ArticlePDF Available

A computational model of perceptual expectation effect based on neural coding principles

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Abstract: Prior expectation affects posterior perceptual experience. This contextual bias is called expectation effect. Previous studies have observed two different patterns of expectation effect: contrast and assimilation. Contrast magnifies the perceived incongruity, and assimilation diminishes the incongruity. This study proposes a computational model that explains the conditions of contrast and assimilation based on neural coding principles. This model proposed that prediction error, uncertainty, and external noise affected the expectation effect. Computer simulations with the model show that the pattern of expectation effect shifted from assimilation to contrast as the prediction error increased, uncertainty decreased the extent of the expectation effect, and external noise increased the assimilation. We conducted an experiment on the size–weight illusion (SWI) as a case of the cross-modal expectation effect and discussed correspondence with the simulation. We discovered conditions where the participants perceived bigger object to be heavier than smaller one, which contradicts to conventional SWI. Practical applications: Expectation effect in sensory perception represents a perceptual bias caused by prior expectation, such as illusions and cross-modality. The computational model proposed in this study guides researchers and practitioners who investigate this bias in sensory studies to set a hypothesis with appropriate experimental factors. For example, the model suggests that prediction error can be used as a main factor to identify a condition at which assimilation switches over to contrast. The model provided how expectation uncertainty and noise of stimulus affect the switchover point of prediction error and extent of expectation effect. Uncertainty, which may differ from person to person, can be used as a factor to explain personal differences in the extent of expectation effect.
Content may be subject to copyright.
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
A computational model of perceptual expectation eect based
on neural coding principles
Hideyoshi Yanagisawa
Department of Mechanical Engineering, The
University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
Correspondence
H. Yanagisawa, Department of Mechanical
Engineering, The University of Tokyo, 7-3-
1, Hongo, Bunkyo, Tokyo, Japan.
Email: hide@mail.design.t.u-tokyo.ac.jp
Funding Information
This work was supported by KAKEN (No.
23760127).
Abstract
Prior expectation aects posterior perceptual experience. This contextual bias is called expectation
eect. Previous studies have observed two dierent patterns of expectation eect: contrast and
assimilation. Contrast magnies the perceived incongruity, and assimilation diminishes the incon-
gruity. This study proposes a computational model that explains the conditions of contrast and
assimilation based on neural coding principles. This model proposed that prediction error, uncer-
tainty, and external noise aected the expectation eect. Computer simulations with the model
show that the pattern of expectation eect shifted from assimilation to contrast as the prediction
error increased, uncertainty decreased the extent of the expectation eect, and external noise
increased the assimilation. We conducted an experiment on the sizeweight illusion (SWI) as a
case of the cross-modal expectation eect and discussed correspondence with the simulation. We
discovered conditions where the participants perceived bigger object to be heavier than smaller
one, which contradicts to conventional SWI.
Practical applications
Expectation eect in sensory perception represents a perceptual bias caused by prior expectation,
such as illusions and cross-modality. The computational model proposed in this study guides
researchers and practitioners who investigate this bias in sensory studies to set a hypothesis with
appropriate experimental factors. For example, the model suggests that prediction error can be
used as a main factor to identify a condition at which assimilation switches over to contrast. The
model provided how expectation uncertainty and noise of stimulus aect the switchover point of
prediction error and extent of expectation eect. Uncertainty, which may dier from person to
person, can be used as a factor to explain personal dierences in the extent of expectation eect.
1
|
INTRODUCTION
Expectation congruity works as an appraisal component that evokes
emotions, such as surprise (Ludden, Schierstein, & Hekkert, 2012),
satisfaction (Oliver, 1977, 1980), and disappointment (Demir, Desmet,
& Hekkert, 2009). For example, a user expects the usability from a
product appearance (This looks easy to use.), and determines the
actual usability (This is really easy to use.). A positive expectation dis-
crepancy (This is easier to use than expected!) provides a satisfaction.
In contrast, a negative expectation discrepancy (This is harder to use
than expected.) disappoints the user. Furthermore, prior expectations
aect and change posterior perception and experience. Researchers
from a broad range of elds have observed this psychological bias, the
so-called expectation eect, with regard to dierent cognitive proc-
esses, such as desire for rewards (Schultz, Dayan, & Montague, 1997),
emotions (Geers & Lassiter, 1999; Wilson, Lisle, Kraft, & Wetzel, 1989),
and sensory perceptions (Deliza & MacFie, 1996; Schierstein, 2001).
The expectation eect changes the disconrmation between expecta-
tion and experience. Thus, the expectation eect is an essential factor
to ensure the satisfactory design of products and services.
In a time sequence of UX of a product and service, users shift
from one sensory state to another in cyclic interactions involving
action, sensation, and perception (Krippendor, 2005). Figure 1 illus-
trates an example of such state transitions while using a camera. A user
perceives the appearance by looking (vision), the tactile feeling by
grasping (touch), and shutter sound by popping a shutter (audition). We
expect that users would predict subsequent states between these tran-
sitions of state (e.g., we expect a meal to taste a certain way based on
how it looks, the weight of a product before lifting it, the usability of a
mouse by looking at it). This prior prediction aects posterior
430
|
V
C2016 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/joss J. Sens. Stud. 2016; 31: 430-439
Received: 28 April 2016
|
Revised: 25 June 2016
|
Accepted: 16 August 2016
DOI 10.1111/joss.12233
perception, that is, the expectation eect. For example, visual expecta-
tion changes tactile perceptions of surface texture (Yanagisawa &
Takatsuji, 2015a).
We can explain a kind of perceptual illusion using the expectation
eect. For example, people perceive a smaller object as heavier than a
larger one although the weight of both objects is identical (Flanagan,
Bittner, & Johansson, 2008). This well-known sizeweight illusion
(SWI) can be explained as a visual expectation eect. People expect a
larger object to be heavier than a smaller one. Prior visual expectation
of the objectsweights magnies the perception of dierence between
the expected and actual weights. Although many experimental ndings
exist on the expectation eect in dierent disciplines, the general
mechanism on why and how the eect occurs is not yet clearly eluci-
dated. A mathematical model of the expectation eect based on a fun-
damental mechanism enables us to estimate user perception of
product and service. Yanagisawa and Takatsuji (2015b) proposed a
mathematical model of the expectation eect using information theory.
They modeled prior expectation as a subjective probability distribution
and hypothesized that Shannons entropy of the distributions repre-
senting uncertainty of prior expectation determines the occurrence of
the expectation eect. An experimental result of the visual expectation
eect of tactile texture showed that the lower entropy invoked expec-
tation eect in a higher rate than another. This nding suggests that
uncertainty is a factor of occurrence of expectation eect.
On the other hand, two dierent patterns of expectation eect,
contrast and assimilation, were observed (Deliza & MacFie, 1996). Con-
trast is a bias that magnies the dierence between prior expectation
and posterior experience. Assimilation is a bias that diminishes expecta-
tion incongruence. It is important to understand whether the expecta-
tion eect is contrasting or assimilating, because they exaggerate or
diminish the perception of expectation disconrmation as a factor of
satisfaction. However, the mechanisms and conditions governing the
contrasting and assimilating patterns are not yet clearly elucidated. In
this study, we propose a simulation model of the expectation eect
that explains the conditions of contrast and assimilation by applying
neural coding principles, such as Bayesian decoding and the ecient
encoding principles. Based on the proposed model, we conduct com-
puter simulations of the expectation eect and obtain an accurate
hypothesis of the conditions of assimilation and contrast. Finally, we
apply the obtained hypothesis to externalize a perceptual law behind
SWI as an expectation eect from a result of an experiment with the
participants.
2
|
COMPUTATIONAL MODEL OF
EXPECTATION EFFECT
2.1
|
Bayesian coding in perception involving prior
expectation
We dene perception as an estimation of external physical property,
such as the weight of an object. Sensory stimulus from the external
physical world, such as pressure applied to a hand, is transformed to
patterns of neural signals. We call the neural representation of an
external physical variable encoding. Based on the pattern of neural sig-
nals, our brain estimates the physical variable. We call this estimation
process decoding. We assume that sensory stimuli are encoded as cer-
tain ring rates of neural populations. This type of neural coding is
called rate coding. Another type of encoding is a temporal coding
hypothesis where the precise timing of spikes adds information. How-
ever, most studies of temporal coding have concentrated on single
brain areas and only a few have reported how timing codes are trans-
formed across dierent stages of a sensory pathway. Evidence for tem-
poral coding is less established in higher cognitive brain area (Quian
Quiroga & Panzeri, 2009). Thus, we assume rate coding for further dis-
cussion. Based on the ring rate distributions from a sensory stimulus,
R, our brain forms the likelihood function,PRjuðÞ, of a physical variable,
u. On the other hand, a physical property has certain frequency distri-
butions in the world. Human beings learn these frequency distributions
throughout their life. Based on these learnt distributions, human beings
predict a physical variable, u, before experiencing sensory stimulus. For
example, in the SWI, people predict the weight of an object by looking
at it before actually lifting it up. The predicted physical variable should
follow certain probability distributions. We dene this distribution as
prior,PðuÞ. Recent studies in neuroscience showed that the estimation
of a physical variable, that is, decoding, follows the Bayesian estimator
(Brayanov & Smith, 2010; Ernst & Banks, 2002). Based on Bayestheo-
rem, our brain estimates the distributions of perceptions or posterior,
PujR
ðÞ
, using prior and likelihood.
PujRðÞ5PRju
ðÞ
Pu
ðÞ
XXPRjuðÞPuðÞ (1)
Since the denominator of the right-hand side of Equation 1isa
constant for normalization, the posterior is proportional to the product
of prior and likelihood.
PujRðÞ/PRjuðÞPuðÞ (2)
If the distributions of prior and likelihood follow the Gaussian dis-
tributions and are independent from each other, we can calculate the
optimal estimate of posterior ^
uby the following equation (K
ording &
Wolpert, 2004).
^
u5V½PðuÞE½PðRjuÞ1V½PðRjuÞE½PðuÞ
V½PðuÞ1V½PðRjuÞ (3)
where EðpÞdenotes mean of distribution p,andVðpÞdenotes a var-
iance of p. The numerator of the right side in Equation 3represents
sum of prior mean EðPðuÞÞ weighted with likelihood variation VðPðRjuÞÞ
and likelihood mean EðPðRjuÞÞ weighted with prior variation VðPðuÞÞ.
appearance
vision
look
tactile feel
touch
grasp
shutter sound
audition
press button
FIGURE 1 Sensory transitions of product use (a case of camera)
YANAGISAWA
|
431
The denominator of the right side in Equation 3 is a sum of variations
for normalization. The estimate is in the middle of prior mean and likeli-
hood mean when the prior variation and likelihood variation are same.
The estimate comes close to the prior mean when prior variation is big-
ger than likelihood variation. In an opposite manner, the estimate
comes close to likelihood when likelihood variation is bigger than prior
variation. Equation 3 indicates that the posterior estimate, ^
u,isalways
between prior mean and likelihood peak. Figure 2 illustrates how pos-
terior estimate comes close to a peak of prior from a peak of the likeli-
hood estimate of the sensory stimulus. We call the eect attractive
inuence of prior.
2.2
|
Expectation eect and prediction error
Now, we can write the expectation eect, E,andprediction error,d
E5
^
u2u
lik (4)
d5ulik2E½PðuÞ (5)
where ulik denotes a likelihood peak. Using Eand d, we can formalize
the condition of contrast as jd1Ej>jdjand the condition of assimilation
as jd1Ej<jdj.jd1Ejrepresents the extent of perceived discrepancy
with expectation eect. Figure 3 illustrates contrast and assimilation.
Contrast is a type of expectation eect that exaggerates perceived dis-
crepancy, whereas assimilation is another type of expectation eect
that diminishes perceived discrepancy. Assume that the horizontal axis
of Figure 3 is weight, contrast makes people feel heavier than actual
weight, whereas assimilation makes people feel lighter than the actual
weight.
2.3
|
Ecient coding causes contrast
The attractive inuence of prior alone involves assimilation as an
expectation eect. The question then arises: How does contrast occur?
We applied a neural encoding framework based on the ecient coding
principle (Wei & Stocker, 2012). According to the encoding framework,
the Bayesian estimate shifts away from the peaks of the prior distribu-
tion. This phenomenon corresponds to the contrast pattern of the
expectation eect. Ecient coding hypothesis proposes that the tuning
characteristics of a neural population are adapted to the prior distribu-
tion of a sensory variable such that the neural population optimally rep-
resents the sensory variable (Barlow, 1961). This optimal neural coding
makes the total expected spike count of each neuron in the population
similar. To mathematically represent this code, the neural encoding
framework assumed an equivalent homogeneous space with units ~
uin
which the stimulus prior distribution is uniform and mapped the physi-
cal space with units uto the homogeneous space using a mapping
function F. The framework used cumulative of the prior Pas the map-
ping function as follows.
~
u5FuðÞ5ðu
21
PxðÞdx (6)
To map from ~
uto u, we can use the inverse function F21ð~
uÞ.Fig-
ure 4 illustrates the tuning curves of ecient neural populations and
the mapping to/from the homogeneous space for a prior of normal dis-
tributions pðuÞ. In the homogeneous space, we assume similar turning
curves for each neural population. The turning curves are assigned at
equally spaced intervals so that the expected spike count of each pop-
ulation is the same for a uniform prior. Then, we map the tuning curve
of each neural population to the physical space using F21.Figure4
shows that the tuning curves in the physical space are dense around
prior expectation (high prior density) and sparse toward away from the
expectation (on the side of low prior density). In summary, more neural
populations are allocated to sense a range of physical values that are
more likely to occur than others. This coding strategy maximizes
expected information amount for a prior. Due to the mapping using F,
the shape of tuning curve in the physical space gradually becomes
asymmetrical towards the side of lower prior density. This is attributable
to the shape of the mapping function F. The mapping function shapes S-
curve that closes to a linear around the prior expectation, but becomes
nonlinear toward away from prior mean. We assume that the likelihood
shape follows the shape of the tuning curves in the physical space.
The likelihood shape is constrained in physical space by the prior
distribution. The likelihood distributions in physical space transformed
from symmetric distributions, such as Gaussian, in homogeneous space
FIGURE 2 Attractive inuence of prior in Bayesian estimate
FIGURE 3 Illustration of contrast and assimilation
432
|
YANAGISAWA
shows heavier tails on the side of lower prior density. In sum, the e-
cient encoding typically leads to an asymmetrical likelihood function
whose mean value is away from the peak of prior. As shown in Equa-
tion 2, the Bayesian estimate is determined by a combination of prior
and shifted likelihood means,
ulik, and it shifts away from the prior peak
(Figure 5).
We apply this ecient encoding to explain contrast in our model.
The Bayesian estimate (perceived value), ^
u, shifts from a peak of the
asymmetric likelihood function away from a peak of prior. We call the
perceptual shift repulsion inuence.
2.4
|
Perceptual model with prior expectation
From the above discussion using Bayesian decoding and ecient cod-
ing principle, we can summarize our hypothetical model of perception
as shown in Figure 6. Based on the ecient encoding principle, prior
changes the shape of the likelihood function asymmetry while encod-
ing the sensory stimulus of the physical variable, u,asaring rate of
the neuron population R. The Bayesian decoder integrates the prior
distribution, PðuÞ, and asymmetric likelihood function, PðRjuÞ, and forms
posterior distributions. As a result, we perceive a peak of the posterior,
^
u, as an estimate of the physical variable, that is, perception.
2.5
|
Factors of expectation eect: prediction error,
uncertainty, and external noise
Based on the ecient coding, the likelihood shape shows heavier tails
on the side of lower prior density, and the likelihood means shifts away
from the prior peak. The extent of likelihood asymmetry and the likeli-
hood mean shift increases towards away from a prior peak. Thus, we
can predict that the repulsion bias increases towards away from a prior
peak. (Wei & Stocker, 2012) mathematically derived the prediction.
Based on their mathematic derivation, we can approximate the repul-
sion inuence at prediction error, bðuÞ, in the following dierential
equation.
bðuÞCd
du
1
PðuÞ2
! (7)
where Cis a positive constant, and PðuÞis prior distributions. If the
prior is Gaussian with zero mean, the gradient of 1=Pðjd2is positive
for any prediction error dand increases away from mean of prior. Thus,
Equation 7 indicates that the repulsion bias increases as the absolute
prediction error jdjincreases. This is because the extent of asymmetry
of likelihood increases away from the peak of prior. Repulsion inuence
involves contrast. Thus, the prediction error is a key factor that decides
the condition of the expectation eect.
We assume two more factors of the expectation eect: external
noise and uncertainty. The shape of the likelihood function is aected
by the noise of the external stimulus. An external noise modies the
FIGURE 4 How ecient coding makes likelihood in physical space asymmetry
lik
θ
lik
θ
ˆ
θ
δ
ε
FIGURE 5 Repulsion inuence due to asymmetry of likelihood
function
YANAGISAWA
|
433
shape of the likelihood function by convolving it with noise
distributions.
ðfgÞðuÞ5ðfðxÞgðu2xÞdx (8)
Where fis the asymmetric likelihood function, and gis a symmet-
ric external noise distribution. We assume that the external noise distri-
bution follows the Gaussian distribution.
gðuÞNðulik;r2noise Þ(9)
Symmetric external noise distributions do not change the mean of
likelihood, but they increase its overall width. Thus, the attractive inu-
ence of prior relatively increases, and the Bayesian estimate, ^
u,shifts
toward the peak of prior. If the attractive inuence of prior exceeds
the repulsion inuence of the asymmetric likelihood, the expectation
eect may change into assimilation from contrast.
Variations of prior distributions are indicators of prediction uncer-
tainty. As shown in formula (3), the variation in prior impacts the attrac-
tive inuence. In the Bayesian estimation, a small variation in prior
denotes small uncertainty and involves a strong attractive inuence.
Conversely, a big variation in prior means uncertain prediction and
involves weak attractive inuence.
From the above discussions, we can assume that the condition and
extent of expectation eect alters with the three factors, namely, pre-
diction error, d, uncertainty, V½PðuÞ, and external noise, r2noise.
3
|
EFFECT OF PREDICTION ERROR,
EXTERNAL NOISE, AND UNCERTAINTY ON
EXPECTATION EFFECT
3.1
|
Method
To investigate the eects of the three factors, that is, prediction error,
uncertainty, and external noise on the expectation eect, we con-
ducted a computer simulation using abovementioned model. We used
Gaussian distributions for prior, homogeneous likelihood, and posterior.
The following conditions were selected as simulation parameters: pre-
diction error, d, within [0, 100]; uncertainty, V½PðuÞ, within [50, 200];
and external noise, r2noise , within [5, 50]. The standard deviation of the
homogeneous likelihood is set as 0.04. We calculated the expectation
eect, E, using the simulation model for all combinations of the above-
mentioned conditions for the three factors. We use MATLAB® to con-
duct the simulations. The code is provided as a supporting information.
3.2
|
Result and discussion
Figure 7 shows an example of the simulation result of the expectation
eect as a function of the prediction error. Each line represents a con-
dition of uncertainty (small: 150, big: 180) and external noise (small: 30,
big: 40). A positive value represents contrast, and a negative value,
assimilation. Figure 7 reveals three ndings.
The expectation eect functions as an assimilating eect when
the prediction error is small. As the prediction error increases, the
expectation eect increases and changes to the contrasting condition
((1) in Figure 7). Around the peak of prior, where the prediction error is
small, the shape of the likelihood function was close to symmetry, the
repulsion inuence was small, and the attractive inuence of prior is
dominant. Thus, assimilation occurs. As the prediction error increases,
the extent of the likelihood asymmetry increases, and the repulsion
inuence increases. Thus, the expectation eect shifts to the contrast
condition.
ˆ
θ
P( )
θ
P( | )R
θ
θ
P( )|R
θ
FIGURE 6 A model of perception involving prior expectation
0 20 40 60 80 100 120 140 160 180 200
Prediction error
-1.5
-1
-0.5
0
0.5
1
1.5
2
2.5
Expectation effect
uncertainty=150, noise=30
uncertainty=150, noise=40
uncertainty=180, noise=30
uncertainty=180, noise=40
(2)
(2)
(2)
(2)
(3)
(1)
FIGURE 7 Expectation eect as a function of expectation error
for dierent conditions of expectation uncertainty and external
noise
434
|
YANAGISAWA
The extent of the expectation eect, jEj, is bigger when uncer-
tainty is lower for both assimilation and contrast ((2) in Figure 7). With
respect to assimilation, the attractive inuence of prior increases in the
Bayesian estimation as the variation of prior (uncertainty) decreases.
On the other hand, the repulsive inuence increases from a certain
value of prediction error as the variation of prior decreases. In sum, cer-
tain predictions involve a sharp expectation eect regardless of the
condition (contrast or assimilation).
The prediction error at which assimilation changes to contrast
increases as the external noise increases ((3) in Figure 7). External noise
weakens the repulsive inuence. In the Bayes estimation, the attractive
inuence of prior becomes stronger than the repulsive inuence of
likelihood. Thus, the area of assimilation in the prediction error
increases when the external noise exceeds prediction error and uncer-
tainty. We observed the abovementioned trends for all possible combi-
nations of conditions for uncertainty and external noise.
Figure 8 shows normalized prediction error when assimilation
switches over to contrast as a function of uncertainty for dierent
external noise condition. The prediction error, the vertical axis, is nor-
malized between zero and one. In sum, the vertical axis represents the
ratio of assimilation area in the range of prediction error. Zero in the
vertical axis represents a case where only contrast occurs, whereas one
represents a case where only assimilation occurs. With respect to small
external noise such as 5, only contrast occurs for all uncertainty condi-
tions. This is because the repulsion inuence due to ecient coding is
dominant with small external noise. Except the condition of noise 5,
only assimilation occurs when uncertainty is small, such as 50 for all
noise conditions. This is because attractive inuence with small uncer-
tainty is dominant in Bayesian decoding (as indicated in formula [3]).
The assimilation area decreases as uncertainty increases from certain
uncertainty condition. This is because the attractive inuence of prior
decreases against repulsion inuence caused by ecient coding.
Uncertainty at the area where assimilation starts to decrease increases
as noise increases because attractive inuence becomes relatively
stronger than noisier likelihood in Bayesian decoding. The above results
suggest that the switchover prediction error, assimilation area, depends
on the balance between attractive inuence of prior uncertainty in
Bayesian decoding and repulsion inuence due to asymmetric likeli-
hood in ecient coding.
Figure 9 shows contours of prediction errors when assimilation
changes to contrast for all combinations of uncertainty and external
noise. The prediction error, the z-axis, is normalized between zero and
one. Zero in the contour represents a case where only contrast occurs,
whereas one represents a case where only assimilation occurs. Figure 9
shows that the area where uncertainty is high and external noise is
small denotes cases where only contrast occurs. In this area, the repul-
sive inuence of asymmetry likelihood is dominant compared to the
attractive inuence of uncertain prediction. On the other hand, the
area with low uncertainty and big external noise shows only assimila-
tion. The attractive inuence of prior is dominant for certain predic-
tions compared to the repulsive inuence, which is weakened by the
external noise.
4
|
EXPERIMENT WITH SIZEWEIGHT
ILLUSION
4.1
|
Method
As discussed in the Introduction Section, SWI is a signicant case of
the cross modal expectation eect, where prior visual expectation
biases weight perception. To investigate the actual eect of the
three factors (i.e., prediction error, uncertainty, and external noise)
on SWI, we conducted a sensory experiment with the participants.
We manipulated the three factors of the expectation eect. For
each condition of the factors, we obtained responses from the par-
ticipants with respect to perceptions of weight, and evaluated the
extent of weight illusions as expectation eects. We presented the
participants with pairs of cubic metal objects. The objects in each
pair had identical weights but dierent sizes. We asked the partici-
pants to compare the weights and obtained their responses for the
dierence in weights. We used the perceptual dierence of weight
as the extent of the expectation eect.
Uncertainty
Switchover from assimilation to contrast
5
15
25
35
45
FIGURE 8 Expectation errors when assimilation switch over to
contrast as a function of uncertainty for dierent external noise
10 20 30 40 50
50
100
150
0.2
0.4
0.6
0.8
1
External noise
Uncertainty
Normalized expectation error
0
FIGURE 9 Expectation errors when assimilation shifts to contrast
for dierent conditions of uncertainty and external noise. Data is
provided as a supporting information
YANAGISAWA
|
435
According to conventional SWI, human beings perceive that a
smaller object is heavier than a larger object, although both objects
may have the same weight. This illusion can be viewed as a contrast of
the expectation eect, where the perception of dierence between
the weight predicted by the objects size and its actual weight, the pre-
diction error, is exaggerated. However, our simulation result in Figure 4
showed that assimilation, an opposite eect to contrast, occurs when
the prediction error is less than a certain value. To validate the simula-
tion results in SWI, we controlled the prediction errors in the experi-
ment. We prepared pairs of objects, called target and reference.
Participants evaluated the weight of the target by comparing it with
that of the reference. Figure 10 illustrates the weight and size of tar-
gets (t1t4) and references (r1r4). All samples have identical surface
material. References are solid objects made by identical material with
equal density. All targets have identical size in appearance. The visual
expectation of weight for all targets are marked xin Figure 10 from
the identical appearance (size and surface material). However, we
adjusted each target weight similar to each compared reference. The
dierence between the visually expected weight and the actual weight
works as the prediction error. In this setting, the target was always big-
ger than the reference for each pair. Therefore, when target is lighter
than the reference denotes contrast, whereas when target is heavier
than the reference denotes assimilation.
To control uncertainty of visual predictions, we used a fogged glass
so that transparency between the participants and target object was
manipulated. We assumed that fuzzy visual images of the targets
would increase the uncertainties of size and weight predictions. To
control the external noise of somatosensory sensation while lifting an
object, we asked the participants to add a weight to the wrist he/she
used for lifting the samples. According to the WeberFechner Law, the
dierence threshold increases as the intensity of stimulus increases.
We assumed that the additional weight of each participantswrist
serves as the external noise of weight perception due to the increasing
dierence threshold.
4.2
|
Material
We prepared ten solid cubic shapes made of duralumin (A2017) as a
set of reference samples. The weights of the reference samples ranged
from 350 g to 1250 g. Their sides were 5076.5 mm long. For the tar-
get samples, we prepared hollow cubes made of duralumin (A2017),
with sides measuring 80 mm. We inserted additional weights into the
hollow cubes so that the weights of both samples were identical for
each pair. We attached a wire to the top of each target and reference
sample and hung them from a steel framework to straighten the wires
without tension. We placed a ring in the middle of each wire to enable
the participants to lift the samples using their index ngers.
4.3
|
Participants
Fifteen (twelve male and three female) volunteers aged 21 to 24 years
served as experiment evaluators. They were undergraduate or graduate
students studying mechanical engineering at the University of Tokyo.
All participants were physically healthy.
4.4
|
Procedure
The participants were invited individually into the isolated test room.
Each participant was seated on a chair in front of the framework, which
was set on a table. After obtaining informed consent, the participants
received written instructions for the procedure. Before starting the
comparison of the pairs, we asked the participants to lift up the ten ref-
erence samples with their index ngers using the wired ring to perceive
the density of the duralumin. After the learning session, the partici-
pants compared the weights of the target and reference samples under
four dierent combinations of external noise and uncertainty (Table 1).
To simulate the condition of big uncertainty (B and D in Table 1), we
placed a fogged glass between the target and the participant so that
the visual image of the target was fuzzy. For the big external noise con-
dition (C and D in Table 1), a participant added a weight to the wrist
he/she used for lifting the samples. For each condition, we randomly
presented each pair of the 10 pairs of the target and reference samples
with identical weights. The presentation order diered between partici-
pants. We asked participants to alternately lift the target sample and
reference sample with the index nger of the dominant arm using the
wired ring. After they had lifted both samples, we asked them to rank
the target weight as very much heavier,”“heavier,”“kind of heavier,
almost the same,”“kind of lighter,”“lighter,and very much lighterin
comparison to the reference sample in that pair. We repeated the
paired comparisons of sample pair weights for all ten pairs for the four
conditions.
size (cm3)
mass (g)
reference
target
prediction error
expectation
comparison
x
r1
r2
r3
r4
t1
t2
t3
t4
FIGURE 10 Method for manipulating expectation error.
Participants compared the weights of the reference and target. The
dierence between the predicted weight (x) and the actual weight
of the target were controlled as prediction errors for each
referencetarget pair
TABLE 1 Experimental conditions regarding uncertainty and exter-
nal noise
External noise
Small Big
Uncertainty Small A C
Big B D
436
|
YANAGISAWA
4.5
|
Data analysis
We used the participantsresponses regarding the relative weights of
the target samples in comparison to those of the reference samples as
an index of the expectation eect. As explained previously, the physical
weight of the target and reference samples in each pair were identical.
If the response was almost the same,we can infer that no expecta-
tion eect was observed. Due to the combination of learned density of
the material and the visually estimated volume, all the target samples
were actually lighter than the expected weight, whereas the reference
samples, which were solid and had congruent expected and actual
weights, were heavier. Thus, the participantsresponses that the target
samples were heavier than the reference samples denote contrast.
Conversely, the participantsresponses that the target samples were
lighter than the reference samples represent assimilation. We com-
pared the participantsresponses of the expectation eect for dierent
combinations of prediction error, uncertainty, and external noise.
4.6
|
Experimental results
Figure 11 shows the averaged responses of the participants regarding
the relative weight of each target sample for four combinations of
uncertainty and external noise. A positive value shows how much
heavier the target (smaller object) was than the reference (bigger
object), whereas the negative value shows the opposite. In sum, the
positive value represents contrast, and the negative value represents
assimilation. The horizontal axis denotes the dierences between the
expected weight and the actual weight of each target (see Figure 10),
that is, the extent of prediction errors for each pair.
The result shows that under all combinations of uncertainty and
external noise, the expectation eect began with assimilation and then
shifted to contrast as the prediction error increased. This trend corre-
sponds to the simulation results shown in Figure 7. As we hypothe-
sized, assimilation occurred in the presence of small prediction errors,
which contradicts the SWI.
We conducted three-way ANOVA with prediction error,uncer-
tainty,andexternal noise as independent variables and the response of
the relative weight of the target sample (i.e., expectation eect, E)as
the dependent variable. The main eects of the prediction error
[F524.83, p<0.001, h250.276, 12b 1:0] were signicant. The
average score of the expectation eect with bigger noise was signi-
cantly smaller than one with smaller noise [F54.65, p50.03,
h250.00576, 12b 5 0.578]. In sum, bigger external noise tended to
have more assimilations than smaller external noise. The main eects
of uncertainty [F50.29, p50.58, h250.00036, 12b 5 0.084] and
interactions were not signicant.
To investigate the inuences of uncertainty and external noise in
detail, we conducted a two-way repeated measure ANOVA with uncer-
tainty and external noise as independent variables and the response of
the relative weight of the target sample as the dependent variable for
each target sample, that is, each prediction error. The results indicate
statistical signicance at the prediction errors of 180 g for assimilation
and of 780 g for contrast.
With respect to the prediction error of 180 g (assimilation), the
main eects of uncertainty [F52.25, p50.16, h250.016,
12b 5 0.341] and external noise [F50.38, p50.55, h250.0049,
12b 5 0.150] were not signicant. However, we observed marginally
signicant interaction between uncertainty and external noise [F52.92,
p50.1, h250.0445 12b 5 0.517]. Figure 7 shows a prominent nega-
tive response, namely, assimilation, for small uncertainty and small
noise at a prediction error of 180 g. We also observed a similar trend
at 280 g (Figure 7). We compared the responses for the prediction
error of 180 g for dierent conditions of uncertainty and external noise
using Bonferroni-corrected paired comparisons. We found that the
negative response (assimilation) for small uncertainty and big noise was
signicantly bigger than the response for big uncertainty and small
noise. For the prediction error of 780 g (contrast), we found signicant
main eects for both uncertainty [F55.18, p50.039, h250.076,
12b 5 0.681] and external noise [F57.88, p50.014, h250.116,
12b 5 0.626]. The interaction between uncertainty and external noise
is not signicant [F50.25, p50.62, h250.0036 12b 5 0.142]. We
observed that smaller uncertainty involves a signicantly bigger expec-
tation eect, namely, contrast, than bigger uncertainty. For external
-2
-1.5
-1
-0.5
0
0.5
1
1.5
2
180 280 380 480 580 680 780 880 980 1080
Average score of weight
expectation effec t
Prediction error (g)
small u ncertai nty & small no ise
big u ncertainty & smal l noise
small u ncertai nty & big noise
big u ncertainty & big no ise
FIGURE 11 Expectation eects in SWI as functions of expectation error for each condition of uncertainty and external noise. Each bar
represents the average responses of the relative weight of each target. The error bars denote standard errors
YANAGISAWA
|
437
noise, smaller noise involves signicantly bigger contrast than bigger
noise.
5
|
DISCUSSION
Both results of the computer simulation (Figure 7) and the experiment
using the SWI (Figure 11) showed that prediction error dominantly
aected the extent of the expectation eect and worked as a factor of
either the assimilation or the contrast condition. The pattern of expec-
tation eect shifted from assimilation to contrast as the prediction
error increased. This correspondence between the simulation and
experiment supports our hypothesis, namely, that the prediction error
increases the likelihood repulsive inuence against prior attractive
inuence during Bayesian estimation (decoding). As we hypothesized,
assimilation occurred in the presence of small errors. In sum, human
beings perceive bigger object as heavier than a smaller object if the
prediction error is small. This result contradicts the idea in the conven-
tional SWI. The law of expectation eect using the model revealed
these undiscovered perceptual phenomena.
We discuss the signicance of the psychological phenomenon
from an ecological viewpoint. Contrast exaggerates expectation discon-
rmation so that human beings pay attention to novel stimuli with sur-
prise (Itti & Baldi, 2009) and try to gain information from unexpected
phenomena. This biological function may provide an opportunity to
learn novel information and renew prior knowledge, that is, prior distri-
butions. However, due to limitations of cognitive resources, such as
short-term memory content and energy, human beings cannot pay
attention to each unexpected phenomenon. Assimilation may work as
alter to select which unexpected phenomena should be paid atten-
tion to. In sum, human beings ignore marginal prediction error. This bio-
logical function is reasonable in that it saves the energy resources of
the human brain.
The second hypothesis was that the trend in the relationship
between the expectation eect and prediction error depends on uncer-
tainty and external noise. The simulation results in Figure 4 show that
uncertainty decreased the extent of the expectation eect and external
noise increased the assimilation due to the decreasing repulsive inu-
ence during the Bayesian estimation. Although these eects were
weak in the SWI experiment, the trend supported the simulation result.
The condition of small uncertainty with big external noise involved
prominent assimilation at 180 g prediction error. The result of 780 g
prediction error showed that the extent of contrast with smaller noise
signicantly exceeded that with bigger noise. Smaller uncertainty
involved a signicantly bigger contrast than bigger uncertainty.
We can explain these phenomena with our hypothetical model as
follows. Prior distributions of low variation, namely, certain predictions,
attracted a Bayesian estimate against the likelihood function of noisy
stimuli when the prediction error and likelihood asymmetry are small.
The repulsive inuence decreased as uncertainty and external noise
increased. The contrast weakened with big uncertainty and big noise.
Human beings rely on their prior distributions when the external stimu-
lus is noisy. Certain prior predictions may increase this dependency,
and thus, the extent of assimilation becomes prominent. On the other
hand, human beings should pay attention to big prediction errors of
certain predictions and clear external stimuli. Therefore, contrast
increased with small uncertainty (certain prediction) and small external
noise (clear stimulus).
6
|
CONCLUSION
We proposed a simulation model of expectation eect based on neural
coding principles. The simulation results using the model revealed that
the condition of expectation eect is altered depending on the three
factors, that is, prediction error, uncertainty, and external noise. We
observed that the trend of the SWI experiment supported the simula-
tion result. The simulation result renewed the conventional SWI thesis
based on the proposed expectation eect model.
Discussions from ecological and neural viewpoints suggested that
the simulation result was reasonable. Although we need to validate the
model with other perceptual phenomena in future study, the proposed
modelhasapotentialtoworkasafundamentaloftheexpectation
eect.
The proposed model guides researchers and practitioners to select
parameter combinations that dierentiate sensory illusions caused by
prior expectation. This provides a guideline to design ecient experi-
ment that maximize response variance with limited trials of partici-
pants. Furthermore, the model helps to explain perceptual bias caused
by prior expectation in the participantsresponses in sensory
experiment.
REFERENCES
Barlow, H. B. (1961). Possible principles underlying the transformation of
sensory messages. In Sensory Communication, (W. Rosenblith, ed.) pp.
217234. MIT Press: Cambridge, MA.
Brayanov, J. B., & Smith, M. A. (2010). Bayesian and anti-Bayesian
biases in sensory integration for action and perception in the size
weight illusion. Journal of Neurophysiology,103, 15181531.
Deliza, R., & Mace, H. J. H. (1996). The generation of sensory expecta-
tion by external cues and its eect on sensory perception and
hedonic ratings: A review. Journal of Sensory Studies,11, 103128.
Demir, E., Desmet, P., & Hekkert, P. (2009). Appraisal patterns of emo-
tions in human-product interaction. International Journal of Design,3,
4151.
Ernst, M. O., & Banks, M. S. (2002). Humans integrate visual and haptic
information in a statistically optimal fashion. Nature,415, 429433.
Flanagan, J. R., Bittner, J. P., & Johansson, R. S. (2008). Experience can
change distinct size-weight priors engaged in lifting objects and judg-
ing their weights. Current Biology,18, 17421747.
Geers, A. L., & Lassiter, G. D. (1999). Aective expectations and informa-
tion gain: Evidence for assimilation and contrast eects in aective
experience. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,35, 394413.
Itti, L., & Baldi, P. (2009). Bayesian surprise attracts human attention.
Vision Research,49, 12951306.
K
ording, K. P., & Wolpert, D. M. (2004). Bayesian integration in sensori-
motor learning. Nature,427, 244247.
Krippendor, K. 2005. The semantic turn: A new foundation for design.
Boca Raton, London, New York: Taylor & Francis, CRC Press.
438
|
YANAGISAWA
Ludden,G.D.S.,Schierstein, H. N. J., & Hekkert, P. (2012).
Beyond surprise: A longitudinal study on the experience of
visual-tactual incongruities in products. International Journal of
Design,6,110.
Oliver, R. L. (1977). Eect of expectation and disconrmation on postex-
posure product evaluations: An alternative interpretation. Journal of
Applied Psychology,62, 480486.
Oliver, R. L. (1980). A cognitive model of the antecedents and conse-
quences of satisfaction decisions. Journal of Marketing Research,7,
460469.
Quian Quiroga, R., & Panzeri, S. (2009). Extracting information from neu-
ronal populations: Information theory and decoding approaches.
Nature Review Neuroscience,10, 173185.
Schierstein, H. N. J. (2001). Eects of product beliefs on product per-
ception and liking. In Food, people and societyed (Lynn J. Frewer, Einar
Risvik, Hendrik Schierstein, eds.), pp. 7396. Springer: Berlin
Heidelberg.
Schultz, W., Dayan, P., & Montague, P. R. (1997). A neural substrate of
prediction and reward. Science,275, 15931599.
Wei, X. X., & Stocker, A. (2012). Bayesian Inference with ecient neural
population codes. In Articial neural networks and machine learning
ICANN 2012 (A. P. Villa, W. Duch, P.
Erdi, F. Masulli, & G. Palm eds.)
(pp. 523530). Springer: Switzerland.
Wilson, T. D., Lisle, D. J., Kraft, D., & Wetzel, C. G. (1989). Preferences as
expectation-driven inferences: Eects of aective expectations on aec-
tive experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,56,519530.
Yanagisawa, H., & Takatsuji, K. (2015a). Eects of visual expectation on
perceived tactile perception: An evaluation method of surface texture
with expectation eect. International Journal of Design,9,3951.
Yanagisawa, H., & Takatsuji, K. (2015b). Expectation eect of perceptual
experience in sensory modality transitions: Modeling with informa-
tion theory. Journal of Intelligent Manufacturing,110. doi: 10.1007/
s10845-015-1096-7.
SUPPORTING INFORMATION
Additional Supporting Information may be found in the online ver-
sion of this article.
YANAGISAWA
|
439
... The minimized free energy is expressed by following equations when a Gaussian generative model is assumed (Taniyama et al., 2021;Yanagisawa, 2016Yanagisawa, , 2021: ...
... represents the upper limit of allowable operation error (e.g., road width). We calculated the SoA based on the free energy model (Taniyama et al., 2021;Yanagisawa, 2016Yanagisawa, , 2021, see Chapter 2 in detail). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Action-feedback delay during operation reduces both task performance and sense of agency (SoA). In this study, using information-theoretic free energy, we formalized a novel mathematical model for explaining the influence of delay on both task performance and SoA in continuous operations. Based on the mathematical model, we propose a novel interface design called Predictive Wand for predicting future outcomes to prevent task performance and SoA degradation resulting from response delays. Model-based simulations and operational experiments with participants confirmed that operational delay considerably reduces both task performance and SoA. Furthermore, the proposed Predictive Wand mitigates these problems. Our findings support the model-based interface design for continuous operations with delay to prevent task performance and SoA degradation.
... Behavioral science studies have shown that the Bayesian method successfully builds computational theories for perception [4,5] and sensorimotor control [6], which led to Bayesian brain theory [7]. A growing body of experimental evidence has proven that human perception is Bayes-optimal [7]- [10], that is, human perceptions can be formulated as the estimations of the hidden states that cause sensory stimuli. Such estimates are given by Bayesian inference based on uncertain prediction and noisy sensory stimuli. ...
... Driver knowledge of the effects of these environmental factors on the SOC and characteristics of lithium-ion batteries may affect the predicted SOC. [4,7,9,10,18]. The inevitability of the Bayesian hypothesis in perception can be deduced from the free-energy principle, which is a unified brain theory that suggests that any self-organizing system (e.g., the brain) in equilibrium with its environment minimizes its free energy [5,19]. ...
Article
Full-text available
State of charge (SOC) management is necessary for electric vehicle (EV) driving safety and comfort. However, the SOC estimated by a human driver may differ from the actual SOC, inducing negative emotions and false charging behaviors. Currently , a model for explaining or estimating driver perceptions and emotions regarding SOC is lacking. Therefore, this paper proposes a Bayesian framework that explains and estimates human SOC perception and related emotions and experimentally verifies the model predictions. The results suggest that the deviations between actual SOC variations and driver expectations are necessary to determine the emotional experiences of drivers caused by managing SOC noise.
... Human perception can be modelled using the concept of Bayesian inference (Ma et al. 2006), and cross-modal perception is also modelled by applying this concept (Yanagisawa 2016). In this study, we model cross-modal effect of olfaction and audio-visual perception and propose effective scent ejection methods based on the model. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
Olfaction is an area of great interest as a new interface for digital experience. To provide rich multimodal experiences with limited amount of scent, we propose efficient olfactory ejection methods: pulse ejection and habituation ejection. These methods are based on Bayesian cross-modal model of olfaction. The pulse ejection gives off odour at regular intervals. Immediately after each ejection, the scent perception’s prior distribution changes, making it easier to perceive presence of scent. Habituation ejection gives off odour for a certain time at the beginning of a scene. As the nose gets accustomed to the scent, the confidence of the scent perception’s likelihood function becomes smaller, making it easier to create an illusion of scent by audio-visual information. An experiment in VR environment showed tendency that the methods, especially the pulse ejection, effectively reduced total ejection amount of the odour while maintaining the richness of multimodal experiences.KeywordsOlfactionMultimodalDigital experienceVirtual realityBayesian model
... Theoretical and empirical studies have supported the Bayesian brain theory from perception to motor-sensory optimizations (K. Friston, 2010;Kersten et al., 2004;Körding & Wolpert, 2004;Wei & Stocker, 2015;Yanagisawa, 2016). We adopt a Bayesian perception model in which that perception is an estimation of the cause of sensory outcomes, and these estimates follow Bayesian inference. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
An appropriate level of arousal induces positive emotions, and a high arousal potential may provoke negative emotions. To explain the effect of arousal on emotional valence, we propose a novel mathematical framework of arousal potential variations in the dual process of human cognition: automatic and controlled process. Although models have been proposed to explain the emotions in the dual process, a suitable mathematical formulation is largely undiscovered. Our model associates free energy with arousal potential and its variations to explain emotional valence. Decreasing and increasing free energy consequently induces positive and negative emotions, respectively. We formalize a transition from the automatic to controlled process in the dual process as a change of Bayesian prior. We model emotion valence using free-energy increase (FI) when one tries to change one's Bayesian prior and its reduction (FR) when one succeeds to recognize the same stimuli with a changed prior and define three emotions: "interest," "confusion," and "boredom" using the variations. The mathematical analysis comparing between varied Gaussian model parameters suggests that: 1) prediction error (PR) increases FR when the first prior variance is greater than the second prior variance, 2) PR always increases FR, and 3) the distance between priors' means always increases FR. We discuss the association of the outcomes with emotions in the controlled process. The mathematical model provides a general framework for predicting and controlling emotional valence in the dual process that varies with viewpoint and stimuli, as well as for understanding the contradictions in the effects of arousal on the valence.
... The difference between the posterior mean and the peak of likelihood represents the gap between perceived and observed safety. This gap is regarded as a cognitive bias termed expectation effect (Yanagisawa, 2016). In our previous study, we modelled the expectation effect as a function of three parameters: a prediction error (difference between prior mean and peak of likelihood), prior precision (inverse variance of the prior), and observation precision (inverse variance of the likelihood). ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
In a human centred society, sense of safety perceived by people is important to consider as well as actual safety. The gap between expectations and reality in safety causes social problems. Overestimation of safety provokes dangerous behaviours, while underestimation of safety causes excessive behavioural restraint. For example, in COVID 19 pandemic, the overestimation may lead to the spread of infection due to dangerous behaviours, and the underestimation may lead to slow consumption due to excessive self-control. In this talk, I propose a mathematical framework to model sense of safety using Bayesian inference. Recent neuroscience studies suggest that human brain activities can be explained as a Bayes’ model(Knill & Pouget, 2004). Here, based on Helmholtz’s epistemology, I assume that safety x is inferred as the cause of observation (data: y) and define the sense of safety as Bayesian posterior p(x|y) that is proportional to a product of a prior p(x) and likelihood p(y|x). The prior and the likelihood represent the expectation of safety and the safety based solely on the data (observed safety), respectively. In this model, the prior (safety belief) is updated to the posterior (perception of safety). The difference between the posterior mean and the peak of likelihood represents the gap between perceived and observed safety. This gap is regarded as a cognitive bias termed expectation effect (Yanagisawa, 2016). In our previous study, we modelled the expectation effect as a function of three parameters: a prediction error (difference between prior mean and peak of likelihood), prior precision (inverse variance of the prior), and observation precision (inverse variance of the likelihood). We found that there are two types: assimilation and contrast. Assimilation diminishes the prediction error, and contrast exaggerates the prediction error. The expectation effect explains several psychological biases that lead to abnormal behaviours, such as normalcy bias, excessive anxiety, and overconfidence. Based on the expectation effect model, I discuss the condition of each psychological bias in safety. In addition, I discuss how to model emotions such as anxious and fear in a mathematical manner by applying information theoretic quantities such as surprisal and free energy (Yanagisawa, Kawamata, & Ueda, 2019).
... Bayesian estimate explains human perception.(Yanagisawa, 2016) Left)Man-machine system model of the SOC(Stage of charge) in an EV. Driver's estimate follows Bayesian posterior given observations.(Right) Experimental result of deviation between observed and recognized SOCs versus deviation between observed and predicted SOCs.Recognized SOCs attracted to predicted SOCs. This supports Bayesian percept ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
The brain always predicts the world. We see brain's predictions as the world. Sensory observations are the only source of correction for predictions. Our perception is an estimate based on predictions and sensory observations. This estimation follows Bayes' theorem. Prediction error is calculated as information surprise. The brain recognizes the world by minimizing prediction errors. This essential function of the brain and its mathematical model explains and predicts human perception, emotions and self-awareness. We discuss how to apply the brain model toward human-centered society.
... Perception is defined as the estimation of the causes of sensory stimuli. Sensory stimuli are coded to neural activity in the brain, such as the firing rate of certain neuronal populations, through sensory organs (Yanagisawa, 2016a(Yanagisawa, , 2016b. We termed the neural codes as sensory data and assumed denoted them as a random variable X that follows a certain probability distribution. ...
Chapter
Emotions consist of two dominant dimensions: arousal (intensity) and valence (positivity and negativity). Psychological theories such as arousal potential suggest that an appropriate level of arousal would induce a positive valence, but extreme arousal potentials induce negative valence. This chapter proposes a general mathematical formulation of the emotional arousal potential elicited by both novelty and complexity. The model formulates the arousal potential as information contents to be processed in the brain after sensory stimuli are perceived or recognized; this is termed as surprisal. I mathematically demonstrate that the surprisal represents free energy, which is originally a thermodynamic quantity, and that it is equivalent to the summation of information contents both from novelty (i.e., information gain) and from complexity or uncertainty (i.e., negative-log-likelihood averaged over Bayesian posterior). Novelty, uncertainty, and complexity all act as factors that form the arousal potential. An aesthetic evaluation experiment using profile shapes supports the model hypothesis that the summation of novelty and complexity determines the inverse U-shaped beauty function. A discussion on the potential of free energy as a mathematical principle is presented to explain the emotion initiators (i.e., arousal potential). The model provides a general mathematical framework for understanding and predicting emotions caused by novelty, uncertainty, and complexity.
Article
Full-text available
An appropriate level of arousal induces positive emotions, and a high arousal potential may provoke negative emotions. To explain the effect of arousal on emotional valence, we propose a novel mathematical framework of arousal potential variations in the dual process of human cognition: automatic and controlled. A suitable mathematical formulation to explain the emotions in the dual process is still absent. Our model associates free energy with arousal potential and its variations to explain emotional valence. Decreasing and increasing free energy consequently induce positive and negative emotions, respectively. We formalize a transition from the automatic to the controlled process in the dual process as a change of Bayesian prior. Further, we model emotional valence using free energy increase (FI) when one tries changing one’s Bayesian prior and its reduction (FR) when one succeeds in recognizing the same stimuli with a changed prior and define three emotions: “interest,” “confusion,” and “boredom” using the variations. The results of our mathematical analysis comparing various Gaussian model parameters reveals the following: (1) prediction error (PR) increases FR (representing “interest”) when the first prior variance is greater than the second prior variance, (2) PR decreases FR when the first prior variance is less than the second prior variance, and (3) the distance between priors’ means always increases FR. We also discuss the association of the outcomes with emotions in the controlled process. The proposed mathematical model provides a general framework for predicting and controlling emotional valence in the dual process that varies with viewpoint and stimuli, as well as for understanding the contradictions in the effects of arousal on the valence.
Article
Full-text available
Human beings predict modal perceptions using a prior modality; for example, they might predict a product’s tactile quality based on its appearance before they have touched it. In this paper, we propose a method for evaluating the quality of surface textures by focusing on the effects of a prior visual prediction on posterior tactual experience, a phenomenon known as the expectation effect. Using a half-mirror apparatus, we synthesized differing combinations of visual and tactile samples. We then compared the expectation effects on perceptual responses to texture samples synthesized using different visual samples. This method was used in an experiment in which participants responded to tactile quality (“nice to touch”) and perceived features (e.g., roughness, hardness, stickiness) of plastic textures under three conditions: visual expectation (V), touch alone (T), and touch following a visual expectation (VT). For the VT condition, participants were asked to evaluate congruent combinations (i.e., identical visual and tactile samples) and incongruent combinations (i.e., tactile samples synthesized with different visual samples). Responses to the tactile samples differed depending on the synthesized visual sample. Comparisons of the responses for V and T conditions revealed expectation effects for contrast and assimilation. It was found that the qualities of both “roughness” and “stickiness” significantly affected tactile quality evaluations. Thus, it is shown that our proposed method can help identify visual-tactile combinations that can improve perceived tactile texture quality.
Article
Full-text available
A user’s experience of a product involves a set of state transitions. For example, the state of a sensory modality may shift from vision to touch to perceive a quality of a product. Between such state transitions, users expect experiences of the subsequent states as well as experience the current state event. A discrepancy between prior expectation and posterior experience evokes emotions, such as surprise, satisfaction, and disappointment, affecting the perceived product value. A noteworthy psychological phenomenon is that expectation affects perceived experience. This phenomenon, called the expectation effect, is a key to designing the affective experience of a product. Although experimental findings of this effect exist in a variety of disciplines, general and theoretical models of the effect are largely unexplored. In this paper, we propose a theoretical model of the expectation effect using information theory and affective expectation model. We hypothesize that Shannon’s entropy of prior subjective probability distributions of posterior experience determines the occurrence of an expectation effect, and the amount of information gained after experiencing a posterior event positively relates to the intensity of the expectation effect. Furthermore, we hypothesize that the conscious awareness of expectation discrepancy discriminates the two types of expectation effect, assimilation and contrast. To verify these hypotheses, we conducted an experiment using the tactile quality of surface texture. In the experiment, we extracted the visual expectation effect on tactile roughness and analyzed the causes of the effect based on these hypotheses. The experimental results validated the proposed model.
Article
Full-text available
When people encounter products with visual-tactual incongruities, they are likely to be surprised because the product feels different than expected. In this paper, we investigate (1) the relationship between surprise and the overall liking of the products, (2) the emotions associated with surprise, and (3) the long-term effects of surprise. We created products that were similar in visual appearance but that differed in their tactual characteristics. Participants evaluated the same products at three different points in time. Surprise was often followed by the emotions interest, fascination, amusement, confusion, indignation and irritation. We suggest that the liking for surprising products may be the composite effect of a decreased liking due to unfamiliar characteristics and increased liking due to positive emotions following surprise. Although the effect of surprise diminishes over time, it persists and can be demonstrated at multiple occasions.
Article
Full-text available
Emotional design, i.e., designing with an intention to evoke or to prevent a particular emotion, can be facilitated by understanding the processes underlying emotions. A promising approach to understanding these processes in the current psychological literature is appraisal theory. Appraisal theory can support this understanding because it explains how different emotions are elicited by different underlying appraisals. This paper reports a study that aimed to identify and specify appraisals that elicit emotions of product users for four emotion groups: happiness/joy, satisfaction/contentment, anger/irritation, and disappointment/dissatisfaction. The study started with a sensitizing task to make participants familiar with reporting their emotional experiences. With a combination of experience sampling and in-depth interviews, the emotions experienced when interacting with products and the causes of these emotions were captured. The results indicated that the appraisal patterns as proposed in general appraisal theory can also be traced in human-product interaction for all four emotion groups. On the basis of the results, an initia specification of those appraisals and design directions are proposed
Article
Full-text available
This chapter is an attempt to formulate ideas about the operations performed by physiological mechanisms, and not merely a discussion of the physiological mechanisms of sensory pathways. It presents three hypotheses regarding the purpose of sensory relays. The first one is the “password” hypothesis, which posits that, since animals respond specifically to specific stimuli, their sensory pathways must possess mechanisms for detecting such stimuli and discriminating between them. The second hypothesis is the fashionable one that relays act as control points at which the flow of information is modulated according to the requirements of other parts of the nervous system. Finally, the third hypothesis theorizes that reduction of redundancy is an important principle guiding the organization of sensory messages and is carried out at relays in the sensory pathways.
Conference Paper
Full-text available
The accuracy with which the brain can infer the value of a stimulus variable depends on both the amount of stimulus information that is represented in sensory neurons (encoding) and the mechanism by which this information is subsequently retrieved from the responses of these neurons (decoding). Previous studies have mainly focused on either the encoding or the decoding aspect. Here, we present a new framework that functionally links the two. More specifically, we demonstrate that optimal (efficient) population codes which guarantee uniform firing rate distributions allow the accurate emulation of optimal (Bayesian) infer-ence using a biophysically plausible neural mechanism. The framework provides predictions for estimation bias and variability as a function of stimulus prior, strength and integration time, as well as physiologi-cal parameters such as tuning curves and spontaneous firing rates. Our framework represents an example of the duality between representation and computation in neural information processing.
Article
A model is proposed which expresses consumer satisfaction as a function of expectation and expectancy disconfirmation. Satisfaction, in turn, is believed to influence attitude change and purchase intention. Results from a two-stage field study support the scheme for consumers and nonconsumers of a flu inoculation.
Article
A model is proposed which expresses consumer satisfaction as a function of expectation and expectancy disconfirmation. Satisfaction, in turn, is believed to influence attitude change and purchase intention. Results from a two-stage field study support the scheme for consumers and nonconsumers of a flu inoculation.
Article
The capacity to predict future events permits a creature to detect, model, and manipulate the causal structure of its interactions with its environment. Behavioral experiments suggest that learning is driven by changes in the expectations about future salient events such as rewards and punishments. Physiological work has recently complemented these studies by identifying dopaminergic neurons in the primate whose fluctuating output apparently signals changes or errors in the predictions of future salient and rewarding events. Taken together, these findings can be understood through quantitative theories of adaptive optimizing control.
Article
Reviews interpretations of the effect of expectation and disconfirmation on perceived product performance. At issue is the relative effect of the initial expectation level and the degree of positive or negative disconfirmation on affective judgments following product exposure. Although the results of prior studies suggest a dominant expectation effect, it is argued that detection of the disconfirmation phenomenon may have been clouded by a conceptual and methodological overdetermination problem. To test this notion, 243 college students responded to expectation and disconfirmation measures in a 3-stage field study of reactions to a recently introduced automobile model. These measures were later related to postexposure affect and intention variables in a hierarchical analysis of variance design. Although the results support earlier conclusions that level of expectation is related to postexposure judgments, it is also shown that the disconfirmation experience may have an independent and equally significant impact. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)