ArticlePDF Available

Activity in superior parietal cortex during training by observation predicts asymmetric learning levels across hands

Springer Nature
Scientific Reports
Authors:

Abstract and Figures

A dominant concept in motor cognition associates action observation with motor control. Previous studies have shown that passive action observation can result in significant performance gains in humans. Nevertheless, it is unclear whether the neural mechanism subserving such learning codes abstract aspects of the action (e.g. goal) or low level aspects such as effector identity. Eighteen healthy subjects learned to perform sequences of finger movements by passively observing right or left hand performing the same sequences in egocentric view. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging we show that during passive observation, activity in the superior parietal lobule (SPL) contralateral to the identity of the observed hand (right\left), predicts subsequent performance gains in individual subjects. Behaviorally, left hand observation resulted in positively correlated performance gains of the two hands. Conversely right hand observation yielded negative correlation - individuals with high performance gains in one hand exhibited low gains in the other. Such behavioral asymmetry is reflected by activity in contralateral SPL during short-term training in the absence of overt physical practice and demonstrates the role of observed hand identity in learning. These results shed new light on the coding level in SPL and have implications for optimizing motor skill learning.
Content may be subject to copyright.
1
Scientific RepoRts | 6:32133 | DOI: 10.1038/srep32133
www.nature.com/scientificreports
Activity in superior parietal cortex
during training by observation
predicts asymmetric learning levels
across hands
Ori Ossmy1,2 & Roy Mukamel1,2
A dominant concept in motor cognition associates action observation with motor control. Previous
studies have shown that passive action observation can result in signicant performance gains in
humans. Nevertheless, it is unclear whether the neural mechanism subserving such learning codes
abstract aspects of the action (e.g. goal) or low level aspects such as eector identity. Eighteen healthy
subjects learned to perform sequences of nger movements by passively observing right or left hand
performing the same sequences in egocentric view. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging
we show that during passive observation, activity in the superior parietal lobule (SPL) contralateral
to the identity of the observed hand (right\left), predicts subsequent performance gains in individual
subjects. Behaviorally, left hand observation resulted in positively correlated performance gains of
the two hands. Conversely right hand observation yielded negative correlation - individuals with high
performance gains in one hand exhibited low gains in the other. Such behavioral asymmetry is reected
by activity in contralateral SPL during short-term training in the absence of overt physical practice and
demonstrates the role of observed hand identity in learning. These results shed new light on the coding
level in SPL and have implications for optimizing motor skill learning.
It has long been proposed that motion is intrinsically linked to perception such that observing an action acti-
vates motor programs that are used to execute the same action1. During the last two decades, there has been an
increasing interest in identifying the neural underpinning of this perceptual-motor link2–5. Direct recordings in
monkeys6 and in humans7,8 demonstrate an overlapping neural representation of observed and executed actions
in frontal and parietal regions. Additionally, neuroimaging studies reveal that action observation evokes activity
within various regions traditionally associated with motor function such as the primary motor cortex (M1),
supplementary motor area (SMA), dorsal premotor cortex (dPMC), supramarginal gyrus, inferior frontal gyrus
(IFG) and the superior parietal lobe (SPL)9–11. Such evoked activity supports the involvement of these regions in
translating perceived actions performed by someone else into a motor plan that can be executed by the observer.
More specically, parieto-frontal regions, that are active during passive action observation, overlap with those
engaged during online imitation12–16. Together, these results suggest a perceptual-motor translation mechanism
in the brain, ideally suited for imitation.
Imitation is an important form by which individuals can learn how to perform an action from observing
others17–20. Indeed, action observation has been shown to facilitate subsequent executed movements and improve
skill performance21–27. e process of learning by observation, was extensively investigated24,28–30 and supports the
idea that the perceptual-motor translation mechanism, which matches observed behaviors with internal motor
representations, facilitates subsequent motor performance. Understanding this mechanism can provide a useful
account of how we acquire new behaviors, as well as correct our movements12,31,32.
To date, there is no consensus regarding the level of abstraction of what is extracted from observed actions
and encoded during the learning process (e.g. kinematics, action goal). Given that the neural representation dur-
ing physical practice is known to have a contra-lateral bias33,34, it is not clear whether during action observation
the perceptual-motor learning mechanism displays a similar bias. In their associative sequence learning theory,
1Sagol School of Neuroscience, Tel-Aviv University, Tel-Aviv 69978, Israel. 2School of Psychological Sciences, Tel-Aviv
University, Tel-Aviv 69978, Israel. Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to R.M. (email:
rmukamel@tau.ac.il)
Received: 11 February 2016
Accepted: 03 August 2016
Published: 18 August 2016
OPEN
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
2
Scientific RepoRts | 6:32133 | DOI: 10.1038/srep32133
Heyes and Ray argue that action observation engages similar learning processes as overt physical practice, and
hence depends on the identity of the observed eector31,35. is theory was supported by a series of empirical
studies in which participants performed a serial reaction time (SRT) task, and performance was facilitated only
when subjects used the same eector as the one they observed during passive training28,36–38. Such evidence are
taken to support the notion that the perceptual-motor learning mechanism uses an eector-dependent coordi-
nate frame. However, other recent studies suggest that the perceptual-motor learning mechanism can generalize
across eectors. In a behavioral study by Williams and Gribble, subjects passively observed videos of another
individual reaching to visual targets in a force eld with the right or le hand39. Results showed that subsequent
right hand performance was facilitated in a similar manner following observation sessions depicting right or le
hand. Another study demonstrates shorter reaction times following observation of the same action performed
with a dierent eector40. ese later results suggest that observed actions evoke neural representations of action
goals which are eector independent.
From a neural perspective, neuroimaging studies showed that activation in the parietal lobe is sensitive to
observed eectors in a somatotopic manner2 and also to visual perspectives (egocentric vs. allocentric)41. However
little is known as to how the identity of the observed eector modulates neural activity and how such modulation
relates to learning by observation. A study by Frey and Gerry showed that activity in the right intraparietal sulcus
predicts learning levels following training by observation. However, they used a bimanual sequential task and
therefore were not able to outline the role of hand identity (right/le) in the perceptual-motor learning process42.
e current study focuses on how learning by observation and its underlying neural mechanisms depend
on the identity of the observed effector (right/left hand). To this end we acquired whole-brain functional
magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data while healthy subjects were engaged in a short-term unimanual
learning-by-observation task.
Results
Eighteen healthy subjects were presented with visual input consisting of two virtual hands in egocentric view
(Fig.1a) while either the right or the le virtual hand performed a to-be-learned sequence of nger movements
(conditions ‘Obs-RH’ and ‘Obs-LH’ respectively). Performance levels on the sequence of nger movements were
evaluated before and aer training (see experimental design in Fig.1b). Le and right hand performance gains
(calculated as the accuracy index G; see Methods) were signicant in both training conditions – demonstrating
signicant learning by observation. Within hands, there was no signicant dierence between performance gains
in the Obs-RH and Obs-LH conditions at the group level (p = 0.55 and p = 0.4 for right and le hand respec-
tively). Regression analysis on individual subject data conrmed no signicant relationship between performance
gains and identity of observed hand. is was true both for le and right hands (See Fig.2a). We also compared
performance gains across hands within each observational training condition. At the group level, there was no
signicant dierence between hands, and this was true both following right or le hand observation (p = 0.68
and p = 0.55 respectively; see Fig.2b). However, regression analysis on individual subjects revealed a signicant
correlation between right and le hand performance gains which depended on the identity of observed hand
during training. Following le hand observation (‘Obs-LH’), performance gains across hands were positively
correlated. Subjects exhibiting high performance gains in the right hand also exhibited high performance gains
in the le hand (r = 0.47, p = 0.04; Fig.2b le panel). Conversely, following right hand observation (‘Obs-RH’),
correlation between the two hands was negative (r = 0.54, p = 0.02; Fig.2b right panel). us subjects exhibiting
high performance gains in the right hand, exhibited low performance gains in the le hand and vice versa. Taken
together, these data demonstrate that although on average at the group level, the identity of observed hand does
not aect post-training performance gains, across individual subjects the identity of observed hand has an asym-
metric impact on short-term learning across the two hands.
Figure 1. Experimental design. (a) Subjects learned a novel sequence of nger movements by observing two
virtual hands while one of the hands performed the sequence. (b) Each subject completed two experimental
runs. In each run, a unique sequence of ve digits was presented together with a sketch of the mapped ngers.
Aer instructions, subjects performed the sequence using their right hand (RH) and their le hand (LH) for
initial evaluation of performance level. In the training stage subjects passively observed either the right or le
virtual hand performing the sequence. Aer training, subjects repeated the evaluation stage for assessment of
changes in performance level.
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
3
Scientific RepoRts | 6:32133 | DOI: 10.1038/srep32133
We also explored putative neural networks underlying these behavioral results. First, to examine global dif-
ferences in activation between le hand observation and right hand observation at the group level, we performed
a general linear model (GLM) analysis on the fMRI data obtained during the training stage. e multi-subject
map in Fig.3a demonstrates the high overlap between activation during le and right hand observation relative
to baseline (contrast Obs-LH > rest and contrast Obs-RH > rest, red and blue respectively). e direct contrast:
Obs-RH vs. Obs-LH, yielded an empty multi-subject map indicating no signicant dierence in the global acti-
vation levels for the two conditions in any particular voxel. ese results suggest that at the group level, observing
right or le hand nger movements engage spatially overlapping neural networks and at similar activation levels.
Next, we performed a regions of interest (ROI) analysis in individual subjects. In each subject, we dened brain
regions engaged during task execution (obtained from the pre-training test period; see multi subject map in Fig.3b
and Methods). For each subject and ROI, fMRI activity during the observational training period was compared
against the subsequent behavioral performance gains of each hand (see Methods). We found that across subjects,
activity in the right SPL during ‘Obs-LH’ training correlated with le (r = 0.72, p = 7·104) and right (r = 0.66,
p = 2·103) hand performance gains (See Fig.3c, le panel; Bonferroni corrected for the 16 regions examined).
is is compatible with our observation that across subjects, le and right hand performance gains are positively
correlated following training by le hand observation. In the remaining 14 ROIs, no signicant correlation values
were obtained. We also examined whether fMRI activity level in the same ROIs during ‘Obs-RH’ training period
corresponds to subsequent le or right hand performance gains. is time, out of all ROIs only activity in le SPL
exhibited a signicant positive correlation with right hand performance gain and a signicant negative correlation
with le hand performance gain (r = 0.68, p = 1.9·103 and r = 0.71, p = 9.6·104 respectively; Bonferroni cor-
rected for multiple comparisons; see Fig.3c, right panel; for Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates
of ROI centers in individual subjects see Table1). is is compatible with our behavioral results demonstrating
that le and right hand performance gains are negatively correlated following training by right hand observation.
ese results imply that in the absence of overt physical practice, the SPL in the hemisphere contralateral to the
identity of the observed hand plays an important role in the process of short term learning by observation.
Discussion
Observing and imitating others, has long been recognized as constituting a powerful learning strategy for
humans. Previous studies evaluating learning by observation report inconsistent results with respect to the level
of abstract representation coded by the perceptual-motor learning mechanism36,37,39,40. e present study was
Figure 2. Behavioral results. (a) Le hand performance gains were signicant relative to baseline following
right (Obs-RH; p = 0.002) and le (Obs-LH; p = 0.006) hand observation but not signicantly dierent between
the two observation condition. Similarly, right hand performance gains were signicant following right and le
hand observation but not signicantly dierent between the two observation conditions. (b) Following le hand
observation there was no signicant dierence in performance gains between right and le hands at the group
level. However, regression analysis on individual subject data revealed a signicant positive correlation between
le and right hand performance gains. Following right hand observation, there was no signicant dierence
between the two hands at the group level. However, regression analysis on individual subject data revealed a
signicant negative correlation.
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
4
Scientific RepoRts | 6:32133 | DOI: 10.1038/srep32133
designed to determine the eect of identity of the observed eector on learning by observation and trace its
underlying neural mechanism.
Behaviorally, the performance gains we found aer training support the idea that humans can indeed acquire
information about “how” to make movements by mere observation26. At the group level, we found that short
term motor learning by observation does not depend on the identity of the observed eector suggesting that the
perceptual-motor learning mechanism codes actions in an abstract manner. is is consistent with previous stud-
ies showing that subjects can acquire a new skill similarly following observed actions performed with dierent
eectors39,40.
However, closer inspection of individual subject data revealed an unexpected asymmetrical eect of the iden-
tity of observed hand. is asymmetry demonstrates that the identity of the observed eector does play a role
Figure 3. Correlation of neural activity with behavior. (a) Random eect multi-subject activation
map (N = 18) displaying signicant regions obtained from the GLM contrasts: Obs-LH > rest (red) and
Obs-RH > rest (blue) in the training stage (q(FDR) < 0.05). Purple regions correspond to overlapping regions
between the two contrasts. (b) ROI analysis. Random eect multi-subject activation map (N = 18) displaying
signicant regions obtained from the GLM contrasts right hand execution > rest and le hand execution > rest
obtained from the pre-training evaluation period (see Methods). For all analyses, ROIs were dened using
this contrast at the individual subject level. (c) Across all ROIs, the fMRI signal during training by observation
in two regions – right and le SPL - correlated signicantly with subsequent performance gains. Each color
denotes peak cluster of each subject dened from the localizer (described in panel b). During training by le
hand observation, activity in the right SPL correlated both with subsequent le and right hand performance
gains. During training by right hand observation, activity in the le SPL showed positive correlation with right
hand performance gain and negative correlation with le hand performance gain.
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
5
Scientific RepoRts | 6:32133 | DOI: 10.1038/srep32133
in short term learning and contributes to the discussion regarding the level of abstract representation during
learning by observation. e behavioral asymmetry we found might be a reection of well documented hemi-
spheric dierences in cortical hand representation43–45. Such cortical dierences are usually associated with hand
dominance33,46 and inter-limb dierences in motor control and learning47,48. Whether or not the asymmetry we
report is related to hand dominance is not known and a further study in le-handers is required to clarify this
point. Nonetheless, the current results do imply that at least in right-handers, le hand rather than right hand
visual input can lead to optimization of short term motor learning by observation.
At the neural level, our results provide strong evidence that in the absence of overt physical practice, the
bilateral SPL plays an important role in enhancing short-term learning during observation. e parietal cortex
has been previously found active during action observation, demonstrating its engagement in visuo-motor con-
trol and imitation learning6,13,24,49,50. Additionally, it has been suggested that the SPL represents low level aspects
(e.g. visual perspective) of the action rather than abstract aspects (e.g. action goal)12,41,51. We extend this view
to the context of learning. Although at the group level we did not nd signicant dierence in activation power
during le and right hand observation, at the individual subject level activation in SPL depended on the identity
of observed hand and correlated with subsequent performance gains. ese correlations during observational
training on a unimanual task are in agreement with a previous study showing that activity level in parietal regions
predicted subsequent performance gains in a bimanual problem solving task42. Taken together, the results empha-
size the key role of parietal regions in short-term learning by observation of complex action sequences.
Although in the current study we focus on the role of hand identity in learning by observation, the current
ndings may well have a bearing on the neural underpinning of motor imitation. Previous studies revealed a
set of parieto-frontal regions active during both observation and synchronous imitation13,14,52. We found simi-
lar regions during the observational training with no signicant dierence in activation level between observa-
tion of right and le hand movements at the group level. A previous imitation study by Aziz-Zadeh et al. also
demonstrated bilateral activations independent of the identity of the observed hand52. us, during imitation, the
perceptual-motor translation mechanism seems to form a neural representation of the observed action in both
hemispheres implying an abstraction with respect to hand identity. However, our nding of hemispheric laterality
in SPLs prediction of subsequent performance gains in individual subjects, suggests that the learning process in
SPL is sensitive to the identity of the observed hand.
Finally, the importance of SPL in coding spatial aspects of movements is well established53,54. In the current
study we used a nger movement task that has a strong spatial component. erefore, additional research will be
necessary to assess whether the SPL involvement we report in learning generalizes to other types of tasks with a
less dominant spatial factor (e.g. temporal learning tasks). Another key question is to what extent the role of SPL
in learning observed actions holds true also for learning motor movements that are not in the repertoire of the
observer55–57. Future studies should go beyond learning how to combine known movement elements in a new
arrangement (e.g. order of nger movements), and examine the acquisition process of entirely new movements
(e.g. when children imitate adults58).
In summary, by examining not only the group level but also the individual subject level, we show asymmetric
performance gains across hands which depend on the identity of the observed hand. Furthermore, we show that
the SPL plays a signicant role in this asymmetric learning by observation. ese ndings have implications for
the debate regarding the coding level in SPL, and demonstrate that for the purpose of learning in both hands, le
rather than right hand observation is preferable.
Subject Le SPL coordinates Right SPL coordinates
1 [ 31 63 50] [24 65 49]
2 [ 22 74 44] [25 76 42]
3 [ 28 64 49] [17 65 54]
4 [ 18 65 54] [25 57 55]
5 [ 25 65 34] [25 65 43]
6 [ 32 51 62] [24 53 53]
7 [ 20 52 51] [23 56 50]
8 [ 34 51 67] [13 63 63]
9 [ 25 66 54] [25 66 50]
10 [ 24 59 61] [21 54 56]
11 [ 29 58 59] [30 57 56]
12 [ 34 64 56] [22 63 62]
13 [ 27 52 60] [22 60 62]
14 [ 31 51 49] [31 53 56]
15 [ 31 54 56] [29 54 57]
16 [ 29 63 47] [24 65 45]
17 [ 35 72 35] [28 66 41]
18 [ 23 55 55] [24 54 66]
Table 1. MNI locations of each subject’s peak voxels in the le and right SPL ROIs (Fig.3c).
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
6
Scientific RepoRts | 6:32133 | DOI: 10.1038/srep32133
Methods
Subjects. Eighteen healthy subjects (10 females, mean age: 27.4, range: 22–34 years), naïve to the purpose
of the study, participated aer providing written informed consent. All subjects were right handed according to
the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory, with normal or corrected-to-normal vision and no reported cognitive or
neurological decits. Subjects were recruited according to the standard safety criteria for fMRI studies and were
compensated by payment (55 NIS/hour). e study protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of Tel-Aviv
University and the Helsinki committee at the Tel-Aviv Sourasky Medical Center, and carried out in accordance
with the approved guidelines.
Procedure and Task. During fMRI scans, subjects completed 5 consecutive experimental runs, in which
they learned to perform unique sequences of nger movements. During 3 runs, subjects physically practiced a
sequence of nger movements (results reported in a separate manuscript). In two runs, subjects passively learned
to perform a sequence of nger movements by observing two virtual hands on the screen while either the le
or the right virtual hand performed the sequence (Fig.1a). roughout these runs the subjects’ real hands were
immobile.
Fingers were numbered from index (1) to little nger (4) and subjects were instructed to learn a unique
sequence in each experimental run (one of 5 optional sequences: 4-1-3-2-4, 4-2-3-1-4, 3-2-4-1-3, 3-1-4-2-3, 2-1-
4-3-2). e subjects were instructed to learn the sequence and knew that they would be tested on it in pre- and
post- evaluation stages. Run order and the nger sequence associated with each run were counter balanced across
subjects. Subjects’ nger movements were monitored using motion-sensing MR-compatible gloves (5DT Data
Glove Ultra). We programmed a custom-build soware, based on the application programming interface pro-
vided by 5DT, to extract nger movements from 14 dierent sensors and control the presentation of the virtual
hands (http://www.5dt.com). is allowed us to verify that the subjects hands are immobile during the train-
ing stage and also allowed us to yoke virtual hand movement presented on the screen to real hand movement
during the evaluation stages (see below). Small delays in feedback are unavoidable due to sampling rate of the
motion-sensing gloves (up to 16 ms) and the refresh rate of the screen (up to 16 ms). We veried that the soware
itself does not introduce additional delays (time duration for calculating ngers’ position and updating the hands
animation was less than 15 ms). erefore we estimate that delays in visual feedback were no longer than 3 refresh
rates in the worst case scenario. During experimental runs, virtual hands were presented with a black background
on a screen. Subjects lied supine with their arms to the side of their body and palms facing up. Subjects could not
see their real hands during the scans and viewed the screen through a tilted mirror mounted in front of their eyes.
In the beginning of each run (Fig.1b), subjects were presented with an instructions slide that depicted two
hand illustrations with numbered ngers and a 5-digit sequence underneath representing the sequence of nger
movements to be learned. e instructions slide was presented for 12 seconds and was followed by a pre-training
evaluation stage in which baseline performance level of each hand was separately assessed. During the evaluation,
subjects physically performed the required sequence with one hand repeatedly, as fast and as accurate as possible,
for 30 seconds (hand order evaluation right\le was counter balanced across all sessions). During the evaluation
stage, real-time visual feedback was provided by the virtual hands’ movements which were yoked to the subjects’
real hand movements.
Following the pre-training evaluation stage, a “Start Training” slide appeared for 9 seconds and cued the sub-
jects to the upcoming training stage. In the training stage subjects observed the le or right virtual hand perform-
ing the sequence (order of training conditions was counter balanced across subjects). Subject were instructed to
observe the screen and refrain from moving their hands. e pace of virtual hand movement during training was
constant across both conditions and was set based on the average pace of the subject during previous execution
sessions. In cases where the training by observation run was rst, the pace was set based on the average pace of
previous subjects (range across subject: 4–9 digit sequences per block). Each training block lasted 15 seconds and
was followed by a 9-second resting period, in which a blank yellow screen appeared. e training stage consisted
of 20 such training blocks (with overall experimental run duration lasting 8 minutes). Aer the training stage,
subject’s performance was evaluated again for 30 seconds in each hand. Similar to the pre-training evaluation,
subjects were instructed to repeatedly execute the sequence as fast and as accurately as possible.
Performance evaluation. In all evaluation stages, we calculated subject’s performance using the formula
(1) below:
=
+
__
__
G
pp
pp
(1)
post training pretraining
post training pretraining
where p is the number of times the subject performed a complete sequence with no errors. erefore, a positive
G index reects improvement in performance. We calculated this performance gain index for each subject, each
training condition (Obs-RH, Obs-LH), and each hand (le/right) - allowing us to compare improvement under
dierent motor trainings conditions.
Behavioral analysis. We used the data from the motion-detection gloves to verify that the subjects did not
move their ngers during the observational training conditions. Each sensor of the glove provided the angle of
each nger joint (sampling rate = 16 ms) and the subjects always started the training sessions with the hand in the
same orientation. e maximal angle of each nger during observational training was not signicantly dierent
from the maximal angle during rest. is was true for all ngers of both the right and the le hand (minimal
p = 0.42; two-tailed paired t-test; in the main text, unless otherwise stated, all signicance tests were two-tailed
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
7
Scientific RepoRts | 6:32133 | DOI: 10.1038/srep32133
paired t-test and all correlation tests were Pearson correlation). is analysis rules out an alternative explanation
that dierences in performance gains (across individuals or conditions) are due to subliminal physical movement
of the subjects during observation.
fMRI data acquisition. Blood oxygenation level dependent (BOLD) images were obtained on a 3 T General
Electric scanner with an 8 channel head coil located at the Tel-Aviv Souraski Medical Center, Tel-Aviv, Israel. An
echo-planar imaging sequence was used to obtain the functional data (39 ascending interleaved axial slices, 4 mm
thickness, slice gaps = 0; TR = 3000 ms; ip angle = 90°; TE = 30 ms; in-plane resolution = 1.72 × 1.72 mm; matrix
size = 128 × 128). In addition, anatomical reference was obtained by T1-weighted scan (voxel size = 1 × 1 × 1 mm)
for each subject.
fMRI Preprocessing. All fMRI data were processed using the BrainVoyager QX soware (version 2.6, Brain
Innovation, Maastricht, Netherlands; http://www.brainvoyager.com). Prior to statistical analysis, a preprocess-
ing procedure was performed on all functional images and included cubic spline slice-time correction, trilinear
3D motion correction, and high-pass ltering (above 0.006 Hz). In addition, we assessed head movements and
veried no scans contained head movement exceeding 2 mm in either direction. e 2D functional images were
co-registered to the anatomical images and the complete dataset was transformed into the Talairach coordinate
system for multi-subject comparisons59. Functional data of individual subjects was spatially smoothed (Gaussian
lter, FWHM 6 mm) prior to statistical analysis. We also transformed the dataset to Montreal Neurological
Institute coordinate system (MNI-305) in order to extract the individual subject peak coordinates that are pro-
vided in Table1.
ROI Analysis. Regions of interest (ROIs; see Fig.3b) were dened at an individual subject level. We used
general linear model (GLM) contrasts to reveal brain regions active during the pre-training evaluations collapsed
across all sessions of each hand: (Pre_Trainingright_hand > rest) and (Pre_Trainingle_hand > rest). e resulting maps
were corrected by controlling the False Discovery Rate (FDR60) and thresholded at q(FDR) < 0.05. For each sub-
ject, we dened a sphere, with maximum cluster size of 12 mm radius around the peak activation within each
anatomically dened region according to Mai et al. 1997. Regions revealed in this analysis include the right and
le pre-motor cortex (R-PMc/L-PMc), primary motor cortex (R- 1/L-M1), visual cortex (R-Visual/L-Visual),
post-central gyrus (R-PoG/L-PoG), superior parietal lobule (R-SPL/L-SPL), supplementary motor area
(R-SMA/L-SMA) and subcortical regions (R-lamus/L-alamus/R-Striatum/L-Striatum). Next, for each of the
eighteen subjects, we calculated separately in each ROI the average fMRI activity level across all signicant voxels
during training (either Obs-RH or Obs-LH; yielding a vector of 18 values for each ROI and training condition).
As the behavioral measure we took the corresponding performance gain (G) in each hand of each subject (See
Fig.3c).
References
1. James, W. e principles ofpsychology (Vol. 1). New Yor: Holt (1890).
2. Buccino, G. et al. Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMI study. European
journal of neuroscience 13, 400–404, doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2001.01385 (2001).
3. Blaemore, S.-J. & Decety, J. From the perception of action to the understanding of intention. Nature eviews Neuroscience 2,
561–567, doi: 10.1038/35086023 (2001).
4. Flanagan, J. . & Johansson, . S. Action plans used in action observation. Nature 424, 769–771, doi: 10.1038/nature01861 (2003).
5. Prinz, W. Perception and action planning. European journal of cognitive psychology 9, 129–154, doi: 10.1080/713752551 (1997).
6. izzolatti, G., Fogassi, L. & Gallese, V. Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nat
ev Neurosci 2, 661–670, doi: 10.1038/35090060 (2001).
7. Muamel, ., Estrom, A. D., aplan, J., Iacoboni, M. & Fried, I. Single-neuron responses in humans during execution and
observation of actions. Current biology : CB 20, 750–756, doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.02.045 (2010).
8. Tremblay, C. et al. Action observation and execution Intracranial recordings in a human subject. Neurology 63, 937–938, doi: 10.
1212/ 01. WNL. 0000137111. 16767. C6 (2004).
9. Grezes, J. & Decety, J. Functional anatomy of execution, mental simulation, observation, and verb generation of actions: a meta
analysis. Human brain mapping 12, 1–19, doi: 10.1002/1097-0193(200101)12 (2001).
10. Tanaa, S. & Inui, T. Cortical involvement for action imitation of hand/arm postures versus nger congurations: an fMI study.
Neuroreport 13, 1599–1602 (2002).
11. Gazzola, V., Aziz-Zadeh, L. & eysers, C. Empathy and the somatotopic auditory mirror system in humans. Current biology 16,
1824–1829, doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.072 (2006).
12. Iacoboni, M. et al. Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science 286, 2526–2528, doi: 10.1126/science.286.5449.2526 (1999).
13. Buccino, G. et al. Neural circuits underlying imitation learning of hand actions: an event-related fMI study. Neuron 42, 323–334,
doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(04)00181-3 (2004).
14. Macuga, . L. & Frey, S. H. Neural representations involved in observed, imagined, and imitated actions are dissociable and
hierarchically organized. NeuroImage 59, 2798–2807, doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.083 (2012).
15. Higuchi, S., Holle, H., oberts, N., Eicho, S. B. & Vogt, S. Imitation and observational learning of hand actions: prefrontal
involvement and connectivity. NeuroImage 59, 1668–1683, doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.021 (2012).
16. rüger, B. et al. Parietal and premotor cortices: Activation reects imitation accuracy during observation, delayed imitation and
concurrent imitation. NeuroImage 100, 39–50, doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.05.074 (2014).
17. Meltzo, A. N. & Prinz, W. e imitative mind: Development, evolution and brain bases. Vol. 6 (Cambridge University Press, 2002).
18. Byrne, . W. Imitation as behaviour parsing. Philosophical Transactions of the oyal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 358,
529–536, doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1219 (2003).
19. Tomasello, M. & Call, J. Primate cognition. Vol. 24 (Oxford University Press, USA, 1997).
20. Bandura, A. Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. (Prentice-Hall, Inc, 1986).
21. Brass, M., Beering, H. & Prinz, W. Movement observation aects movement execution in a simple response tas. Acta psychologica
106, 3–22, doi: 10.1016/S0001-6918(00)00024-X (2001).
22. ilner, J. M., Paulignan, Y. & Blaemore, S. J. An Interference Eect of Observed Biological Movement on Action. Current Biology
13, 522–525, doi: 10.1016/s0960-9822(03)00165-9 (2003).
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
8
Scientific RepoRts | 6:32133 | DOI: 10.1038/srep32133
23. Boutin, A., Fries, U., Panzer, S., Shea, C. H. & Blandin, Y. ole of action observation and action in sequence learning and coding.
Acta psychologica 135, 240–251, doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.07.005 (2010).
24. Cross, E. S., raemer, D. J., Hamilton, A. F., elley, W. M. & Graon, S. T. Sensitivity of the action observation networ to physical
and observational learning. Cerebral cortex 19, 315–326, doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn083 (2009).
25. elly, S. W., Burton, A. M., iedel, B. & Lynch, E. Sequence learning by action and observation: evidence for separate mechanisms.
British journal of psychology 94, 355–372, doi: 10.1348/000712603767876271 (2003).
26. Mattar, A. A. & Gribble, P. L. Motor learning by observing. Neuron 46, 153–160, doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.02.009 (2005).
27. Nojima, I., oganemaru, S., awamata, T., Fuuyama, H. & Mima, T. Action observation with inesthetic illusion can produce
human motor plasticity. e European journal of neuroscience 41, 1614–1623, doi: 10.1111/ejn.12921 (2015).
28. Heyes, C. M. & Foster, C. L. Motor learning by observation: evidence from a serial reaction time tas. e Quarterly journal of
experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology 55, 593–607, doi: 10.1080/02724980143000389 (2002).
29. Petrosini, L. et al. Watch how to do it! New advances in learning by observation. Brain esearch ev iews 42, 252–264, doi: 10.1016/
S0165-0173(03)00176-0 (2003).
30. Wulf, G., Chiviacowsy, S. & Lewthwaite, . Normative feedbac eects on learning a timing tas. esearch quarterly for exercise and
sport 81, 425–431, doi: 10.1080/02701367.2010.10599703 (2010).
31. Heyes, C. Causes and consequences of imitation. Trends in cognitive sciences 5, 253–261, doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01661-2 (2001).
32. Catmur, C., Walsh, V. & Heyes, C. Associative sequence learning: the role of experience in the development of imitation and the
mirror system. Philosophical Transactions of the oyal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 364, 2369–2380, doi: 10.1098/
rstb.2009.0048 (2009).
33. im, S.-G. et al. Functional magnetic resonance imaging of motor cortex: hemispheric asymmetry and handedness. Science 261,
615–617, doi: 10.1126/science.8342027 (1993).
34. Halsband, U. & Lange, . . Motor learning in man: a review of functional and clinical studies. Journal of physiology, Paris 99,
414–424, doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2006.03.007 (2006).
35. ay, M., Dewey, D., ooistra, L. & Welsh, T. N. e relationship between the motor system activation during action observation and
adaptation in the motor system following repeated action observation. Human movement science 32, 400–411, doi: 10.1016/j.
humov.2012.02.003 (2013).
36. Osman, M., Bird, G. & Heyes, C. Action observation supports effector-dependent learning of finger movement sequences.
Experimental brain research 165, 19–27, doi: 10.1007/s00221-005-2275-0 (2005).
37. Bird, G. & Heyes, C. Eector-dependent learning by observation of a nger movement sequence. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform
31, 262–275, doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.31.2.262 (2005).
38. Bird, G., Osman, M., Saggerson, A. & Heyes, C. Sequence learning by action, observation and action observation. British journal of
psychology 96, 371–388, doi: 10.1348/000712605X47440 (2005).
39. Williams, A. & Gribble, P. L. Observed effector-independent motor learning by observing. Journal of neurophysiology 107,
1564–1570, doi: 10.1152/jn.00748.2011 (2012).
40. Costantini, M. & Committeri, G. & Galati, G. Eector-and target-independent representation of observed actions: evidence from
incidental repetition priming. Experimental brain research 188, 341–351, doi: 10. 1007/ s00221-008-1369-x (2008).
41. Shmuelof, L. & Zohary, E. Mirror-image representation of action in the anterior parietal cortex. Nature neuroscience 11, 1267–1269,
doi: 10.1038/nn.2196 (2008).
42. Frey, S. H. & Gerry, V. E. Modulation of neural activity during observational learning of actions and their sequential orders. e
Journal of neuroscience: the ocial journal of the Society for Neuroscience 26, 13194–13201, doi: 10.1523/JNEUOSCI.3914-06.2006
(2006).
43. Volmann, J., Schnitzler, A., Witte, O. & Freund, H.-J. Handedness and asymmetry of hand representation in human motor cortex.
Journal of neurophysiology 79, 2149–2154 (1998).
44. Hammond, G. Correlates of human handedness in primary motor cortex: a review and hypothesis. Neuroscience & biobehavioral
reviews 26, 285–292, doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(02)00003-9 (2002).
45. Cashmore, L., Uomini, N. & Chapelain, A. e evolution of handedness in humans and great apes: a review and current issues. J
Anthropol Sci 86, 7–35 (2008).
46. Amunts, ., Jänce, L., Mohlberg, H., Steinmetz, H. & Zilles, . Interhemispheric asymmetry of the human motor cortex related to
handedness and gender. Neuropsychologia 38, 304–312, doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00075-5 (2000).
47. Serrien, D. J., Ivry, . B. & Swinnen, S. P. Dynamics of hemispheric specialization and integration in the context of motor control.
Nature eviews Neuroscience 7, 160–166, doi: 10.1038/nrn1849 (2006).
48. Garry, M. I., amen, G. & Nordstrom, M. A. Hemispheric dierences in the relationship between corticomotor excitability changes
following a ne-motor tas and motor learning. Journal of neurophysiology 91, 1570–1578, doi: 10.1152/jn.00595.2003 (2004).
49. Culham, J. C. et al. Visually guided grasping produces fMI activation in dorsal but not ventral stream brain areas. Experimental
brain research 153, 180–189, doi: 10.1167/1.3.194 (2003).
50. Tracy, J. et al. egional brain activation associated with dierent performance patterns during learning of a complex motor sill.
Cerebral cortex 13, 904–910, doi: 10.1093/cercor/13.9.904 (2003).
51. Shmuelof, L. & Zohary, E. A mirror representation of others’ actions in the human anterior parietal cortex. The Journal of
neuroscience: the ocial journal of the Society for Neuroscience 26, 9736–9742, doi: 10.1523/JNEUOSCI.1836-06.2006 (2006).
52. Aziz-Zadeh, L., osi, L., Zaidel, E., Mazziotta, J. & Iacoboni, M. Lateralization of the human mirror neuron system. e Journal of
neuroscience : the ocial journal of the Society for Neuroscience 26, 2964–2970, doi: 10.1523/JNEUOSCI.2921-05.2006 (2006).
53. Galletti, C., Battaglini, P. P. & Fattori, P. Parietal neurons encoding spatial locations in craniotopic coordinates. Experimental brain
research 96, 221–229, doi: 10.1007/BF00227102 (1993).
54. Vandenberghe, ., Gitelman, D., Parrish, T. & Mesulam, M. Functional specicity of superior parietal mediation of spatial shiing.
NeuroImage 14, 661–673, doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0860 (2001).
55. Calvo-Merino, B., Grezes, J., Glaser, D. E., Passingham, . E. & Haggard, P. S eeing or doing? Inuence of visual and motor familiarity
in action observation. Current biology : CB 16, 1905–1910, doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.065 (2006).
56. Vogt, S. et al. Prefrontal involvement in imitation learning of hand actions: eects of practice and expertise. NeuroImage 37,
1371–1383, doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.005 (2007).
57. Avanzino, L. et al. Action observation: mirroring across our spontaneous movement tempo. Scientic reports 5, 10325, doi: 10.1038/
srep10325 (2015).
58. Tizard, B. & Hughes, M. Young children learning. (John Wiley & Sons, 2008).
59. Talairach, J. & Tornoux, P. Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain: 3-dimensional proportional system: an approach to cerebral
imaging. (Georg ieme, 1988).
60. Benjamini, Y. & Hochberg, Y. Controlling the False Discovery ate - a Practical and Powerful Approach to Multiple Testing. J oy
Stat Soc B Met 57, 289–300 (1995).
Acknowledgements
is study was supported by the I-CORE Program of the Planning and Budgeting Committee and the Israel
Science Foundation (grant No. 51/11), e Israel Science Foundation (grants No. 1771/13 and 2043/13), and
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
9
Scientific RepoRts | 6:32133 | DOI: 10.1038/srep32133
Human Frontiers Science Project (HFSP) Career Development Award (CDA00078/2011-C) (R.M.); e Yosef
Sagol Scholarship for Neuroscience Research, e Israeli Presidential Honorary Scholarship for Neuroscience
Research, and the Sagol School of Neuroscience fellowship (O.O.). e authors thank A. Shuster and R. Gilron
for fruitful comments on the manuscript. e funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis,
decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Author Contributions
R.M. and O.O. developed the study concept and designed the experiment; O.O. programmed and collected data;
O.O. carried out data analysis under supervision from R.M; O.O. and R.M. wrote the paper and approved the
nal version.
Additional Information
Competing nancial interests: e authors declare no competing nancial interests.
How to cite this article: Ossmy, O. and Mukamel, R. Activity in superior parietal cortex during training by
observation predicts asymmetric learning levels across hands. Sci. Rep. 6, 32133; doi: 10.1038/srep32133 (2016).
is work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License. e images
or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license,
unless indicated otherwise in the credit line; if the material is not included under the Creative Commons license,
users will need to obtain permission from the license holder to reproduce the material. To view a copy of this
license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
© e Author(s) 2016
... One indirect approach to facilitate the performance of the affected UL is to use visual input which provides a rich source of information supporting motor behaviour. It is now well established that performance level on a motor task can increase following passive observation of someone else performing a similar task [15,16]. The physiological mechanism that underlies this phenomenon is believed to rely on activation of mirror neurons in regions within the fronto-parietal cortex of the observer [17][18][19]. ...
... The physiological mechanism that underlies this phenomenon is believed to rely on activation of mirror neurons in regions within the fronto-parietal cortex of the observer [17][18][19]. We have recently demonstrated in healthy participants that the activity in the superior parietal lobule (SPL) during passive action observation may play an important role in such a learning process [16]. The effectiveness of visual feedback and training by observation as an additional rehabilitation tool has been also examined with patients. ...
... Therefore, it is particularly important to examine alternative approaches, especially for the more severe cases in which direct training of the affected UL is not suitable. The experimental intervention employed in the current study is such an alternative approach, motivated by knowledge gained from recent motor learning research in healthy subjects [16,23]. The behavioural part of the study shows that this approach can have a useful clinical application in neuro-rehabilitation. ...
Article
Aims Modalities for rehabilitation of the neurologically affected upper-limb (UL) are generally of limited benefit. The majority of patients seriously affected by UL paresis remain with severe motor disability, despite all rehabilitation efforts. Consequently, extensive clinical research is dedicated to develop novel strategies aimed to improve the functional outcome of the affected UL. We have developed a novel virtual-reality training tool that exploits the voluntary control of one hand and provides real-time movement-based manipulated sensory feedback as if the other hand is the one that moves. The aim of this study was to expand our previous results, obtained in healthy subjects, to examine the utility of this training setup in the context of neuro-rehabilitation. Methods We tested the training setup in patient LA, a young man with significant unilateral UL dysfunction stemming from hemi-parkinsonism. LA underwent daily intervention in which he intensively trained the non-affected upper limb, while receiving online sensory feedback that created an illusory perception of control over the affected limb. Neural changes were assessed using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) scans before and after training. Results Training-induced behavioral gains were accompanied by enhanced activation in the pre-frontal cortex and a widespread increase in resting-state functional connectivity. Discussion Our combination of cutting edge technologies, insights gained from basic motor neuroscience in healthy subjects and well-known clinical treatments, hold promise for the pursuit of finding novel and more efficient rehabilitation schemes for patients suffering from hemiplegia. • Implications for rehabilitation • Assistive devices used in hospitals to support patients with hemiparesis require expensive equipment and trained personnel – constraining the amount of training that a given patient can receive. • The setup we describe is simple and can be easily used at home with the assistance of an untrained caregiver/family member. • Once installed at the patient's home, the setup is lightweight, mobile, and can be used with minimal maintenance . • Building on advances in machine learning, our software can be adapted to personal use at homes. • Our findings can be translated into practice with relatively few adjustments, and our experimental design may be used as an important adjuvant to standard clinical care for upper limb hemiparesis.
... One indirect approach to facilitate the performance of the affected UL is to use visual input which provides a rich source of information supporting motor behaviour. It is now well established that performance level on a motor task can increase following passive observation of someone else performing a similar task [15,16]. The physiological mechanism that underlies this phenomenon is believed to rely on activation of mirror neurons in regions within the fronto-parietal cortex of the observer [17][18][19]. ...
... The physiological mechanism that underlies this phenomenon is believed to rely on activation of mirror neurons in regions within the fronto-parietal cortex of the observer [17][18][19]. We have recently demonstrated in healthy participants that the activity in the superior parietal lobule (SPL) during passive action observation may play an important role in such a learning process [16]. The effectiveness of visual feedback and training by observation as an additional rehabilitation tool has been also examined with patients. ...
... Therefore, it is particularly important to examine alternative approaches, especially for the more severe cases in which direct training of the affected UL is not suitable. The experimental intervention employed in the current study is such an alternative approach, motivated by knowledge gained from recent motor learning research in healthy subjects [16,23]. The behavioural part of the study shows that this approach can have a useful clinical application in neuro-rehabilitation. ...
... One indirect approach to facilitate the performance of the affected UL is to use visual input which provides a rich source of information supporting motor behaviour. It is now well established that performance level on a motor task can increase following passive observation of someone else performing a similar task [15,16]. The physiological mechanism that underlies this phenomenon is believed to rely on activation of mirror neurons in regions within the fronto-parietal cortex of the observer [17][18][19]. ...
... The physiological mechanism that underlies this phenomenon is believed to rely on activation of mirror neurons in regions within the fronto-parietal cortex of the observer [17][18][19]. We have recently demonstrated in healthy participants that the activity in the superior parietal lobule (SPL) during passive action observation may play an important role in such a learning process [16]. The effectiveness of visual feedback and training by observation as an additional rehabilitation tool has been also examined with patients. ...
... Therefore, it is particularly important to examine alternative approaches, especially for the more severe cases in which direct training of the affected UL is not suitable. The experimental intervention employed in the current study is such an alternative approach, motivated by knowledge gained from recent motor learning research in healthy subjects [16,23]. The behavioural part of the study shows that this approach can have a useful clinical application in neuro-rehabilitation. ...
Preprint
Cross education is a phenomenon in which motor training of one hand induces motor learning in the other hand. We have recently shown in healthy subjects that the effect of cross-education is significantly augmented by provision of real-time manipulated bi-modal (visual and kinesthetic) sensory feedback, creating an illusory sensation of voluntary training with the other hand. Here we tested whether this training method may be applicable also in pathological conditions affecting one side of the body. We present here data showing behavioral gain accompanied by functional magnetic resonance imaging dynamics following training with this setup in the case of patient LA, a young man with significant unilateral upper-limb dysfunction stemming from hemi-Parkinson’s disease. Following two weeks of daily sessions in which he intensively trained the non-affected upper limb, he showed improvement in motor capacity of the affected limb, accompanied by enhanced activation in the pre-frontal cortex and a widespread increase in functional coupling in the brain. Results from the current case study suggest that combining cross-education with manipulated sensory input may also have beneficial effects in clinical conditions.
... The SMA corresponds exactly to area 6mp: SMA and mesial superior frontal gyrus, according to recent work using probabilistic cytoarchitectonic mapping 38,39 . Furthermore, the subareas SPL 7A,7P as well as IPS hIP1 appear in manipulation of both hands, suggesting short-term learning by observation of complex action sequences 40 , or an effect of short term working memory involved in the reorganization of information at a critical site mediating sensory and haptic processing 41,42 . As evidenced by high nodal strength, the anterior-dorsal part of precuneus plays a prominent role in both action observation and motor manipulation, which is consistent with the finding of Sakai et al. 43 that the dorsal anterior precuneus was specifically involved in the transition from declarative learning of a visuomotor sequence to the effective execution. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study aims to establish an imitation task of multi-finger haptics in the context of regular grasping and regrasping processes during activities of daily living. A video guided the 26 healthy, right-handed volunteers through the three phases of the task: (1) fixation of a hand holding a cuboid, (2) observation of the sensori-motor manipulation, (3) imitation of that motor action. fMRI recorded the task; graph analysis of the acquisitions revealed the associated functional cerebral connectivity patterns. Inferred from four 60 ROI weighted graphs, the functional connectivities are consistent with a motor plan for observation and manipulation in the left hemisphere and with a network in the right hemisphere involving the inferior frontal gyrus, the site of intentional control of imitation. The networks exhibit (1) rich clubs which include sensori-motor hand, dorsal attention and cingulo-opercular communities for observation and motor execution in both hemispheres and (2) diversity clubs, significant only for manipulation and observation of the left hand, which include the dorsal visual association cortex, suggesting enhanced visual perception required for guiding the movement-limited left fingers. Short pathway analyses are consistent with these findings, confirming preferential involvement of ventral premotor cortices in the mirror network.
... Indeed, the right superior parietal gyrus is a relevant brain region involved in sustained attention, a crucial component of learning and memory 57,58 . Additionally, it has also been reported that the bilateral superior parietal gyrus plays an important role in enhancing short-term MSL during observation of hand movements 59 . Furthermore, the increased FC in the right superior parietal gyrus found in our study was mainly related to increased FC in the left posterior-superior temporal gyrus and left superior temporal sulcus. ...
Article
Full-text available
Although shared behavioral and neural mechanisms between working memory (WM) and motor sequence learning (MSL) have been suggested, the additive and interactive effects of training have not been studied. This study aimed at investigating changes in brain functional connectivity (FC) induced by sequential (WM + MSL and MSL + WM) and combined (WM × MSL) training programs. 54 healthy subjects (27 women; mean age: 30.2 ± 8.6 years) allocated to three training groups underwent twenty-four 40-min training sessions over 6 weeks and four cognitive assessments including functional MRI. A double-baseline approach was applied to account for practice effects. Test performances were compared using linear mixed-effects models and t-tests. Resting state fMRI data were analysed using FSL. Processing speed, verbal WM and manual dexterity increased following training in all groups. MSL + WM training led to additive effects in processing speed and verbal WM. Increased FC was found after training in a network including the right angular gyrus, left superior temporal sulcus, right superior parietal gyrus, bilateral middle temporal gyri and left precentral gyrus. No difference in FC was found between double baselines. Results indicate distinct patterns of resting state FC modulation related to sequential and combined WM and MSL training suggesting a relevance of the order of training performance. These observations could provide new insight for the planning of effective training/rehabilitation.
Preprint
Full-text available
This study seeks to establish a reference for monitoring sensori-motor rehabilitation from hand paresis. The reference task utilizes finger gaiting in the context of within hand prehensile object manipulation. A video guided the 26 healthy, right-handed volunteers through the three phases of the task: (1) fixation of a hand holding a cuboid, (2) observation of the sensori-motor manipulation, (3) imitation of that motor action. fMRI recorded the task; graph analysis of the acquisitions revealed the associated functional cerebral connectivity patterns. Inferred from four 60 ROI, weighted graphs, the functional connectivities are consistent with a motor plan for observation and manipulation in the left hemisphere and with a network in the right hemisphere involving the inferior frontal gyrus, the site of intentional control of imitation. The networks exhibit (1) rich clubs which include sensori-motor hand, dorsal attention, cingulo-opercular and dorsal attention communities for observation and motor execution in both hemispheres, and (2) diversity clubs, significant only for manipulation and observation of the left hand, which include the dorsal visual association cortex, suggesting enhanced visual perception required for guiding the movement-limited left fingers. Short pathway analyses are consistent with these findings, confirming preferential involvement of ventral premotor cortices in the mirror network.
Article
Full-text available
During the preadolescent period, when the cerebral thickness, curvature, and myelin are constantly changing, the brain's regionalization patterns underwent persistent development, contributing to the continuous improvements of various higher cognitive functions. Using a brain atlas to study the development of these functions has attracted much attention. However, the brains of children do not always have the same topological patterns as those of adults. Therefore, age-specific brain mapping is particularly important, serving as a basic and indispensable tool to study the normal development of children. In this study, we took advantage of longitudinal data to create the brain atlas specifically for preadolescent children. The resulting human Child Brainnetome Atlas, with 188 cortical and 36 subcortical subregions, provides a precise period-specific and cross-validated version of the brain atlas that is more appropriate for adoption in the preadolescent period. In addition, we compared and illustrated for regions with different topological patterns in the child and adult atlases, providing a topologically consistent reference for subsequent research studying child and adolescent development.
Article
Virtual reality (VR) is effectively used to evoke the mirror illusion, and transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) synergistically facilitates this illusion. This study investigated whether a mirror virtual hand illusion (MVHI) induced by an immersive, first-person-perspective, virtual mirror system could be modulated by tDCS of the primary motor cortex. Fourteen healthy adults (average age 21.86 years ±0.47, seven men and seven women) participated in this study, and they experienced VR with and without tDCS-the tDCS and sham conditions, each of which takes ∼30 minutes-on separate days to allow the washout of the tDCS effect. While experiencing VR, the movements of the virtual left hand reflected the flexion and extension of the real right hand. Subsequently, electroencephalogram was recorded, the magnitude of the proprioceptive shift was measured, and the participants provided responses to a questionnaire regarding hand ownership. A significant difference in the proprioceptive shift was observed between the tDCS and sham conditions. In addition, there was significant suppression of the mu power in Pz, and augmentation of the beta power in the Pz, P4, O1, and O2 channels. The difference in proprioceptive deviation between the two conditions showed significant negative correlation with mu suppression over the left frontal lobe in the tDCS condition. Finally, the question "I felt that the virtual hand was my own hand" received a significantly higher score under the tDCS condition. In short, applying tDCS over the motor cortex facilitates the MVHI by activating the attentional network over the parietal and frontal lobes such that the MVHI induces more proprioceptive drift, which suggests that the combination of VR and tDCS can enhance the immersive effect in VR. This result provides better support for the use of the MVHI paradigm in combination with tDCS for recovery from illnesses such as stroke.
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this research was to determine the effect of the model’s skill level and frequency of feedback on learning a simple serial aiming task. For this purpose, 48 female students aged 19 to 25 years old were selected as the sample. Based on pre-test movement times, they were divided into four groups of 12 subjects (expert model and 100% feedback, expert model and 50% feedback, novice model and 100% feedback, and novice model and 50% feedback). In the acquisition phase, the groups performed the serial aiming task 80 times according to specific instructions. The immediate retention test was conducted 15 minutes after completion of the acquisition phase, and the delayed retention test was conducted 24 hours later. The results of mixed analysis of variance with repeated measures on training blocks in the acquisition phase showed that the main effect of the model in both variables of movement time and spatial error were significant, and expert model observer groups had longer movement times and fewer spatial errors than other groups. The results of two-way analysis of variance in the immediate and delayed retention tests indicated that in both variables of movement time and spatial error, the main effect of the model is significant, but the main effect of the frequency of feedback is not meaningful. Comparing the means showed that the expert model observer groups had longer movement times and fewer spatial errors than other groups that observed novice models.
Article
Full-text available
During action observation (AO), the activity of the "mirror system" is influenced by the viewer's expertise in the observed action. A question that remains open is whether the temporal aspects of the subjective motor repertoire can influence the "mirror system" activation.
Article
The common approach to the multiplicity problem calls for controlling the familywise error rate (FWER). This approach, though, has faults, and we point out a few. A different approach to problems of multiple significance testing is presented. It calls for controlling the expected proportion of falsely rejected hypotheses — the false discovery rate. This error rate is equivalent to the FWER when all hypotheses are true but is smaller otherwise. Therefore, in problems where the control of the false discovery rate rather than that of the FWER is desired, there is potential for a gain in power. A simple sequential Bonferronitype procedure is proved to control the false discovery rate for independent test statistics, and a simulation study shows that the gain in power is substantial. The use of the new procedure and the appropriateness of the criterion are illustrated with examples.
Article
Most people are right-handed, preferring the right hand for skilled as well as unskilled activities, but a notable proportion are mixed-handed, preferring to use the right hand for some actions and the left hand for others. Assuming a structural/functional correlation in the motor system we tested whether asymmetries in hand performance in consistent right and left handers as well as in mixed handers are associated with anatomical asymmetries in the motor cortex. In vivo MR morphometry was used for analyzing interhemispheric asymmetry in the depth of the central sulcus in the region of cortical hand representation of 103 healthy subjects. Subjects were tested both for hand preference and hand performance. As expected, left-right differences in hand performance differed significantly between consistent right, consistent left and mixed handers and were independent on gender. Male consistent right handers showed a significant deeper central sulcus on the left hemisphere than on the right. Anatomical asymmetries decreased significantly from male consistent right over mixed to consistent left handers. Sixty two per cent of consistent left handers revealed a deeper central sulcus on the right than on the left hemisphere, but for the group as a whole this rightward asymmetry was not significant. No interhemispheric asymmetry was found in females. Thus, anatomical asymmetry was associated with handedness only in males, but not in females, suggesting sex differences in the cortical organization of hand movements.
Article
Most people are right-handed, preferring the right hand for skilled as well as unskilled activities, but a notable proportion are mixed-handed, preferring to use the right hand for some actions and the left hand for others. Assuming a structural/functional correlation in the motor system we tested whether asymmetries in hand performance in consistent right and left handers as well as in mixed handers are associated with anatomical asymmetries in the motor cortex. In vivo MR morphometry was used for analyzing interhemispheric asymmetry in the depth of the central sulcus in the region of cortical hand representation of 103 healthy subjects. Subjects were tested both for hand preference and hand performance. As expected, left-right differences in hand performance differed significantly between consistent right, consistent left and mixed handers and were independent on gender. Male consistent right handers showed a significant deeper central sulcus on the left hemisphere than on the right. Anatomical asymmetries decreased significantly from male consistent right over mixed to consistent left handers. Sixty two per cent of consistent left handers revealed a deeper central sulcus on the right than on the left hemisphere, but for the group as a whole this rightward asymmetry was not significant. No interhemispheric asymmetry was found in females. Thus, anatomical asymmetry was associated with handedness only in males, but not in females, suggesting sex differences in the cortical organization of hand movements.
Article
Modern neurosurgical concepts call for not only "seeing" but also for "localizing" structures in three-dimensional space in relationship to each other. Hence there is a need for a reference system. This book aims to put this notion into practice by means of anatomical and MRI sections with the same stereotaxic orientation. The purpose is to display the fundamental distribution of structures in three-dimensional space and their spatial evolution within the brain as a whole, while facilitating their identification; to make comparative studies of cortico-subcortical lesions possible on a basis of an equivalent reference system; to exploit the anatomo-functional data such as those furnished by SEEG in epilepsy and to enable the localization of special regions such as the SMA in three-dimensional space; and to apply the anatomical correlations of this reference system to neurophysiological investigations lacking sufficient anatomical back-up (including PET scan).
Article
After watching sports, people often feel as if their sports skills might have been improved, even without any actual training. On some occasions, this motor skill learning through observation actually occurs. This phenomenon may be due to the fact that both action and action observation (AO) can activate shared cortical areas. However, the neural basis of performance gain through AO has not yet been fully clarified. In the present study, we used transcranial magnetic stimulation to investigate whether primary motor cortex (M1) plasticity is a physiological substrate of AO-induced performance gain and whether AO itself is sufficient to change motor performance. The excitability of M1, especially that of its intracortical excitatory circuit, was enhanced after and during AO with kinesthetic illusion but not in interventions without this illusion. Moreover, behavioral improvement occurred only after AO with kinesthetic illusion, and a significant correlation existed between the performance gain and the degree of illusion. Our findings indicated that kinesthetic illusion is an essential component of the motor learning and M1 plasticity induced by AO, and this insight may be useful for the strategic rehabilitation of stroke patients. This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved. This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
Article
This study investigated whether activation within areas belonging to the action observation and imitation network reveals a linear relation to the subsequent accuracy of imitating a bimanual rhythmic movement measured via a motion capturing system. 20 participants were scanned with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) when asked to imitate observed bimanual movements either concurrently versus with a delay (2s) or simply to observe the movements without imitation. Results showed that action observation relates to activation within classic mirror-related areas. Activation patterns were more widespread when participants were asked to imitate the movement. During observation with concurrent imitation, activation in the left inferior parietal lobe (IPL) was associated negatively with imitation accuracy. During observation in the delayed imitation condition, higher subsequent imitation accuracy was coupled with higher activation in the right superior parietal lobe (SPL) and the left parietal operculum (POp). During the delayed imitation itself, a negative association between imitation accuracy and brain activation was revealed in the right ventral premotor cortex (vPMC). We conclude that the IPL is involved in online comparison and visuospatial attention processes during imitation, the SPL provides a kinesthetic blueprint during movement observation, the POp preserves body identity, and the vPMC recruits motor representations-especially when no concurrent visual guidance is possible.
Article
There is a large body of psychological and neuroimaging experiments that have interpreted their findings in favor of a functional equivalence between action generation, action simulation, action verbalization, and perception of action. On the basis of these data, the concept of shared motor representations has been proposed. Indeed several authors have argued that our capacity to understand other people’s behavior and to attribute intention or beliefs to others is rooted in a neural, most likely distributed, execution/observation mechanism. Recent neuroimaging studies have explored the neural network engaged during motor execution, simulation, verbalization, and observation. The focus of this metaanalysis is to evaluate in specific detail to what extent the activated foci elicited by these studies overlap.
Article
Repeated action observation has been shown to alter the cortical representation of the observed movement in the motor system. This change in cortical representation is thought to reflect a motor adaptation to observational training (observational training effect). One factor that may impact the observational training effect is the degree of motor system activation that occurs during the observation of the action (i.e., individual differences in the responsiveness of the motor system during action observation). The present study was conducted to test this hypothesis by assessing the relationship between the change in motor system activity during action observation and the change in cortical representation of action following repeated action observation. To this end, transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) was used to evoke contractions of thumb muscles in two different protocols: 1) during the observation of thumb movements to assess the responsiveness of each individual's corticospinal system during action observation; and, 2) after the observation of 1800 thumb movements to assess the amount of adaptation in the representation of the thumb following repeated action observation. The key finding was the significant positive relationship between the level of corticospinal system activation during action observation and the amount of change in the direction of TMS evoked thumb movements. These data support the hypothesized relationship between motor system activation during action observation and the motor systems adaptation following observational training. They are also consistent with the notion that a common neural mechanism underlies these effects.