ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Pathological buying is characterized by irrepressible buying behaviour and its negative consequences. A possible mechanism contributing to its development and maintenance is that buying episodes act as a maladaptive strategy to cope with negative emotions. Accordingly, pathological buying has been repeatedly associated with impulsivity, in particular with the tendency to experience strong reactions under negative affect. Relying on an experimental mood induction procedure, the present study tested in a sample of 100 individuals (a) whether individuals with pathological buying symptoms respond more impulsively in the Go/No-Go Task (as a measure of the behavioural inhibition aspect of impulsivity) and (b) whether this association is more pronounced in a negative mood. While controlling for comorbidities, the results show that pathological buying is associated with faster responses and a larger number of commission errors. Moreover, a significant interaction indicated that the association between pathological buying and performance the Go/No-Go Task was stronger in the negative mood condition. The present study thus shows that that pathological buying is associated with deficits in the behavioural inhibition component of impulsivity. These deficits are most pronounced when mood is negative; in turn, this provides an explanation for the occurrence of excessive buying episodes following negative affect.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Effects of mood state on impulsivity in pathological buying
Jennifer Nicolai
a,
n
, Stefaniá Darancó
b
, Morten Moshagen
c
a
Cognition and Individual Differences, University of Mannheim, Schloss, EO 254, 68133 Mannheim, Germany
b
University of Glasgow, UK
c
University of Kassel, Germany
article info
Article history:
Received 14 December 2015
Received in revised form
25 July 2016
Accepted 4 August 2016
Available online 4 August 2016
Keywords:
Pathological buying
Compulsive buying
Addictive buying
Mood
Impulsivity
abstract
Pathological buying is characterized by irrepressible buying behaviour and its negative consequences. A
possible mechanism contributing to its development and maintenance is that buying episodes act as a
maladaptive strategy to cope with negative emotions. Accordingly, pathological buying has been re-
peatedly associated with impulsivity, in particular with the tendency to experience strong reactions
under negative affect. Relying on an experimental mood induction procedure, the present study tested in
a sample of 100 individuals (a) whether individuals with pathological buying symptoms respond more
impulsively in the Go/No-Go Task (as a measure of the behavioural inhibition aspect of impulsivity) and
(b) whether this association is more pronounced in a negative mood. While controlling for comorbidities,
the results show that pathological buying is associated with faster responses and a larger number of
commission errors. Moreover, a signicant interaction indicated that the association between patholo-
gical buying and performance the Go/No-Go Task was stronger in the negative mood condition. The
present study thus shows that pathological buying is associated with decits in the behavioural in-
hibition component of impulsivity. These decits are most pronounced when mood is negative; in turn,
this provides an explanation for the occurrence of excessive buying episodes following negative affect.
&2016 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.
1. Introduction
Pathological buying is characterized by irrepressible and un-
controllable buying behaviour and its associated negative con-
sequences (Dittmar, 2005). More specically, it can be dened as:
(1) a recurrent preoccupation with buying and/or irresistible urges
to buy; (2) repetitive buying habits that are difcult to control
(with frequent buying of more than can be afforded, frequent
buying of items that are not needed, or shopping for longer peri-
ods of time than intended associated with an unavoidable urge to
buy); (3) a cause of negative consequences leading to personal
distress and signicant social and/or nancial problems (Dittmar,
2004;McElroy et al., 1994;Racine et al., 2014). According to a
recent meta-analysis, the prevalence of pathological buying ap-
proximates 5% among adults in the general population (Maraz
et al., 2016). Pathological buying demonstrates high comorbidities
with other Axis I disorders, including mood, anxiety, and sub-
stance use disorders (de Zwaan, 2011;Müller et al., 2010). The
issue of how to classify pathological buying is still under debate
(Grüsser et al., 2007;Hollander and Allen, 2006;Racine et al.,
2014). Propositions in this regard include classifying pathological
buying as an obsessivecompulsive disorder (Hollander et al.,
2011;Hollander and Allen, 2006), mood disorder (Lejoyeux et al.,
1997), behavioural addiction (Lawrence et al., 2014), or impulse
control disorder (Müller et al., 2015a).
Pathological buyers experience a strong and repetitive urge to
purchase goods, associated with growing tensions that can only be
relieved by the act of buying (Christenson et al., 1994;Faber and
Christenson, 1996;McElroy et al., 1994). A number of environ-
mental stimuli are known to trigger buying episodes (such as
colours, sounds, and smells) (Kellett and Bolton, 2009) and it
seems likely that pathological buyers have difculties to resist
those cues (Trotzke et al., 2014). Correspondingly, evidence relying
on self-report measures suggests that pathological buying is as-
sociated with higher trait-impulsivity, in particular with the ten-
dency to experience strong reactions, frequently under conditions
of negative affect (Black et al., 2012;Lejoyeux et al., 1997;Rose and
Segrist, 2014;Vogt et al., 2015;Williams and Grisham, 2012; but
see Müller et al. (2014)). This, in turn, leads to difculties to de-
liberately suppress automatized responses (Billieux et al., 2008;
Williams and Grisham, 2011).
Indeed, the experience of negative affect or negative emotions
is often reported as internal antecedent of pathological buying
(Faber and Christenson, 1996;Miltenberger et al., 2003) and in-
dividuals suffering from pathological buying frequently believe
that purchasing items acts as a deterrent for negative mood (Faber
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/psychres
Psychiatry Research
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2016.08.009
0165-1781/&2016 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.
n
Corresponding author.
E-mail address: nicolai@uni-mannheim.de (J. Nicolai).
Psychiatry Research 244 (2016) 351356
and Christenson, 1996;Kyrios et al., 2013). Studies relying on real-
time assessments have shown that pathological buyers suffer an
increased negative affect prior to a compulsive buying episode,
which is alleviated later on (Miltenberger et al., 2003;Müller et al.,
2012). This experience of negative reinforcement may contribute
to the use of buying in order to cope with negative affect (Faber
and Christenson, 1996). Therefore, individuals with pathological
buying disorder may engage in excessive buying as a maladaptive
self-regulatory mechanism to reduce negative feelings, which is
particularly the case for those who exhibit decits in emotion
regulation (Williams and Grisham, 2011). According to this mood-
repair hypothesis it can be expected that negative affect leads to
the goal of mood repair which is realized in purchasing behaviour;
consequently, purchasing behaviour functions as a mean to repair
negative mood (Kellett and Bolton, 2009;Williams, 2012). How-
ever, studies that experimentally induced negative affect using a
mood induction procedure failed to support this contention (Vogt
et al., 2014;Williams, 2012), as mood state did not alter the be-
haviour of individuals with pathological buying disorder in com-
puterized buying or shopping related tasks. However, the applied
tasks differed in various important aspects from natural buying
situations (Williams, 2012).
Because of the difculties to establish a controlled experi-
mental environment that maintains the relevant ecological cues
and rewards in natural buying situations, a more viable approach
may lie in investigating the effect of mood state on specic cog-
nitive processes that are presumably involved in acute buying
episodes. In this context, state-impulsivity and inhibition-related
processes may be of particular importance. It is generally assumed
that (lack of) impulse control can be evident at different steps
during goal selection and goal pursuit, with each step in the se-
lection-action cycle being associated with different cognitive
processes (Cyders and Coskunpinar, 2011;Stahl et al., 2014). The
present study focuses on the behavioural inhibition component of
state-impulsivity (Stahl et al., 2014; called prepotent response
inhibition by Cyders and Coskunpinar (2011)) which refers to the
ability to withhold or stop an already initiated or ongoing re-
sponse. Consequently, the present study evaluated the effect of
mood state using a behavioural task (the Go/No-Go Task) to assess
the behavioural inhibition component of state-impulsivity. In line
with the considerations above, it was expected that (1) pathologi-
cal buying is generally associated with poorer performance on the
Go/No-Go Task and that (2) performance deteriorates most when
experiencing negative mood.
2. Methods
The study has been approved by the ethics committee of the
Psychology Department at the University of Mannheim. All parti-
cipants provided written informed consent by ticking a checkbox
linked to a respective statement.
2.1. Participants
An a-priori power analysis indicated that a sample of size
N¼95 is required to detect a moderate interaction effect (corre-
sponding to a standardized regression coefcient of .25) with a
power of 80% given an alpha error of 5%. Participants were re-
cruited via postings on social networks and by asking members of
self-help groups pertaining to pathological buying to participate.
The study was announced as investigation on buying and cogni-
tion. This led to a convenience sample of 100 (70 female, 28 male,
2prefer not to say) individuals. Mean age was 27.83 years
(SD¼12.2; range 1868). Most of the participants (90%) reported
native or uent English language skills. The majority of
participants were students (59%) and another 31% of participants
were currently employed or self-employed. Participants com-
pleted the study on an anonymous and voluntary basis without
any compensation.
2.2. Measures
The primary outcome of the present study was the perfor-
mance in the Go/No-Go Task. Questionnaire measures used in-
cluded a scale assessing pathological buying and a set of poten-
tially relevant clinical background variables. The latter measures
were included to allow for the investigation of whether the results
pertaining to pathological buying change when controlling for
comorbid disorders. In addition, background measures included a
scale assessing symptoms of obsessivecompulsive disorder (OCD)
to determine whether individuals with OCD symptoms behave
similar to those with symptoms of pathological buying, thereby
contributing evidence toward the classication of pathological
buying.
Go/No-Go Task. In each trial, participants were shown a con-
sonant (randomly selected from 21 consonants) in the centre of
the screen for a maximum of 1500 ms. The task was to press the
space key as fast as possible as soon as a letter appeared on screen
(go trials), except for an X, to which any response should be
withheld (no-go trials). In case of an incorrect response, a red cross
briey appeared. Letters were presented in white against a black
background. Each trial was preceded by a xation cross that was
shown for 200 ms. Stimulus onset time was determined at random
to be either 600, 700, 800, or 900 ms. A training block of 10 trials
was followed by a test block comprising 100 trials (70 go and 30
no-go trials). The Go/No-Go Task elicits two types of responses, the
number of commission errors (failure to inhibit a response) in the
no-go trials and the response times for correct responses in the go-
trials. A higher level of impulsivity is evident in a larger number of
commission errors and/or shorter reaction times.
2.2.1. Pathological Buying Screener (PBS)
The PBS (Müller et al., 2015b) is a 13-item screening instrument
that measures pathological buying with the two factors loss of
control/consequences and excessive buying behaviour. The reliability
of the total score as well as of the two subscales proved to be good.
Evidence of incremental validity and convergent validity of the PBS
using the Compulsive Buying Scale (Faber and OGuinn, 1992) has
been provided.
2.2.2. Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS)
Mood was assessed by the PANAS (Krohne et al., 1996;Watson
et al., 1988) before and after the mood induction procedure. The
PANAS consists of two 10-item scales that measure Positive Affect
(PA) and Negative Affect (NA). Respondents were asked to rate how
they felt at the present moment on a 5-point Likert scale ranging
from 1 (very slightly or not at all) to 5 (extremely). The PANAS
shows good psychometric properties (Watson et al., 1988).
2.2.3. Mood Disorder Questionnaire (MDQ)
The MDQ (Hirschfeld et al., 2003,2000) is a 13-item self-report
screening instrument for bipolar spectrum disorders that is di-
vided into three parts: Part 1 asks in a dichotomous response (yes/
no) format about lifetime (hypo-)maniac symptoms (hyperactivity,
irritability, sleeping behaviour, concentration, activity levels, and
risky behaviour), Part 2 is about symptom co-occurrence, and Part
3 asks about symptom severity on a four point scale (no problem,
minor problem, moderate problem, serious problem). The sensi-
tivity and specicity have been estimated in a psychiatric popu-
lation (Hirschfeld et al., 2000) and in the general population
(Hirschfeld et al., 2003). In the present study, we used the sum
J. Nicolai et al. / Psychiatry Research 244 (2016) 351356352
score across all items to measure the severity of mood disorder
symptoms.
2.2.4. Modied Beck Depression Inventory (M-BDI)
A simplied 20 item-version (Schmitt et al., 2003;Schmitt and
Maes, 2000) of the BDI (Beck and Steer,1987) was used. The M-BDI
asks respondents to indicate the frequency of experiencing
symptoms of depression on a 6-point Likert scale ranging from 0
(never) to 5 (almost always), with higher scores indicating higher
levels of depression. The reliability of the M-BDI proved to be
good. Its construct validity and measurement equivalence as
compared to the original BDI have been demonstrated (Schmitt
et al., 2003).
2.2.5. Borderline Symptom List 23 (BSL-23)
The BSL (Bohus et al., 2009) is a 23-item self-report instrument
that measures borderline symptom severity on a ve-point Likert
scale ranging from 0 (not at all) to 4 (very strong). The psycho-
metric properties of the BSL-23 were assessed in ve different
borderline personality disorder (BPD) patient samples. The BSL has
good internal consistency. The BSL-23 has been shown to ade-
quately discriminate BPD patients from patients with an axis I
diagnosis.
2.2.6. Obsessive Compulsive Inventory (OCI-R)
The OCI-R (Foa et al., 2002) is an 18-item self-report measure
designed to assess symptoms of obsessivecompulsive disorder
(OCD) across six subscales: washing, checking, ordering, obsessing,
hoarding, and neutralizing. Respondents assess the degree to
which they are distressed by OCD symptoms in the past month on
a 5-point scale from 0 (not at all) to 4 (extremely). The OCI-R has
demonstrated excellent psychometric properties in a sample of
patients with OCD, other anxiety disorders, and in non-anxious
individuals (Foa et al., 2002). It has been shown that it differ-
entiates well between people with and without OCD. Foa and
colleagues also found good internal consistency, adequate test
retest reliability, and evidence of convergent and discriminant
validity.
2.3. Procedures
The study was conducted (in the English language) via the In-
ternet, closely adhering to standards of web-based experimenting
(Reips, 2002). After receiving general information about the study
and providing informed consent, participants completed the PA-
NAS (baseline measurement). Next, participants were randomly
assigned to either a positive mood or a negative mood condition.
Mood induction proceeded by using a subset of the Velten State-
ments (Velten, 1968), accompanied by either cheerful or sad in-
strumental music. Participants were requested to turn on the
audio and were provided the opportunity to check whether the
audio was working. The Velten technique presents a series of self-
referent mood statements such as I feel enthusiastic and con-
dent now,Im pleased that most people are so friendly to me,
and I have a sense of power and vigour(positive mood condition)
or I feel rather sluggish now,People annoy me; I wish I could be
by myself, and Just to stand up would take a big effort(negative
mood condition). Participants were instructed to read each of the
statements carefully and to try to be open to their meaning. The
effectiveness of the Velten technique in an online context has been
demonstrated (Göritz, 2007), as has the combination of the Velten
technique with music (Westermann et al., 1996). Either 30 positive
or 30 negative Velten statements were presented consecutively for
510 s each (with an average of 8.2 s), depending on the length of
the respective statement. In total, the mood induction phase took
approximately 5 min. Immediately after the mood induction,
participants completed the PANAS again (post-mood induction)
and took the Go/No-Go Task. Thereafter, the questionnaire mea-
sures were presented in randomized order. Finally, demographic
background data were collected, participants were debriefed, and
the study closed with a mood-neutralization phase (for the par-
ticipants in the negative mood condition). The study took ap-
proximately 20 min to complete.
2.4. Statistical analyses
To evaluate our hypotheses multiple regression analyses with
the PBS score (as measure of pathological buying), mood condition
(coded "1 for negative and 1 for positive mood), the PBS #mood
interaction, and the remaining measures (as covariates) were
performed. We followed the recommended approach in linear
regression analysis involving main effects and interactions to in-
clude all relevant predictors simultaneously in a single model,
since testing the interaction term requires the presence of the
main effects (e.g., Cohen et al., 2003). The dependent variable was
behavioural impulsivity as measured through the performance in
the Go/No-Go Task. We considered both, the number of commis-
sion errors in the no-go trials and the mean response times in the
go-trials. All variables (except for the mood condition) were
z-standardized prior to the analyses, in order to obtain centred
predictors and standardized regression coefcients that can be
readily compared across predictors that are measured on different
scales. All statistical analyses were performed in R (version 3.0.2; R
Core Team, 2013).
3. Results
Detailed descriptive results are presented in Table 1. According
to the tentative guidelines suggested by Müller et al. (2015b) 36%
and 13% of the sample, respectively, would be diagnosed as pa-
thological buyers using cut-off scores of 2.23 (2 SD above the mean
in a general population sample) and 3.00 (on average, each pro-
blematic behaviour occurs at least sometimes), respectively. All
questionnaire measures exhibited good internal consistencies. On
a bivariate level, the PBS showed moderately positive correlations
to the BSL-23, M-BDI, MDQ, and the OCI. However, the PBS was the
only measure that was signicantly correlated with performance
in the Go/No-Go Task, both with respect to response times
(r¼".28) and the number of commission errors (r¼.20).
Table 1
Means, standard deviations, and correlations (Cronbach's αEstimate of Internal
Consistency on the Diagonal).
Variable Mean (SD) Correlations
12345
1. PBS 1.99 (0.78) (.91)
2. M-BDI 2.30 (0.71) .44
**
(.91)
3. OCI 1.83 (0.62) .40
**
.44
**
(.88)
4. MDQ 1.56 (0.27) .36
**
.30
**
.29
**
(.83)
5. BSL-23 1.71 (0.71) .30
**
.80
**
.53
**
.32
**
(.95)
GNGT response time 477 (75.1) ".28
**
.03 ".17 ".08 .00
GNGT commission errors 3.17 (2.59) .20
*
".09 .01 .09 ".07
PBS ¼Pathological Buying Screener, M-BDI ¼Modied Beck Depression Inventory,
OCI ¼Obsessive Compulsive Disorder Inventory, MDQ ¼Mood Disorder Ques-
tionnaire, BSL-23¼Borderline Symptom Checklist, GNGT ¼Go/No-Go Task.
*
po.05.
**
po.01.
J. Nicolai et al. / Psychiatry Research 244 (2016) 351356 353
3.1. Manipulation check
Two linear regressions were performed to evaluate the effec-
tiveness of the mood manipulation by considering the PA and NA
subscales, respectively, of the PANAS as criterion variables. Speci-
cally, the respective PANAS subscale assessed after mood induc-
tion was regressed on mood condition (coded "1 for negative and
1 for positive mood) and the like PANAS subscale measured at
baseline (to control for pre-existing mood differences across con-
ditions). Results show that the mood manipulation yielded sig-
nicant effects on both subscales of the PANAS (PA:
β
¼0.72,
po.01; NA:
β
¼"0.75, po.01). In addition, compared to partici-
pants in the negative mood induction condition, participants in
the positive mood induction condition exhibited smaller values on
the post-mood induction PANAS-NA (M¼1.83, SD¼0.75 vs.
M¼1.41, SD¼0.43; d¼"0.70; t(98)¼3.49, po.01) and higher
values on the PANAS-PA (M¼2.21, SD¼0.85 vs. M¼2.59, SD¼0.95;
Cohen's d¼0.43; t(98)¼2.11, po.05), thereby conrming that the
mood induction was successful. Apart from the PANAS, there was
no effect of mood condition on either of the remaining ques-
tionnaire measures.
3.2. Effect of pathological buying and mood state on state
impulsivity
The multiple regression predicting Go/No-Go Task reaction
times yielded a signicant effect of PBS (
β
¼"0.30, po.01), but no
signicant effect of mood condition (
β
¼"0.07, p¼.45) and no
signicant interaction between PBS and mood condition (
β
¼0.14,
p¼.17). The model explained 10.1% of the variance (po.05). This
pattern indicates that participants with higher values on the PBS
acted more impulsively by responding faster, irrespectively of
mood. This pattern of result did not change when adding the re-
maining variables (BSL-23, M-BDI, MDQ, and OCI) as covariates to
the model: PBS showed a similar effect (
β
¼"0.35, po.01),
whereas no other predictor signicantly contributed to the model.
The multiple regression predicting the number of commission
errors in the Go/No-Go Task yielded a signicant effect of PBS
(
β
¼0.24, po.05), indicating that participants with higher values
on the PBS reacted more impulsively by making more commission
errors. There was no signicant effect of mood condition (
β
¼0.15,
p¼.13). The main effect of PBS was qualied by a signicant in-
teraction between PBS and mood condition (
β
¼"0.25, po.05),
showing that mood state moderates the association between PBS
and impulsivity. The multiple R
2
was .12 (po.01). Note that we did
not perform a stepwise regression analysis, but simultaneously
included the relevant predictors in a single model, in line with
contemporary recommendations (Cohen et al., 2003). However,
performing a stepwise regression analysis leads to the same con-
clusions. Predicting the number of errors by the PBS score as the
only predictor yielded a signicant R
2
of .04, F(1,98)¼4.2, po.05.
Including the mood condition in the second step did not sig-
nicantly improve the model,
Δ
R
2
¼0.02, F(1,97) ¼2.1, p¼.15. In-
cluding the interaction between PBS and mood condition as a third
step signicantly contributed to the prediction of the number of
errors,
Δ
R
2
¼0.06, F(1,96)¼6.6, po.05. Fig. 1 shows the effect of
PBS on the number of commission errors separately for partici-
pants in the positive and negative mood condition. It is evident
that PBS shows virtually no association with the number of com-
mission errors in the positive mood condition (simple slope
β
¼"0.01, p¼.95). By contrast, there was a pronounced positive
association between PBS and the number of commission errors in
the negative mood condition (simple slope
β
¼0.49, po.001).
Thus, the interaction shows that higher (compared to lower) PBS
scores were associated with more impulsive responding when
mood was negative, whereas PBS did not predict impulsivity when
mood was positive. Adding the covariates to the model did not
change the results. Again, PBS (
β
¼0.31, po.01) and the interaction
between PBS and mood condition (
β
¼"0.25, po.05) were the
only signicant predictors.
4. Discussion
Pathological buying has been repeatedly associated with de-
cits in impulse control. A possible mechanism contributing to the
development and maintenance of pathological buying is that
buying acts as a maladaptive strategy to cope with negative affect.
In the present study, we tested (a) whether pathological buying
symptoms are associated with impaired performance in the Go/
No-Go Task as a measure of the behavioural inhibition component
of state-impulsivity and (b) whether this association is more
pronounced in negative mood. In agreement with the rst hy-
pothesis, the study showed that higher scores on the PBS were
associated with faster responses and a larger number of commis-
sion errors in the Go/No-Go Task, thus indicating that individuals
exhibiting pathological buying symptoms generally behave more
impulsive. Corroborating the second hypothesis, the results fur-
ther provide evidence for the notion that decits in impulse
control are most striking when mood is negative, providing a
possible explanation for the occurrence of excessive buying epi-
sodes following negative affect.
In line with related work (de Zwaan, 2011;McElroy et al., 1994;
Müller et al., 2010), the present study revealed high comorbidities
of pathological buying to other disorders. Pathological buying ex-
hibited substantial correlations with symptoms of depression and
mood disorder, but also to OCD, borderline, and mania. Given
these comorbidities, it is interesting to note that both principal
ndings of more impulsive responding with more pathological
buying symptoms and stronger decits in impulse control for
negative mood still hold when controlling for the inuence of the
remaining variables. Thus, the results indicate that the suggested
mechanism may indeed be unique to pathological buying. As such,
these results also provide evidence concerning the classication of
Fig. 1. Effect of pathological buying and mood on the number of commission errors
in the Go/No-Go Task.
J. Nicolai et al. / Psychiatry Research 244 (2016) 351356354
pathological buying, given that OCD was not related to impulsivity.
This suggests that different cognitive mechanisms apply to OCD
and thus questions the categorization of pathological buying as
obsessive-compulsive disorder. Apart from OCD, pathological
buying also shares many similarities with (behavioural) addictions,
including a craving-like urge before engaging in buying, a re-
petitive loss of control, the use of buying to alleviate negative af-
fect, and symptoms similar to withdrawal when not buying.
Likewise, research has consistently demonstrated decits in im-
pulse control for individuals with (behavioural) addictions (Cos-
kunpinar and Cyders, 2013;Karim and Chaudhri, 2012). Thus, al-
though we did not investigate this issue explicitly in the present
study, the results are closely aligned with the notion that patho-
logical buying resembles (behavioural) addictions (Lawrence et al.,
2014;Racine et al., 2014). However, despite these overlaps, it
should be noted that pathological buying unlike addictions
does not seem to alter serotonergic, dopaminergic, or opioid reg-
ulatory systems (Piquet-Pessôa et al., 2014), so that a classication
as an impulse control disorder seems equally plausible (Müller
et al., 2015a).
A related issue is whether pathological buying is associated
with decits executive functions. Studies investigating perfor-
mance in tasks that seek to measure decision-making under am-
biguity and under risk provided mixed results (Black et al., 2012;
Derbyshire et al., 2014;Trotzke et al., 2015;Voth et al., 2014). If
anything, the available evidence indicates a trend for pathological
buyers to exhibit impairments in situations when ambiguity oc-
curs, but not in situations where the outcome of a decision is
explicit and stable (Trotzke et al., 2015). However, decision-making
is comprised of a number of different cognitive processes that
occur on different levels of the cognitive system. It might be
possible that pathological buying is associated with decits in
rather specic cognitive processes that may be difcult to isolate
when considering complex decision-making tasks. To the best of
our knowledge, the present study is the rst that investigated the
behavioural inhibition component of impulsivity via the Go/No-Go
task. It seems plausible that the observed impairments in this
particular component do not readily generalize to other aspects of
impulsivity or entirely different executive functions.
The results of the present study may also inform approaches to
treatment of individuals suffering from pathological buying.
Treatment programs may support clients in identifying and
managing their reactions to negative affect by promoting affect
regulation and distress tolerance skills.
Some limitations should be considered when interpreting the
results of the present study. First, the study is based on a non-
representative analog sample (instead of comparing individuals
clinically diagnosed as pathological buyers versus healthy con-
trols). Although the use of analog samples is subject to several
shortcomings (Coyne, 1994), most authors conceive pathological
buying as a dimensional phenomenon with normal variations (e.g.,
Vogt et al., 2014), so that analog samples are well suited to gain
insights into the underlying mechanisms distinguishing absence of
problems or subclinical from clinical states (Abramowitz et al.,
2014). A second limitation is that the present study was performed
online, thus giving rise to concerns pertaining to insufcient ex-
perimental control. It is therefore recommended to replicate the
results using in a laboratory setting, which would also allow for
applying more sophisticated and thus arguably more effective
methods of mood induction (Hewig et al., 2005). In addition, to
obtain further and more direct support for the mood-repair hy-
pothesis, a replication using buying related cues is recommended.
Moreover, the present study did not include a self-report measure
assessing trait impulsivity. Given that self-report and behavioural-
based measures of impulsivity exhibit only little overlap and
presumably assess different aspects of impulsivity (Cyders and
Coskunpinar, 2011;Stahl et al., 2014), it would be interesting to
examine trait-impulsivity in conjunction with the tasks used
herein. Finally, the presence of disorders has been assessed after
the mood induction and the Go/No-Go task, because certain
questionnaire items could potentially trigger negative emotions
and thus conict with the mood induction procedure. Although
there were no effects of mood condition on measures of psycho-
logical disorders, a superior approach is to assess the presence of
disorders several days in advance in a separate session.
Despite these limitations, the present study demonstrates
decits in the behavioural inhibition component of state im-
pulsivity using a behavioural paradigm for individuals with pa-
thological buying symptoms and showed that these decits are
most pronounced in a state of negative affect. It was further shown
that this mechanism is related to pathological buying, rather than
to comorbid disorders. The results of the present study can thus be
interpreted to provide initial support for the notion that patho-
logical buyers may use buying episodes as a maladaptive self-
regulatory mechanism to reduce negative feelings.
References
Abramowitz, J.S., Fabricant, L.E., Taylor, S., Deacon, B.J., McKay, D., Storch, E.A., 2014.
The relevance of analogue studies for understanding obsessions and compul-
sions. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 34, 206217.
Beck, A.T., Steer, R.A., 1987. Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI). The
Psychological Corporation, San Antonio (TX).
Billieux, J., Rochat, L., Rebetez, M.M.L., Van der Linden, M., 2008. Are all facets of
impulsivity related to self-reported compulsive buying behavior? Pers. Individ.
Differ. 44, 14321442.
Black, D.W., Shaw, M., McCormick, B., Bayless, J.D., Allen, J., 2012. Neuropsycholo-
gical performance, impulsivity, ADHD symptoms, and novelty seeking in
compulsive buying disorder. Psychiatry Res. 200, 581587.
Bohus, M., Kleindienst, N., Limberger, M.F., Stieglitz, R.-D., Domsalla, M., Chapman,
A.L., Steil, R., Philipsen, A., Wolf, M., 2009. The short version of the borderline
symptom list (BSL-23): development and initial data on psychometric proper-
ties. Psychopathology 42, 3239.
Christenson, G.A., Faber, R.J., de Zwaan, M., Raymond, N.C., Specker, S.M., Ekern, M.
D., Mackenzie, T.B., Crosby, R.D., Crow, S.J., Eckert, E.D., 1994. Compulsive
buying: descriptive characteristics and psychiatric comorbidity. J. Clin. Psy-
chiatry 55, 511.
Coskunpinar, A., Cyders, M.A., 2013. Impulsivity and substance-related attentional
bias: a meta-analytic review. Drug Alcohol Depend. 133, 114.
Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S.G., Aiken, L.S., 2003. Applied Multiple Regression/Cor-
relation Analysis in the Behavioral Sciences, 3rd ed Erlbaum, Mahwah.
Coyne, J.C., 1994. Self-reported distress: analog or ersatz depression? Psychol. Bull.
116, 2945.
Cyders, M.A., Coskunpinar, A., 2011. Measurement of constructs using self-report
and behavioral lab tasks: is there overlap in nomothetic span and construct
representation for impulsivity? Clin. Psychol. Rev. 31, 965982.
Derbyshire, K.L., Chamberlain, S.R., Odlaug, B.L., Schreiber, L.R., Grant, J.E., 2014.
Neurocognitive functioning in compulsive buying disorder. Ann. Clin. Psy-
chiatry 26, 5763.
de Zwaan, M., 2011. Psychiatric comorbidity and compulsive buying, Compulsive
Buying: Clinical Foundations and Treatment. Routledge, New York, pp. 87104.
Dittmar, H., 2005. Compulsive buyinga growing concern? An examination of
gender, age, and endorsement of materialistic values as predictors. Br. J. Psy-
chol. 96, 467491.
Dittmar, H., 2004. Understanding and diagnosing compulsive buying. In: Coombs,
R.H. (Ed.), Handbook of Addictive Disorders: A Practical Guide to Diagnosis and
Treatment. Wiley, New York, pp. 411450.
Faber, R.J., Christenson, G.A., 1996. Differences in the mood states experienced by
compulsive buyers and other consumers. Psychol. Mark. 13, 803819.
Faber, R.J., OGuinn, T.C., 1992. A clinical screener for compulsive buying. J. Consum.
Res., 459469.
Foa, E.B., Huppert, J.D., Leiberg, S., Langner, R., Kichic, R., Hajcak, G., Salkovskis, P.M.,
2002. The Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory: development and validation of a
short version. Psychol. Assess. 14, 485496.
Göritz, A.S., 2007. The induction of mood via the WWW. Motiv. Emot. 31, 3547.
Grüsser, S.M., Poppelreuter, S., Heinz, A., Albrecht, U., Sass, H., 2007. Verhaltens-
sucht: Eine eigenständige diagnostische Einheit? [Behavioural addiction: an
independent diagnostic category?]. Nervenarzt 78, 9971002.
Hewig, J., Hagemann, D., Seifert, J., Gollwitzer, M., Naumann, E., Bartussek, D., 2005.
A revised lm set for the induction of basic emotions. Cogn. Emot. 19,
10951109.
Hirschfeld, R.M.A., Holzer, C., Calabrese, J.R., Weissman, M., Reed, M., Davies, M.,
Frye, M.A., Keck, P., McElroy, S., Lewis, L., Tierce, J., Wagner, K.D., Hazard, E.,
2003. Validity of the mood disorder questionnaire: a general population study.
J. Nicolai et al. / Psychiatry Research 244 (2016) 351356 355
Am. J. Psychiatry 160, 178180.
Hirschfeld, R.M.A., Williams, J.B.W., Spitzer, R.L., Calabrese, J.R., Flynn, L., Keck, P.E.,
Lewis, L., McElroy, S.L., Post, R.M., Rapport, D.J., Russell, J.M., Sachs, G.S., Zajecka,
J., 2000. Development and validation of a screening instrument for bipolar
spectrum disorder: the mood disorder questionnaire. Am. J. Psychiatry 157,
18731875.
Hollander, E., Allen, A., 2006. Is compulsive buying a real disorder, and is it really
compulsive? Am. J. Psychiatry 163, 16701672.
Hollander, E., Zohar, J., Sirovatka, P.J., Regier, D.A., 2011. Obsessive-compulsive
spectrum disorders: Rening the research agenda for DSM-V, 1st ed. American
Psychiatric Pub, Washington, DC.
Karim, R., Chaudhri, P., 2012. Behavioral addictions: an overview. J. Psychoact.
Drugs 44, 517.
Kellett, S., Bolton, J.V., 2009. Compulsive buying: a cognitivebehavioural model.
Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 16, 8399.
Krohne, H.W., Egloff, B., Kohlmann, C.W., Tausch, A., 1996. Investigations with a
German version of the positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS). Diag-
nostica 42, 139156.
Kyrios, M., McQueen, P., Moulding, R., 2013. Experimental analysis of the relation-
ship between depressed mood and compulsive buying. J. Behav. Ther. Exp.
Psychiatry 44, 194200.
Lawrence, L.M., Ciorciari, J., Kyrios, M., 2014. Relationships that compulsive buying
has with addiction, obsessive-compulsiveness, hoarding, and depression.
Compr. Psychiatry 55, 11371145.
Lejoyeux, M., Tassain, V., Solomon, J., Ades, J., 1997. Study of compulsive buying in
depressed patients. J. Clin. Psychiatry 58, 169173.
Maraz, A., Grifths, M.D., Demetrovics, Z., 2016. The prevalence of compulsive
buying: a meta-analysis. Addiction 111, 408419.
McElroy, S.L., Keck, P.E., Pope, H.G., Smith, J.M., Strakowski, S.M., 1994. Compulsive
buying: a report of 20 cases. J. Clin. Psychiatry 55, 242248.
Miltenberger, R.G., Redlin, J., Crosby, R., Stickney, M., Mitchell, J., Wonderlich, S.,
Faber, R., Smyth, J., 2003. Direct and retrospective assessment of factors con-
tributing to compulsive buying. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 34, 19.
Müller, A., Claes, L., Georgiadou, E., Möllenkamp, M., Voth, E.M., Faber, R.J., Mitchell,
J.E., de Zwaan, M., 2014. Is compulsive buying related to materialism, depres-
sion or temperament? Findings from a sample of treatment-seeking patients
with CB. Psychiatry Res. 216, 103107.
Müller, A., Mitchell, J.E., Black, D.W., Crosby, R.D., Berg, K., de Zwaan, M., 2010.
Latent prole analysis and comorbidity in a sample of individuals with com-
pulsive buying disorder. Psychiatry Res. 178, 348353.
Müller, A., Mitchell, J.E., Crosby, R.D., Cao, L., Johnson, J., Claes, L., de Zwaan, M.,
2012. Mood states preceding and following compulsive buying episodes: an
ecological momentary assessment study. Psychiatry Res. 200, 575580.
Müller, A., Mitchell, J.E., De Zwaan, M., 2015a. Compulsive buying. Am. J. Addict. 24,
132137.
Müller, A., Trotzke, P., Mitchell, J.E., de Zwaan, M., Brand, M., 2015b. The patholo-
gical buying screener: development and psychometric properties of a new
screening instrument for the assessment of pathological buying symptoms.
PLoS One 10, e0141094.
Piquet-Pessôa, M., Ferreira, G.M., Melca, I.A., Fontenelle, L.F., 2014. DSM-5 and the
decision not to include sex, shopping or stealing as addictions. Curr. Addict.
Rep. 1, 172176 .
R Core Team, 2013. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R
Foundation ForStatistical Computing, Vienna, Austria.
Racine, E., Kahn, T., Hollander, E., 2014. Compulsive buying disorder, Behavioral
Addictions: Criteria, Evidence, and Treatment. Academic Press, New York,
pp. 285316.
Reips, U.-D., 2002. Standards for Internet-based experimenting. Exp. Psychol. 49,
243256.
Rose, P., Segrist, D.J., 2014. Negative and positive urgency may both be risk factors
for compulsive buying. J. Behav. Addict. 3, 128132.
Schmitt, M., Beckmann, M., Dusi, D., Maes, J., Schiller, A., Schonauer, K., 20 03.
Messgüte des vereinfachten Beck-Depressions-Inventars (BDI-V) [Psychometric
properties of the simplied Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-V)]. Diagnostica 49,
147156.
Schmitt, M., Maes, J., 2000. Vorschlag zur Vereinfachung des Beck-Depressions-
Inventars (BDI) [A proposition for a simplied Beck Depression Inventory
(BDI)]. Diagnostica 46, 3846.
Stahl, C., Voss, A., Schmitz, F., Nuszbaum, M., Tüscher, O., Lieb, K., Klauer, K.C., 2014.
Behavioral components of impulsivity. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 143, 850886.
Trotzke, P., Starcke, K., Pedersen, A., Brand, M., 2014. Cue-induced craving in pa-
thological buying: empirical evidence and clinical implications. Psychosom.
Med. 76, 694700.
Trotzke, P., Starcke, K., Pedersen, A., Müller, A., Brand, M., 2015. Impaired decision
making under ambiguity but not under risk in individuals with pathological
buying-behavioral and psychophysiological evidence. Psychiatry Res. 229,
551558.
Velten, E., 1968. A laboratory task for induction of mood states. Behav. Res. Ther. 6,
473482.
Vogt, S., Hunger, A., Pietrowsky, R., Gerlach, A.L., 2015. Impulsivity in consumers
with high compulsive buying propensity. J. Obsessive-Compuls. Relat. Disord. 7,
5464.
Vogt, S., Hunger, A., Türpe, T., Pietrowsky, R., Gerlach, A.L., 2014. Effects of mood
induction on consumers with vs. without compulsive buying propensity: an
experimental study. Psychiatry Res. 220, 342347.
Voth, E.M., Claes, L., Georgiadou, E., Selle, J., Trotzke, P., Brand, M., de Zwaan, M.,
Müller, A., 2014. Reactive and regulative temperament in patients with com-
pulsive buying and non-clinical controls measured by self-report and perfor-
mance-based tasks. Compr. Psychiatry 55, 15051512.
Watson, D., Clark, L.A., Tellegen, A., 1988. Development and validation of brief
measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol.
54, 10631070.
Westermann, R., Spies, K., Stahl, G., Hesse, F.W., 1996. Relative effectiveness and
validity of mood induction procedures: a meta-analysis. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 26,
557580.
Williams, A.D., 2012. Evaluation of the mood repair hypothesis of compulsive
buying. Open J. Psychiatry 2, 8390.
Williams, A.D., Grisham, J.R., 2012. Impulsivity, emotion regulation, and mindful
attentional focus in compulsive buying. Cogn. Ther. Res. 36, 451457.
Williams, A.D., Grisham, J.R., 2011. Impulsivity, emotion regulation, and mindful
attentional focus in compulsive buying. Cogn. Ther. Res. 36, 451457.
J. Nicolai et al. / Psychiatry Research 244 (2016) 351356356
... The role of impulsive action, or response inhibition, in BD is mixed; four studies (Nicolai et al., 2016;Trotzke et al., 2020;Vogel et al., 2019;Williams, 2012) used a type of go-task (i.e., Affective Shifting Task; GoStop; Go/No-Go Task), and two studies (Derbyshire et al., 2014;Vogt et al., 2015) used stop signaling tasks. Of the six studies, two (Vogel et al., 2019;Vogt et al., 2015) did not find differences in response inhibition compared to controls. ...
... However, one of these studies excluded individuals with BD (i.e., exceeding cut-off score on one out of two BD screening instruments) who had elevated probability to respond to a stop signal (i.e., rarely inhibited response) for concern that instructions were misunderstood (Vogt et al., 2015), and the other study posited that individuals in both the BD and control groups may have similar approach tendencies to shopping, or that individuals with BD may have been previously treated to learn avoidance tendencies toward shopping-related stimuli (Vogel et al., 2019) since this sample included treatment-seekers with BD. However, in a convenience sample of individuals assessed for BD on the Pathological Buying Screener (PBS; Müller et al., 2015), BD was associated with greater response inhibition under heightened negative affect (Nicolai et al., 2016). These findings remained significant when controlling for comorbid disorders, suggesting a unique relationship between impulsivity and BD. ...
... Based on the evidence, BD fits more closely to behavioral addictions than OCD, which has also been proposed by Nicolai et al. (2016). This is due to similarities to SUDs and GD on impulsive action, impulsive choice, and delay discounting. ...
Article
Full-text available
Although Buying Disorder (BD) is not a formal diagnosis in commonly used diagnostic systems, this condition can cause significant impairments. Additionally, there is an ongoing discussion about the most appropriate conceptualization and classification of BD. Most often, BD is considered either an obsessive–compulsive and related disorder (OCRD) or an addictive behavior (i.e., substance use disorders or behavioral addictions), and was previously recognized as an impulse control disorder. The present narrative review examines the cognitive processes of impulsivity (i.e., impulsive action, impulsive choice, decision making, and personality), compulsivity, and reward processing (i.e., cue reactivity and craving), in BD, obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD), and addictive behaviors. Most evidence supports BD having overlapping features with behavioral addictions more so than with OCD due to similar impairments in decision-making and inhibition, as well as similar motivations behind BD. Further, BD demonstrates cue-reactivity and craving similar to behavioral addictions. There were also similar elevations on personality inventories between BD and addictive behaviors, which were less relevant in OCD. Although studies in these specific cognitive domains suggest similarities between BD and behavioral addiction, more studies are needed to further elucidate BD processes, which would in turn assist with the classification of BD. Further, despite similarities across conditions, directly comparing BD to these conditions on the aforementioned processes is needed. In future, study designs should directly compare BD to disorders within each classification to elucidate shared and distinct functions of these processes.
... Normal (0-7) 127 Mild (8)(9) 38 Moderate (10)(11)(12)(13)(14) 106 Severe (15)(16)(17)(18)(19) 46 Extreme Severe (>20) 93 Stress level 13.331±9.454 Normal (0- 14) 262 Mild (15)(16)(17)(18) 51 Moderate (19)(20)(21)(22)(23)(24)(25) 40 Severe (26)(27)(28)(29)(30)(31)(32)(33) 45 Extreme Severe (>34) 14 to engage in compulsive online shopping and connect to online shopping sites longer and more frequently. [17] However, Duroy et al. [5] did not find a significant association between online compulsive buying and Internet addiction. ...
... Contrary to our study findings, evidence from other studies indicates that individuals with pathological buying behavior exhibit higher levels of impulsivity compared to healthy controls, and this impulsivity is also associated with negative affect. [28,29] One other study reported a positive and statistically significant correlation between smartphone addiction, social media addiction, FOMO, impulsive buying, and online compulsive buying. [30] Researchers did not find any relevance or evidence of the effectiveness of pharmacological and psychological interventions in treating pathological buying behavior. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Compulsive buying behavior [CBB] or pathological buying (PB) is a psychiatric disorder of growing recognition and many psychosocial factors have been proposed to predispose or precipitate this predicament. Aim To find out the association of compulsive buying behavior, emotional distress, self-esteem, internet addiction, and impulsiveness among nursing students. Materials and Methods A web-based cross-sectional, online survey was conducted among nursing undergraduates to assess the association of compulsive buying behavior, emotional distress, self-esteem, internet addiction, and impulsiveness using total enumerative sampling from February 2024 to April 2024. Data were collected using self-administered PBS, IAT, Rosenberg self-esteem scale, BIS-Brief, and DASS-21 scales. Descriptive and inferential statistics were used. Frequency distribution, Bivariate correlation, and Multiple regression analysis were also used. Results There were 410 participants with valid responses, and 207 [50.5%] fell into the category of pathological buying. The participant group “with pathological buying” was comparable with the group “not having pathological buying” in terms of sociodemographic statistics, preferred modality of purchasing stuff, and the “duration of daily internet use”. However, the DASS-21 and IAT scores were significantly higher among the participants “with pathological buying”. On regression analysis, both DASS and IAT scores were predicting the PBS scores. Conclusion There is a significant association between pathological buying with internet addiction and psychological distress, but not with self-esteem, and impulsivity.
... However, according to Müller et al. (2019), both etiologic and phenomenological similarities with other behavioural addictions are ignored: the act of buying implies a positive reinforcement at first, the purpose of it is to alleviate emotions and situations perceived as negative and/or stressful. This shift in the reinforcing value of the buying behaviour is consistent with the current addiction models Nicolai et al., 2016;Pickering et al., 2023). Other studies found compulsive buyers are likely to experiment symptoms of withdrawal (Ausburguer et al., 2020), which fits more a behavioural addiction rather than a residual subcategory of impulse control disorders. ...
... Over the last decade, the correlation between impulsivity and compulsive buying as well as impulsivity's prediction over compulsive buying has been demonstrated (Aydin et al., 2021;De Paula et al., 2015;González & Lemos, 2020;Lindheimer et al., 2020;Nicolai et al., 2016;Tiegoe et al., 2019). Moreover, correlations between pathological buying and deficits on self-control and executive functions (Billeux et al., 2008;Racine et al., 2014). ...
Article
Compulsive buying is a phenomenon that involves dysfunctional shopping behaviors together with a continuous concern about buying and problems derived from said behaviour that impact different aspects of daily life. This study aims to analyse some of the psychological factors related to compulsive buying both online and offline, such as anxiety and impulsivity. 202 Volunteers from both sexes between the ages 18 and 69 participated. A questionnarie was designed based on the Barrat Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11, 1995), the Bergen Shopping Adicction Scale (BSAS, 2015) and the State-Trait Anxiety Questionnarie (STAI, 1983). It was verified that the most impulsive and anxious individuals tend to perform more behaviors related to compulsive buying. No differences in compulsive buying based on age were found. It is interesting to delve into the implications derived from dysfunctional purchasing behaviors and the psychosocial implications of such practices.
... Sin embargo, según Müller et al. (2019), se ignoran las similitudes fenomenológicas y etiológicas que comparte con otros trastornos por adicción: aunque el acto de comprar implica un refuerzo inicialmente positivo a lo largo del proceso de desarrollo del trastorno de compra compulsiva el motivo es aliviar y afrontar estados emocionales y situaciones percibidas como estresantes y/o negativas. Este cambio en el valor reforzador de la conducta de compra coincide con los modelos actuales de la adicción Nicolai, et al., 2016;Pickering et al., 2023). En otros estudios también se encontró que los compradores compulsivos son propensos a experimentar síntomas de abstinencia (Augsburger et al., 2020), dato que corresponde con un trastorno por conducta adictiva más que como una subcategoría residual de trastornos del control de impulsos. ...
... A lo largo de la última década se ha demostrado la asociación entre la impulsividad y la compra compulsiva, así como la función predictora de la misma (Aydin et al., 2021;De Paula et al., 2015;González y Lemos, 2020;Lindheimer et al., 2020;Nicolai et al., 2016;Tiego et al., 2019). Asimismo, existen correlaciones entre la compra patológica y déficits en el autocontrol y las funciones ejecutivas (Billeux et al., 2008;Racine et al., 2014). ...
Article
La compra compulsiva es un fenómeno que implica conductas de compra disfuncionales junto a una continua preocupación por comprar y problemas derivados del propio comportamiento en distintos ámbitos de la vida cotidiana. El objetivo de este estudio es analizar algunos de los factores psicológicos relacionados con la compra compulsiva tanto online como offline, tales como la ansiedad y la impulsividad. Participaron 202 voluntarios de ambos sexos entre 18 y 69 años. Se diseñó un cuestionario utilizando la “Escala de impulsividad de Barrat” (BIS-11, 1995), la “Escala de adicción a las compras de Bergen” (BSAS, 2015) y el “Cuestionario de ansiedad estado-rasgo” (STAI, 1983). Se comprobó que los individuos más impulsivos y ansiosos tienden a realizar más comportamientos relacionados con la compra compulsiva. No se constataron diferencias en la compra compulsiva en función de la edad. Resulta de interés profundizar en las implicaciones derivadas de las conductas de compra disfuncionales y en las implicaciones psicosociales de tales prácticas.
... Individuals who believe that online discounts are beneficial for increasing customer loyalty are more inclined to embrace the idea that online discounts help to nurturing consumer loyalty, according to the findings of this study. Given the considerable association that exists between these parameters, as revealed by the findings of the ChiSquare test, it is important to highlight the potential influence that online discounts might have on customer loyalty campaigns (Nicolai, 2016). The necessity of businesses strategically incorporating online discount tactics into their plans for retaining customers is brought to light by this statistic. ...
Article
Full-text available
With the increasing preference of Indians to shop online instead of visiting physical stores, the retail sector in the country is undergoing significant transformation. Several factors are fueling the rapid rise in popularity of online shopping in India. These include the increasing internet penetration, a growing number of internet-savvy consumers, a larger labour force, a wide range of online stores offering goods from around the globe, and improved web accessibility. The concept of "e-tailing" refers to the business model where products are bought and sold through an online platform. India's retail sector is currently undergoing a significant transformation due to the transition from traditional stores to online marketplaces. During the festival season, online retailers are currently offering a variety of deals to attract more customers. In recent years, the popularity of online shopping has soared, allowing consumers to easily purchase a wide range of products, from everyday essentials to cutting-edge technology. Online shopping and digital shops are becoming increasingly popular in rural areas, reaching beyond the confines of city life. Several factors contribute to this phenomenon, such as increasing incomes, a stronger economy, a wide range of choices for consumers, and the active involvement of lenders in promoting digitalization and online shopping. The festival shopping sector is a significant driver behind the growth of e-commerce platforms. During holiday seasons, companies frequently offer promotions and significant discounts on both new and used products. In addition, they offer additional incentives such as cash-back deals and buy-one-get-one sales. At this time, companies would organise sales events with distinctive names such as "Big Billion Days" on Flipkart, "Flash Sales" on Myntra, and "Great Festive Sale" on Amazon. The aim of this study is to examine the impact of virtual shopping festivals on local communities and individual shoppers.
... "zwanghafte Kauf-Shopping-Störung", "Kauf-Shopping-Störung" (Georgiadou & Tahmassebi, 2022;Laskowski, Trotzke, de Zwaan, Brand & Müller, 2021) oder "Shoppingstörung" (Rumpf et al., 2021). In der derzeitigen deutschen Entwurfsfassung der ICD-11 des Bundesinstituts für Arzneimittel und Medizinprodukte findet sich der von Iver Hand geprägte Begriff "pathologisches Kaufen" (BfArM, 2022), der in den letzten 25 Jahren häufig in der deutschen (Hand, 1998;Müller & de Zwaan, 2008) und auch internationalen Fachliteratur als "pathological buying" (Claes, Luyckx, Vogel, Verschueren & Müller, 2018;Fernandez-Aranda et al., 2019;Müller, Trotzke, Mitchell, de Zwaan & Brand, 2015;Nicolai, Daranco & Moshagen, 2016;Trotzke, Starcke, Müller & Brand, 2015) verwendet wurde. ...
Article
Full-text available
Zusammenfassung: Zielsetzung: Psychiatriehistorische Betrachtung der Begriffsbildung und nosologischen Einordnung der „Oniomanie“. Methodik: Fokussierter narrativer Überblick. Ergebnisse: Der Begriff „Oniomanie“ wurde vermutlich im 19. Jahrhundert von dem französischen Psychiater Magnan geprägt und später vor allem mit Kraepelin in Verbindung gebracht. Kraepelin und Bleuler beschrieben die „Oniomanie“ in kurzen Absätzen ihrer psychiatrischen Lehrbücher. Die nosologische Zuordnung geschah zunächst im Rahmen der „Entartungslehre“ des 19. Jahrhunderts, Anfang des 20. Jahrhunderts sprach Kraepelin von „krankhaften Trieben“, Bleuler in Anlehnung an Kraepelin von „impulsivem Irresein“, später von „psychogenen Störungen“ und von „krankhaften Reaktionen thymopsychischer Art“. Der Begriff „Oniomanie“ wird in der aktuellen Literatur kaum noch gebraucht. Stattdessen werden Bezeichnungen wie z. B. „Kaufsucht“, „pathologisches Kaufen“ oder „zwanghafte Kauf-Shopping-Störung“ bevorzugt. In der ICD-11 wird die „compulsive buying-shopping disorder“ als ein Beispiel für eine sonstige näher bezeichnete Störung der Impulskontrolle genannt. Neuropsychologische Befunde sprechen für eine Einordnung als Verhaltenssucht. Schlussfolgerungen: Der Begriff „Oniomanie“ wurde vor mehr als 100 Jahren geprägt und kann als Vorläufer der ICD-11 Bezeichnung „zwanghafte Kauf-Shopping-Störung“ gesehen werden. Aktuell sieht man die Störung nosologisch entweder bei den Impulskontrollstörungen oder Verhaltenssüchten angesiedelt.
Article
E-commerce platforms have proliferated, revolutionising consumer purchasing behaviour and enabling more convenient shopping from any location at any time. But because of its ease, there has been a marked increase in impulsive purchases, which has greatly increased the amount of plastic waste. This study investigates the relationship between the rise in plastic waste that occurs from impulsive purcphasing on e-commerce platforms. The purpose of this study is to demonstrate the negative environmental effects of the current e-commerce ecosystem and offer viable solutions to reduce plastic waste by examining consumer behaviour, packaging standards, and waste management concerns. The relationship between impulsive purchasing on e-commerce platforms and the rise in plastic waste is examined in this qualitative study. The study attempts to determine the reasons behind through in-depth interviews and thematic analysis
Article
Full-text available
Purpose of the Review To provide a systematic review of experimental studies concerning cognitive functions in compulsive buying-shopping disorder (CBSD) and to evaluate the studies as supporting or not supporting the affective and cognitive interactions proposed by the Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model for addictive behaviors. Recent Findings The results of the present review concerning CBSD mirror findings regarding cue reactivity and disadvantageous decision making in other addictive behaviors, but they also demonstrate a relative lack of experimental studies addressing other cognitive domains such as attentional bias, inhibitory control, implicit associations, or Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer. Experimental work on physiological and neural correlates of affective and cognitive mechanisms and their interaction in CBSD is still at the beginning. Summary While a reasonable number of experimental neuropsychological studies support the application of the I-PACE framework to CBSD, future research is required to systematically examine affective and cognitive interactions in CBSD.
Article
Full-text available
Aims: To estimate the pooled prevalence of compulsive buying behaviour (CBB) in different populations and to determine the effect of age, gender, location, and screening instrument on the reported heterogeneity in estimates CBB and whether publication bias could be identified. Methods: Three databases were searched (Medline, PsychInfo, Web of Science) using the terms "compulsive buying", "pathological buying" and "compulsive shopping" to estimate the pooled prevalence of CBB in different populations. Forty studies reporting 49 prevalence estimates from 16 countries were located (n=32,000). To conduct the meta-analysis, data from non-clinical studies regarding mean age and gender proportion, geographical study location, and screening instrument used to assess CBB were extracted by multiple independent observers and evaluated using a random effects model. Four a-priori subgroups were analysed using pooled estimation (Cohen's Q) and covariate testing (moderator and meta-regression analysis). Results: The CBB pooled prevalence of adult representative studies was 4.9% [3.4%-6.9%, eight estimates, 10,102 participants] although estimates were higher among university students: 8.3% [5.9%-11.5%, 19 estimates, 14,947 participants], in adult non-representative samples: 12.3% [7.6%-19.1%, 11 estimates, 3929 participants], and in shopping-specific samples: 16.2% [8.8%-27.8%, 11 estimates, 4,686 participants]. Being young and female were associated with increased tendency, but not location (USA vs. non-USA). Meta-regression revealed large heterogeneity within subgroups, mostly due to diverse measures and timeframes (current vs. lifetime) used to assess CBB. Conclusions: A pooled estimate of compulsive buying behaviour in the populations studied is around 5% but there is large variation between samples largely accounted for by use of different time frames and measures.
Article
Full-text available
The study was designed to develop a new screening instrument for pathological buying (PB), and to examine its psychometric properties in a large-scale sample. By using a facet theoretical approach and based on literature as well as on clinical experience, a 20-item Pathological Buying Screener (PBS) was developed and administered to a representative German sample (n = 2,539). Valid data were available from 2,403 participants who were subjects for three subsequent empirical studies. The first study explored the factor structure using exploratory factor analyses in a subsample of 498 participants. Based on factor loadings, a 13-item version with the two factors loss of control / consequences and excessive buying behavior was revealed. This two-factor model was confirmed in study 2 by confirmatory factor analysis performed on another subsample (n = 1,905). Study 3 investigated age and gender effects and convergent validity of the PBS using the Compulsive Buying Scale (CBS) in the full sample (N = 2,403). The total PBS score was adequately correlated with the CBS score. Hierarchical regression analyses with the CBS score as the dependent variable and the two PBS factors as the predictors indicated an own incremental validity of the two factors in participants ≤ 65 years. The reliability of the total score as well as of the two subscales was good to excellent. Overall, the PBS represents a useful measure for PB. Future studies are needed to replicate the two-factor structure in clinical samples and to define a valid cutoff for PB.
Chapter
This chapter explores compulsive buying disorder (CBD) both as a unique impulsive control disorder and as a disorder that shares features with other compulsive and impulsive disorders. The chapter begins with a description of CBD derived from the existing literature, followed by a section on the epidemiology of CBD, with a focus on the distribution of CBD across different industrialized nations, genders, ages, and socioeconomic statuses. The chapter continues with a discussion of the proposed diagnostic criteria for CBD and an analysis of the disorder's presenting clinical characteristics. This section also reviews the clinically designed diagnostic scales for CBD as well as the most common comorbidities seen with CBD: common Axis I and Axis II disorders, particularly mood and anxiety disorders. The pathophysiology of CBD is then discussed, with a brief exploration of the possible hereditary nature of the disorder. The chapter concludes with a section on treatment and management methods for CBD, both psychological and pharmacological, as well as suggestions for future research needed to further the understanding and treatment of CBD.
Article
Compulsive buying (CB) is a pathological behavior leading to impairment and distress. Evidence suggests that CB is associated with impulsivity and hoarding. However, multi-method investigations of impulsivity in CB with and without hoarding are sparse. In this study, consumers with high CB propensity (CBs; n=28) and controls (n=21) were compared with respect to self-reported and behavioral impulsivity. We investigated the association between impulsivity and hoarding in CBs and compared CBs with hoarding (HCBs) and CBs without hoarding symptomatology (NCBs) with respect to self-reported impulsivity.Compared to controls, CBs scored higher on self-reported impulsivity, except for "sensation seeking", but performed equally on behavioral assessments. Hoarding was more prevalent in CBs. Self-reported but not behavioral impulsivity was significantly correlated to hoarding symptoms. Compared to NCBs, HCBs reported to be more impulsive. However, the impulsivity facet "urgency" was the only significant predictor of CB although other impulsivity facets and acquisition-unrelated hoarding symptoms were considered.CBs and especially HCBs perceive themselves as more impulsive. However, this is not reflected on a behavioral level of impulsivity. Future studies should investigate impulsivity in CB under the consideration of hoarding using larger samples and more ecologically valid behavioral assessments.
Article
A German adaptation of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) is presented, the factorial structure as well as the internal consistency are analyzed and external associations are described. A principal component analysis of state affect (N=349; instruction: "How do you feel at the moment") and trait affect(N=480; instruction: .,... In general") resulted in a distinct two-factor solution labeled "Positive Affectivity" (PA) and "Negative Affectivity" (NA). Applying four additional time-oriented instructions ("How did you feel today - during the past few days - weeks - the past yea"), an internal validation study indicated that as time intervals increase, the influence of the state affect decreases, whereas that of the trait affect increases. Trait PA and NA furthermore yielded differential associations with global personality traits and variables within the areas of anxiety, reporting of symptoms, and emotions, as well as stress management.
Article
Pathological buying (PB) is described as dysfunctional buying behavior, associated with harmful consequences. It is discussed whether decision-making deficits are related to PB, because affected individuals often choose the short-term rewarding option of buying despite persistent negative long-term consequences. We investigated 30 patients suffering from PB and 30 matched control participants with two different decision-making tasks: the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) measures decisions under ambiguity and involves emotional feedback processing, whereas the Game of Dice Task (GDT) measures decisions under risk and can be solved strategically. Potential emotional and cognitive correlates of decision making were investigated by assessing skin conductance response (SCR) and executive functioning. In comparison to the control participants, the patients showed more disadvantageous decisions under ambiguity in the IGT. These data were supported by the SCR results: patients failed to generate SCRs that usually occur before disadvantageous decisions. The physiological and behavioral performance on decisions under risk and executive functioning did not differ between groups. Thus, deficits in emotional feedback processing might be one potential factor in etiology and pathogenesis of PB and should be considered in theory and treatment. Copyright © 2015. Published by Elsevier Ireland Ltd.
Article
Although compulsive buying (CB) seems to be not only prevalent but even increasing in prevalence, it often remains neglected or minimized in clinical settings. There is a need for a greater understanding and recognition of this problem. The aim of this article is to summarize the current knowledge regarding CB and to offer thoughts regarding classification. Review of published literature over the period 1994-2013 through Pubmed/Medline, PsychINFO, and Google Scholar using the key words 'compulsive buying', 'impulsive buying' and 'addictive buying'. CB is defined by a preoccupation with buying and shopping, by frequent buying episodes, or overpowering urges to buy that are experienced as irresistible and senseless. The maladaptive spending behavior is associated with serious psychological, social, occupational, and financial problems. Treatment-seeking patients with CB suffer from substantial psychiatric comorbidity (eg, anxiety and depressive mood disorders, compulsive hoarding, binge eating disorder). Representative surveys revealed prevalence estimates of CB between 6% and 7% and indicate that younger people are more prone to develop CB. Moreover, European data suggest an increase of CB in the adult population over the last 20 years. While there is no evidence for the efficacy of psychopharmacological treatment, group cognitive behavioral therapy has been shown to be effective. The relevance of recognition of CB as mental disorder is undeniable in the face of its estimated prevalence and associated burden. As our understanding of contributing neurobiological and etiological factors is limited, further research should focus on these topics, taking into account the heterogeneity of individuals with CB. There is also a need for specific treatment options and for the development of prevention strategies. (Am J Addict 2015;24:132-137). © American Academy of Addiction Psychiatry.