ArticlePDF Available

The inseparable role of emotions in the teaching and learning of primary school science

Authors:

Abstract

In this paper, we seek to explore the inseparable role of emotions in the teaching and the learning of science at the primary school level, as we elaborate the theoretical underpinnings and personal experiences that lead us to this notion of inseparability. We situate our perspectives on the complexity of science education in primary schools, draw on existing literature on emotions in science, and present arguments for the necessity of working towards positive emotions in our work with young children and their teachers. We layer our own perspectives and experiences as teachers and as researchers onto methodological arguments through narratives to emerge with a reflective essay that seeks to highlight the importance of emotions in our work with children and their teachers in elementary school science.
Guest Editors: S. Ritchie and K. Tobin.
This article is part of the Special Issue on Researching Emotions in Science Education.
Christina Siry ( )
The University of Luxembourg, 2 avenue de l’Université, L-4365 Esch-sur-Alzette, Luxembourg
Email: christina.siry@uni.lu
Michelle Brendel
The University of Luxembourg, Esch-sur-Alzette, Luxembourg
!
The Inseparable Role of Emotions in the Teaching and Learning of Primary
School Science
Christina Siry and Michelle Brendel
Author’s pre-print copy of accepted version of manuscript. Print version
available at: http://rd.springer.com/article/10.1007/s11422-016-9781-1
2!
The Inseparable Role of Emotions in the Teaching and Learning of Primary
School Science
Christina Siry and Michelle Brendel
Abstract In this paper, we seek to explore the inseparable role of emotions in the
teaching and the learning of science at the primary school level, as we elaborate the
theoretical underpinnings and personal experiences that lead us to this notion of
inseparability. We situate our perspectives on the complexity of science education in
primary schools, draw on existing literature on emotions in science, and present
arguments for the necessity of working towards positive emotions in our work with
young children and their teachers. We layer our own perspectives and experiences as
teachers and as researchers onto methodological arguments through narratives to
emerge with a reflective essay that seeks to highlight the importance of emotions in
our work with children and their teachers in elementary school science.
Keywords dialogue · embodied science · doing science · elementary science
Resume Dans cet article, nous allons explorer le rôle inséparable des émotions dans
l'enseignement et l'apprentissage des sciences à l'école primaire. A cette fin, nous
élaborons les fondements théoriques ainsi que les expériences personnelles qui nous
mènent à cette notion d'inséparabilité. Nous situons notre vue de la complexité de
l'enseignement des sciences dans les classes de l'école primaire, en nous référant à la
littérature existante sur les émotions en sciences et présentons des arguments qui
mettent en évidence la nécessité de favoriser les émotions positives dans notre travail
avec de jeunes enfants et leurs enseignants. A travers une approche narrative, nous
confrontons nos propres perspectives et expériences comme enseignantes et
chercheures à des arguments méthodologiques afin de faire émerger un essai réflexif
qui cherche à mettre en évidence l'importance des émotions dans notre travail avec les
enfants et leurs enseignants dans l'enseignement des sciences à l'école primaire.
I needed to know that somebody else believed that my work had promise and
direction, and I have often been surprised that others had faith in me when I
had very little in myself. (Bateson 1971, xix)
We consider learning as expanding the possibilities to have the world at one's disposal
(« Verfügung über die Welt », Holzkamp 1995). Perceiving of the world as social and
physical, of science as the understanding of phenomena, of culture as a system of
beliefs, values, customs, behaviors, and artifacts that are used in a society, enables us
to find one's way in the world, one's place - or not - in the community. To this end of
expanding possibilities, we heed Gregory Bateson’s point to ‘believe’ in the promise
and direction of our students’ work. Learning plays a key role in the transmission of
culture from one generation to the next, and in some ways, so does teaching and
schooling. Central to these complex processes is the role of emotions, which we frame
as deeply connected and woven together with teaching, schooling, and learning.
Emotions are fluid and complex, and it has been suggested that there are three reasons
that there has been a dearth of research on the role of emotions in education; first, that
3!
they are fluid and more difficult to describe than cognition, second, due to the
dominance of cognitive psychology over research in education, and lastly, due to the
legacy of dualism that has positioned reason in opposition to emotion (Zembylas
2005). In this paper, we seek to explore the inseparable role of emotions in the
teaching and the learning of science at the primary school level, as we elaborate the
theoretical underpinnings and personal experiences that lead us to this notion of
inseparability.
Young children’s science learning
Learning is a basic and ubiquitous human activity. Society is based on
learning; communities are held together by learning; and people construct
identities through learning. Yet learning becomes problematic in school,
where it is assumed that some people will learn and others will not, and where
it is assumed that learning is something that kids will only do under coercion.
(Eckert, Goldman and Wenger 1997)
Certainly, learning is a core component of societies, yet there is a contradiction that
can be found when teaching and schooling itself are structured in the problematic
ways introduced in the quote above. In the sections that follow, we situate our
perspectives on the complexity of science education in primary schools, draw on
existing literature on emotions in science, and present arguments for the necessity of
working towards positive emotions in our work with young children and their
teachers. We layer our own perspectives and experiences as teachers and as
researchers onto methodological arguments through narratives to emerge with a
reflective essay that seeks to highlight the importance of emotions in our work with
children and their teachers in elementary school science. We regularly experience
children engaged in everyday, non-school activities, that show scientific thinking
(Bruner 1986), expressed by children’s understanding of evidence, their ability to
distinguish between the physical causes of an event, and a person’s reasons for
believing something.
On a walk through the woods 4-year-old Lia and her friend Martha were
running down a little hill, when they stopped where the track flattened and
looked down the valley. Martha exclaimed, "I did not see how beautiful it is
here, did you, Lia?" "No!" said Lia, "I think, when you run as fast as you can,
you don't see anymore. Let’s test, you see that big tree ahead? Lets run as fast
as we can." Arriving at the tree, Martha said: "I did not see anything around
me, did you?" Lia replied "No!" as Martha said to Michelle, "Michelle, when
you run as fast as you can, you don't see anything anymore!" And they walked
on, discussing how when you focus on your running, you don't always notice
your surroundings.
This moment between Martha, Lia, and Michelle (the second author of this paper)
provides an example of something that those who interact with young children have
no doubt experienced countless times; that is, children observe their environment and
theorize the ways in which they interact and experience their surroundings to arrive at
observations about causes and effects of specific phenomena, such as how Lia and
Martha sensed their environment based on what they were doing in a given moment.
Terry Eagleton has written that young children are wonderful theorists, as they have
4!
not yet been educated into the social practices that assume what is the norm, and in
turn, ask questions of the routine, as “they do not see why we might not do things
differently” (1990, p. 34). As researchers we regularly have discussions with
colleagues, who draw on Jean Piaget's developmental stage theory (1950) to ground
their explanations that children under 11 years are not able to show hypothetic-
deductive scientific reasoning, and thus are not able to form hypotheses and carry out
experiments. We work in the European country of Luxembourg, where Piaget's
understanding of the interrelationship of intelligence and affectivity in children's
development (1954) still serves as an implicit foundation of the school science
curriculum, which, as we will elaborate later, constitutes a dilemma for teachers when
they collaborate with us. Our work is grounded in sociocultural theoretical
perspectives (e.g., Sewell 1992) and we examine the complex individual as well as
collective ways in which children engage in describing and exploring natural
phenomena – as such, we are troubled by strict notions of what children can, and
cannot, do in science. When we consider our colleagues’ strong commitment to
adopting a perspective of the concrete stages of children’s development, we wonder
how this contrasts with what we hear when we adopt critical approaches to radically
listen to children in schools, as well as in non-school activities, and “believe” in them
in the ways that Bateson has described in the opening quote above.
Science as embodied and “done"
Through the frameworks and perspectives that ground our work in educational
research, science is an enacted and embodied process, one that is lived in the practice.
We have written previously about science as something that is “done” (e.g., Siry,
Ziegler and Max 2012) in discursive interactions. We currently are co-PIs on a
research project that situates the assessment of science processes and understandings
through dialogue (“Assessing Science Processes in Narratives”, ASPIN, 2013-2016).
Through this project, we work closely with teachers of early elementary students to
examine their own praxis as well as the practices their students engage in as they “do
science.” This and other previous work we have done with young children and their
teachers provides us with a specific lens on the deep connections between emotions
and science learning and teaching, one that goes far beyond the cognitive dimensions
of learning alone. The linear, rational model of science has been critiqued in the
literature (e.g., Roth and McGinn 1997), and there is a body of literature that has
emerged in particular over the past decade that argues for students to be engaged in
authentic science learning practices (e.g., Quigley 2014). We seek to gain
understandings of the complex ways in which children engage in the practices of
doing science, to provide us, as teachers and as researchers, with a particular lens on
how these understandings mediate our work with teachers in professional
development.
Common in many early childhood and elementary classrooms internationally
are investigations around the topic of water, and we have previously written about the
possibilities for constructing teaching and learning experiences from children’s
wonderings around water investigations (e.g., Siry and Max, 2012). We now draw on
narrative examples of students investigating water as they interact with volume
capacities of different containers to highlight the ways in which emotional
engagement is inherent to, and inseparable from, learning and doing science. We have
constructed narrative “impressionist tales” (van Maanen 1988) to guide our theoretical
and interpretive reflections that follow.
5!
A Kindergarten class is outside the school building and children are spread out
around the schoolyard. The teacher has set up a variety of large basins filled
with water and containers of different shapes and sizes. At one of these basins,
a group of five children stands around the edge, all looking into the water with
their hands in the water, splashing vigorously. As they splash around, the
water sprays their faces and, as they laugh collectively, there are shrieks of joy
as the sun shines and the water glistens.
Five-year-old Jan takes the largest container, scoops up some water, and turns
to his classmate Nina standing next to him. “I’ll put it in yours, then you put it
in mine” he says, as he pours the water from his container into her (smaller)
container. “Yes!” Nina shouts “Again! Again!” As the water from Jan’s
container overflows in hers, she grabs his hand smiling and pours the water
back into his. Jan’s face is lit up in a huge smile as he raises his eyebrows,
looks at his container and starts to jump and up and down eagerly. “First in
yours, now in mine! Let’s try it again!” he says. Suddenly the other children
around the basin all begin pouring water from full containers to empty ones, as
they giggle, bounce, and enthusiastically move water around from one
container to another.
At the heart of the “doing” of science is wonder, curiosity, and risk-taking. A central
consideration of engaging in science is “the relationship between scientific thinking
and larger conceptualizations of the world, and indeed the universe, as an object of
wonder and questioning” (Gallas 1994, p. 77). Jan, Nina, and their classmates
exchange utterances imbued with excitement, and these utterances are filled with
sounds of wonder and playfulness. Their curiosity is embodied, as they use their
gestures, expressions, pitch, and tone to emphasize their discoveries. The interaction
is filled with children saying “Let’s try this …” and “now in this one …” as they laugh,
smile, and bounce up and down on their toes. Much has been written in the field of
science education on the value of engaging students in the practices of science (e.g.,
Liljeström, Enkenberg and Pöllänen 2013)) and the focus on inquiry and science
practices has been written about for generations (e.g., Dewey 1910). Such authentic
practices are evident, and indeed central, when elementary aged children engage
multimodally / multisensorially in science investigations. Focusing on embodied
science (doing science as well as talking about science) provides a lens for teachers,
researchers, and the students themselves to reflexively consider not only what is
known, but how it is engaged in as an enacted (i.e., emotional, lived) practice. In the
next sections, we highlight several questions for reflexive consideration; How is
science learning an emotional practice? In what ways do science understandings
evolve in the emotions and science relationship? How do listening, belonging, and
becoming connect to this relationship? As we engage with these questions, we
wonder (and ask readers of this essay to do the same); where is the joy? Where is the
love? In the exploration of these questions, we hope to provide a lens on the
emotionally embedded ways in which children and their teachers engage with science
practices and concepts.
How is science learning an emotional practice?
Emotions are as ubiquitous as breathing. (Radford 2015)
6!
This special issue of the journal Cultural Studies of Science Education illustrates an
international focus across the papers on researchers’ emphasis on emotions in their
work in science education. Emotions are contextually situated in time and space, and
in our work in elementary schools we often witness students’ embodied emotional
performances as they engage in science investigations, such as those of Lia, Martha
and Michelle, as well as those of Jan and Nina. These moments are mediated by
individual as well as collective emotional energies. Emotions are relational, and as
cultural constructs they are socioculturally as well as historically constructed. Jan and
Nina’s interaction around the pouring of water into differently sized and shaped
containers is filled with collective effervescence and collective laughter, which has
been theorized as the social glue that holds groups together (Collins 2005).
Our collaborative research with teachers is guided in part by Margaret Carr’s
(2001) approach to learning stories, in which she accentuates the importance of
wellbeing and the feeling of belonging to the community in children's learning. There
is a necessity of a mutuality of positive feeling and a mutual affection in order to
facilitate learning (Bronfenbrenner 1979). To meet Bronfenbrenners' claims, in such a
learning community, all members are considered, and consider themselves, as
learners, and thus form the community of learners (Rogoff, Matusov and White
1998). In short, children ought to be considered experts of their own learning, which
requires a shift in framing the roles and power relations in the classroom. This shift in
the balance of power Bronfenbrenner is speaking of also corresponds to the notion of
responsibility emphasized by Barbara Rogoff, Eugene Matusov, and Cynthia White,
who have elaborated that:
In a classroom functioning as a community of learners, organization involves
dynamic and complementary group relations among class members who learn
to take responsibility for their contribution to their own learning and to the
group’s functioning. (…) (T)he organization involves a community working
together with all serving as resources to the others, with varying roles
according to their understanding of the activity at hand and differing (and
shifting) responsibilities in the system. (1998, p. 397)
It is precisely this mutual sense of belonging to a community of learners, and the
sense of responsibility of all the members of the community to work collectively
towards positive, mutual, feelings of belonging, that we hope to work towards in our
interactions with children and their teachers around the learning of science. As we
hear children’s ideas, as with Lia and Martha’s observation on how what we see is
shaped by how we interact with our environment, it is critical to take these ideas
seriously and to set aside any assumptions and expectations on what they “should” be
saying relative to cause and effect, for example. Inspired by radical listening (e.g.,
Tobin and Kincheloe, 2010), humility and openness are necessary to listen to, and
respect, the theories and questions that emerge from children’s observations and
interactions. We hope that in doing so we can push back against the prevalent notions
of strict stages that define what children can, or should, say and do at a level of
schooling, and instead create spaces in which each child can develop and grow in
ways that are most suited to them. Lois Holzman has written about young children’s
development and noted that, “the process of learning and the product of learning are
created together” (2009, p. 18). We embrace this notion of the unification of learning
and developing, and next elaborate how we have ‘seen’ science understandings evolve
in the emotions and science relationship.
7!
In what ways do science understandings evolve in the emotions / science
relationship?
The sense of wonder vividly apparent as youngsters gaze enthralled at art, at
nature, at falling leaves, reminds us of our responsibility to keep that feeling
alive, to amplify and mirror it as valuable and humanizing. (Arnold 2010, p.
41)
This sense of wonder that Arnold alludes to is a central component of engaging in
science, and we have written previously about the possibilities in building science
curriculum from students’ wonderings (Siry and Max 2013). The term “wonderings”
is one that we use to encompass the embodied, often non-verbal, ways in which
children interact around their questions and discoveries of science phenomena.
Children such as Jan and Nina, and myriad others we have encountered through the
years, try something out (like pouring water from a bigger container to a smaller one),
observe what happens, and then typically try again (and again and again as in Nina
and Jan’s case). As they engage in such ritualistic behaviors, their ideas shift and
change, which can be seen in the new ways in which they try their next investigations.
When such rituals in a group are positive, members of the group can experience
positive emotional energy, which over time supports a sense of belonging (Collins
2002). This sense of belonging supports children’s inquiry, as each moment of
children’s positively-infused wondering can lead to new ones – new questions, new
ideas, new explorations. This act of wondering is one that we see as central to the
processes of inquiry – in fact, it is perhaps the most central aspect of doing science –
and it is often only visible in examining nonverbal engagement of children. We now
return to Jan and Nina to explore their wonderings around the water containers of
varying sizes.
Jan has said to Nina “First in yours, now in mine! Let’s try it again!” She
smiles, nods, and turns her container upside down. She taps the bottom of the
container, presumably to get all the water out. Turning it back right-side up,
she nods and smiles at Jan. He fills his (larger) container as full as he can
manage, and makes eye contact with her, eyes wide open and eyebrows raised.
Tilting his head, he holds her hand with her container steady, and pours the
water from his container into hers. She leans over, concentrating as she
watches what happens as the water fills her container and overflows into the
basin and over her and Jan’s hands. Her eyes get wider and wider, until Jan’s
container is empty. They both pause briefly, bounce up and down on their toes
while turning their heads to make eye contact and their faces burst into smiles
as they giggle and move back and forth from side to side. “Again?” Nina asks.
Yes, but different?” Jan replies, and the both begin to laugh again.
As Jan, Nina and their classmates explore the containers and the differing amounts
that they can hold, they refine their approaches to pouring and filling as they go.
Hands are held more steady, often with the help of another, as in the vignette above.
Giggling and dancing around is balanced with holding very still as new methods of
getting as much water as possible into the different containers are tried. Very few
words are exchanged, and no words are exchanged that directly refer to water or
volume or water flow, yet by looking and listening closely to the embodied emotional
8!
ways in which the children are investigating, it becomes evident that they are
wondering how to get as much water as possible into the different sized containers. It
also is clear that the children are enjoying their interactions and their investigations, as
evidenced by laughter and shrieks of joy, and expressive movements such as dancing
and jumping on their toes. Despite the fact that there are very few words used in this
interaction, the children’s wonderings about the relations between the size and shape
of the containers and the amount of water that they can hold are evident. While we
can of course not “know” what the children were thinking, we can examine the
interaction for clues to children’s interests and questions, and in doing so, we focus on
the embodied, joyful ways in which children demonstrate and develop their science
understandings. While it is beyond the scope of our research to frame our work
around students’ “thinking,” we take inspiration from Lev Vygotsky’s latest works, in
which he reflected on the relations between thought, affect, language and
consciousness
(Thought) is not born of other thoughts. Thought has its origins in the
motivating sphere of consciousness, a sphere that includes our inclinations and
needs, our interests and impulses, and our affect and emotions. The affective
and volitional tendency stands behind thought. Only here do we find the
answer to final 'why' in the analysis of thinking. (Vygotsky 1934/1987, p.282)
Mahn and John-Steiner expand on this often neglected aspect of Vygotsky’s work,
emotions, by drawing on their own work (e,g., Mahn 1997; John-Steiner 2000) and
other scholars (e.g., Damasio 1999) who describe emotions as coming into being
through the interplay of interpersonal and intrapersonal dynamics. They describe the
zone of proximal development - the distance between the learner's actual
development level and the level the learner could reach through collaboration - as a
complex whole, in which negative affective factors diminish the zone in which
effective teaching and learning can occur (Mahn and John-Steiner 2002). We consider
a range of positive emotions as being not only critical for such effective teaching and
learning, but just as importantly, for supporting students’ sense of belonging to a
community of science learners. As we perceive of emotions and emotional energy
along a continuum, then positive emotions are a motivating force that can lead to
feelings of happiness and solidarity among a group (Collins 1990). Randall Collins
has theorized that people seek out situations in which they expect positive emotional
energy, and thus experiences that can lead to higher levels of positive emotions are
motivating, as these emotions “serve as a powerful means to connect children to their
learning” (Hughes 2013, p. 12). As Jan, Nina, Lia, Martha, and Michelle investigate
with positive emotions their science understandings may grow and evolve, as they are
positioned to take risks and pursue their wonderings.
Critical reflection on my experience as a student in unexciting classrooms
enabled me not only to imagine that the classroom could be exciting but that this
excitement could co-exist with and even stimulate serious intellectual and / or
academic engagement” (hooks 1994, p.7) As bell hooks has captured through
reflection on her own experiences as a student, classrooms filled with moments of
excitement can stimulate serious growth and engagement. Thus, focusing on positive
emotions is not an add-on to the teaching and learning of science, but rather is an
inseparable component that mediates children’s and teachers’ meaning-making.
9!
How do listening, belonging, and becoming connect to the relationship of science
learning and emotions?
Education through our lenses is a continual process of being and becoming (Siry,
Brendel and Frisch in press) and we hope that this process mediates learning science;
in particular, learning science as part of having the world at our disposal (Holzkamp
1995). Earlier we suggested that radical listening is central to working with research
participants, and indeed, we believe that ethical, compassionate, critical approaches to
research can be accomplished through providing spaces in which participants can
engage in dialogue around their experiences and perspectives. In these dialogic
encounters, listening is central. To hear each other, to listen to one another, is an
exercise in recognition (hooks 1994). As we listen to what the other says, we open
ourselves up to the perspectives of those that might be very different from our own, in
an effort to understand the positionalities and perspectives of those we work with.
We believe that we need to not only “give voice” to participants in our research, but
we need to listen to, and act on, these voices that we hear. This in turn can lead us to
more ethical, empathetic, work with research participants and engage them in
processes of teaching, learning, and researching in ways that connect to the emotional
practices of belonging to a community. “As a classroom community, our capacity to
generate excitement is deeply affected by our interest in one another, in hearing one
another’s voices, in recognizing one another’s presence” (hooks 1994, p. 8).
Becoming a member of a classroom community is a deeply emotional experience, one
that is grounded on trust, respect, and the ability to take risks and feel safe, heard, and
recognized by the community. Part of this process, we contend, is built upon
experiences (which in our work are situated in science classes) in which children and
teachers together open themselves to what the other has to say. We contrast Nina and
Jan above to another narrative elaboration of a lesson in a similar Kindergarten class.
Children are seated at their desks, which have been arranged into groups of
four. In the center of these now-larger tables created by the four desks are
three containers of varying shapes and sizes. “Which one holds the most
water?” The classroom teacher asks. Six-year-old Tessy and Dylan exchange
glances with furrowed brows as they then look at and point to each of the
different containers in front of their group of four children. Tessy holds up one
and looks at the group with a quizzical expression, as if asking a question of
the group. The other children raise their eyebrows, look at the containers, and
slowly each of them nods. Tessy then holds up the one container proudly and
shows it to the teacher. “You predict this one Tessy? Try with your group now
to see if you are right” the teacher says while smiling. “Take them to the sink
and try to pour water from one to the other to see if you are right that this one
is the biggest one” she instructs them next.
The four children go to the sink with the containers. They stand for a moment
and look at the containers and the sink, with their mouths tightly closed and
their brows furrowed. Their teacher comes over and suggests they take the one
they think is the smallest and pour it into the next one, and so on, to see if they
are correct in their guess. The children then order the three containers from the
shortest to the tallest (important to note is that the containers vary greatly in
width, and thus the shortest is not the one that holds the least amount of
water…) and they begin. No words are exchanged, and the children stand
10!
quietly and watch intensely as Tessy fills the first container and pours it into
the next one. Indeed, the first is smaller than the second, and the children
collectively smile and exhale making sounds of “phew!” as they have had
apparent success. The next try is to take the second container and pour the
water into the one that Tessy predicted was the one to hold the most water.
Again the children are deeply focused, wordlessly watching and holding very
still as Tessy pours the water from the second to the third container. However,
with this try, the water overflows, as the third container does not hold more
water than the second. Tessy’s facial expression shifts to one of
disappointment, and her shoulders drop. “Why’d you choose the wrong one?”
Dylan asks her “Now we don't have it right!” he scolds her.
We question whether in classroom moments that are imbued with negative emotions,
such as this one with Tessy and her classmates, children develop a sense of belonging
that can support their science investigations in ways that allow them to take risks and
express and pursue their wonderings. Transient negative emotions, including
embarrassment, fear and anger during interactions have an influence on people’s
levels of emotional energy (Turner and Stets 2005). As we position one of the goals of
education as learning to have the world at our disposal, we hope for educational
experiences that inspire children to pursue their curiosity and wonder. With this as an
overarching goal, moments like these with Tessy and her classmates, which focus
solely on achieving one correct answer, can act to counter a goal of supporting
children’s open exploration and pursuit of wonderings. We echo a question posed by
Wolk, as she asks “Is it really possible to inspire people to live a life of learning and
wonder, if throughout their schooling children are always told what to learn, when to
learn, and how to learn?” (2007, p. 652). Belonging to a learning community requires
a commitment to engaged pedagogical approaches that focus on the well-being of the
children and all the participants in the classroom, in order “to educate as the practice
of freedom is a way of teaching that anyone can learn” (hooks 1994, p. 13).
Classroom teachers as well as we as researchers are members of the children's
learning community, and thus in considering engaged pedagogical approaches and the
role of emotions in learning, we ask, how are we, as researchers, positioned in this
community? Are we inadvertently representing the teaching-as-usual the classroom
teachers may know and expect from their training and experience; the mainstream
science education research approaches that perpetuate positivist expectations of
research processes? Rich normality requiring that "(w)ithin an environment and a
society that are made of exceptions and special cases, the design and didactic result
should be that of dynamic normality" (Ceppi and Zini 1998) the Other to the norms
defined by the national curriculum? Margery Wolf stresses that "(N)o matter how
careful, I fear all of us who do research must be prepared to be the represented Other
to the "objects" of our study" (1992, p.13). The teachers participating in our study
may feel torn between the community of Luxembourgish teachers, in which the
competent teachers are positioned to know and transmit solid knowledge to their
students on one hand, and the research community we belong to in which all
members, children and adults, are positioned as learners in the "doing" of science on
the other hand. Thus, collaborating in research becomes destabilizing, confusing, and
evokes feelings of uncertainty and lack of confidence. The teachers' emotional need to
be positioned as competent teachers by the other members of the school community
competes with their desire to be recognized by the members of the research group,
entailing the loss of their feeling of belonging to either of these communities.
11!
We have heard teachers express this contradiction countless times in our work
with them, as teachers express their frustration about wanting to engage in new
pedagogical approaches, but feeling confusion about how to do that and still have
their students emerge with the “right” conceptual understandings relating to science.
For example, one teacher we recently collaborated with summed this feeling up when
she said “but if the students are investigating on their own, I still need to be sure they
know the right facts at the end.|As pointed out by Walsh (2005, p. 42), "(T)he deep
retention of Piaget’s theory long after the shortcomings have been identified is
curious. (...) (The) focus on development, rather than on other areas of schooling, for
example, curriculum, instruction, and assessment, has remained dominant in the
field.” We have noted that for our participating teachers, theoretical frameworks are
not considered as tools that might be useful for the development of teaching and
learning, but as truth to which the own practice needs to correspond. A Piagetian
understanding of young children's developmental levels calling for teachers who
structure the children's learning and provide the right knowledge is in conflict with
perspectives that position even very young children as authors of their own learning in
a community. Tessy makes a wrong assumption about the different sizes and volume
capacity of the containers, and is scolded by Dylan – how do we avoid this in other
work in classrooms? We look for approaches to support our participants to become
attuned to the emotions of those they are interacting with – the emotional landscape of
the classroom. In the focus on right and wrong, instead of the process of learning and
investigating, the interaction became negative and the one point is achieving solely
the right answer.
In childhood, emotions are linked to experiences of attachment, happiness,
need, and anger. Sometimes a child's needs are not met, s/he doesn't feel bonds of
attachment or belonging to the community; then an awareness of helplessness may
become overwhelming. The remnants of negative childhood experiences may lead to
a re-emergence of feelings of despair and loss in adults. Not only are the students’
emotions inseparable from their learning experiences in science, but teachers’
emotions are also intertwined with successful learning in the classroom. We risk
neglecting that teachers go through an emotionally highly demanding process of
learning when changing their teaching practice in science. We might adopt Bronwyn
Davies' definition of intra-action, "where "intra-action" refers to more than interaction
(a meeting of two pre-existing entities); it is an encounter where each participant
affects, and is open to being affected by, the other (Barad 2007: Lenz Taguchi 2010)"
(Davies 2014).
Where is the joy? Where is the love?
What avail is it to win prescribed amounts of information about geography and
history, to win the ability to read and write, if in the process the individual
loses his own soul? (Dewey 1938, p. 49)
We have experienced countless times, as teachers and as researchers, the
performative, playful, and joyful ways in which children engage in science
investigations – in particular, when children are given the space to pursue their
wonderings and questions; individually or collectively, in classrooms or informal
settings, guided or free. The boundaries that are set by structures such as science
curriculum policy are porous, and a focus on enjoyment, and ideally on happiness, can
serve as an approach to transgress such boundaries. “To teach in a manner that
12!
respects and cares for the souls of our students is essential if we are to provide the
necessary conditions where learning can most deeply and intimately begin” (hooks, p.
13). Moments such as Lia and Martha’s discoveries and amazement as they explore
the movement of their bodies in space and time and their related sensory perception,
as well as Jan and Nina’s water filling and pouring, are filled with emotional
effervescence and enjoyment, which can support positive emotions within science-
related investigations. We hope that these positive emotions towards science leads to
further science explorations and we take inspiration from Jonathan Turner and Jan
Stets, who have written that “people pursue lines of conduct because they bring about
positive (emotional) outcomes, while avoiding those that lead to negative (emotional)
consequences” (2005, p. 22). It has been suggested that happiness ought to be a main
aim of education (Noddings 2003), and we believe that radical listening and love and
caring are necessary to work towards this aim in ways that facilitate children’s sense
of belonging to a community of learners and mediates their developing science
understandings. In our conceptualization of the value of caring and happiness in
education, we are reminded of Freire’s (1998) point that teaching is an act of love,
one that is requires a “well-thought-out capacity to love” (p. 3). His work draws on
such love as central for true dialogue. Love has been theorized as a “transformative
and revolutionary force” (Zembylas 2015, p. 32) yet caring is often perceived as being
soft – not rigorous or technical – and thus is often considered to only be perhaps for
teachers at the Kindergarten and pre-Kindergarten levels. Our work through the years
with teachers and their students as they engage in teaching and learning science has
reinforced our commitment to dialogic practices that are grounded on listening,
humility, and dialogue.
Radical listening, in which we try to put aside our own perspectives and open
ourselves up to understanding the perspectives of those that we work with, can
position us to imagine new possibilities for science education, as we are positioned to
experience children’s investigations and perspectives in different ways. “Working
with young children requires being able to begin to see them in all their possibilities
and potentials.” (Walsh 2005, p. 46). As we open ourselves up to radically listen to
what children say in science, we draw on Freire’s conceptualization of dialogue and a
foundation of radical love, which he explained, “love is at the same time the
foundation of dialogue and dialogue itself” (2000, pp. 89-90). Radical listening is a
humble approach to respecting those we work with, one that positions participants,
ourselves included, to work towards horizontal power relationships and to try to truly
understand perspectives that might be very different from their own perspectives or
expectations. As participants are positioned, and position themselves, to engage in
dialogic practices, there can emerge a space to understand each other; a space for
dialogue, reflection, and ideally for action. Enacting radical listening and embracing
radical love highlights for us the inseparability of emotions from the practices and
processes of teaching and learning science, an approach that supports our own critical
pedagogical practices and one that we hope provides spaces for children to question,
to learn, and to develop their sense of belonging to a classroom community. Once we
find the love in the classroom, the students can be situated to find the joy in science
learning and exploration and we hope that it positions them to engage with each other
and with the discipline in new ways.
13!
References
Arnold, R. (2010). Teaching heaven and earth: Beyond philosophy. In R. Lake [Ed.]
Dear Maxine: Letters from the unfinished conversation with Maxine Greene (pp. 40-
42). New York: Teachers College Press.
Barad, K. (2007). Meeting the universe halfway: Quantum physics and the
entanglement of matter and meaning. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.
Bateson, G. (1971). Steps to an ecology of mind. Chicago: The University of Chicago
Press.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
Bruner, J. (1986). Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Carr, M. (2001). Assessment in early childhood settings. Learning stories. London:
Sage.
Ceppi, G., Zinni, M. [eds.] (1998). Children, spaces, relations. Metaproject for an
environment for young children. Milano: Reggio Children.
Collins, R. (2005). Interaction ritual chains. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Davies, B. (2014). Listening to children. Being and becoming. London: Routledge.
Damasio, A. (1999). The feeling of what happens: Body and emotions in the making
of consciousness. New York: Harcourt Brace.
Dewey, J. (1910). Science as subject matter and method. Science, 31 (787) 121-128.
Dewey, J. (1938). Experience and education. Kappa Delta Pi Press.
Diamond, J. (2008). Welcome to the aquarium: A year in the lives of children. New
York: The New Press.
Eagleton, T. (1990). The significance of theory. Oxford: Basil Blackwood.
Freire, P. (2000). Pedagogy of the oppressed. New York: Bloomsbury.
Gallas, K. (1994). The languages of learning: How children talk, write, dance, draw,
and sing their understanding of the world. New York: Teachers College Press.
Holzkamp, K. (1995). Lernen. Subjektwissenschaftliche Grundlegung.
Frankfurt/Main: Campus.
Holzman, L. (2009). Vygotsky at work and at play. Oxon: Routledge.
14!
hooks, b. (1995). Teaching to transgress: Education as the practice of freedom. New
York: Routledge.
Jaber, L. Z. & Hammer, D. (2016). Engaging in science: A feeling for the discipline.
Journal of the Learning Sciences, 25 (2), 156-202. doi: 10.1080/10508406.
John-Steiner, V. (2000). Creative collaborations. New York: Oxford University Press
Lanas, M. & Zembylas, M. (2015). Towards a transformational political concept of
love in critical education. Studies in the Philosophy of Education, 34, 31-44.
doi: 10.1007/s11217-014-9424-5
Lentz Taguchi, H. (2010). Going beyond the theory/practice divide in early childhood
education: Introducing an intra-active pedagogy. London: Routledge/Falmer.
Liljeström, A., Enkenberg, J., Pöllänen, S. (2013). Making learning whole: An
instructional approach for mediating the practices of authentic science inquiries.
Cultural Studies of Science Education, 8, 51-86. doi: 10.1007/s11422-012-9416-0
Mahn, H. and John-Steiner, V. (2002). The gift of confidence: A Vygotskian view of
emotions. In G. Wells & G. Claxton [Eds.] Learning for life in the 21st century:
Sociocultural perspectives on the future of education, Blackwell Publishing Ltd,
Oxford, UK. doi: 10.1002/9780470753545.ch4
Mahn, H. (1997). Dialogue journals: Perspectives of second language learners in a
Vygotskian theoretical framework. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of
New Mexico, Albuquerque.
Otrel-Cass, K. (this issue). Towards a geography of emotional analysis. Cultural
Studies of Science Education. doi: 10.1007/s11422-015-9693-5
Piaget, J. (1950). Introduction à l'épistémologie génétique. (III) La pensée biologique.
La pensée psychologique. La pensée sociologique. Paris: Presses Univ. de France.
Piaget, J. (1954). Les relations entre l'intelligence et l'affectivité dans le
développement de l'enfant. Paris: Centre de documentation universitaire, 195 p.
(Cours à la Sorbonne 1953-1954)
Quigley, C. (2014). Expanding our view of authentic learning: Bridging in and out-of-
school experiences. Cultural Studies of Science Education, 9, 115-122. doi:
10.1007/s11422-013-9535-2
Radford, L. (2015). Of love, frustration, and mathematics: A Cultural-Historical
approach to emotions in mathematics teaching and learning. In B. Pepin, B. Roesken-
Winter [Eds.] From beliefs to dynamic affect systems in mathematics education, (pp.
25-49). Dordrecht: Springer.
Rogoff, B., Matusov, E. & White, C. (1998). Models of teaching and learning:
Participation in a community of learners. In D. R. Olson & N. Torrance [Eds.], The
handbook of education and human development (pp. 388-413). Oxford and Malden,
15!
MA: Blackwell.
Siry, C., Brendel, M., & Frisch, R. (in press). Radical listening and dialogue in
educational research. To be published in the International Journal of Critical
Pedagogy.
Siry, C. and Max. C. (2013). The collective construction of a science unit: Framing
curricula as emergent from Kindergarteners’ wonderings. Science Education, 97 (6),
878-902. doi: 10.1002/sce.21076
Stern, D. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant: A view from psychoanalysis
and developmental psychology. New York: Basic Books.
Tobin, K. & Kincheloe, J. (2010). Doing educational research: A handbook.
Rotterdam: Sense Publishing.
Turner, J. & Stets, J. (2005). The sociology of emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
van Maanen, J. (1988). Tales of the field: On writing ethnography. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Walsh, D.J. (2005). Developmental theory and early childhood education: necessary
but not sufficient. In N. Yelland [Ed.] Critical issues in early childhood education
(pp. 40-48). England: Open University Press.
Wolf, M. (1992) A thrice told tale: Feminism, postmodernism and ethnographic
responsibility. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Wolk, S. (2007). Why go to school? Phi Delta Kappan, 88(9), 648–658.
Zembylas, M. (2005). Emotions and science teaching: Present research and future
agendas. In S. Alsop [Ed.] Beyond Cartesian dualism: Encountering affect in the
teaching and learning of science (pp. 123-132). Dordrecht: Springer.
16!
Authors’ biographies
Christina Siry is an elementary science education researcher and teacher educator, and
she has worked in the US prior to relocating to Luxembourg, where she is currently a
professor of learning and instruction at the University of Luxembourg. Her research is
grounded in critical theoretical approaches and she draws on participatory and visual
methodologies in her work with teachers and children around the teaching and
learning of science.
Michelle Brendel is a trained in family therapist and Gestalt-therapist. She worked
with children in foster care before she joined the initial teacher training for preschool
and primary school at the University of Luxembourg, where her research interests are
collaborative teacher research, inclusive education, and portfolio approaches.
... Ultimately, learners' immersion in these diversified and multimodal EL experiences responds to learners diverse learning needs and promotes their cognitive, social, and affective learning (Larsen, 2017;Liu et al., 2024;Wesely, 2021). Recently, there is a growing interest in affective and emotional aspects of science teaching and learning (Hufnagel & Kelly, 2018;Siry & Brendel, 2016;Tytler & Ferguson, 2023), and since this field of science education is less studied, we offer below a general overview about this field of research. ...
... 168). Indeed, research on affect and emotions is growing with a strong realization of their importance for science learning and pedagogy (see, e.g., Bruckermann et al., 2021, Davidson et al., 2020Hufnagel & Kelly, 2018;Parong & Mayer, 2021;Siry & Brendel, 2016;Tytler & Ferguson, 2023;Zembylas, 2016). The notion of affect has been suggested to entail emotions, attitudes, motivations, and values; emotions though are distinguished from attitudes because they entail "aboutness," being experienced about an urgent and immediate matter (Hufnagel & Kelly, 2018). ...
... In science education, Zembylas (2021) has invariably argued that provoking affect and emotion is at the heart of pedagogy. Similarly, Siry and Brendel (2016) and Parce (2022) underscore the importance of developing a learning community while learning science, whereby teachers and learners collectively strive for positive and mutual feelings of belonging. ...
Article
Background: Youths’ declining interest and engagement in science due to traditional content-driven curricula and teaching methods remains a concern. Thus, innovative pedagogical approaches and curricula are needed, specifically at upper elementary and middle school, to counter learners’ disengagement and negative attitudes toward science. The purpose of this study was to explore the effect of an experiential learning (EL) curriculum on students’ ability to solve problems in new contexts and their attitudes and affective engagement with science. Methodology/Approach: A multimethod case study approach was used. Quantitative data included a science attitude survey and a feedback loop reasoning pre and posttest given to the regular and EL classrooms. Qualitative data included classroom observations and student focus group interviews. We conducted a thematic analysis to systematically analyze classroom observation data. Also, internal reliability for the attitude survey pretests data was established via Cronbach's alpha. Findings/Conclusion: The results revealed that the EL group's average on the attitude scale somewhat increased, whereas the average of the regular class decreased significantly. The problem-solving pre- and posttests revealed a sizable increase in learning progression levels of the EL group compared to the regular group. Class observations showed mostly manifestations of positive affective engagement in the EL class, with some challenges connected to a competitive culture. Implications: EL provides opportunities to enhance students’ solving problems skills and positive engagement and attitudes toward science.
... These strategies help students navigate the emotional challenges associated with learning complex scientific concepts and tasks, leading to more persistent and resilient learners. Some authors (Siry & Brendel, 2016) offer a more narrative-driven perspective on the importance of emotions in science education, illustrating through personal experiences and stories how positive emotional engagement can drive scientific inquiry and exploration. They argue for a holistic approach to science education that integrates emotional well-being with cognitive learning. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study investigates the impact of interactive science workshops participation on primary school children's emotions and attitudes towards science. The sample consisted of 186 students aged 9 to 13 from five different schools. The interactive science workshops consisted of a series of practical, active, and hands-on workshops where primary school students could learn about different aspects of science. These workshops, designed and conducted by prospective teachers from the Primary Education degree program, were non-competitive and did not involve participants in competitive activities. Using pre-and post-event questionnaires, changes in emotions and science-related attitudes were assessed. Results indicated a significant increase in positive emotions such as joy and enthusiasm, alongside a reduction in negative emotions like boredom and anxiety. Specifically, the post-test results showed a higher interest in discussing science, greater appreciation for experiments, and an increased desire for science-related careers. These changes were statistically significant, accentuating the transformative potential of interactive, hands-on learning experiences. The findings highlight the importance of integrating such activities into primary education curricula to enhance students' engagement and promote positive perceptions of science.
... The aesthetic and humanistic elements infused in the activities may have triggered students' emotional responses (Elliott, 2022). Emotions are inherent in science learning (Siry & Brendel, 2016;Wickman et al., 2022). This study attested to inducing affect such as enjoyment, boredom, anxiety and sense of hope in the process of science learning scrutinised in previous research (Raker et al., 2019;Pierson et al., 2023). ...
Article
Full-text available
Science education bears the broader objective of nurturing students today to be scientifically-literate citizens of tomorrow who are able to foresee challenges, invent solutions and make responsible decisions for global issues. As a prelude to the new focus of agency in the Anthropocene, this paper presents an intervention on climate change with upper secondary students in a museum of natural history in England. Instructional strategies such as infusing scenarios and arts into scientific discussions were adopted to induce imagination, future-oriented thinking and emotional responses. Statistical results showed that the intervention significantly enhanced participants’ futures literacy, environmental agency and positive emotions. However, it did not increase their interests in learning science in out-of-school context. Implications of this study will shed light on futurising science and climate education in research and practice.
... En los últimos veinticinco años, la investigación alrededor del dominio afectivo ha ido en aumento dentro de la didáctica de las ciencias experimentales. Esto responde a estudios empíricos que han demostrado que componentes tales como las actitudes y las emociones influyen fuertemente tanto en la enseñanza como en el aprendizaje de las ciencias (Watts y Alsop, 2000;Zembylas, 2002Zembylas, , 2004Zembylas, , 2005Brígido et al., 2009;Mellado et al., 2012;Cripps y Groves, 2012;Hugo et al., 2013;Olitsky, 2013;Bland y Morrison, 2015;King et al., 2015;Powietrzyńska y Gangji, 2016;Siry y Brendel, 2016;Rinchen et al., 2016;De la Blanca et al., 2017;Retana-Alvarado et al., 2017;Nichols et al., 2017;Sánchez et al., 2017;Del Rosal y Bermejo, 2018;Hufnagel, 2018;Hufnagel y Kelly, 2018;Retana-Alvarado et al., 2018;Talavera et al., 2018;Bravo et al., 2019;Hufnagel, 2019aHufnagel, , 2019bMarcos et al., 2019;Pipitone y García, 2020;Jimenez-Liso, et al., 2021;Del Rosal et al., 2022;Blumenkranc, et al., 2023;Membiela, et al., 2023, Pérez-Bueno et al., 2024. ...
Article
Full-text available
En la actualidad, uno de los temas que más interés concita en la agenda de investigación de didáctica de las ciencias experimentales son las emociones y su rol en el aprendizaje y la enseñanza. Dado lo anterior, se presenta aquí una revisión de la literatura al respecto. Nuestro estudio tiene una primera fase de identificación de publicaciones en bases de datos como Scopus, Dialnet y Redalyc y una segunda fase de selección con criterios teóricos, en la cual se recuperaron aquellos documentos que presentaban revisiones y metaanálisis frente a los avances investigativos en esta línea. Posteriormente los datos de estos documentos seleccionados fueron organizados, codificados y analizados con ayuda del software procesador de datos Atlas.ti24, obteniéndose como resultado el reconocimiento de unos “momentos clave” dentro de la investigación sobre las emociones en la didáctica de las ciencias. Iniciamos nuestro análisis con la identificación de algunas causas sobre la falta de investigación en la temática y llegamos al punto de hacer consideraciones para su auge en la última década y proyecciones para el futuro próximo.
... No wonder, as thinking programs serve as a source to provide individuals with a set of strategies, enriching the children's environment with knowledge and skills, and driving a passion for creative learning (Al-Salihin et al., 2020). An expert teacher allows children to deal with problems and the environment without interruption, as such interventions often hinder creative flows (Christina and Brendel, 2017). In this regard, Vivian M.Y. ...
Article
This study investigates environmental factors and their correlation with potential influences on students' creativity and the reinforcement of creative behavior along the trajectory of primary schools. This attempt falls within the research that confirms the positive relationship between an active learning environment and its experiences, its connection to students' abilities and interests, and their future success. It explores how its flexibility allows for more opportunities for learner creativity. This study aims to identify common change or the degree of change in variables by examining the relationship between the educational environment and creative learning. A coefficient analysis was conducted to explore the relationship between creative thinking skills scores of students in both urban and rural environments, revealing a moderate positive correlation reaching statistical significance levels ranging from 0.56 to 0.59. Statistical methods, including arithmetic mean calculations and independent T-Test analysis, were employed to analyze the data. The results shed light on the significance of the environment in facilitating creative learning, showing statistically significant differences attributed to the environmental variable in favor of urban areas. Considering these results, it is recommended to incorporate thinking skills in curricula to enhance the potential of the environment and enrich it with possible programs that increase opportunities for creative learning.
... No wonder, as thinking programs serve as a source to provide individuals with a set of strategies, enriching the children's environment with knowledge and skills, and driving a passion for creative learning (Al-Salihin et al., 2020). An expert teacher allows children to deal with problems and the environment without interruption, as such interventions often hinder creative flows (Christina and Brendel, 2017). In this regard, Vivian M.Y. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study investigates environmental factors and their correlation with potential influences on students' creativity and the reinforcement of creative behavior along the trajectory of primary schools. This attempt falls within the research that confirms the positive relationship between an active learning environment and its experiences, its connection to students' abilities and interests, and their future success. It explores how its flexibility allows for more opportunities for learner creativity. This study aims to identify common change or the degree of change in variables by examining the relationship between the educational environment and creative learning. A coefficient analysis was conducted to explore the relationship between creative thinking skills scores of students in both urban and rural environments, revealing a moderate positive correlation reaching statistical significance levels ranging from 0.56 to 0.59. Statistical methods, including arithmetic mean calculations and independent T-Test analysis, were employed to analyze the data. The results shed light on the significance of the environment in facilitating creative learning, showing statistically significant differences attributed to the environmental variable in favor of urban areas. Considering these results, it is recommended to incorporate thinking skills in curricula to enhance the potential of the environment and enrich it with possible programs that increase opportunities for creative learning.
... Aktiveringen av lärare och elever i dialog, snarare än synen på elever som passiva mottagare av NOkunskaper överförd av lärare och läromedel, främjar en ökad förståelse för omvärlden och hur man skapar kunskap och mening . Att involvera hela klassen i samkonstruktion av kunskap och förståelse kan också bidra med en känsla av gemensam ansträngning, vilket i sin tur kan framkalla de positiva känslor som kopplats ihop med framgångsrikt lärande i naturvetenskap (Siry & Brendel, 2016). ...
Thesis
Full-text available
This thesis is about the pedagogical considerations and decisions that upper primary school teachers (students age 10-12) engage with as they plan and conduct teaching in science class. It is also about the factors that have an impact on these considerations and decisions. Science education in upper primary school has been neglected in Sweden and internationally, in both research and development efforts. These years are important because children of upper primary school age are often interested in science, yet they are challenged by for example the presence of more abstract concepts in grade 4 which might negatively affect their science interest. Sociocultural perspectives, curriculum theory, and science didactics are the theoretical points of departure and have guided the planning and implementation of my research as well as the analytical process. By conducting three qualitative studies, consisting of interviews, classroom observations, document reviews, and a practice-based research and development project, I gathered useful and diverse empirical data to enable exploration of current upper primary school science education. 14 teachers were interviewed, an intrinsic case study of one grade six science class’ work on a whole topic (seven weeks) was conducted, and curriculum and planning documents from one teacher were gathered. In addition four science teachers from different grade levels (4-6 and 7-9) were invited and participated in pedagogical discussions to contribute knowledge about teachers’ collaboration on didactical questions related to continuity in science education. I used qualitative thematic and content analysis to analyze the empirical material from the three studies. The results show that upper primary school teachers place a lot of effort and time into planning and conducting a varied science instruction. They also express and show a great deal of care for and encouragement of their students in science class. In terms of factors that have an impact on the pedagogical considerations and decisions of teachers in upper primary school science, all three studies shared the main results. The following aspects were prominent in terms of the teachers’ views and actions: the view of science as a set of static and irrefutable facts, a high regard for the factual content of science curriculum, and varied views of the purpose of practical work in science education. In comparison with previous research, the findings presented in this thesis suggest that teaching practices in upper primary school are often in line with an academic tradition and a view of science as authoritative. Further, the finding that many of the participating teachers often refer to the extensive core content found in science curriculum and emphasize the pressure that this places on them, implies that the purpose of qualification is predominant in their view of the purpose of science education.
Article
Equity‐focused calls for elementary education reform recognize the importance of student and teacher translanguaging, yet nuances of how this process unfolds in early childhood science is an underexplored area. This study examines young plurilingual children's participation in science investigations, with a view toward understanding how open‐ended pedagogical structures supported their communication and engagement as related to science learning. We examine the work of 4‐ to 6‐year‐olds as they participated in a 3‐week unit exploring worms and draw upon translanguaging theoretical perspectives to interpretively analyze their interactions in science. Situated in the multilingual national context of Luxembourg, the study examines the interactions of these plurilingual children and their teacher as they investigated worms in varied open‐ended pedagogical structures. Schools are trilingual in Luxembourg, yet approximately half of the students in the country's elementary schools do not come to school with proficiency in any of the three languages of instruction. Issues of equity in schooling are thus heavily bound in languages. The robust dataset incorporating video data were examined using multimodal interaction analysis, and three vignettes zoom in on children's actions, utterances, and materials in open‐ended science learning spaces, providing rich examples of classroom structures that support meaningful translanguaging through students' agentic science communication. Young students' communication and science engagement are inseparable, and this study shows that these intertwine through translanguaging, in a process which is emergent when children are able to agentically draw upon diverse resources to make meanings.
Article
Creating, visualizing, and critiquing data are integral knowledge‐building practices within science, as well as many other fields. Yet data is often treated as neutral and value‐free, perpetuating narratives of science as a dispassionate discipline where data are merely extracted, repackaged, and distributed anew. As researchers and educators seek to imagine, design and support more humanistic accounts of young people's data practices, attuning to emotion will be a key area for further study. Recent scholarship in science education has elevated emotion as inseparable from disciplinary knowledge‐building practices, yet studies of emotion emergent within young people's data practices remain less understood. As part of the special issue Centering Affect and Emotion Toward Justice and Dignity in Science Education, this article explores how emotion and data are co‐produced as children construct and critique data visualizations together, collectively transforming notes, tallies and sketches into class‐level aggregated data displays within a fifth grade ecology curriculum. Analyzing interviews, classroom video recordings, and newly constructed data visualizations, I reveal how feeling, sensemaking and practice are entangled as children and their classmates build a paper data map and a dot plot graph, shaped by personal, cultural and sociopolitical dimensions. I show how these emergent emotional configurations shift activity from collaborative to competitive, in turn changing attention to the data itself (properties, histories, visualizations), the underlying organisms being studied and the ecological system‐level interactions. I argue that as science and data science education push forward a zeal for data, it will be important to attune to the emotional and relational dimensions that undergird children's emergent data practices.
Article
Full-text available
Emotions associated with prior mathematics learning experiences endure for some pre-service teachers, leaching into their own teaching of mathematics. Taking a sociology of emotions framing, the naturalistic study used event-oriented social inquiry and employed multiple methods (classroom transcripts, interviews, email conversations and reflective notes). In the event selected for this paper, we identified the importance of teacher educators intentionally attending to emotions in pre-service mathematics classes to address a dominant cognitive emphasis in learning to teach mathematics. We found that collective empathy was central to an emotional climate in which responses to individual and collective emotions were considered. We argue that collectively creating an emotionally safe environment in pre-service teacher education can help disrupt cycles of negative emotion associated with mathematics.
Chapter
Full-text available
Emotions have traditionally been characterized as inner, subjective, and physiological experiences, usually of an irrational nature. Against this subjectivist and physiological position, drawing on cultural psychology and anthropological research, in this article I advocate for a cultural conception of emotions and their role in thinking in general and mathematical thinking in particular. I argue that, rather than momentarily subjective phenomena, emotions (for instance, anger, frustration, love) are historically constituted. Emotions, I contend, are not opposed to thinking, but are an integral part of it. Emotions are as ubiquitous as breathing. I illustrate these ideas through the analysis of Grade 4 students working on a mathematical problem.
Chapter
The aim of this chapter is to distinguish theories of development that cast learning as a community process of transformation of participation in sociocultural activities from theories that cast learning as a one-sided process in which only teachers or learners are responsible for learning, either through transmission of knowledge from experts or acquisition of knowledge by learners by themselves. To distinguish these perspectives and highlight the theoretical stance of transformation of participation, we take a developmental approach by examining the transformation in understanding that occurs as adults who have been used to functioning in institutions employing transmission theories attempt to understand a new institution employing a participation theory.
Article
Preface PART 1: TWO NATURAL KINDS 1. Approaching the Literary 2. Two Modes of Thought 3. Possible Castles PART 2: LANGUAGE AND REALITY 4. The Transactional Self 5. The Inspiration of Vygotsky 6. Psychological Reality 7. Nelson Goodman's Worlds 8. Thought and Emotion PART 3: ACTING IN CONSTRUCTED WORLDS 9. The Language of Education 10. Developmental Theory as Culture Afterword Appendix: A Reader's Retelling of "Clay" by James Joyce Notes Credits Index
Book
When parents are asked what they want for their children, they usually answer that they want their children to be happy. Why, then, is happiness rarely mentioned as a goal of education? This book explores what we might teach if we were to take happiness seriously as a goal of education. It asks, first, what it means to be happy and, second, how we can help children to understand it. It notes that we have to develop a capacity for unhappiness and a willingness to alleviate the suffering of others to be truly happy. Criticizing our current almost exclusive emphasis on economic well-being and pleasure, Nel Noddings discusses the contributions of making a home, parenting, cherishing a place, the development of character, interpersonal growth, finding work that one loves, and participating in a democratic way of life. Finally, she explores ways in which to make schools and classrooms cheerful places. Nell Noddings is Lee L. Jacks Professor of Education, Emerita, at Stanford University. She is past president of the Philosophy of Education Society and of the John Dewey Society. In addition to twelve books, she is the author of more than 170 articles and chapters on various topics ranging from the ethics of care to mathematical problem solving. Her latest books are Starting at Home: Caring and Social Policy (University of California Press) and Educating Moral People: A Caring Alternative to Character Education (Teachers College Press), both published in 2002.
Book
Through a series of exquisite encounters with children, and through a lucid opening up of new aspects of poststructuralist theorizing, Bronwyn Davies opens up new ways of thinking about, and intra-acting with, children. This book carefully guides the reader through a wave of thought that turns the known into the unknown, and then slowly, carefully, makes new forms of thought comprehensible, opening, through all the senses, a deep understanding of our embeddedness in encounters with each other and with the material world. This book takes us into Reggio-Emilia-inspired Swedish preschools in Sweden, into the author’s own community in Australia, into poignant memories of childhood, and offers the reader insights into: new ways of thinking about children and their communities; the act of listening as emergent and alive; ourselves as mobile and multiple subjects; the importance of remaining open to the not-yet-known. Defining research as diffractive, and as experimental, Davies’ relationship to the teachers and pedagogues she worked with is one of co-experimentation. Her relationship with the children is one in which she explores the ways in which her own new thinking and being might emerge, even as old ways of thinking and being assert themselves and interfere with the unfolding of the new. She draws us into her ongoing experimentation, asking that we think hard, all the while delighting our senses with the poetry of her writing, and the stories of her encounters with children.
Article
This book offers rare and fascinating glimpses into the dynamic alliances from which some of our most important scholarly ideas, scientific theories, and art forms are born. It shows the creative process unfolding in the intimate relationships of Jean-Paul Sartre and Simone de Beauvoir, Henry Miller and Anais Nin, Marie and Pierre Curie, Martha Graham and Erick Hawkins, and Georgia O'Keeffe and Alfred Stieglitz; the productive partnerships of Pablo Picasso and Georges Braque, Albert Einstein and Marcel Grossmann, Aaron Copland and Leonard Bernstein, and Freeman Dyson and Richard Feynman; the familial collaborations of Thomas and Heinrich Mann, Hubert and Stuart Dreyfus, and Margaret Mead, Gregory Bateson and Mary Catherine Bateson; and the larger ensembles of The Guarneri String Quartet, Lee Strasburg, Harold Clurman and The Group Theater, and such feminist groups as The Stone Center and the authors of Women's Ways of Knowing. Many of these collaborators complemented each other, meshing different backgrounds and forms into fresh styles, while others completely transformed their fields. This book offers a unique cultural and historical perspective on the creative process and a compelling depiction of the associations that nurtured our most talented artists and thinkers. By delving into these complex collaborations, the book illustrates that the mind-rather than thriving on solitude-is clearly dependent upon the reflection, renewal, and trust inherent in sustained human relationships.