly and less
consistently linked to low agreeableness than other-oriented perfectionism.
As regards conscientiousness, self-oriented perfectionism showed large-sized positive
relationships with the domain score and all facets across bivariate and partial correlations,
confirming the FFM finding that self-oriented perfectionists are primarily characterized by high
Stoeber, Corr, Smith, & Saklofske 13
conscientiousness. As expected, there were no significant relationships between other-oriented
perfectionism and conscientiousness or any of the facets scores once the overlap with the other
perfectionism dimensions was controlled. In contrast, socially prescribed perfectionism showed
unique negative relationships with the domain score and all facet scores once the overlap with
the other two perfectionism dimensions was controlled. This suggests that socially prescribed
perfectionists are not very conscientious, and corroborates the studies that found socially
prescribed perfectionism to show significant negative correlations with FFM conscientiousness
(e.g., Hewitt & Flett, 2004; Molnar et al., 2012).
Finally, as regards openness, no perfectionism dimension showed any significant unique
relationships with the domain score or any of the facet scores, except that socially prescribed
perfectionism showed a small negative partial correlation with creativity. This finding is in line
with the FFM findings indicating that multidimensional perfectionism is largely unrelated to
openness, but if perfectionism shows small negative relationships with openness and its facets, it
is most likely socially prescribed perfectionism that will show these relationships.
Overall, the findings with the HEXACO dimensions and facets show considerable
correspondence with the findings from studies of the FFM dimensions and facets with respect to
extraversion, openness, agreeableness, and conscientiousness (but not emotionality). Going
beyond the FFM, the HEXACO findings indicate that both other-oriented perfectionism and
socially prescribed perfectionism are associated with low honesty-humility (even though they
showed somewhat different relationships with the honesty-humility facets). This suggests that
not only other-oriented perfectionism is a personality disposition that has “dark” features (cf.
Marcus & Zeigler-Hill, 2015), but so also is socially prescribed perfectionism, which
complements prior findings that socially prescribed perfectionism showed unique positive
relationships with callousness and deceitfulness (Stoeber, 2014b, 2015).
Neuropsychological Models of Personality
In contrast to structural models of personality, neuropsychological models of personality
aim to provide an account of the underlying emotion, motivation, and learning bases of
individual differences and, more specifically, to provide neuropsychologically anchored
principles and constructs to understand the foundations of temperament and the underpinnings of
general personality descriptive systems, including the FFM (Corr, DeYoung, & McNaughton,
2013). The major assumption of this specific approach is that a small number of approach and
avoidance systems underlie many general personality factors.
Stoeber, Corr, Smith, & Saklofske 14
Eysencks PEN Theory
A prominent neuropsychological model of personality is Eysencks PEN theory (Eysenck,
1970). Whereas the PEN theory also functions as a structural model of personality, it is not based
on psycholexical analyses, but on theory and research on individual differences in
neuropsychological functioning (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1985).
The PEN theory differentiates three broad personality dimensions: psychoticism (P),
extraversion (E), and neuroticism (N). Factor analytic studies suggest that the PEN dimensions
of extraversion and neuroticism closely correspond to the FFM dimensions of extraversion and
neuroticism, whereas psychoticism appears to be a combination of low agreeableness and low
conscientiousness (Costa & McCrae, 1995b). Unfortunately, there is only one study (Hewitt,
Flett, & Blankstein, 1991) examining how Hewitt and Fletts three perfectionism dimensions are
related to psychoticism (as conceptualized by Eysenck’s PEN theory), and the findings were
mixed: In male participants, perfectionism showed no significant correlations with psychoticism,
whereas self-oriented and socially prescribed perfectionism showed a positive correlation in
female students and other-oriented perfectionism showed a positive correlation in female
patients. Still, Eysencks PEN theory of personality is important in the present context because
the E and N factors of the theory (regarding the neuropsychological foundations of extraversion
and neuroticism) laid the foundation for Grays reinforcement sensitivity theory (Gray, 1982;
Gray & McNaughton, 2000; for a review, see Corr, 2008).
Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory (RST)
The reinforcement sensitivity theory (RST) is a prominent neuropsychological theory of
personality explaining individual differences in approach- and avoidance-related behaviors and
associated conflicts. It assumes the existence of three emotional-motivational systems: one
approach system (the behavioral approach system [BAS]) and two avoidance systems (the
behavioral inhibition system [BIS] and the fight-flight-freeze system [FFFS]). The most
distinctive features of the two avoidance systems are emotional output and defensive direction:
The BIS activates behavioral repertoire when moving toward threat, eliciting the emotional state
of anxiety; in contrast, the FFFS activates behavior that moves the individual away from threat,
eliciting the emotional state of fear. Further refinement and theoretical elaboration of RST
resulted in a progressive revision of RST (Corr & McNaughton, 2008, 2012; McNaughton &
Corr, 2004). Consequently, the latest measure of RSTthe Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory
Personality Questionnaire (RST-PQ; Corr & Cooper, in press)captures individual differences
Stoeber, Corr, Smith, & Saklofske 15
in seven RST components: four components of the BAS (reward interest, goal-drive persistence,
reward reactivity, and impulsivity) plus BIS, FFFS, and a separate factor of defensive fight.
Three studies have investigated how the three dimensions of Hewitt and Flett’s model
relate to the components of revised RST. The first study (Randles, Flett, Nash, McGregor, &
Hewitt, 2010) examined two samples of university students using Carver and White’s (1994)
BIS/BAS Scales to differentiate five RST components: BAS drive, BAS fun seeking, BAS
reward responsiveness, the BIS, and the FFFS.
5
Across the two samples, self-oriented
perfectionism showed positive correlations with BAS reward responsiveness, BAS drive, and the
BIS whereas socially prescribed perfectionism only showed a positive correlation with the BIS.
Otherwise, findings were mixed. In particular, other-oriented perfectionism did not show a clear
pattern of significant relationships across the two samples. The second and third study (Stoeber
& Corr, 2015, 2017) also examined university students, but this time used Corr and Cooper’s
RST-PQ differentiating all seven components of revised RST. Moreover, the studies not only
examined bivariate correlations, but also computed multiple regressions to examine the
perfectionism dimensions unique relationships with the RST components. If we focus on the
unique relationships that were significant across both studies, the following picture emerges.
Self-oriented perfectionism showed unique positive relationships with BAS goal-drive
persistence, BAS reward reactivity, and BIS. Other-oriented perfectionism showed a unique
positive relationship with defensive fight, and a unique negative relationship with the BIS. In
contrast, socially prescribed perfectionism showed unique positive relationships with BAS
impulsivity and the BIS, and a unique negative relationship with BAS goal-drive persistence.
Taken together, the studies examining multidimensional perfectionism from the
perspective of revised RST suggest that the three perfectionism dimensions of Hewitt and Flett’s
model have unique profiles of relationships with emotional-motivational systems and associated
approach- and avoidance-related behaviors. Self-oriented perfectionists appear to be highly goal-
directeddriven by goals and persistent in the pursuit of goalswhile at the same time highly
reactive to both positive and negative reinforcing stimuli. Socially prescribed perfectionists are
5
Note that the BIS/BAS Scales are based on the old, unrevised RST and do not
differentiate the BIS and the FFFS, but some items of the BIS Scale can be used to assess the
FFFS (Heym, Ferguson, & Lawrence, 2008).
Stoeber, Corr, Smith, & Saklofske 16
highly reactive only to negative reinforcing stimuli, and their approach-related behaviors are
impulsive. By contrast, other-oriented perfectionists appear to show a reduced reactivity to
negative reinforcing stimuli, which differentiates them from other perfectionists. Whereas both
self-oriented and socially prescribed perfectionism were associated with higher BIS levels
(suggesting that they are prone to experience anxiety), other-oriented perfectionists reported
lower BIS levels (suggesting they are unlikely to experience anxiety). In addition, other-oriented
perfectionism was the only dimension that showed a unique positive relation with defensive
fight. This suggests that other-oriented perfectionists may become highly defensive when
attacked, and will attack back. The combination of an overactive defensive fight system with an
underactive BIS (indicating a reduced sensitivity to negative reinforcers) dovetails with findings
that other-oriented perfectionism shows links with aggression and psychopathy (Stoeber, 2014b,
2015).
Furthermore, these results show that it is important to go beyond structural models of
personality (like the FFM and the HEXACO model) and also examine neuropsychological
models if we want to gain a deeper understanding of how multidimensional perfectionism is
linked with emotional-motivational systems that directly feed into approach- versus avoidance-
related behavior. This is important because different dimensions of perfectionism show different
profiles of relationships with approach and avoidance motivation (see Chapter 2). Moreover,
individual differences in the sensitivity to positive and negative reinforcers may determine
whether perfectionism takes on forms that have adaptive aspects, or forms that are maladaptive
and lacking any adaptive aspects (Slade & Owens, 1998).
Open Questions and Future Research
In concluding this chapter, it is important to point out that our review focused on Hewitt
and Flett’s (1991) tripartite model of perfectionism. On the one hand, this focus provided us with
a coherent framework when reviewing the different relationships of different perfectionism
dimensions with the FFM and HEXACO dimensions and facets and with the revised RST
components. On the other hand, it also presented a limitation as there are other prominent
multidimensional models of perfectionism, most notably those of Frost et al. (Frost, Marten,
Lahart, & Rosenblate, 1990), Slaney et al. (Slaney, Rice, Mobley, Trippi, & Ashby, 2001), and
Hill et al. (Hill et al., 2004). We note, however, that self-oriented perfectionism is a key indicator
of perfectionistic strivings, and socially prescribed perfectionism a key indicator of
perfectionistic concerns (Stoeber & Otto, 2006). Consequently, one can expect the respective key
Stoeber, Corr, Smith, & Saklofske 17
indicators in these other models to show comparable patterns of relationships with the FFM,
HEXACO model, and revised RST dimensions, facets, and components. In particular, personal
standards (Frost et al., 1990), high standards (Slaney et al., 2001), and striving for excellence
(Hill et al., 2004) are key indicators of perfectionistic strivings, and should show similar
relationships as self-oriented perfectionism. As well, concern over mistakes (Frost et al., 1990;
Hill et al., 2004) and discrepancy (Slaney et al., 2001) are key indicators of perfectionistic
concerns and should show similar relationships as socially prescribed perfectionism. Two studies
employing the FFM confirm this expectation (Cruce, Pashak, Handal, Munz, & Gfeller, 2012;
Rice et al., 2007). In contrast, for both the HEXACO model and revised RST, this is an open
question that needs to be answered in future research.
There are further questions that remain to be answered. One question regarding the FFM
findings concerns the degree to which the overlap between the three perfectionism dimensions of
Hewitt and Flett’s model influenced the findings. Self-oriented, other-oriented, and socially
prescribed perfectionism show substantial overlap: Intercorrelations are often in the .40s, but can
be in the .50s (e.g., Hewitt & Flett, 2004). Consequently, when this overlap is controlled and
unique relationships are regarded, the findings may be different (Stoeber & Gaudreau, 2017). For
example, socially prescribed perfectionism tends to show significant negative correlations with
agreeableness, but this may be due to its overlap with other-oriented perfectionism (which shows
consistent negative correlations with agreeableness). Once this overlap is removed, socially
prescribed perfectionism may show nonsignificant relationships with FFM agreeableness, as was
the case for HEXACO agreeableness (see Table 2). Furthermore, socially prescribed
perfectionism tends to show nonsignificant correlations with conscientiousness, but this may be
due to its overlap with self-oriented perfectionism (which shows consistent positive correlations
with conscientiousness). Once this overlap is removed, it remains to be seen if socially
prescribed perfectionism is negatively related with conscientiousness, as was the case for
HEXACO conscientiousness (see again Table 2).
Another question is whether there are gender differences in the perfectionismpersonality
relationships. For example, Hewitt et al. (1991) found that multidimensional perfectionism
showed significant correlations with psychoticism in women, but not in men (see the above
section on Eysenck’s PEN theory). In addition, they found that self-oriented perfectionism was
positively correlated with neuroticism only in women, but not men. Furthermore, Hill, Zrull, and
Turlington (1997) investigated perfectionism and personality from an interpersonal circumplex
Stoeber, Corr, Smith, & Saklofske 18
perspective. They found that male self-oriented perfectionists tended to be arrogant-calculating
whereas female self-oriented perfectionists tended to be warm-agreeable (cf. Chapter 6) which
suggests that there also may be gender differences in how self-oriented perfectionism relates to
agreeableness.
Finally, the perhaps most important question is whether individual differences in
personality contribute to the development of individual differences in perfectionism. Flett,
Hewitt, Oliver, and Macdonald (2002) provided a comprehensive analysis of potential factors
contributing to the development of perfectionism, and one factor they suggested to play a role
was the child’s “temperament. If we replace “temperament” with “personality,” this would
suggest that personality contributes to the development of perfectionism. Furthermore, the
findings presented in this chapter suggest that different personality dimensions contribute to the
development of different perfectionism dimensions. This suggestion was put to the test in a two-
wave longitudinal study examining whether the FFM dimensions predicted changes in
adolescents’ self-oriented and socially prescribed perfectionism (Stoeber et al., 2009). In line
with cross-sectional findings linking self-oriented perfectionism with conscientiousness and
socially prescribed perfectionism with neuroticism, conscientiousness was expected to predict
increases in self-oriented perfectionism, and neuroticism was expected to predict increases in
socially prescribed perfectionism. Even though the study found support only for one of the
expectationsconscientiousness predicted increases in self-oriented perfectionism, but
neuroticism did not predict increases in socially prescribed perfectionismthe study is important
as it is the first to demonstrate that personality may play a role in the development of
perfectionism. Unfortunately, longitudinal studies examining developmental antecedents of
perfectionism are scarce and usually focus on parental factors, but do not include measures of the
child’s personality (Stoeber, Edbrooke-Childs, & Damian, in press). Further longitudinal
research on perfectionism and personality is neededincluding other models of perfectionism as
well as other models of personalityto determine which perfectionismpersonality relationships
reflect mere covariations showing us where the different personality dimensions fit” within
broader personality theories and models, and which relationships reflect dynamic processes of
personality dimensions contributing to the development of perfectionism.
Stoeber, Corr, Smith, & Saklofske 19
References
Allport, G. W., & Odbert, H. S. (1936). Trait-names: A psycho-lexical study. Princeton, NJ:
Psychological Review Company.
Ashton, M. C. (2013). Individual differences and personality (2nd ed.). London: Academic
Press.
Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2007). Empirical, theoretical, and practical advantages of the
HEXACO model of personality structure. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11,
150-166.
Ashton, M. C., Lee, K., Perugini, M., Szarota, P., de Vries, R. E., Di Blas, . . . De Raad, B.
(2004). A six-factor structure of personality-descriptive adjectives: Solutions from
psycholexical studies in seven languages. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
86, 356-366.
Bowden, S. C., Saklofske, D. H., van de Vijver, F. J. R., Sudarshan, N. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G.
(in press). Cross-cultural measurement invariance of the Eysenck Personality
Questionnaire across 33 countries. Personality and Individual Differences.
Campbell, J. D., & Di Paula, A. (2002). Perfectionistic self-beliefs: Their relation to personality
and goal pursuit. In G. L. Flett & P. L. Hewitt (Eds.), Perfectionism: Theory, research, and
treatment (pp. 181-198). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective
responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS Scales. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 319-333.
Cattell, R. B. (1943). The description of personality: Basic traits resolved into clusters. Journal
of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 38, 476506.
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155-159.
Corr, P. J. (Ed.) (2008). The reinforcement sensitivity theory of personality. Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press
Corr, P. J., & Cooper, A. (in press). The Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory of Personality
Questionnaire (RST-PQ): Development and validation. Psychological Assessment.
Corr, P. J., DeYoung, C. G. & McNaughton, N. (2013). Motivation and personality: A
neuropsychological perspective. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 7, 158-175.
Corr, P. J., & McNaughton, N. (2008). Reinforcement sensitivity theory and personality. In P. J.
Corr (Ed.), The reinforcement sensitivity theory of personality (pp. 155-187). Cambridge,
Stoeber, Corr, Smith, & Saklofske 20
UK: Cambridge University Press.
Corr, P. J., & McNaughton, N. (2012). Neuroscience and approach/avoidance personality traits:
A two stage (valuationmotivation) approach. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews,
36, 2339-2354.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R) and
NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI). Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment
Resources.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1995a). Domains and facets: Hierarchical personality
assessment using the Revised NEO Personality Inventory. Journal of Personality
Assessment, 64, 21-50.
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1995b). Primary traits of Eysencks P-E-N system: Three- and
five-factor solutions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 308-317.
Cruce, S. E., Pashak, T. J., Handal, P. J., Munz, D. C., & Gfeller, J. D. (2012). Conscientious
perfectionism, self-evaluative perfectionism, and the five-factor model of personality traits.
Personality and Individual Differences, 53, 268-273.
Davis, C., Karvinen, K., & McCreary, D. R. (2005). Personality correlates of a drive for
muscularity in young men. Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 349-359.
Dunkley, D. M., Blankstein, K. R., & Berg, J.-L. (2012). Perfectionism dimensions and the five-
factor model of personality. European Journal of Personality, 26, 233-244.
Dunkley, D. M., & Kyparissis, A. (2008). What is DAS self-critical perfectionism really
measuring? Relations with the five-factor model of personality and depressive symptoms.
Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 1295-1305.
Enns, M. W., & Cox, B. J. (1999). Perfectionism and depressive symptom severity in major
depressive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37, 783-794.
Enns, M. W., Cox, B. J., & Clara, I. P. (2005). Perfectionism and neuroticism: A longitudinal
study of specific vulnerability and diathesis-stress models. Cognitive Therapy and
Research, 29, 463-478.
Eysenck, H. J. (1970). The structure of human personality (3rd ed.). London: Methuen.
Eysenck, H. J. (1991). Dimensions of personality: 16, 5 or 3?Criteria for a taxonomic
paradigm. Personality and Individual Differences, 12, 773-790.
Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, M. W. (1985). Personality and individual differences: A natural
science approach. New York: Plenum.
Stoeber, Corr, Smith, & Saklofske 21
Flett, G. L., Hewitt, P. L., Blankstein, K., & OBrien, S. (1991). Perfectionism and learned
resourcefulness in depression and self-esteem. Personality and Individual Differences, 12,
61-68.
Flett, G. L., Hewitt, P. L., & De Rosa, T. (1996). Dimensions of perfectionism, psychosocial
adjustment, and social skills. Personality and Individual Differences, 20, 143-150.
Flett, G. L., Hewitt, P. L., Oliver, J. M., & Macdonald, S. (2002). Perfectionism in children and
their parents: A developmental analysis. In G. L. Flett & P. L. Hewitt (Eds.),
Perfectionism: Theory, research, and treatment (pp. 89-132). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Frost, R. O., Heimberg, R. G., Holt, C. S., Mattia, J. I., & Neubauer, A. L. (1993). A comparison
of two measures of perfectionism. Personality and Individual Differences, 14, 119-126.
Frost, R. O., Marten, P., Lahart, C., & Rosenblate, R. (1990). The dimensions of perfectionism.
Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14, 449-468.
Gray, J. A. (1982). The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the functions of the septo-
hippocampal system. New York: Oxford University Press.
Gray, J. A., & McNaughton, N. (2000). The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the
functions of the septo-hippocampal system. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Hamachek, D. E. (1978). Psychodynamics of normal and neurotic perfectionism. Psychology,
15(1), 27-33.
Hewitt, P. L., & Flett, G. L. (1991). Perfectionism in the self and social contexts:
Conceptualization, assessment, and association with psychopathology. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 456-470.
Hewitt, P. L., & Flett, G. L. (2004). Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale (MPS): Technical
manual. Toronto: Multi-Health Systems.
Hewitt, P. L., Flett, G. L., & Blankstein, K. R. (1991). Perfectionism and neuroticism in
psychiatric patients and college students. Personality and Individual Differences, 12, 273-
279.
Heym, N., Ferguson, E., & Lawrence, C. (2008). An evaluation of the relationship between
Gray’s revised RST and Eysenck’s PEN: Distinguishing BIS and FFFS in Carver and
White’s BIS/BAS scales. Personality and Individual Differences, 45, 709-715.
Hill, R. W., Huelsman, T. J., Furr, R. M., Kibler, J., Vicente, B. B., & Kennedy, C. (2004). A
new measure of perfectionism: The Perfectionism Inventory. Journal of Personality
Stoeber, Corr, Smith, & Saklofske 22
Assessment, 82, 80-91.
Hill, R. W., McIntire, K., & Bacharach, V. R. (1997). Perfectionism and the Big Five factors.
Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 12, 257-270.
Hill, R. W., Zrull, M. C., & Turlington, S. (1997). Perfectionism and interpersonal problems.
Journal of Personality Assessment, 69, 81-103.
John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The Big Five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and
theoretical perspectives. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality:
Theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 102-138). New York: Guilford.
Khazanov, G. K., & Ruscio, A. M. (2016). Is low positive emotionality a specific risk factor for
depression? A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin, 142, 991-
1015.
Klibert, J. J., Langhinrichsen-Rohling, J., & Saito, M. (2005). Adaptive and maladaptive aspects
of self-oriented versus socially prescribed perfectionism. Journal of College Student
Development, 46, 141-156.
Lee, K., & Ashton, M. C. (n.d.). HEXACO-PI-R self-report form, English, 100-item version.
Retrieved from http://hexaco.org/hexaco-inventory
Marcus, D. K., & Zeigler-Hill, V. (2015). A big tent of dark personality traits. Social and
Personality Psychology Compass, 9, 434-446.
McAdams, D. P. (2006). The person: A new introduction to personality psychology (4th ed.).
Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1999). A five-factor theory of personality. In L. A. Pervin &
O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 139-153).
New York: Guilford.
McCrae, R. R., Costa, P. T., Jr., & Martin, T. A. (2005). The NEO-PI-3: A more readable revised
NEO Personality Inventory. Journal of Personality Assessment, 84, 261-270.
McNaughton, N., & Corr, P. J. (2004). A two-dimensional view of defensive systems: Defensive
distance and fear/anxiety. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 28, 285-305.
Molnar, D. S., Sadava, S. W., Flett, G. L., & Colautti, J. (2012). Perfectionism and health: A
mediational analysis of the roles of stress, social support and health-related behaviours.
Psychology & Health, 27, 846-864.
Nathanson, C., Paulhus, D. L., & Williams, K. M. (2006). Predictors of a behavioral measure of
scholastic cheating: Personality and competence but not demographics. Contemporary
Stoeber, Corr, Smith, & Saklofske 23
Educational Psychology, 31, 97-122.
Norman, W. T. (1963). Toward an adequate taxonomy of personality attributes: Replicated factor
structure in peer nomination personality ratings. Journal of Abnormal and Social
Psychology, 66, 574-583.
Pervin, L. A., Cervone, D., & John, O. P. (2005). Personality: Theory and research (9th ed.).
Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Randles, D., Flett, G. L., Nash, K. A., McGregor, I. D., & Hewitt, P. L. (2010). Dimensions of
perfectionism, behavioral inhibition, and rumination. Personality and Individual
Differences, 49, 83-87.
Rice, K. G., Ashby, J. S., & Slaney, R. B. (2007). Perfectionism and the five-factor model of
personality. Assessment, 14, 385-398.
Saucier, G., & Goldberg, L. R. (1996). Evidence for the Big Five in analyses of familiar English
personality adjectives. European Journal of Personality, 10, 61-77.
Sherry, S. B., Hewitt, P. L., Flett, G. L., Lee-Baggley, D. L., & Hall, P. A. (2007). Trait
perfectionism and perfectionistic self-presentation in personality pathology. Personality
and Individual Differences, 42, 477-490.
Sherry, S. B., Hewitt, P. L., Sherry, D. L., Flett, G. L., & Graham, A. R. (2010). Perfectionism
dimensions and research productivity in psychology professors: Implications for
understanding the (mal)adaptiveness of perfectionism. Canadian Journal of Behavioural
Science, 42, 273-283.
Sherry, S. B., Mackinnon, S. P., & Gautreau, C. M. (2016). Perfectionists don't play nicely with
others: Expanding the social disconnection model. In F. M. Sirois & D. S. Molnar (Eds.),
Perfectionism, health, and well-being (pp. 225-243). New York: Springer.
Slade, P. D., & Owens, R. G. (1998). A dual process model of perfectionism based on
reinforcement theory. Behavior Modification, 22, 372-390.
Slaney, R. B., Rice, K. G., Mobley, M., Trippi, J., & Ashby, J. S. (2001). The revised Almost
Perfect Scale. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 34, 130-145.
Smith, M. M., Sherry, S. B., Chen, S., Saklofske, D. H., Flett, G. L., & Hewitt, P. L. (in press).
Perfectionism and narcissism: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Research in Personality.
Smith, M. M., Sherry, S. B., Rnic, K., Saklofske, D. H., Enns, M. W., & Gralnick, T. M. (2016).
Are perfectionism dimensions vulnerability factors for depressive symptoms after
controlling for neuroticism? A meta-analysis of 10 longitudinal studies. European Journal
Stoeber, Corr, Smith, & Saklofske 24
of Personality, 30, 201-212.
Stairs, A. M., Smith, G. T., Zapolski, T. C. B., Combs, J. L., & Settles, R. E. (2012). Clarifying
the construct of perfectionism. Assessment, 19, 146-166.
Stoeber, J. (2012). Dyadic perfectionism in romantic relationships: Predicting relationship
satisfaction and longterm commitment. Personality and Individual Differences, 53, 300-
305.
Stoeber, J. (2014a). How other-oriented perfectionism differs from self-oriented and socially
prescribed perfectionism. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 36,
329-338.
Stoeber, J. (2014b). Multidimensional perfectionism and the DSM-5 personality traits.
Personality and Individual Differences, 64, 115-120.
Stoeber, J. (2015). How other-oriented perfectionism differs from self-oriented and socially
prescribed perfectionism: Further findings. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral
Assessment, 37, 611-623.
Stoeber, J. (in press). Comparing two short forms of the HewittFlett Multidimensional
Perfectionism Scale. Assessment.
Stoeber, J., & Corr, P. J. (2015). Perfectionism, personality, and affective experiences: New
insights from revised Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory. Personality and Individual
Differences, 86, 354-359.
Stoeber, J., & Corr, P. J. (2017). Perfectionism, personality, and future-directed thinking: Further
insights from revised Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory. Personality and Individual
Differences, 105, 78-83.
Stoeber, J., Edbrooke-Childs, J. H., & Damian, L. E. (in press). Perfectionism. In R. J. R.
Levesque (Ed.), Encyclopedia of adolescence (2nd ed.). New York: Springer.
Stoeber, J., & Gaudreau, P. (2017). The advantages of partialling perfectionistic strivings and
perfectionistic concerns: Critical issues and recommendations. Personality and Individual
Differences, 104, 379-386.
Stoeber, J., & Otto, K. (2006). Positive conceptions of perfectionism: Approaches, evidence,
challenges. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 295-319.
Stoeber, J., Otto, K., & Dalbert, C. (2009). Perfectionism and the Big Five: Conscientiousness
predicts longitudinal increases in self-oriented perfectionism. Personality and Individual
Differences, 47, 363-368.
Stoeber, Corr, Smith, & Saklofske 25
Stoeber, J., Sherry, S. B., & Nealis, L. J. (2015). Multidimensional perfectionism and narcissism:
Grandiose or vulnerable? Personality and Individual Differences, 80, 85-90.
Stoeber, Corr, Smith, & Saklofske 26
Table 1
FFM Domains and Facets
Neuroticism
Openness
Agreeableness
Conscientiousness
Anxiety
Fantasy
Trust
Competence
Angry hostility
Aesthetics
Straightforwardness
Order
Depression
Feelings
Altruism
Dutifulness
Self-consciousness
Actions
Compliance
Achievement striving
Impulsiveness
Ideas
Modesty
Self-discipline
Vulnerability
Values
Tender-mindedness
Deliberation
Note. FFM = five-factor model of personality. Domain and facet scales from the NEO-PI-R (Costa & McCrae, 1992, 1995a).
Stoeber, Corr, Smith, & Saklofske 27
Table 2
Multidimensional Perfectionism: Correlations with the HEXACO Model of Personality
Domains and Facets
Bivariate correlations
Partial correlations
Domains and facets
SOP
OOP
SPP
SOP
OOP
SPP
Honesty-humility
Sincerity
.01
.15**
.14**
.10
.11*
.10
Fairness
.12*
.09
.13*
.21***
.06
.17**
Greed-avoidance
.29***
.33***
.31***
.15**
.18**
.11
Modesty
.11
.36***
.27***
.07
.27***
.12*
Domain score
.11
.34***
.31***
.09
.22***
.19***
Emotionality
Fearfulness
.12*
.17**
.06
.07
.14*
.05
Anxiety
.26***
.08
.15**
.22***
.05
.05
Dependence
.03
.12*
.03
.00
.13*
.04
Sentimentality
.13*
.06
.07
.11*
.01
.01
Domain score
.18**
.15**
.11
.13*
.09
.01
Extraversion
Social self-esteem
.01
.02
.36***
.17**
.23***
.46***
Social boldness
.10
.25***
.03
.05
.26***
.13*
Sociability
.14*
.08
.00
.15**
.07
.10
Liveliness
.07
.03
.17**
.15**
.12*
.25***
Domain score
.11
.13*
.16**
.16**
.23***
.31***
Agreeableness
Forgiveness
.07
.19***
.12*
.02
.15**
.03
Gentleness
.01
.29***
.09
.09
.30***
.05
Flexibility
.21***
.28***
.17*
.12*
.20***
.01
Patience
.04
.16**
.13*
.14*
.14*
.09
Domain score
.08
.30***
.17**
.04
.26***
.03
Stoeber, Corr, Smith, & Saklofske 28
Conscientiousness
Organization
.41***
.05
.03
.45***
.05
.15**
Diligence
.60***
.17**
.04
.65***
.07
.33***
Perfectionism
.67***
.15**
.13*
.69***
.04
.23***
Prudence
.35***
.02
.05
.42***
.02
.22***
Domain score
.64***
.12*
.05
.70***
.02
.32***
Openness
Aesthetic appreciation
.04
.14*
.10
.02
.11
.03
Inquisitiveness
.06
.09
.09
.02
.04
.04
Creativity
.04
.03
.12*
.10
.02
.14**
Unconventionality
.05
.02
.07
.03
.03
.06
Domain score
.04
.10
.13*
.03
.04
.09
Note. N = 321 university students (50 male, 271 female). SOP = self-oriented perfectionism,
OOP = other-oriented perfectionism, SPP = socially prescribed perfectionism. Partial
correlations = correlations of SOP controlling for OOP and SPP, SPP controlling for SOP
and OOP, and OOP controlling for SOP and SPP. Domain score = total score aggregated
across the four facets. Data from Stoeber (2014a, Study 2).
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
... Perfectionism can be considered a multidimensional personality disposition that interferes with the life domains of an individual in the course of their existence (Stoeber, 2018) and, perhaps, for this reason, has generated a great deal of curiosity and investigation, especially in the sports context (Hill & Madigan, 2017;Stoeber, 2018). When deemed a personality disposition, perfectionism is understood as a phenomenon that can be learned and shaped by experiences throughout life and changed over time through situations given by the social context (Stoeber, 2018), differently from when it is considered a personality trait, which is regarded as general, stable and responsible for consistent patterns of behavior (Stoeber, Corr, Smith, & Saklofske, 2018). ...
... Perfectionism can be considered a multidimensional personality disposition that interferes with the life domains of an individual in the course of their existence (Stoeber, 2018) and, perhaps, for this reason, has generated a great deal of curiosity and investigation, especially in the sports context (Hill & Madigan, 2017;Stoeber, 2018). When deemed a personality disposition, perfectionism is understood as a phenomenon that can be learned and shaped by experiences throughout life and changed over time through situations given by the social context (Stoeber, 2018), differently from when it is considered a personality trait, which is regarded as general, stable and responsible for consistent patterns of behavior (Stoeber, Corr, Smith, & Saklofske, 2018). ...
... Perfectionism can be considered a multidimensional personality disposition that interferes with the life domains of an individual in the course of their existence (Stoeber, 2018) and, perhaps, for this reason, has generated a great deal of curiosity and investigation, especially in the sports context (Hill & Madigan, 2017;Stoeber, 2018). When deemed a personality disposition, perfectionism is understood as a phenomenon that can be learned and shaped by experiences throughout life and changed over time through situations given by the social context (Stoeber, 2018), differently from when it is considered a personality trait, which is regarded as general, stable and responsible for consistent patterns of behavior (Stoeber, Corr, Smith, & Saklofske, 2018). Currently, perfectionism is taken as a complex event associated with personal and interpersonal components that establishes relations with different psychological phenomena, such as stress and psychological wellbeing (Chang, 2006). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study investigated the association between the perfectionism traits of 140 futsal athletes and their teams’ performance markers. The athletes were divided into two groups: Medalists (n = 29) and Non-medalists (n = 111). The instruments used were The Sport Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale-2 (SMPS-II) and the official game bulletins for collecting performance markers (goals scored, goals conceded, number of wins, number of losses, and points added in the competition). Data analysis was conducted using the Mann-Whitney “U,” Spearman correlation, and Path Analysis. The results showed that the non-medal players present more doubts in action than the medalists (p = 0.008). Perfectionist strivings had a significant and positive association with performance markers (b = 0.17) and negative association with goals conceded (b = -0.23) in medal players, while perfectionist concerns were negatively associated (b > -0.20) with non-medalist markers. It was concluded that adaptive perfectionism could be considered an intervening factor in collective performance in futsal, especially in successful teams.
... Early approaches describe perfectionism as a one-dimensional construct (e.g., Burns, 1980); however, the model that currently seems most appropriate is a multidimensional conceptualization that considers that psychological processes and consequences may differ between various dimensions of perfectionism (Stoeber, 2018a). The multidimensional model of perfectionism, introduced by Hewitt and Flett (1991), is one of the most common models in perfectionism research (Stoeber et al., 2018). Acknowledging that perfectionism has not only personal but also social aspects, the model and its associated measurethe multidimensional perfectionism scale (MPS; Flett, 1991, 2004)-propose three dimensions of perfectionism depending on source and direction: self-oriented, socially prescribed, and other-oriented perfectionism. ...
... We could not provide evidence for a link between leaders' perfectionism and transformational leadership; however, for monitoring (i.e., management by exception) and servant leadership behaviors, our findings support our assumptions. Selforiented perfectionism has been linked to nurturance goals and altruism, indicating that self-oriented perfectionists are inclined to behave in a prosocial manner (Stoeber, 2014;Stoeber et al., 2018). Accordingly, self-oriented perfectionistic leaders were less likely to display management by exception (Study 1) and were more likely to manifest servant leadership behavior (Study 2). ...
Article
Full-text available
Despite the growing interest in perfectionism and its many facets, there is a lack of research on this phenomenon in the context of leadership. Attending to this deficit, the present study is the first to investigate the relationship between the three facets of perfectionism (self-oriented, socially prescribed, and other-oriented perfectionism) and three types of self-rated leadership behavior. In Study 1 (N = 182), leaders’ perfectionism and its association to their organizational, goal-oriented leadership behavior—self-rated as transactional (management by exception) and transformational leadership—is explored. In Study 2 (N = 185), the relationship of leaders’ perfectionism to their servant leadership as a people-centered leadership behavior is investigated. In line with the perfectionism social disconnection model (PSDM), we assume other-oriented and socially prescribed perfectionism to be positively related to management by exception (i.e., monitoring behavior) and negatively related to transformational and servant leadership, whereas the opposite pattern is primarily predicted for self-oriented perfectionism. Our findings in Study 1 reveal a negative relationship between leaders’ self-oriented perfectionism as well as positive relationships to their other-oriented and socially prescribed perfectionism in management by exception, while no substantial correlations with transformational leadership have emerged. In Study 2, a negative association between other-oriented perfectionism and the forgiveness dimension of servant leadership is revealed, indicating a possible barrier to building interpersonal relationships of acceptance and trust. Additionally, self-oriented perfectionism has been proven to be a rather favorable trait in servant leadership.
... Despite these well-established general views, scientific literature on perfectionism has been and still is debating some basic issues about the construct, ranging from whether it represents a personality trait or a disposition to which life experiences contribute to its development [2,3]. ...
... In a very recent contribution, perfectionism is conceptualized and defined as "a multidimensional personality disposition characterized by a striving for flawlessness and setting exceedingly high standards of performance accompanied by overly critical evaluations of one's behavior" [8] (p. 3). This definition captures the core idea embedded in early efforts [9] posing that one's tendency to perfectionism may influence personal and interpersonal experiences. ...
Article
Full-text available
The present research focused on the general theme of perfectionism in the sport domain, and it provided the first empirical validation of the original 72-item “Multidimensional Inventory of Perfectionism in Sport” (MIPS) among Italian athletes. The study, specifically, also focused on the relations linking personal and interpersonal components of perfectionism to athletes’ competitive anxiety. The research overall relied on data from 644 Italian sport science students and professional athletes and included both cross-sectional and longitudinal designs. Data analyses primarily focused on structural equation modeling, and the findings overall supported the psychometric and construct validity of the Italian version of the MIPS, also highlighting the key role of the personal components of perfectionism.
... There is a consensus among researchers that the five-factor model (FFM) characterizes the key structure of personality, encompassing neuroticism, extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness, and conscientiousness (Costa & McCrae, 1992). These personality dimensions, referred to as the "Big Five", are sometimes represented through a bipolar continuum (neuroticism versus emotional stability, extraversion versus introversion) or labelled with different term (extraversion may be labelled energy, openness may be labelled intellect) (Stoeber et al., 2017). However, they all describe the same five broad personality dimensions. ...
Article
This study investigated the relationship between the Big Three Perfectionism Scale–Short Form (BTPS-SF) (Rigid, Self-critical, and Narcissistic perfectionism) and the ten facets of the Big Five Questionnaire (BFQ) through network analysis. The BTPS-SF and the BFQ were administered to 473 Italian workers. Both centrality indexes (Expected Influence and Node Predictability) and bridge dimensions were calculated. Stability and accuracy were also checked to ensure the trustworthiness and reliability of the findings. The network analysis showed that BTPS-SF Rigid Perfectionism had the highest centrality and a bridge function, whereas BFQ Dominance had a bridge function. Findings suggested that BTPS-SF Rigid Perfectionism and BFQ Dominance are relevant in linking perfectionism and personality traits. Although further studies are needed, the network approach can provide a promising realm for understanding relationships between perfectionism and personality traits.
... In order to thoroughly explain and understand perfectionism, it is very important to examine the dimensions of perfectionism in the context of personality theories and models, based on the psycholexical and psychobiological paradigm (Stoeber, Corr, Smith, & Saklofske 2018). The Revised Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory (rRST; Gray & McNaughton 2000), which represents the psychobiological personality theory, was chosen as the reference framework of this research. ...
Article
Full-text available
The main goal of this research was to examine the role of personality traits in predicting the two aspects of perfectionism – maladaptive evaluation concerns and positive strivings. Personality traits were postulated by the Revised Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory, while the dimensions of perfectionism were defined by the Model of Perfectionism developed by Frost et al. The sample consisted of 299 students of the Faculty of Philosophy in Niš (79.3% were female), aged 19 to 29 years (M = 20.34; SD = 1.83). The instruments used were: the Reinforcement Sensitivity Questionnaire (RSQ), and the Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale (MPS). The main takeaways of this research indicate that the Behavioural Inhibition System (BIS) is the strongest and positive correlate of maladaptive evaluation concerns, followed by Freeze and Fight. Unlike personality defence systems, the Behavioural Approach System (BAS) shows a negative correlation with the abovementioned criterion. The total percentage of explained variance for maladaptive evaluation concerns is 23%. When positive strivings are used as the criterion, BAS is seen as a positive correlate only in the first step of the hierarchical regression analysis. However, Flight and Fight represent consistent and positive correlates of this criterion. The total percentage of explained variance for positive strivings is 6.9%. The findings are consistent with the Revised Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory and available empirical findings from previous studies. Moreover, the results indicate that in the context of the abovementioned personality theory, maladaptive evaluation concerns and positive strivings can be understood as maladaptive forms of perfectionism, which deviates from previous understandings of perfectionism.
... As for Extraversion, existing empirical findings are uncertain (Dunkley & Kyparissis, 2008;Flett & Hewitt, 2006;Molnar et al., 2012;Rice et al., 2007;Stoeber et al., 2009). A possible explanation may be the factors related to extraversion, such as assertiveness and activity (Stoeber, Corr, Smith, & Saklofske, 2018). The latter is worth discussing as it challenges the view of a positive association between adaptive perfectionism and well-being. ...
Article
Full-text available
Perfectionism is a personality trait that plays an important role in understanding human behavior and functioning. There has been a focus on the negative aspects and outcomes of perfectionism, and less is known about whether and how perfectionism relates to adaptive characteristics of personality and normal functioning. We investigated associations between different aspects of perfectionism and psychological well-being in two studies by determining the role of dispositional flow and personality traits in this relationship. In Study 1, participants completed questionnaires for perfectionism, psychological well-being and flow. In Study 2, personality traits from the HEXACO model of personality were additionally measured. We found that psychological well-being had a positive correlation with conscientious perfectionism and a negative correlation with self-evaluative perfectionism. Flow mediates the relationship between conscientious perfectionism and psychological well-being. There was no correlation between self-evaluative perfectionism and dispositional flow. After controlling for relevant personality traits, dispositional flow remains the mediator between conscientious perfectionism and psychological well-being, but the relation becomes negative. Implications for the understanding of how different components of perfectionism are related to psychological well-being and how flow experience contributes to this relationship are discussed.
... Additionally, each of these dimensions has unique relationships with basic personality traits. Perfectionistic strivings correlate positively with conscientiousness, whereas perfectionistic concerns with neuroticism (Ulu & Tezer, 2010;Stoeber et al., 2018). Moreover, perfectionism, especially perfectionistic concerns play a significant role in the development of mental disorders, such as depression, anxiety, eating, obsessive-compulsive and somatic disorders (Enns et al., 2002;Reuther et al., 2013;Sirois & Molnar, 2014;Slof-Op't et al., 2016). ...
Article
Full-text available
Recent years have been marked by a rise in perfectionism in developed countries. Studies have shown that dimensions of perfectionism are related to the way people handle identity crises, whose successful resolution leads to finding a place within the adult community. In the present study, we aimed to show that two dimensions of perfectionism-perfectionistic strivings and perfectionistic concerns-are related to the outcomes of identity formation and that these relationships are mediated by shame and guilt-proneness, and self-esteem. A path mediation model showed that shame and self-esteem fully mediate the relationship between two dimensions of perfectionism and community-identity. These findings have both theoretical and practical implications. Firstly, they show the mechanism by which perfectionism may be related to identity stage resolution. Secondly, our results support the distinction between perfectionistic strivings expressing drive towards excellence, and perfectionistic concerns expressing fear of failure and fear of being judged in identity studies. This in turn may contribute to more fitting and nuanced psychotherapeutic interventions, as it justifies the application of psychotherapeutic tools aimed at reducing perfectionistic concerns and enhancing perfectionistic strivings.
... Perfectionism was generally unrelated to concurrent levels of pain severity and functional disability at IIPT admission and discharge, with only SPP demonstrating a small association with functional disability at discharge; these findings are generally consistent with prior research which fails to demonstrate proximal relations between perfectionism and pain-related outcomes (Randall et al., 2018c). This study also supports the conceptualization of perfectionism as a stable personality characteristic (Stoeber et al., 2018) that is perhaps less amenable to change during a 3-6 week-long IIPT. Trajectory analyses indicated that SPP did not significantly change across treatment, and although the "No/Minimal Disability" group appreciated a statistically significant decrease in SOP, this reduction was minute. ...
Article
Objective Several factors are known to impact response to the intensive interdisciplinary pain treatment (IIPT) program described in this study, yet no research has explored the role of perfectionism. This secondary data analysis explored direct and indirect relations between perfectionism and functional disability (primary outcome) and pain severity (secondary outcome) after IIPT, with pain catastrophizing and fear of pain as mediators. Methods Youth (N = 253) aged 8–21 with chronic pain and associated disability completed pre- and post-IIPT measures of self-oriented perfectionism (SOP), socially prescribed perfectionism (SPP), pain catastrophizing, fear of pain, functional disability, and pain characteristics for routine clinical care and this nonrandomized trial. Eight mediated models were run for the two predictors, two mediators, and two outcomes. Results Pretreatment perfectionism (SOP and SPP) led to greater reductions in pain catastrophizing over the course of IIPT, which resulted in lower pain severity (β = −.02 [CI = −0.07, −0.01] for SOP and β = −.02 [CI = −0.06, −0.003] for SPP) and less functional disability (β = −.06 [CI = −0.13, −0.01] for SOP and β = −.06 [CI = −0.14, −0.01] for SPP). Independent of pain catastrophizing, pretreatment SPP was directly associated with more posttreatment functional disability (β = .16 [CI = 0.05, 0.27]). Fear of pain was not a mediator. Conclusions Findings suggest perfectionism has the potential to negatively impact IIPT outcomes. However, when perfectionistic youth with chronic pain learn to manage pain-related distress, they benefit. Results highlight the importance of assessing for and treating perfectionism and pain-related distress in youth with chronic pain.
... The HEXACO model is a six-dimension framework for personality that accommodates several personality variables that are poorly assimilated within the five-factor model (Ashton & Lee, 2007). For example, the HEXACO model suggests that the fivefactor model is missing an important dimension of personality labeled honesty-humility, which differentiates people who are sincere from those who are sly, greedy, and hypocritical (Stoeber et al., 2018). Moreover, previous studies have shown that both Other-Oriented Perfectionism and Socially Prescribed Perfectionism are associated with low honesty-humility (Stoeber, 2014). ...
Article
Previous investigations on the factor structure of perfectionism have largely focused on the Frost Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale and the Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale. The current study aimed to identify the underlying factor structure of perfectionism, based on several widely used measures, and to examine how these factors related to psychopathology and personality broadly. College students (N = 598) completed several measures of perfectionism and broadband measures of psychopathology and personality. Exploratory structural equation modeling (ESEM) was conducted to examine the hierarchical structure of perfectionism followed by exploratory factor analysis. The hierarchical structure examined provides a framework for understanding the relationship between models of perfectionism at different levels of the hierarchy. The exploratory factor analysis revealed five dimensions of perfectionism: Achievement Striving, Evaluative Concerns, Expectations From Others, Narcissistic Perfectionism, and Organization. These dimensions were associated with psychopathology to differing degrees and were differentially related to personality. These results support using a multidimensional perspective to understand perfectionism.
Article
Full-text available
Cél A Hewitt–Flett Multidimenzionális Perfekcionizmus Kérdőív validálása, a tehetség-tanácsadásban felhasználható magyar verzió kialakítása. Az adaptív és maladaptív perfekcionizmussal együtt járó motivációs mintázatok feltérképezése, továbbá a perfekcionizmus dimenzióinak feltáró vizsgálata különböző oktatási szinteken, illetve tehetséggondozó tagozaton tanuló és normál középiskolai populáció között. Módszer önbeszámolós kérdőívek, megerősítő faktoranalízis, nonparaméteres eljárások. Minta A vizsgálatban összesen 446 fő vett részt. A 16–19 éves középiskolás tanulók száma: 265 fő (59,4%); a felsőoktatásban tanulók létszáma: 181 fő (40,6%). A középiskolai tanulók közül 123 f\xC5\x91 (a teljes minta 27,6%-a) tehetséggondozó tagozaton tanul. Eszközök Kérdőíves eljárásokat alkalmaztunk, melyek a következők: Hewitt–Flett-féle (1991) Multidimenzionális perfekcionizmus skála, Lang és Fries-féle (2006) Teljesítménymotiváció Kérdőív, illetve a Tehetség Motivációs Öndefiníciós Kérdőív (Dávid, 2011). Eredmények A magyar mintán kapott eredmények összhangban állnak a nemzetközi standardokkal. A tehetségesek szignifikánsan magasabb perfekcionizmussal jellemezhetők, s nemi különbségek is igazolhatók. A társas előírásként megélt (maladaptív) perfekcionizmus együtt jár a kudarckerüléssel, negatívan korrelál az akarat, kitartás, általános aktivitás faktorokkal. A szelforientált (adaptív) perfekcionizmus pozitívan korrelál a magas igényszinttel, a sikerkereséssel, továbbá a szándék/akarat, kitartás, érdeklődés/tudásvágy, valamint általános aktivitás faktorokkal. Igazoltuk a motivációs korrelátumok eltérő mintázatát, illetve a perfekcionizmus skálákban kimutatható különbségek létét almintánként és oktatási szintenként egyaránt.
Article
Full-text available
Depression is well known to share a negative cross-sectional relationship with personality constructs defined by positive emotion (positive affect, extraversion, behavioral activation). These Positive Emotionality (PE) constructs have been proposed to represent stable temperamental risk factors for depression, not merely current mood state. These constructs have also been proposed to increase risk specifically for depression, relative to anxiety. We performed a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies to examine the relationship of PE to depression (59 effect sizes) and anxiety (26 effect sizes). In cross-sectional analyses, PE constructs were negatively associated with depression (r = -.34) and anxiety (r = -.24). PE constructs also prospectively predicted depression (r = -.26) and anxiety (r = -.19). These relationships remained statistically significant, but were markedly attenuated, when baseline levels of depression (β = -.08) and anxiety (β = -.06) were controlled. Moreover, depression and anxiety were equally strong predictors of subsequent changes in PE (β = -.07 and -.09, respectively). These findings are consistent with theoretical accounts of low PE as a temperamental vulnerability for depression, but suggest that the prospective relationship of PE to depression may be weaker and less specific than previously assumed. (PsycINFO Database Record
Article
In a recent study, Stoeber and Corr (2015) examined how three forms of perfectionism (self-oriented, other-oriented, socially prescribed) predicted participants’ affective experiences in the past two weeks, and found that revised Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory (rRST) components explained the relations between perfectionism and affective experiences. As an extension, this study investigated whether rRST components—capturing individual differences in the Behavioral Approach System (BAS), Behavioral Inhibition System (BIS), Fight-Flight-Freeze System (FFFS), and defensive fight—also explained the relations between perfectionism and future-directed thinking. 343 university students completed measures of perfectionism, rRST, and positive and negative expectations for the next two weeks. Mediation analyses showed that all BAS components (reward interest, goal-drive persistence, reward reactivity, impulsivity) and the BIS, but not the FFFS and defensive fight, explained how the different forms of perfectionism predicted future-directed expectations. The findings suggest that the BAS and BIS components of rRST, which reflect fundamental emotion-motivational systems of personality, play a role not only in the relations of perfectionism and past affective experiences, but also in those of perfectionism and future-directed thinking.
Article
Cross-cultural invariance of personality measurement provides important information regarding the universality of personality traits. With the recent release of historical data from 33 countries on the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQ) the opportunity arose to test the invariance of the three personality dimensions measured by the EPQ, together with the response set scale. Although the factor structure of the EPQ has been much studied in previous decades, there was a need to validate the previously reported four-factor structure using modern factor analytic techniques. As anticipated, both exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis provided unsatisfactory models, for different reasons. Instead, exploratory structural equation modeling provided confirmation of the hypothesized oblique, four-factor structure. Results of measurement invariance comparisons across countries, separately by gender, showed striking evidence of the generalizability of the four-factor structure in pairwise comparisons with the English sample as the reference group. In addition, there was evidence of failure of item invariance, for a varying subset of items, in all of the between-country comparisons. However, the pattern of partial measurement invariance does not preclude effective use of the EPQ as a research tool in diverse cultures. The results provide strong theoretical convergence, with other published studies, on the universality of the four-factor structure.
Article
According to the two-factor theory of perfectionism (Stoeber & Otto, 2006), perfectionism comprises two superordinate dimensions—perfectionistic strivings (PS) and perfectionistic concerns (PC)—that show different, and often opposite, relations with psychological adjustment and maladjustment, particularly when their overlap is partialled out. Recently, Hill (2014) raised concerns about the interpretation of the relations that PS show after partialling. The present article aims to alleviate these concerns. First, we address the concern that partialling changes the conceptual meaning of PS. Second, we explain how the relations of residual PS (i.e., PS with PC partialled out) differ from those of PS, and how to interpret these differences. In this, we also discuss suppressor effects and how mutual suppression affects the relations of both PS and PC with outcomes. Furthermore, we provide recommendations of how to report and interpret findings of analyses partialling out the effects of PS and PC. We conclude that, if properly understood and reported, there is nothing to be concerned about when partialling PS and PC. On the contrary, partialling is essential if we want to understand the shared, unique, combined, and interactive relations of the different dimensions of perfectionism.
Article
We report the development of a self-report questionnaire of the reinforcement sensitivity theory (RST) of personality for use with children. Focus groups were held with children to sample their experiences of situations modelled on components of three RST systems: fight-flight-freeze system (FFFS, related to fear), behavioural inhibition system (BIS, related to anxiety), and behavioural approach system (BAS, related to approach). The thematic responses formed the conceptual anchors to the development of test items that were examined using exploratory factor analysis in a sample of 288 9–13 year olds. After eliminating items that did not load on their designated factor, or substantially cross-loaded across factors, the original 48 items were reduced to 21 items: 7 items for each of the BIS, FFFS and BAS factors extracted from the data. The separation of the BIS and FFFS items across two factors is consistent with the revised model of RST. We offer this new questionnaire as a RST measure of fundamental motivation and emotion traits in children.