ArticlePDF Available

A BRIEF HISTORY OF METACOGNITION AND PRINCIPLES OF METACOGNITIVE INSTRUCTION IN LEARNING

Authors:
  • Iran Language Institute

Abstract

The very concept of metacognition has been the focal point of metacognitive instruction for many years. This paper provides a brief review of the history of metacognition and principles of metacognitive Instruction in learning. Two extensively used models of metacognition, namely Flavell's (1979) model of cognitive monitoring and A. L. Brown's (1987) model of metacognition have been presented in the subsequent sections of the paper.
editor.bestjournals@gmail.com www.bestjournals.in
A BRIEF HISTORY OF METACOGNITION AND PRINCIPLES OF METACOGNITIVE
INSTRUCTION IN LEARNING
MEHRDAD NAZARIEH
Department of English Language, Faculty of Foreign Languages, Kerman
Institute of Higher Education, Kerman, Iran
ABSTRACT
The very concept of metacognition has been the focal point of metacognitive instruction for many years. This
paper provides a brief review of the history of metacognition and principles of metacognitive Instruction in learning. Two
extensively used models of metacognition, namely Flavell’s (1979) model of cognitive monitoring and A. L. Brown’s
(1987) model of metacognition have been presented in the subsequent sections of the paper.
KEYWORDS:
Metacognition, Cognitive Monitoring, Metacognitive
INTRODUCTION
The very concept of metacognition has been the focal point of metacognitive instruction for many years. In the
field of educational psychology, metacognition has been simply defined as thinking about thinking; Devine (1993) stated
that metacognition is a form of cognition which includes active control over cognitive procedure.
The first researcher to introduce and use the term metacognition to the realm of educational and cognitive
psychology was Flavell (1976). He used the term to refer to an individual’s awareness of thinking and learning. He
provided the following explanation: “Metacognition refers to one’s knowledge concerning one’s own cognitive processes
and products or anything related to them, e.g. the learning-relevant properties of information or data” (p. 232). He also
added: “Metacognition refers, among other things, to active monitoring and consequent regulation and orchestration of
these processes in relation to the cognitive objects or data on which they bear, usually in the service of some concrete goal
or objective” (p. 232).
The two facets of metacognitive awareness are metacognitive experiences or regulations and metacognitive
knowledge. Flavell (1979) defined metacognitive experience as: “any conscious cognitive or affective experience that
accompanies [sic] and pertain to any intellectual enterprise” (p. 906). In fact, metacognitive experience mentions the
different adjustments a person makes to his/her cognitive processes to help manage and control his/her own learning. An
instance of this in L2 listening is when a student is encountering a task of word recognition and abruptly remembers a
similar problem that he or she was able to solve and handle in another listening event. As a result, the learner, using such a
metacognitive experience, applies a similar strategy to solve and handle the new word recognition challenge.
Metacognitive knowledge, on the other hand, consists of an individual’s beliefs and knowledge about themselves
as thinking beings and many different approaches that can be used by them for learning. In the example just mentioned, the
strategy choice of the learner depends on his or her previously existed metacognitive knowledge.
The term the seventh sense was used by Nisbet and Shuck smith (1986) to refer to metacognition. Metacognition,
BEST: Journal of Humanities, Arts,
Medicine and Sciences (BEST: JHAMS)
Vol. 2, Issue 2, Dec 2016, 61-64
© BEST Journals
62
Mehrdad Nazarieh
editor.bestjournals@gmail.com www.bestjournals.in
also, has been appeared to be one of the essential prognosticators of learning (Wang, Haertel, & Walberg, 1990), and the
benefits of metacognitive instruction have been irrefutable in areas such as listening, reading, and mathematics (Goh,
2008).
Regarding the fluctuating historical roots of metacognition in educational psychology, the study and the
standpoint of metacognition have remained enigmatic (Georghiades, 2004). As stated by A. L. Brown (1987),
“metacognition is not only a monster of obscure parentage, but a many-headed monster at that” (p. 105). Two extensively
used models of metacognition, namely Flavell’s (1979) model of cognitive monitoring and A. L. Brown’s (1987) model of
metacognition have been presented in the following sections.
FLAVELL’S MODEL OF COGNITIVE MONITORING
Flavell’s (1979) proposed a model of cognitive monitoring including four interactive sub-parts: metacognitive
knowledge, metacognitive experiences, goals, and strategies. According to Flavell (1979), metacognitive knowledge was
defined as “that segment of your stored knowledge that has to do with people as cognitive creatures and with their diverse
cognitive tasks, goals, actions, and experiences” (p. 906). In fact, this type of knowledge is the part of knowledge which
deals with individuals as cognitive beings and considers their different cognitive tasks, aims, behavior, and experiences as
well. It was later on when Wenden (1998), applied Flavell’s model of metacognitive knowledge to the realm of L2
learning. This model of metacognitive knowledge makes up three different categories: person knowledge, task knowledge,
and strategy knowledge.
Person knowledge included learners’ general knowledge about humans as thinking organisms. Person knowledge
comprised judgments about one’s learning abilities and knowledge about internal and external factors that influence the
success or failure in one’s learning process (Vandergrift et al., 2006).
Task knowledge referred to learners’ knowledge about the purpose, nature, and demands of learning tasks. It also
involved knowledge about the difficulty differences between two specified tasks. Task knowledge could also enable the
learners to consider factors that might be involved in difficulty of a task, together with the features of the oral message
(Vandergrift et al., 2006).
Finally, strategy knowledge referred to learners’ knowledge about using strategies to achieve cognitive goals.
According to Nisbet and Shuck smith (1986), strategy knowledge could be useful for achieving learning goals and aiding
the learners with having a choice in their strategy use and preference.
BROWN’S MODEL OF METACOGNITION
A. L. Brown (1987) proposed his model in which metacognition was composed of two dimensions: knowledge
about cognition and regulation of cognition. Knowledge about cognition referred to what individuals knew about their own
cognitive processes which facilitated the reflective aspects of metacognition (A. L. Brown, 1987). In additional studies,
knowledge about cognition was characterized into declarative knowledge, procedural knowledge, and conditional
knowledge (Jacobs & Paris, 1987). Declarative knowledge includes knowledge about oneself as a learner and about the
factors that influence one’s performance. Knowledge about self and strategies are other constituent parts of declarative
knowledge (Schraw & Moshman, 1995). For example, one might know that goal-setting is an effective strategy before
starting a learning task.
A Brief History of Metacognition and Principles of Metacognitive Instruction in Learning
63
editor.bestjournals@gmail.com www.bestjournals.in
Procedural knowledge denotes knowledge about the execution of procedural skills and how to use strategies.
Individuals with a higher degree of procedural knowledge use skills more automatically, are expected to structure strategies
effectively, and use qualitatively different strategies to resolve problems and difficulties (Schraw & Moshman, 1995).
Procedural knowledge, as mentioned, includes knowledge about how to use the strategies. For example, one might know
how to set goals before going through a specific task.
Conditional knowledge refers to knowing when and why to apply various cognitive actions. In a better word it
deals with utilizing declarative and procedural knowledge. It may be regarded as declarative knowledge about the relative
utility of cognitive procedures (Garner, 1990; Schraw & Moshman, 1995). For example, before a doing a specific task, one
might know that goal-setting would be much more appropriate. In A. L Brown’s (1987) model, knowledge about cognition
was characteristically stable, every so often imperfect, and frequently late developing. Conditional knowledge is important
since it aids learners in selectively allot their resources and use strategies more efficiently (Reynolds, 1992). Conditional
knowledge also allows learners to adjust themselves to the varying situational demands of a specific learning task.
Regulation of cognition referred to a set of activities that help the learners regulate and monitor learning, which
facilitate the control or executive aspect of learning (A. L. Brown, 1987). A quantity of studies report noteworthy
improvements in learning when regulatory skills and an understanding of how to use and apply these skills are contained
within classroom instruction (Cross & Paris, 1988; Brown & Palincsar, 1989). Regulation of cognition entailed three
metacognitive strategies: planning, monitoring, and evaluating strategies.
Planning included the selection of appropriate strategies and the allocation of appropriate resources that affect
performance. Instances include making predictions before doing a task, sequencing strategies, and allocating time or
thoughtfulness selectively before starting a specific task (Berieter & Scardamalia, 1987).
Monitoring referred to one’s on-line and regular awareness of comprehension and presentation of a task. For
instance being able to involve in self-testing periodically, while learning, is a good example. Studies also indicate that
monitoring as ability develops quite slowly and is quite poor in children and even adults (as cited in Schraw & Moshman,
1995).
Evaluating strategies referred to assessing the products and regulatory processes of an individual are learning.
They also referred to assessing the outcome of comprehension or the learning processes after accomplishing a task. Re-
evaluating one’s goals and conclusions after a specific task is a representative example for that.
According to A. L. Brown (1987), compared with features of knowledge about cognition, regulation of cognition
was considered to be unstable and also age independent. That is to say, adults might not use strategies when solving a
simple problem (unstable); young learners might not have the ability to monitor and regulate their strategies (age
independent). Regulatory processes—planning, monitoring, and evaluation—may not be conscious or statable in many
learning situations. One reason is that many of these processes are extremely automated, at least amongst adults. The
second reason is that some of these procedures have developed without any conscious reflection and consequently are
problematic to report to others (Brown, 1987).
64
Mehrdad Nazarieh
editor.bestjournals@gmail.com www.bestjournals.in
PRINCIPLES OF METACOGNITIVE INSTRUCTION
As stated by Veenman, Van Hout-Wolters, and Afflerbach (2006), while metacognitive development can be learnt
implicitly, it can be enhanced through explicit instruction. Veenman et al. (2006) identified three key principles for fruitful
metacognitive instruction. The first was to mix metacognitive instruction with the subject matter to foster connectivity. The
second was to notify the learners of the usefulness of metacognitive activities to have them make extra effort, and the third
was to provide the learners with extensive prolonged instruction to ensure the maintenance of the metacognitive activities.
REFERENCES
1. Brown, A. L. (1987). Metacognition, executive control, self-regulation, and other more mysterious mechanisms.
In F. E. Weinert& R. H. Kluwe (Eds.), Metacognition, motivation, and understanding (pp. 65-116). Hillsdale,
New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
2. Brown, A.L. & Palincsar, A.S. (1989). Guided, cooperative learning and individual knowledge acquisition. In
L.B. Resnick, ed., Knowing and Learning: Essays in Honor of Robert Glaser (pp. 393–451). Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
3. Brown, A.L. & Palincsar, A.S. (1989). Guided, cooperative learning and individual knowledge acquisition. In
L.B. Resnick, ed., Knowing and Learning: Essays in Honor of Robert Glaser (pp. 393–451). Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
4. Cross, D.R., & Paris, S.G. (1988). Developmental and instructional analyses of children’s metacognition and
reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology 80: 131– 142.
5. Devine, J. (1993). The role of metacognition in second language reading and writing. In J. G. Carson & I. Leki
(Eds.). Reading in the composition classroom: Second language perspectives (pp. 105-127). Boston: Heinle and
Heinle.
6. Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive development enquiry.
American Psychologist, 34, 906-911.
7. Nisbet, J., & Shucksmith, J. (1986). Learning strategies. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
8. Schraw, G., & Moshman, D. (1995). Metacognitive theories. Educational Psychology Review, 7(4), 351–371.
9. Vandergrift, L., Goh, C. C. M., Mareschal, C. J., & Tafaghodtari, M. H. (2006). The metacognitive awareness
listening questionnaire (MALQ): Development and validation, Language Learning, 56, 431-62.
10. Veenman, M. V. J., Van Hout-Wolters, B. H. A. M., & Afflerbach, P. (2006). Metacognition and learning:
conceptual and methodological considerations. Metacognition and Learning, 1, 3-14. doi: 10.1007/s11409-006-
6893-0.
... Flavell (1979) introduced two facets of metacognition as, "metacognitive knowledge (knowledge about our own and other people's cognitive processes) and metacognitive experiences (conscious cognitive or affective experiences that accompany current behaviour" (Weil et al., 2013). As stated by Nazarieh (2016), "the term the seventh sense was used by Nisbet and Smith (1986) to refer to metacognition". In addition to Flavell's classification, Brown (1987) proposed two dimensions of metacognition: knowledge about cognition and regulation of cognition. ...
... They also referred to assessing the outcome of comprehension or the learning processes after accomplishing a task. Re-evaluating one's goals and conclusions after a specific task is a representative example for that (Nazarieh, 2016). Table 1 that in the stage of planning, 49.2% of students are able to think and discuss the topics handled in the class outside the classroom in English, 41% of students are 'sometimes' conscious of their thought process, 42.6% of students are 'always' able to think effectively to gather ideas, 72.1% of them are 'always' able to brainstorm and draw mind map before writing, 37.7% of them are able to think about the topic in detail before writing, and 39.3% of them use feedback given for the previous task. ...
Article
Full-text available
Writing is a cardinal skill for effective communication practised extensively from primary education, but the students are not exhibiting adequate writing proficiency in their higher education and at their workplace. This experimental study focuses on enhancing the students' writing skills by promoting metacognitive strategies in the classroom. The participants of this study are 51 pre-final year Diploma students belonging to the Department of Instrumentation and Control Engineering of an autonomous polytechnic institute in Tamil Nadu. The teacher-researcher has facilitated students' cognizance with metacognitive strategies employed in the writing tasks administered during the course. The results have exhibited improvement apropos of coherence and unity in the students' writing skill. It implies the indispensable role of metacognitive strategies in developing the capacity of the learners' strategic thinking and guiding them to plan, progress, and process their writing into a coherent text.
... Metacognitive skills include self-regulation, self-assessment, monitoring and planning, self-assessment of the learning strategies used, and the effectiveness of the problem-solving process [10]. Metacognitive skills help students to gain knowledge and improve their intellectual abilities from their learning process and learning experiences [11]. The empowerment of metacognitive skills will improve students' ability to think about the results of elaborating their knowledge in learning [12] [13]. ...
Conference Paper
Metacognitive abilities and creative thinking skills are the essential skills for students in the online learning. The students who possess adequate metacognitive abilities and creative thinking skills will be able to stabilize or improve the learning outcomes in online learning. This study aims to investigate the students’ metacognitive abilities and creative thinking skills based on their gender. The study focuses on the students who are engaged in online learning during the pandemic of covid-19. The respondents of this study were 95 students (34 male, and 61 female students). This study employed a qualitative study. The instruments used were a metacognitive ability questionnaire and creative thinking questions. The results of the study revealed an insignificant difference between the score of metacognitive ability in male and female students. Each gender got an average score of 81.84 (male) and 77.23 (female) was in the high and enough metacognitive category. The creative thinking skills, on the other hand, presented an adequate difference between male and female students’ scores. Each gender (male and female) got an average score of 39.01 (low creative thinking category) and 58.70 (moderate creative thinking category). This indicates the students’ metacognitive abilities should be maximized and the students’ creative thinking skills in online learning should also be improved.
... Competencies and skills have to be possessed by students in the 21 st century human resources are by improving the quality of learning, including raising the teaching system and improving the quality of teacher abilities (Perdana et al., 2020). Many things did achieve these goals, such as creating an active, innovative, creative, and fun learning atmosphere for students for they passionate and fully developed during the learning process (Murnawianto et al., 2019;Nazarieh, 2016;Santi et al., 2019). This COVID-19 pandemic situation requires the creativity and innovation of teachers in designing learning services (Ahied et al., 2020;Juanda et al., 2021;Nursetiawati et al., 2020;Yustina et al., 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
Concept mastery is the based ability of higher-order thinking skills in the era of society 5.0. Increased understanding of the concept is in line with the practice of dependent on active learning activities by students. The purpose of the study was to determine the effect of problem-based learning on the concept mastery and learning activities of students. The research method used is a quasi-experimental research design with a non-equivalent pretest-posttest control group. The sample had selected through purposive random sampling. The instrument used is a conceptual understanding test instrument with an indicator of adaptation knowledge dimensions from Bloom's taxonomy and a learning activity instrument of a questionnaire. In the results of this study, there are differences between the experimental class and the control class on understanding concepts and learning activities of students; the PBL model increase students' concept mastery
... Evaluation: Evaluation according to Mehrdad (2016) is referred to assessing the outcome of comprehension or the learning processes after accomplishing a task. It is the explanation of progress being made towards goals which could result to further planning, monitoring and execution. ...
... Since metacognition is multi-faceted, "multidimensional and domain-general in nature" (Teng et al., 2021a, p. 169), a consistent debate has been existing around the definition and components of the concept during its evolution (e.g., Zhang et al., 2021b). Regardless, it is acknowledged that the foundational research on metacognition takes root in two frameworks proposed by Flavell (1979) and Brown (1987) (Sperling et al., 2012;Nazarieh, 2016), in which metacognition is agreed to be comprised of metacognitive knowledge encompassing person/declarative knowledge, task/procedure knowledge and strategy/conditional knowledge, and metacognitive regulation or metacognitive strategies composed of planning (planning individual's learning activities in accordance with their learning objectives prior to L2 learning), monitoring (online monitoring in the individuals' learning process) and evaluating (post-learning evaluating of the learning process) with the three components working independently and interactively (refer to Teng et al., 2021b;Zhang, 2021, for a review). In the most updated model of metacognition, though focusing on writing, established by Teng et al. (2021a), metacognitive strategies are also proposed to comprise three key components: planning, monitoring, and evaluating. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study investigated the concept of individual differences (IDs) in the use of metacognitive strategies (planning, problem-solving, monitoring, and evaluating) and its relationship with task demand and learner performance within Kormos’ Bilingual Speech Production Model from the lens of Chinese English-as-foreign-language (EFL) learners in the context of integrated L2 speaking assessment. To measure metacognitive strategies, we administered an inventory on 134 Chinese EFL learners after they completed four integrated L2 speaking assessment tasks. Descriptive analysis and multiple linear regression were adopted for data analysis, and results show that: (a) IDs displayed variance in Chinese EFL learners’ metacognitive strategy use; (b) among the four metacognitive strategies under investigation, problem-solving was reported to be used the most frequently in sharp contrast to monitoring, which had the lowest frequency; (c) metacognitive strategies worked interactively, responding to task demands involved in the four integrated L2 speaking assessment tasks; and (d) Chinese EFL learners’ use of metacognitive strategies, in individual and interactive working modes, had no relationship with their speaking performance. These results are expected to present some insights into the role of IDs in metacognitive strategy use during L2 speech production under assessment conditions, which will add robust evidence to the existing literature on L2 speaking, in particular on metacognitive strategy use in L2 speaking assessment. In the meantime, the findings will provide some empirical validation support for Kormos’ model, which will further provide some implications for L2 speaking instruction and L2 assessment.
... According to Flavell (1976), metacognition refers to an individual's awareness in their thinking process. Nazarieh (2016) mentioned that metacognition is an active process involved in learning. Even though the students are from different cultures or different backgrounds, there exists similarities between them, which is that their thinking is developed through the learning process. ...
Article
Full-text available
Metacognition in science teaching involves processes that include self-awareness and self-regulation. Metacognition enables the teachers to facilitate student learning and to reflect on their teaching in order to enable themselves to improve or to make any changes to their teaching. In particular, teaching activities, especially in the 21st century, do not merely involve the transfer of knowledge and then applying that knowledge into daily life, but teachers need to reflect, plan and evaluate learning outcomes to enhance further in teaching. This study attempts to gain the perspective and implementation of metacognition skills in teaching science in the primary school classroom. The data was collected through a qualitative research method based on interviews with six science teachers in primary school using semi-structured interview protocol. The interview data were analysed for emerging themes, guided by the research questions. Teachers have a similar perspective of the understanding of metacognition in science teaching. Further discussion focuses on the implementation of metacognition in science teaching. This discussion is divided into three aspects, which are constraints faced, overcoming the constraints, and efforts made to implement metacognition in science teaching. Hence, the understanding of science teachers in regards to metacognition in science teaching is important and gives a positive impact towards teaching and learning in primary science teaching.
... According to Flavell (1976), metacognition refers to an individual's awareness in their thinking process. Nazarieh (2016) mentioned that metacognition is an active process involved in learning. Even though the students are from different cultures or different backgrounds, there exists similarities between them, which is that their thinking is developed through the learning process. ...
Article
Metacognition in science teaching involves processes that include self-awareness and self-regulation. Metacognition enables the teachers to facilitate student learning and to reflect on their teaching in order to enable themselves to improve or to make any changes to their teaching. In particular, teaching activities, especially in the 21st century, do not merely involve the transfer of knowledge and then applying that knowledge into daily life, but teachers need to reflect, plan and evaluate learning outcomes to enhance further in teaching. This study attempts to gain the perspective and implementation of metacognition skills in teaching science in the primary school classroom. The data was collected through a qualitative research method based on interviews with six science teachers in primary school using semi-structured interview protocol. The interview data were analysed for emerging themes, guided by the research questions. Teachers have a similar perspective of the understanding of metacognition in science teaching. Further discussion focuses on the implementation of metacognition in science teaching. This discussion is divided into three aspects, which are constraints faced, overcoming the constraints, and efforts made to implement metacognition in science teaching. Hence, the understanding of science teachers in regards to metacognition in science teaching is important and gives a positive impact towards teaching and learning in primary science teaching.
... Metacognition can be described as an executive control process it literally decides what thought processes need to be used, when they should be used, as well as why and how they should be used. It refers to the brain's ability to monitor, manage and organize our own mental activities [5]. This theory is designed to teach learners how to reflect and process information so they have the skills to learn effectively for themselves. ...
Article
Full-text available
E-learning is an established and effective way to educate learners while tracing their contribution and assessment all at a trivial fraction of the cost of traditional training. E-learning platforms are becoming more widespread for educational organizations and their professional development objectives. Suitable instructional design combined with the use of online tools can endorse learners’ interaction in e-learning platforms. The present research aimed to analyse and explores the interaction between learners and tools in e-learning platforms, so as to enhance their learning capabilities. In order to understand the learners’ interaction Generative Knowledge Interviewing (GKI) technique is used. GKI is a facilitated process that help learners to identify, connect, synthesize and determine knowledge and skills they are acquainting from all areas of life. GKI is a process that empowers learners to realize their skills and strengths in their roles as peer writing tutors. GKI is based on the concept of knowledge management and the resolution of knowledge management is to confirm that the right information is delivered to the suitable place or capable person at the right time to empower informed decisions.
Book
Full-text available
This book explores innovative pedagogical practices and teaching and learning strategies in the engineering curriculum for empowered learning. It highlights the urgency for developing specific skill sets among students that meet the current market recruitment needs. The authors present a detailed framework for fostering a higher level of competence in students especially in their communication skills, their knowledge of media and technology tools, and their leadership skills. The book offers examples of new and effective teaching strategies including cognitive, metacognitive, and socio-affective strategies which align well with the existing and evolving technical curriculum. The book will be of interest to teachers, students, and researchers of education, engineering, and higher education. It will also be useful for English language teachers, educators, and curriculum developers.
Article
Full-text available
This paper proposes a framework for understanding people's theories about their own cognition. Metacognitive theories are defined broadly as systematic frameworks used to explain and direct cognition, metacognitive knowledge, and regulatory skills. We distinguish tacit, informal, and formal metacognitive theories and discuss critical differences among them using criteria borrowed from the developmental literature. We also consider the origin and development of these theories, as well as implications for educational research and practice.
Book
Originally published in 1986, designed for teachers and those concerned with the education of primary and secondary school pupils, Learning Strategies presented a new approach to 'learning to learn'. Its aim was to encourage teachers to start thinking about different approaches to harnessing the potential of young learners. It was also relevant to adult learners, and to those who teach them. Thus, although about learning, the book is also very much about teaching. Learning Strategies presents a critical view of the study skills courses offered in schools at the time, and assesses in non-technical language what contributions could be made to the learning debate by recent developments in cognitive psychology. The traditional curriculum concentrated on 'information' and developing skills in reading, writing, mathematics and specialist subjects, while the more general strategies of how to learn, to solve problems, and to select appropriate methods of working, were too often neglected. Learning to learn involves strategies like planning ahead, monitoring one's performance, checking and self-testing. Strategies like these are taught in schools, but children do not learn to apply them beyond specific applications in narrowly defined tasks. The book examines the broader notion of learning strategies, and the means by which we can control and regulate our use of skills in learning. It also shows how these ideas can be translated into classroom practice. The final chapter reviews the place of learning strategies in the curriculum.
Article
Children's understanding of their own cognitive skills, or metacognition, has been hypothesized to play a major role in learning and development. In this study, we examine the developing relation between children's metacognition and reading comprehension. Children in third- and fifth-grade classes were given an experimental curriculum, Informed Strategies for Learning (ISL), designed to increase their awareness and use of effective reading strategies. In both grades, children in experimental classes made significant gains in metacognition and the use of reading strategies compared with children in control classes. The multivariate profiles of reading skills derived from the developmental analyses helped to identify subgroups of children who responded differently to the metacognitive instruction. Although there were specific aptitude-by-treatment interactions, there was a general trend for metacognition and strategic reading to become more congruent from 8 to 10 years of age.
Article
Studies suggest that young children are quite limited in their knowledge about cognitive phenomena—or in their metacognition—and do relatively little monitoring of their own memory, comprehension, and other cognitive enterprises. Metacognitive knowledge is one's stored knowledge or beliefs about oneself and others as cognitive agents, about tasks, about actions or strategies, and about how all these interact to affect the outcomes of any sort of intellectual enterprise. Metacognitive experiences are conscious cognitive or affective experiences that occur during the enterprise and concern any aspect of it—often, how well it is going. Research is needed to describe and explain spontaneous developmental acquisitions in this area and find effective ways of teaching metacognitive knowledge and cognitive monitoring skills. (9 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
This article describes the development and validation of a listening questionnaire designed to assess second language (L2) listeners' metacognitive awareness and perceived use of strategies while listening to oral texts. The process of instrument development and validation is described, along with a review of the relevant literature related to metacognition and learners' regulation of listening comprehension strategies. An exploratory factor analysis of the responses of a large sample (N= 966) of language learners and a subsequent confirmatory factor analysis with another large but different sample (N= 512) resulted in a 21-item instrument with robust psychometric properties. Five distinct factors emerged: problem-solving, planning and evaluation, mental translation, person knowledge, and directed attention. The reliability and factorial validity of the instrument are presented along with evidence for a statistically significant relationship between student response on the instrument and L2 listening comprehension success. The article concludes with a discussion of the potential uses of the Metacognitive Awareness Listening Questionnaire (MALQ).