ArticlePDF Available

“I don’t want to miss a thing”: Adolescents’ fear of missing out and its relationship to adolescents’ social needs, Facebook use, and Facebook related stress

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

This survey study among adolescents (N = 402) investigates an integrative model that examines (1) the mediating role of adolescents’ fear of missing out (FoMO) in the relationships of adolescents’ need to belong and need for popularity with adolescents’ Facebook use and (2) the relationships of adolescents’ FoMO with adolescents’ perceived stress related to the use of Facebook. Structural equation modeling results indicated that an increased need to belong and an increased need for popularity were associated with an increased use of Facebook. These relationships were mediated by FoMO. Increased FoMO was associated with increased stress related to Facebook use. These results emphasize the important role that FoMO plays in adolescents’ media use and well-being.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Running head: FOMO, SOCIAL NEEDS, FACEBOOK USE, AND FACEBOOK STRESS 1
This is a post print version
“I Don’t Want to Miss a Thing”: Adolescents’ Fear of Missing Out and its Relationship to
Adolescents’ Social Needs, Facebook Use, and Facebook Related Stress
Ine Beyensa*, Eline Frisonb, and Steven Eggermontb
aAmsterdam School of Communication Research, University of Amsterdam, the Netherlands
bLeuven School for Mass Communication Research, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
Please cite as follows:
Beyens, I., Frison, E., & Eggermont, S. (2016). I dont want to miss a thing: Adolescents fear
of missing out and its relationship to adolescents social needs, Facebook use, and Facebook
related stress. Computers in Human Behavior.
_______________________________________________
*Ine Beyens, PhD (corresponding author)
Amsterdam School of Communication Research (ASCOR)
University of Amsterdam
Nieuwe Achtergracht 166
1018 WV Amsterdam
The Netherlands
E-mail: I.Beyens@uva.nl
FOMO, SOCIAL NEEDS, FACEBOOK USE, AND FACEBOOK STRESS 2
Abstract
This survey study among adolescents (N = 402) investigates an integrative model that
examines (1) the mediating role of adolescents’ fear of missing out (FoMO) in the relationships
of adolescents’ need to belong and need for popularity with adolescents’ Facebook use and (2)
the relationships of adolescents’ FoMo with adolescents’ perceived stress related to the use of
Facebook. Structural equation modeling results indicated that an increased need to belong and
an increased need for popularity were associated with an increased use of Facebook. These
relationships were mediated by FoMo, which, in turn, was associated with increased stress
related to Facebook use. These results emphasize the important role that fear of missing out
plays in adolescents’ life.
Keywords: Adolescents, Facebook Use, Facebook Stress, Fear of Missing Out, Need to
Belong, Need for Popularity
FOMO, SOCIAL NEEDS, FACEBOOK USE, AND FACEBOOK STRESS 3
“I Don’t Want to Miss a Thing”: Adolescents’ Fear of Missing Out and its Relationship to
Adolescents’ Social Needs, Facebook Use, and Facebook Related Stress
1. Introduction
Social networking sites (SNSs) are highly integrated in many adolescents’ lives (e.g.,
Lenhart, 2015; Sampasa-Kanyinga & Lewis, 2015) and are considered important channels to
seek and maintain social connections (e.g., Shapiro & Margolin, 2013). A recent American
report revealed that Facebook is the most popular and widely used online social network among
adolescents; more than 71% of 13- to 17-year-olds uses Facebook (Lenhart, 2015). The
popularity of Facebook among adolescents raises questions about what motivates them to use
Facebook. In this respect, social needs may be important, as scholars (e.g., Lee & Chiou, 2013)
argue that individuals are becoming increasingly dependent on SNSs to gratify their social needs.
In particular, the need to belong (i.e., the need to develop and maintain significant and stable
interpersonal relationships; Baumeister & Leary, 1995) and the need for popularity (i.e., the need
to do certain things aimed at increasing one’s popularity; Santor, Messervey, & Kusumakar,
2000) may be important motivations for adolescents’ Facebook use in particular, since these are
key concerns in adolescence (Santor et al., 2000).
Although previous studies confirmed this suggestion (e.g., Utz, Tanis, & Vermeulen,
2012), our understanding of potential underlying mechanisms that could explain this relationship
is still limited. However, given the potential harmful outcomes of using Facebook for young
people’s well-being (e.g., Kross et al., 2013), scholars agree that it is important to better
understand how certain factors relate to adolescents’ Facebook use. Przybylski, Murayama,
DeHaan, and Gladwell (2013) recently proposed that fear of missing out (FoMO) or “a pervasive
apprehension that others might be having rewarding experiences from which one is absent”, may
FOMO, SOCIAL NEEDS, FACEBOOK USE, AND FACEBOOK STRESS 4
be an important explanatory mechanism. They suggest, in line with self-determination theory
(SDT; Deci & Ryan, 1985; Ryan & Deci, 2000), that deficits in psychological needs may
increase people’s sensitivity to fear missing out on things, which in turn may drive people
towards social media, as social media may provide individuals an effective self-regulation tool to
satisfy their psychological needs (Przybylski et al., 2013). Therefore, the first aim of the present
study is to investigate the mediating role of FoMO in the association between adolescents’ social
needs (i.e., need to belong and need for popularity) and Facebook use.
Recently, studies reported associations between FoMO and perceived stress (e.g., Jones
2014). At the same time, scholars argued that SNSs may function as a new setting for perceiving
stress (e.g., Fox & Moreland, 2015), as peer interactions increasingly take place in an online
setting (e.g., Shapiro & Margolin, 2013) and are often accompanied by peer-related or
interpersonal stress (e.g., Byrne, Davenport, & Mazanov, 2007). Because Facebook may be a
new stress-inducing platform for teens (e.g., Fox & Moreland, 2015) and given the harmful
correlates of perceived peer-related stress in adolescence (e.g., Sontag & Graber, 2010), it is
critical to determine potential risk factors of perceived stress related to new settings for peer
interactions, such as Facebook. Therefore, the second aim of the present study is to examine
whether FoMO may predict increases in adolescents’ perceived stress related to a Facebook
setting.
In sum, the present study aims to test an integrative model that examines (1) the
mediating role of FoMO in the relationships between need to belong and need for popularity and
adolescents’ Facebook use and (2) the relationships between FoMO and adolescents’ perceived
stress related to the use of Facebook. We integrate these closely related needs in an integrative
model as this allows us to control for the need for popularity when examining the relationship
FOMO, SOCIAL NEEDS, FACEBOOK USE, AND FACEBOOK STRESS 5
between the need to belong and adolescents’ Facebook use, and vice versa, and as this enables a
more comprehensive understanding of the relationships between adolescents’ social needs and
their Facebook use than considering single needs alone.
1.1. The Relationships of Adolescents’ Social Needs with Adolescents’ Facebook Use
Adolescence is a critical developmental period, marked by an increased importance of the
peer group (Brown & Larson, 2009; Pombeni, Kirchler, & Palmonari, 1990). During this stage
of life, adolescents rely more and more on their peers and less on their parents. Peer networks
expand and interactions with peers become more intimate (Steinberg, 2005). As a result, peers
become primary sources of social support (Bokhorst, Sumter, & Westenberg, 2010). Adolescents
in particular feel a strong need to affiliate with their peers and belong to their peers, as well as to
feel popular among their peers. According to SDT (Deci & Ryan, 1985; Ryan & Deci, 2000),
adolescents, and people more generally, seek to gratify their need to feel socially connected with
others. In this vein, scholars have suggested that SNSs may be excellent tools for adolescents to
gratify their need to belong (Nadkarni & Hofmann, 2012) and their need for popularity (Utz et
al., 2012). This is very likely, since media connect adolescents to their peers and contribute to
their socialization with peers (Arnett, 1995; Suoninen, 2001).
First, indications exist that young people’s need to belong is associated with an increased
use of Facebook. More specifically, studies found empirical support for a link between
individuals’ need to belong and willingness to join a SNS (Gangadharbatla, 2008), the preference
for passive browsing on SNSs (Reich & Vorderer, 2013), and the disclosure of more intimate
information in status updates (Winter et al., 2014). Second, indications exist that adolescents’
need for popularity is associated with an increased Facebook use. In particular, research reported
a positive relationship between individuals’ need for popularity and online self-disclosure
FOMO, SOCIAL NEEDS, FACEBOOK USE, AND FACEBOOK STRESS 6
(Christofides et al., 2009; Utz et al., 2012) as well as with various SNS behaviors, including
socializing behaviors on SNSs and routine SNS use (Utz et al., 2012). Based on SDT (Deci &
Ryan, 1985; Ryan & Deci, 2000) and evidence from previous studies (Utz et al., 2012; Winter et
al., 2014), we hypothesize that:
H1: Adolescents’ need to belong is positively associated with adolescents’ Facebook use.
H2: Adolescents’ need for popularity is positively associated with adolescents’ Facebook
use.
1.2. The Mediating Role of Adolescents’ Fear of Missing Out
One of the mechanisms that has been proposed to link individuals’ social needs with their
engagement on SNSs is individuals’ fear of missing out (FoMO; Przybylski et al., 2013).
Individuals who fear to miss out on social opportunities want to stay continually connected with
others and updated about what others are doing (Przybylski et al., 2013). Based on SDT (Deci &
Ryan, 1985; Ryan & Deci, 2000), Przybylski and colleagues (2013) argue that deficits in
psychological need satisfaction may predict a fear of missing out. Adolescents’ fear to miss out
on events, in turn, likely drives their Facebook use, as Facebook enables adolescents to stay
continually connected with their peers (Shapiro & Margolin, 2013).
In line with these assumptions, studies have found empirical evidence for the role of
FoMO as the mediating mechanism that explains the relationships of adults’ and college
students’ needs with their engagement in social media (Alt, 2015; Przybylski et al., 2013). For
instance, in a sample of adults, Przybylski and colleagues (2013) found that FoMO was the
mechanism linking adults’ need satisfaction with social media engagement. More specifically,
adults who experienced less need satisfaction experienced higher overall levels of FoMO, which,
in turn, was associated with seeking out social media engagement. More recently, Alt (2015)
FOMO, SOCIAL NEEDS, FACEBOOK USE, AND FACEBOOK STRESS 7
confirmed the evidence for the mediating role of FoMO, as she found support for a mediating
role of FoMO in the relationship between college students’ academic motivations and social
media engagement.
In line with the premises of SDT (Deci & Ryan, 1985; Ryan & Deci, 2000) and based on
evidence accumulated by previous studies (Alt, 2015; Przybylski et al., 2013), it can be assumed
that FoMO mediates the relationships between adolescents’ social needs and Facebook use.
Therefore, we hypothesize that:
H3: Adolescents’ fear of missing out (FoMO) mediates the relationships of need to
belong and need for popularity with Facebook use.
1.3. The Relationships of Adolescents’ Fear of Missing Out With Adolescents’ Facebook
Related Stress
According to the social selection hypothesis (Hurst, 2007), emotional problems
negatively affect individuals’ social position or factors associated with one’s social position,
including stressful life events. In line with this hypothesis, Kim, Conger, Elder, and Lorenz
(2011) found that anxiety symptoms predicted higher levels of stressful life events among
adolescents. Based on the premises of the social selection hypothesis (Hurst, 2007) and
empirical evidence (e.g., Kim et al., 2011), we expect that FoMO may predict higher levels of
perceived stress related to Facebook use among adolescents.
First, anxiety is an important component of FoMO, as FoMO refers to fears, worries, and
anxieties people have about missing out (Przybylski et al., 2013). In this respect, Rosen,
Whaling, Carrier, Cheever, and Rokkum (2013) found support for a relationship between anxiety
and not being able to check in with various technologies. In addition, Cheever, Rosen, Carrier,
and Chavez (2014) found an association between anxiety and not being able to use wireless
FOMO, SOCIAL NEEDS, FACEBOOK USE, AND FACEBOOK STRESS 8
mobile devices. Because FoMO is closely related to anxiety and anxiety is often associated with
feelings of stress among adolescents (e.g., Kim et al., 2011), it is very likely that adolescents’
FoMO is related with higher levels of perceived stress.
Second, the feelings of stress that adolescents likely experience when they feel that they
do not belong to their peers or feel that they are not popular among their peers (Frankel, 1990;
Leary, 1990), may be particularly salient on SNSs, since adolescents can easily be socially
excluded by their peers on SNSs (e.g., not being invited by peers to an upcoming event on
Facebook) or not being perceived as popular by their peers on SNSs (e.g., receiving no or few
likes on a Facebook post) (e.g., Fox & Moreland, 2015). For adolescents, being excluded from
interactions with their peers and being neglected by peers is stressful (Frankel, 1990) and creates
feelings of stress (Leary, 1990). Therefore, feelings of missing out from peer interactions can be
perceived as very stressful for adolescents. As such, adolescents’ FoMO likely increases
adolescents’ stress related to using Facebook, in particular stress due to not belonging to peers on
Facebook and stress due to not being popular on Facebook.
No study thus far has investigated the relationships between FoMO and perceived stress
related to a Facebook context. However, previous studies have reported a link between
individuals’ (fear of) missing out and perceived stress related to an offline context (e.g.,
Australian Psychological Society, 2015; Chiou et al., 2015; Jones, 2014). For instance, in
experiments involving Facebook users, Chiou and colleagues (2015) found that thinking about
the unavailability of SNSs intensified distress due to being socially excluded. In line with these
findings, Jones (2014) found that students reported to feel stressed when they did not carry their
cell phones with them. In line with several indications in the literature (e.g., Fox & Moreland,
FOMO, SOCIAL NEEDS, FACEBOOK USE, AND FACEBOOK STRESS 9
2015; Rosen et al., 2013), and based on the empirical evidence provided by previous studies
(e.g., Chiou et al., 2015; Jones, 2014), we expect that:
H4: Adolescents’ fear of missing out (FoMO) is positively associated with perceived
stress related to Facebook use.
2. Method
2.1. Participants and Procedure
We conducted a cross-sectional study among adolescents. A total of 402 adolescents
(43% boys) participated in the study and completed a paper-and-pencil survey. Completion of
the survey took approximately 20 minutes. Study participants were recruited from different high
schools in Belgium offering different educational levels, including general education (i.e.,
education preparing students for college or university education), technical education (i.e.,
education with a focus on technical skills), and vocational education (i.e., education preparing
students for specific professions). Students in grades nine to twelve were eligible to participate
in the study. As is customary in Belgium, informed consent for the study was received from the
school principals.
After school principals had granted permission to participate in the study, a trained
research assistant visited the school and distributed paper surveys. Participants were informed
that the study was anonymous and were guaranteed that all responses to the survey would be
processed confidentially. All participants completed the survey at school during regular school
hours. Students who completed the survey were offered a chance to win a raffle. On average,
participants were 16.41 years old (SD = 1.43 years). A total of 371 adolescents had a Facebook
account (92.29%).
FOMO, SOCIAL NEEDS, FACEBOOK USE, AND FACEBOOK STRESS 10
2.2 Measures
Need to belong. Adolescents’ need to belong was measured with the Need to Belong
Scale (NTBS; Leary, Kelly, Cottrell, & Schreindorfer, 2013). Adolescents indicated their
agreement with the ten items of the NTBS, including “I want other people to accept me,” “I have
a strong need to belong,” and “It bothers me a great deal when I am not included in other
people’s plans” (Cronbach’s α = .83; M = 3.19, SD = .76). Response options ranged from
strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (5). Confirmatory factor analysis indicated that seven
items of the original scale loaded on the need to belong factor and showed that the construct
validity of the scale was good (CFI = .95, RMSEA = .05, SRMR = .05). Three items that did not
load on the need to belong factor were removed from the scale.
Need for popularity. Adolescents’ need for popularity was assessed using 11 items of
the popularity scale of Santor and colleagues (2000), including “I have done things to make me
more popular, even when it meant doing something I would not usually do,” “It’s important that
people think I’m popular,” and “At times, I’ve hung out with some people, so others wouldn’t
think I was unpopular” (Cronbach’s α = .91; M = 2.15, SD = .76). Participants rated their
responses on a scale ranging from strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (5). Confirmatory
factor analysis indicated that all items loaded on the need for popularity factor and showed that
the construct validity of the scale was very good (CFI = .98, RMSEA = .03, SRMR = .03).
Fear of missing out. The Fear of Missing Out scale (FoMOs; Przybylski et al., 2013)
was used to measure adolescents’ fear of missing out. Items included “I get anxious when I
don’t know what my friends are up to” and “It bothers me when I miss an opportunity to meet up
with friends”. Two highly similar items of the original scale (i.e., “I fear others have more
rewarding experiences than me” and “I fear my friends have more rewarding experiences than
FOMO, SOCIAL NEEDS, FACEBOOK USE, AND FACEBOOK STRESS 11
me”) were combined in one item, resulting in a scale with nine items. This version of the scale
turned out to be reliable (Cronbach’s α = .84; M = 2.50, SD = .76). Confirmatory factor analysis
indicated that all items loaded on the fear of missing out factor and showed that the construct
validity of the scale was very good (CFI = .99, RMSEA = .02, SRMR = .03). Participants rated
the nine items on a scale ranging from strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (5).
Facebook use. The Facebook Intensity Scale (Ellison, Steinfield, & Lampe, 2007) was
used to measure adolescents’ Facebook use. The scale comprises three measures: 1) the number
of ‘Facebook friends’, 2) the amount of time spent on Facebook, and 3) a series of six attitude
items, including items such as “Facebook has become part of my daily routine”, “I feel out of
touch when I haven’t logged onto Facebook for a while”, and “I would be sorry if Facebook shut
down”, that were assessed using response options ranging from strongly disagree (1) to strongly
agree (5). Following Ellison and colleagues’ (2007) approach, we first standardized the items of
the Facebook Intensity scale before averaging all items to create a total score, because of the
different scale ranges of the three measures. Confirmatory factor analysis indicated that all items
loaded on the Facebook use factor and showed that the construct validity of the scale was very
good (CFI = .97, RMSEA = .03, SRMR = .04). The scale showed good reliability (Cronbach’s α
= .78).
Perceived stress related to Facebook use. A perceived stress related to Facebook use
scale was specifically developed for this study to measure the extent to which adolescents
experience stress related to feedback from peers in the context of their Facebook use. Two types
of perceived stress related to Facebook use were measured. First, perceived stress due to not
belonging on Facebook was assessed using two items. Items included “Not being invited for a
Facebook group” and “Not being invited for an event through Facebook”. Participants rated
FOMO, SOCIAL NEEDS, FACEBOOK USE, AND FACEBOOK STRESS 12
their responses on a scale ranging from not stressful at all (1) to extremely stressful (5). The
scale showed good reliability (Cronbach’s α = .80; M = 2.02, SD = .95). Second, perceived
stress due to not being popular on Facebook was assessed using three items. Items included
“Receiving little or no ‘likes’ on a post, picture, or video that I posted on my timeline” and
“Receiving no reactions on a post, picture, or video that I posted on my timeline”. Participants
rated their responses on a scale ranging from not stressful at all (1) to extremely stressful (5).
The scale showed good reliability (Cronbach’s α = .82; M = 2.66, SD = 1.10). Confirmatory
factor analysis confirmed the two-factor structure of the perceived stress related to Facebook use
measures, with two items loading on the perceived stress due to not belonging factor and three
items loading on the perceived stress due to not being popular factor and showed that the
construct validity of the scales was very good (CFI = .99, RMSEA = .03, SRMR = .02). Both
types of stress were included separately in the models.
Control variables. Respondents indicated their gender (1 = boys, 2 = girls) and date of
birth, which we converted into age. Gender and age were included in the model as control
variables. In particular, gender was controlled for by including paths from gender to FoMO,
Facebook use, and the two measures concerning perceived stress related to Facebook use; age
was controlled for by including paths from age to Facebook use and the two measures
concerning perceived stress related to Facebook use.
2.3. Analyses
We tested the hypothesized relationships with Structural Equation Modeling (SEM) by
using Amos (Arbuckle, 2010). The goodness of fit of the models was determined using the ratio
of chi-square to degrees of freedom ²/df), the comparative fit index (CFI), the root mean square
error of approximation (RMSEA), and the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR).
FOMO, SOCIAL NEEDS, FACEBOOK USE, AND FACEBOOK STRESS 13
Indirect effects were computed by calculating the product of the relevant standardized path
coefficients (Cohen & Cohen, 1983). To examine the significance of the indirect effects, we
conducted bootstrapping analysis for indirect effects (Arbuckle, 2010) based on 1,000 bootstrap
resamples and a 95% bias-corrected confidence interval.
In order to account for missing values, multiple imputation was performed before
proceeding with the bootstrapping method (Honaker & King, 2010) using the SPSS multiple
imputation procedure to impute missing values. In the multiple imputation procedure, multiple
data sets were produced in which the missing values were imputed based on the data available.
The fully conditional specification (FCS) method was applied, which is an iterative Markov
chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) algorithm that uses all other available variables to fit a univariate
model and imputes missing values. The imputed values were saved to the imputed dataset after
10 iterations were performed (i.e., the maximum number of iterations reached).
3. Results
On average, 33.7% of the adolescents (fully) agreed with the items concerning the need
to belong and 4.7% of the adolescents (fully) agreed with the items concerning the need for
popularity. Approximately one in ten adolescents (fully) agreed with the items concerning
FoMO (8.8%). On average, adolescents reported that they had 400 to 500 friends on Facebook
(M = 4.94, SD = 2.56) and spent one to two hours using Facebook a day, on average (M = 4.48,
SD = 2.64). On average, adolescents did not agree nor disagree with the items assessing their
attitude towards Facebook (M = 2.75, SD = .81), but one in five adolescents (strongly) agreed
with the attitude items (20.1%). Approximately one in ten adolescents reported that the feeling
of not belonging to their peers on Facebook was stressful to extremely stressful (9.3%) and one
FOMO, SOCIAL NEEDS, FACEBOOK USE, AND FACEBOOK STRESS 14
in four adolescents indicated that not being popular on Facebook was stressful to extremely
stressful (24.8%).
Zero-order correlations of all variables included in the model are provided in Table 1.
Adolescents’ age was negatively related to both adolescents’ need to belong and need for
popularity, indicating that these needs decrease with age. Adolescents’ perceived stress due to
not being popular on Facebook also decreased with adolescents’ age. Furthermore, girls reported
higher levels of need to belong, fear of missing out, and perceived stress due to not being popular
on Facebook. In addition, strong positive relationships were found between adolescents’ need
for popularity, need to belong, fear of missing out, Facebook use, perceived stress due to not
being popular on Facebook, and perceived stress due to not belonging on Facebook.
[TABLE 1]
Structural equation modeling showed that the hypothesized model yielded a good fit,
χ²(3915) = 7327.96, χ²/df = 1.872, p < .001, CFI = .90, RMSEA = .02, SRMR = .06. In the first
part of the model, which comprises the relationships among social needs, FoMO, and Facebook
use (see Figure 1), the direct relationships between adolescents’ social needs and Facebook use
were not significant (β = .02, B = .01, SE = .03, p = .87 for need to belong; β = .02, B = .01, SE
= .03, p = .87 for need for popularity). Thus, H1 and H2 were not supported.1
[FIGURE 1]
However, the results indicated an indirect relationship between adolescents’ social needs
and Facebook use through FoMO (see Figure 1). First, adolescents’ social needs were
significantly associated with FoMO. More specifically, the results indicated that an increased
need to belong (β = .43, B = .50, SE = .08, p < .001) and an increased need for popularity (β
= .50, B = .51, SE = .07, p < .001) were strongly associated with an increased fear of missing out.
FOMO, SOCIAL NEEDS, FACEBOOK USE, AND FACEBOOK STRESS 15
Need to belong and need for popularity collectively explained 69% of the variance in FoMO (
= .69). Second, fear of missing out was significantly associated with Facebook use (β = .50, B
= .15, SE = .06, p < .01). More specifically, the results indicated that adolescents who
experienced more fear of missing out, reported a higher Facebook use. Bootstrapping analyses
revealed that the relationships of adolescents’ need to belong (.22 = .43 x .50; CI = .11 – .35) and
need for popularity (.25 = .50 x .50; CI = .12 – .40) with Facebook use were significantly
mediated by FoMO. As such, H3 was supported. Adolescents’ need to belong, need for
popularity, and fear of missing out collectively explained 26% of the variance in Facebook use
( = .26).
In the second part of the model, which comprises the relationships among FoMO and
perceived stress of social exclusion on Facebook, several significant relationships were found as
well (see Figure 1). In particular, the results showed that adolescents’ fear of missing out was
significantly associated with perceived stress of social exclusion on Facebook. More
specifically, we found that an increased FoMO was significantly associated with increased stress
due to not belonging = .41, B = .54, SE = .17, p < .001) and increased stress due to not being
popular (β = .41, B = .59, SE = .16, p < .001). Thus, H4 was supported.
The predictors in the model collectively explained 63% of the variance in stress due to
not being popular ( = .63) and 45% of the variance in stress due to not belonging on Facebook
( = .45).
4. Discussion
The aim of the present study was to investigate the relationships among adolescents’
social needs, fear of missing out (FoMO), Facebook use, and perceived stress related to their use
of Facebook. In a cross-sectional study among 402 adolescents, we tested an integrative model
FOMO, SOCIAL NEEDS, FACEBOOK USE, AND FACEBOOK STRESS 16
that examines (1) the mediating role of FoMO in the relationships between need to belong and
need for popularity and adolescents’ Facebook use and (2) the relationships between FoMO and
adolescents’ perceived stress related to their use of Facebook. Structural equation modeling
revealed several strong relationships.
4.1. The Mediating Role of Adolescents’ Fear of Missing Out
First, the present study increases our understanding of how adolescents’ social needs are
related to their Facebook use. In particular, the results indicate that a strong need to belong and
need for popularity is associated with increased Facebook use. An explanation for this
relationship that has emerged from this study involves adolescents’ FoMO. More specifically,
the results indicate that adolescents’ need to belong and need for popularity were related with
more FoMO, which, in turn, was associated with increased Facebook use. As such, the present
study highlights the importance of FoMO in explaining adolescents’ Facebook use and extends
prior research that focused on the role of FoMO among college students and adults (e.g., Alt,
2015; Przybylski et al., 2013).
By investigating the mediating role of FoMO, the present study offers a more
comprehensive understanding of the associations between adolescents’ social needs and
Facebook use. Interestingly, the results indicated that when FoMO was taken into consideration,
the direct associations of adolescents’ social needs with their Facebook use disappeared: While
we did find support for the direct associations between adolescents’ social needs and their
Facebook use when FoMO was not considered1, no support was found for these direct
associations in the integrative model that we tested. This finding highlights the importance of
FoMO as an explanatory mechanism in the association between adolescents’ social needs and
Facebook use and emphasizes the importance of investigating mediation models.
FOMO, SOCIAL NEEDS, FACEBOOK USE, AND FACEBOOK STRESS 17
In addition, although not hypothesized by the theoretical assumptions driving this study,
the results of our study suggested that FoMO plays a mediating role in the relationships of
adolescents’ need to belong and need for popularity with adolescents’ perceived stress due to not
belonging and not being popular on Facebook. That is, adolescents with a stronger need to
belong and need for popularity experienced more FoMO, which, in turn, increased adolescents’
perceived stress when being unpopular on Facebook and when not belonging on Facebook.
Additional post hoc bootstrapping analyses revealed that the relationships of adolescents’ need to
belong and need for popularity with perceived stress due to not belonging on Facebook (need to
belong: .18 = .43 x .41; CI = .07 – .30; need for popularity: .21 = .50 x .41; CI = .09 – .35) and
not being popular on Facebook (need to belong: .18 = .43 x .41; CI = .08 – .29; need for
popularity: .21 = .50 x .41; CI = .08 – .37) were significantly mediated by FoMO.
Individuals’ FoMO represents a growing but relatively understudied phenomenon,
especially among adolescents. The possibility to socialize with peers through social media
anywhere and anytime that is inherent to today’s mobile media environment likely increases the
prevalence of FoMO among adolescents. That is, the constant access to social media and the
possibility to be continuously connected with others may further feed adolescents’ FoMO. As
such, a vicious circle may develop in which FoMO may drive people toward social media,
which, in turn, may further increase adolescents’ FoMO. Research is needed to test this idea of
reinforcing spirals (Slater, 2007) in the relationship between adolescents’ FoMO and social
media use.
Also, the effects that FoMO may have on adolescents’ behavior, well-being, and health,
through affecting adolescents’ media use, warrant further research. For instance, indications
exist that adolescents may lose sleep or develop unhealthy eating patterns out of FoMO, because
FOMO, SOCIAL NEEDS, FACEBOOK USE, AND FACEBOOK STRESS 18
they increasingly feel a need to use media, likely out of FoMO. Adolescents’ nighttime media
use (Oshima et al., 2012; Van den Bulck, 2007) may be partially rooted in adolescents’ FoMO, as
research has shown that adolescents report keeping their mobile phones under their pillows in
order to avoid missing messages at night (Lenhart, Ling, Campbell, & Purcell, 2010). In
addition, adolescents may develop patterns of media-induced meal skipping or fast eating (Van
den Bulck & Eggermont, 2006) due to FoMO. That is, adolescents who fear to miss out on
events may skip a meal or eat faster so that they can connect with their peers through their media
devices and cope with their FoMO. As such, adolescents’ FoMO may be an unexplored
explanation for adolescents’ poor sleep hygiene and sleep problems, as well as their media-
induced meal skipping or fast eating. Research is needed that tests these assumptions.
4.2. Fear of Missing Out and Adolescents’ Perceived Stress Related to their Facebook Use
Furthermore, the present study increases our understanding of the relationship between
adolescents’ FoMO and perceived stress related to their Facebook use. In particular, the results
showed that adolescents’ FoMO was related with increased perceived stress due to not belonging
to their peers on Facebook and not being popular among their peers on Facebook.
As such, the current study adds to our understanding of the role of peer feedback on
SNSs. Providing and receiving peer feedback is an important aspect of SNSs, and Facebook in
particular. Facebook is oriented toward commenting and liking other users’ messages, pictures,
and videos. From prior research we know that, for the largest part, adolescents receive positive
feedback from their peers on SNSs, and only to a limited extent negative feedback (e.g.,
Koutamanis, Vossen, & Valkenburg, 2015; Valkenburg, Peter, & Schouten, 2006). While
receiving positive (Frison & Eggermont, 2015; Lee, Kim, & Ahn, 2014) and negative
(Valkenburg, Peter, & Schouten, 2006) peer feedback both affect adolescents’ well-being, the
FOMO, SOCIAL NEEDS, FACEBOOK USE, AND FACEBOOK STRESS 19
findings of the current study indicate that not receiving peer feedback affects adolescents’ well-
being as well. That is, not receiving any comments or likes from peers on messages, pictures, or
videos that adolescents post on Facebook increases feelings of stress among adolescents. Since
adolescents are highly sensitive to feedback from their peers (Thomaes, Reijntjes, Orobio de
Castro, Bushman, Poorthuis, & Telch, 2010), it seems that both receiving peer feedback (i.e.,
positive and negative) and not receiving peer feedback affect adolescents’ well-being.
Together, the present study is among the first to identify FoMO as a correlate of
perceived stress related to the use of Facebook among adolescents. Our findings show that
adolescents’ FoMO is associated not only with an increased use of Facebook, but also with
feelings of stress related to this Facebook use. However, future research is needed that
investigates other potential correlates of adolescents’ experiences of stress related to the use of
SNSs, since the prevalence rate of Facebook related stress among adolescents was relatively
high, in particular stress related to not being perceived as popular on Facebook, with one in four
adolescents indicating that not being popular on Facebook was stressful to extremely stressful
(24.8%). These high rates represent a cause for concern and call for further research that
investigates determinants of perceived stress specific to the use of SNSs and Facebook. For
instance, research is needed that identifies which adolescents are more susceptible to experience
Facebook induced stress, looking at dispositional, developmental, and social susceptibility
factors (Valkenburg & Peter, 2013).
4.3. Limitations and Directions for Future Research
While the present study improves our understanding of the associations among
adolescents’ social needs, FoMO, Facebook use, and feelings of stress related to Facebook use,
the study is not without limitations. First, the cross-sectional study design prevents us from
FOMO, SOCIAL NEEDS, FACEBOOK USE, AND FACEBOOK STRESS 20
making causal inferences. Future studies using longitudinal data are needed to investigate the
causal order of the relationships between adolescents’ social needs and FoMO, and adolescents’
FoMO and perceived stress related to Facebook use.
Second, the generalizability of our findings beyond those studied is limited, as the present
study relied on a relatively small sample. We therefore recommend that future research examine
these relationships in a nationally representative sample to complement the data provided by the
present study. However, although the sample was relatively small in size, the relationships
observed in this study were particularly strong.
Third, the Facebook intensity scale (Ellison et al., 2007) that was used to asses
adolescents’ Facebook use may not fully capture the diversity of adolescents’ Facebook use.
While the Facebook intensity scale has been used and validated in a wide range of previous
studies that investigate young people’s Facebook use (e.g., Jenkins-Guarnieri, Wright, &
Hudiburgh, 2012; Steinfield, Ellison, & Lampe, 2008), scholars (e.g., Frison & Eggermont,
2015) have emphasized the need to differentiate between specific Facebook activities.
Therefore, research is needed that investigates the associations among adolescents’ social needs
and different Facebook activities, as different Facebook activities may differently relate to
adolescents’ social needs. For instance, adolescents’ need to belong, which reflects the need to
develop and maintain significant and stable interpersonal relationships (Baumeister & Leary,
1995), may be particularly associated with sending private messages through the private
Facebook chat environment. Adolescents’ need for popularity, which reflects the need to
increase one’s popularity, may be particularly associated with posting status updates, pictures,
and videos in the public Facebook environment, because adolescents may receive likes for these
public messages, which likely increases their popularity.
FOMO, SOCIAL NEEDS, FACEBOOK USE, AND FACEBOOK STRESS 21
Fourth, the present study is limited by the fact that we examined the relationship between
FoMO and adolescents’ perceived stress related to Facebook use, rather than focusing on general
perceived stress in life or other well-being indicators. Although we may expect similar outcomes
of stress perceived in an online and offline context, as Facebook may offer users a new context to
perceive and experience stress (e.g., Fox & Moreland, 2015; Morin-Major et al., 2016), this is
only an assumption. No study thus far has examined the health correlates or outcomes of
perceiving stress in a Facebook context. We therefore recommend that future studies examine
the association between adolescents’ FoMO and well-being (e.g., general life stress, anxiety,
depression) as well as empirically test our assumption that perceived stress related to the use of
Facebook is a precursor for negative well-being outcomes.
4.4. Conclusion
The present study contributes to our understanding of the role of FoMO in the associations
between adolescents’ social needs and Facebook use, and the relationship between adolescents’
FoMO and perceived stress related to their Facebook use. The present study shows, on the one
hand, that FoMO mediates the relationships between social needs (i.e., need to belong and need
for popularity) and adolescents’ Facebook use, and on the other hand that FoMO is positively
related with adolescents’ perceived stress related to the use of Facebook. Based on these results,
we can conclude that the impact of FoMO for adolescents may not be underestimated. The fact
that adolescents have access to an accumulating set of SNSs, through an ever increasing range of
platforms, including laptops, tablet computers, and smartphones, implies a near constant
connectivity to their peers through SNSs. This highly mediatized environment requires a clearer
understanding of the role of FoMO in adolescents’ lives and the experience of stress related to the
use of SNSs.
FOMO, SOCIAL NEEDS, FACEBOOK USE, AND FACEBOOK STRESS 22
References
Alt, D. (2015). College students’ academic motivation, media engagement and fear of missing
out. Computers in Human Behavior, 49, 111–119. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2015.02.057
Arbuckle, J. (2010). AmosTM 19 user’s guide. Chicago: Amos Development Corporation.
Arnett, J. J. (1995). Adolescents’ uses of media for self-socialization. Journal of Youth and
Adolescence, 24, 519533. doi:10.1007/BF01537054
Arnett, J. J. (1999). Adolescent storm and stress, reconsidered. American Psychologist, 5, 317–
326. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.54.5.317
Australian Psychological Society (2015). The national stress and well-being in Australia survey.
Melbourne, Australia.
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal
attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529.
doi:10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497
Bokhorst, C. L., Sumter, S. R., & Westenberg, P. M. (2010). Social support from parents, friends,
classmates, and teachers in children and adolescents aged 9 to 18 years: Who is perceived as
most supportive? Social Development, 19, 417–426. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2009.00540.x
Brown, B. B., & Larson, J. (2009). Peer relationships in adolescence. In R. M. Lerner & L.
Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology (pp. 74–103). New York: Wiley.
Byrne, D. G., Davenport, S. C., & Mazanov, J. (2007). Profiles of adolescent stress: The
development of the adolescent stress questionnaire (ASQ). Journal of Adolescence, 30,
393–416. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2006.04.004
Cheever, N. A., Rosen, L. D., Carrier, L. M., & Chavez, A. (2014). Out of sight is not out of
mind: The impact of restricting wireless mobile device use on anxiety levels among low,
FOMO, SOCIAL NEEDS, FACEBOOK USE, AND FACEBOOK STRESS 23
moderate and high users. Computers in Human Behavior, 37, 290–297.
doi:10.1016/j.chb.2014.05.002
Chiou, W.-B., Lee, C.-C., & Liao, D.-C. (2015). Facebook effects on social distress: Priming
with online social networking thoughts can alter the perceived distress due to social
exclusion. Computers in Human Behavior, 49, 230–236. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2015.02.064
Christofides, E., Muise, A., & Desmarais, S. (2009). Information disclosure and control on
Facebook: Are they two sides of the same coin or two different processes?
Cyberpscyhology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 12, 341–345.
doi:10.1089/cpb.2008.0226
Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the
behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Compas, B. E., Orosan, P. G., & Grant, K. E. (1993). Adolescent stress and coping: Implications
for psychopathology during adolescence. Journal of Adolescence.
doi:10.1006/jado.1993.1028
Deci, E., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior.
New York: Plenum Press.
Ellison, N. B., Steinfield, C., & Lampe, C. (2007). The benefits of facebook “friends:” Social
capital and college students’ use of online social network sites. Journal of Computer-
Mediated Communication, 12, 1143–1168. doi:10.1111/j.1083-6101.2007.00367.x
Fox, J., & Moreland, J. J. (2015). The dark side of social networking sites: An exploration of the
relational and psychological stressors associated with Facebook use and affordances.
Computers in Human Behavior, 45, 168–176. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2014.11.083
FOMO, SOCIAL NEEDS, FACEBOOK USE, AND FACEBOOK STRESS 24
Frison, E., & Eggermont, S. (2015). Toward an integrated and differential approach to the
relationships between loneliness, different types of Facebook use and adolescents’
depressed mood. Communication Research. doi: 10.1177/0093650215617506
Gangadharbatla, H. (2008). Facebook me. Journal of Interactive Advertising, 8, 5–15.
doi:10.1080/15252019.2008.10722138
Honaker, J., & King, G. (2010). What to do about missing values in time-series cross-section
data. American Journal of Political Science, 54, 561–581.
Hurst, C. E. (2007). Social Inequality: Forms, Causes, and Consequences. Boston: Pearson
Education.
Jenkins-Guarnieri, M. A., Wright, S. L., & Hudiburgh, L. M. (2012). The relationships among
attachment style, personality traits, interpersonal competency, and Facebook use. Journal of
Applied Developmental Psychology, 33, 294–301. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2012.08.001
Jones, T. (2014). Students’ cell phone addiction and their opinions. The Elon Journal of
Undergraduate Research in Communications, 5, 74–80.
Kim, K. J., Conger, R. D., Elder, G. H., & Lorenz, F. O. (2011). Reciprocal influences between
stressful life events and adolescent internalizing and externalizing problems. Child
Development, 74, 127–143. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00525
Koutamanis, M., Vossen, H. G. M., & Valkenburg, P. M. (2015). Adolescents’ comments in
social media: Why do adolescents receive negative feedback and who is most at risk?
Computers in Human Behavior, 53, 486-494. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.07.016
Kross, E., Verduyn, P., Demiralp, E., Park, J., Lee, D. S., Lin, N., ... & Ybarra, O. (2013).
Facebook use predicts declines in subjective well-being in young adults. PloS one, 8,
e69841.
FOMO, SOCIAL NEEDS, FACEBOOK USE, AND FACEBOOK STRESS 25
Leary, M. R., Kelly, K. M., Cottrell, C. a, & Schreindorfer, L. S. (2013). Construct validity of the
need to belong scale: mapping the nomological network. Journal of Personality Assessment,
95, 610–624. doi:10.1080/00223891.2013.819511
Lee, C.-C., & Chiou, W.-B. (2013). Keep Logging In! Experimental evidence showing the
relation of affiliation needs to the idea of online social networking. Cyberpsychology,
Behavior, and Social Networking, 16, 419–422. doi:10.1089/cyber.2012.0544
Lee, E., Kim, Y. J., & Ahn, J. (2014). How do people use Facebook features to manage social
capital? Computers in Human Behavior, 36, 440–445. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2014.04.007
Lenhart, A. (2015). Teens, Social Media and Technology Overview 2015. Washington, DC: Pew
Internet & American Life Project.
Lenhart, A., Ling, R., Campbell, S., & Purcell, K. (2010). Teens and Mobile Phones.
Washington, DC: Pew Internet & American Life Project.
Morin-Major, J. K., Marin, M. F., Durand, N., Wan, N., Juster, R. P., & Lupien, S. J. (2016).
Facebook behaviors associated with diurnal cortisol in adolescents: Is befriending stressful?
Psychoneuroendocrinology, 63, 238–246.
Nadkarni, A., & Hofmann, S. G. (2012). Why do people use Facebook? Personality and
Individual Differences, 52, 243–249. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.11.007
Oshima, N., Nishida, A., Shimodera, S., Tochigi, M., Ando, S., Yamasaki, S., Okazaki, Y., &
Sasaki, T. (2012). The suicidal feelings, self-injury, and mobile phone use after lights out in
adolescents. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 37, 1023-1030. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jss072
Park, N., & Lee, S. (2014). College students’ motivations for Facebook use and psychological
outcomes. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 58, 601–620.
doi:10.1080/08838151.2014.966355
FOMO, SOCIAL NEEDS, FACEBOOK USE, AND FACEBOOK STRESS 26
Pombeni, M. L., Kirchler, E., & Palmonari, A. (1990). Identification with peers as a strategy to
muddle through the troubles of the adolescent years. Journal of Adolescence, 13, 351–369.
doi:10.1016/0140-1971(90)90029-7
Przybylski, A. K., Murayama, K., DeHaan, C. R., & Gladwell, V. (2013). Motivational,
emotional, and behavioral correlates of fear of missing out. Computers in Human Behavior,
29, 1841–1848. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2013.02.014
Reich, S., & Vorderer, P. (2013). Individual differences in need to belong in users of social
networking sites. In P. Moy (Ed.), Communication and Community (pp. 129–148). New
York, NY: Hampton PRess.
Rosen, L. D., Whaling, K., Carrier, L. M., Cheever, N. a, & Rokkum, J. (2013). The media and
technology usage and attitudes scale: An empirical investigation. Computers in Human
Behavior, 29, 2501–2511.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic
motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 68–78.
doi:10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.68
Sampasa-Kanyinga, H., & Lewis, R. F. (2015). Frequent use of social networking sites is
associated with poor psychological functioning among children and adolescents.
Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 18, 380–385.
doi:10.1089/cyber.2015.0055
Santor, D. A., Messervey, D., & Kusumakar, V. (2000). Measuring peer pressure, popularity, and
conformity in adolescent boys and girls: Predicting school performance, sexual attitudes,
and substance abuse. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 29, 163–182.
doi:10.1023/A:1005152515264
FOMO, SOCIAL NEEDS, FACEBOOK USE, AND FACEBOOK STRESS 27
Slater, M. D. (2007). Reinforcing spirals: The mutual influence of media selectivity and media
effects and their impact on individual behavior and social identity. Communication Theory,
17, 281-303. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2885.2007.00296.x
Shapiro, L. A. S., & Margolin, G. (2013). Growing up wired: Social networking sites and
adolescent psychosocial development. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 17,
1–18. doi:10.1007/s10567-013-0135-1
Steinberg, L. (2005). Adolescence. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Steinfield, C., Ellison, N. B., & Lampe, C. (2008). Social capital, self-esteem, and use of online
social network sites: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology,
29, 434–445. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2008.07.002
Suoninen, A. (2001). The role of media in peer group relations. In S. Livingstone & M. Bovill
(Eds.), Children and their changing media environment: A European comparative study (pp.
201–219). London: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Thomaes, S., Reijntjes, A., Orobio de Castro, B., Bushman, B. J., Poorthuis, A., & Telch, M. J.
(2010). I like me if you like me: On the interpersonal modulation and regulation of
preadolescents’ state self-esteem. Child Development, 81, 811–825.
Turkle, S. (2011). Alone together: Why we expect more from technology and less from each
other. New York: Basic Books.
Utz, S., Tanis, M., & Vermeulen, I. (2012). It is all about being popular: The effects of need for
popularity on social network site use. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking,
15, 37–42. doi:10.1089/cyber.2010.0651
Valkenburg, P.M., & Peter, J. (2013). The differential susceptibility to media effects model.
Journal of Communication, 63, 221-243. doi: 10.1111/jcom.12024
FOMO, SOCIAL NEEDS, FACEBOOK USE, AND FACEBOOK STRESS 28
Valkenburg, P. M., Peter, J., & Schouten, A. P. (2006). Friend networking sites and their
relationship to adolescents’ well-being and social self-esteem. Cyberpsychology, Behavior
and Social Networking, 9, 584–590. doi:10.1089/cpb.2006.9.584
Van den Bulck, J. (2007). Adolescent use of mobile phones for calling and for sending text
messages after lights out: Results from a prospective cohort study with a one-year follow-
up. Sleep, 30, 1220-1223. doi: 10.1016/S0084-3954(08)79019-5
Van den Bulck, J., & Eggermont, S. (2006). Media use as a reason for meal skipping and fast
eating in secondary school children. Journal of Human Nutrition and Dietetics, 19, 91-100.
doi: 10.1111/j.1365-277X.2006.00683.x
Washburn-Ormachea, J. M., Hillman, S. B., & Sawilowsky, S. S. (2004). Gender and gender-role
orientation differences on adolescents’ coping with peer stressors. Journal of Youth and
Adolescence, 33, 31–40.
Winter, S., Neubaum, G., Eimler, S. C., Gordon, V., Theil, J., Herrmann, J., … Krämer, N. C.
(2014). Another brick in the Facebook wall – How personality traits relate to the content of
status updates. Computers in Human Behavior, 34, 194–202. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2014.01.048
FOMO, SOCIAL NEEDS, FACEBOOK USE, AND FACEBOOK STRESS 29
Footnote
1 In a structural equation model including need to belong, need for popularity, and
Facebook use (and excluding FoMO), the direct relationships of adolescents’ need to belong (β
= .23, B = .08, SE = .03, p < .05) and need for popularity (β = .26, B = .08, SE = .03, p < .01)
with Facebook use were significant. However, when FoMO was considered (i.e., in the full
structural equation model that was tested), the direct relationships of adolescents’ need to belong
and need for popularity with Facebook use were not significant.
FOMO, SOCIAL NEEDS, FACEBOOK USE, AND FACEBOOK STRESS 30
Table 1 Zero-Order Correlations
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
7.
8.
1. Age
1
.09
-.23***
-.19***
-.10
-.02
-.15**
-.04
2. Gender
1
-.02
.17**
.15**
.07
.22***
.01
3. Need for Popularity
1
.43***
.48***
.31***
.43***
.40***
4. Need to Belong
1
.53***
.24***
.54***
.41***
5. Fear of Missing Out
1
.50***
.88***
.74***
6. Facebook Use
1
.39***
.33***
7. Perceived Stress
Due to Not Being
Popular on Facebook
1
.50***
8. Perceived Stress
Due to Not Belonging
on Facebook
1
Note. +p < .10. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
FOMO, SOCIAL NEEDS, FACEBOOK USE, AND FACEBOOK STRESS 31
Figure 1. Observed structural equation model for the relationships among social needs (i.e.,
need for popularity and need to belong), fear of missing out, Facebook use, and perceived
stress related to Facebook use (i.e., perceived stress due to not being popular on Facebook
and perceived stress due to not belonging on Facebook).
Note. Path coefficients are presented in standardized form. Dashed lines represent
insignificant paths. Ovals represent latent constructs. For clarity of presentation, control
variables, observed indicators, and error terms are not shown.
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
Need for
Popularity
Need to
Belong
Fear of
Missing Out
Facebook Use
Perceived
Facebook Stress
Popularity
Perceived
Facebook Stress
Belonging
.50**
.50***
.43***
-.02
.01
.41***
.41***
... Moreover, the fear of missing out (FOMO) and 'nomophobia'the fear of being without mobile phone access (Floros et al., 2021) may exacerbate the discomfort in disconnecting. Individuals often fear social exclusion or a perceived decrease in popularity without constant connectivity (Beyens et al., 2016). Thus, we interpret 'effort expectancy' as the perceived difficulty in disconnecting from technology during travels. ...
... Despite being physically distant, individuals are socially present and relate via technology (Floros et al., 2021). This is further reinforced by fears of social exclusion and a potential decline in popularity if individuals disengage from technology (Beyens et al., 2016). Similarly, White and White (2007) highlighted that there is an omnipresent societal expectation to be continually connected. ...
... For instance, it is conjectured that collectivist individuals, given their social affiliations, might perceive lesser benefits from disconnecting digitally, thus affecting their performance expectancy and attitude towards digital-free tourism. Similarly, the effort expectancy of these individuals might be adversely affected as they could find it more difficult to disconnect from technology due to societal expectations of being constantly connected (Beyens et al., 2016;Floros et al., 2021). As a result, the following hypotheses are proposed: H 5a . ...
... The results also revealed a significant association between FoMO and PSMU, which is consistent with the findings of previous studies (Beyens, Frison, & Eggermont, 2016;Dempsey, O'Brien, Tiamiyu, & Elhai, 2019), and (Rozgonjuk et al., 2020). FoMO, the fear that others might experience rewarding events or opportunities without you, has been associated with PSMU (Alutaybi, Al-Thani, McAlaney, & Ali, 2020;Przybylski, Murayama, Dehaan, & Gladwell, 2013) and has been identified as a predictor of PSMU (Franchina, Vanden Abeele, van Rooij, Lo Coco, & De Marez, 2018). ...
... experiencing FoMO may be more likely to engage in the excessive use of social networks to stay constantly connected, updated, and informed about their other's activities (Beyens et al., 2016). Moreover, social networks platforms design triggers and feeds FoMO by employing various features, such as notifications and real-time information, that keep users constantly engaged and attached to their devices This interplay between FoMO and social media platform design can create a reinforcing cycle, making it difficult for individuals to disengage from social media, potentially leading to addictive behaviors (Fioravanti et al., 2021). ...
Article
Recent meta-analytical evidence indicates a mild association between higher neuroticism and lower conscientiousness scores and a tendency towards problematic social media use (PSMU). However, fear of missing out (FoMO) has emerged as a critical variable in understanding the positive link between neuroticism and PSMU. Given the replication crisis in psychology, this study aimed to reinvestigate personality-PSMU associations and, crucially, the less-studied FoMO mediation effect. To ensure generalizability of the findings, we recruited two diverse samples with European and Arabian backgrounds. The results revealed a significant total effect of neuroticism on PSMU for both cultural groups, with the European sample demonstrating a fully mediated effect via FoMO, whereas the Arab sample showed a partially mediated effect via FoMO, along with a significant direct effect. This study contributes to the existing literature by highlighting the importance of FoMO as a mediator between neuroticism and PSMU and some minor potential cultural differences in this association.
... According to Zhang and Mani (2021), positive market shocks have a bigger influence on the volatility of these financial assets than negative shocks of equal magnitude due to the FOMO effect. Some studies have shown that women experience more FOMO than men (Stead and Bibby 2017;Beyens et al. 2016), although Przybylski et al. (2013) discovered the contrary. Thus, the fourth hypothesis of the study is: H 4 FOMO has a significant influence on crypto investment decisions. ...
Article
Full-text available
The study's purpose is to examine the effect of herding, loss aversion, overconfidence, and fear of missing out (FOMO) biases on crypto investors’ investment decisions. The study also looks at how FOMO plays a mediating role between herding, loss aversion, overconfidence, and crypto investment decisions. To acquire data from crypto retail investors, the study used a questionnaire survey. A total of 473 responses were gathered and analyzed with SmartPLS. To achieve the study's aims, factor analysis and partial least square structural equation modelling were used. The study's findings found that FOMO, herding, loss aversion, and overconfidence biases have a substantial effect on the investment decisions of crypto investors, in respective order. In addition, FOMO bias establishes a complementary partial mediation on the relationship between herding, loss aversion, and crypto investors’ decision-making behavior. Ergo, the present study assisted individual and institutional cryptocurrency investors, crypto portfolio managers, policymakers, researchers, and market regulators in broadening their knowledge base about cryptocurrency and forecasting investors' behavior. Hence, this study contributes to the field of behavioral finance.
... According to recent studies (Beyens, Frison & Eggermont, 2016;Dempsey, O'Brien, Tiamiyu & Elhai, 2019;Wegmann et al., 2017;Zhang et al., 2020), FoMO has been proposed as a mediator variable in the usage of internet communication apps. Previous studies have revealed that FoMO mediates the relationship between social media interactions and individual differences such as needs, emotions, motivations (Alt, 2015;Przybylski et al., 2013;Wegmann et al., 2017). ...
Article
Full-text available
Second screen usage by fans while watching sports events on TV has been increasing in recent years. Although second screens are frequently used in two-way communication with fans and sports clubs, few studies have examined the subject from a theoretical perspective to understand it better. This study aims to determine whether the fear of missing out (FoMO [personal FoMO and social FoMO]) mediates the relationship between fan passion (harmonious passion and obsessive passion) and second screen usage through the dualistic model of passion (DMP) perspective. For that purpose, we conducted an online survey for data collection along with fans in Turkey and analysed 300 valid responses (79.3% male, aged 18-59 years) via structural equation modelling. The results showed that harmonious and obsessive passion had no direct effect on second screen usage. However, they had indirect effect on second screen usage through personal FoMO. In addition, personal FoMO had a prediction on second screen usage. As a result, this study highlights the importance of the second screen in the sports industry and the effect of fans' passion and FoMO levels on second screen usage from the DMP view. To our knowledge, the present study provides the first empirical evidence for the mediating role of FoMO in the relationship between fan passion and second screen use.
... In other words, FOMO is the anxiety about losing social interactions and meaningful experiences, especially on social media platforms (Alt & Boniel-Nissim, 2018). The FOMO concept usually goes along with low self-concept (Dutot, 2020), social anxiety (Zhang et al., 2020), the need to belong (Wang et al., 2019), the desire to stay in touch (Beyens et al., 2016), worry about losing social interaction (Przybylski et al., 2013), or the fear of falling behind (Good & Hyman, 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
Social media influencers, a new type of opinion leader, are considered followers’ role models and strongly affect their attitudes and behaviors. Although their power on social media platforms is an emerging phenomenon, the studies exploring the impact of influencers’ endorsement of products on their followers’ buying intentions are still limited. By employing 272 respondents exposed to influencers, this study used PROCESS models to test hypotheses. The results demonstrate that the desire to mimic influencers and the sense of materialism mediate the effect of influencers on followers’ buying intentions. In addition, fear of missing out (FOMO) plays a moderation role in these relationships by altering the impact of influencer exposure on materialism and buying intention. These findings contribute to the current literature on social media influencers, the desire to mimic, materialism, and FOMO and also yield suggestions for influencers and marketers in their strategies.
... In most of the research, the effect of social media (Strasburger et al., 2010, O'Keeffe and Clarke-Pearson, 2011, Kross et al., 2013, Beyens et al., 2016, Vannucci et al., 2017, Thomée et al., 2011, Cheever et al., 2014, Park and Park, 2014 on stress, sleep disorders, and various other social and psychological factors have been analyzed with a focus on children, young adults and university students. The effect of social media usage on stress in middle-aged adults is a comparatively less explored field. ...
Article
Full-text available
Screen media technologies (SMTs) has become an essential part of human life and almost everybody, irrespective of their age group, uses one or the other screen media technologies. Increased dependency on SMTs is raising concerns over their ill effect on the psychological health of its users. The present work aims to study the impact of social media usage and laptop/computer on psychological and physical health. This is a cross-sectional study of the middle management employees of a major Indian telecom organization. The analyses were carried out using structural equation modelling (SEM) approach. Results suggested that neck pain is directly related to cognitive stress, somatic stress, and laptop/computer usage. Cognitive stress was indirectly related to Instagram and WhatsApp use. Behavioural stress had no direct or indirect relationship with social media or laptop/computer use. Using a laptop/computer is found to be the most critical factor contributing to neck pain in Indian middle-aged adults working in an office environment.
... Para los adolescentes pertenecer a una RSV incide en su forma de relacionarse; así como, en la configuración de su identidad. De hecho, se ha encontrado que ante la necesidad de tener aprobación social o de mostrar determinados perfiles en línea se puede generar un uso problemático de las RSV (Hawk et al., 2019;Masanet et al., 2021;Marciano et al., 2022;Beyens et al., 2016) y que ante la falta de reconocimiento en estos escenarios virtuales se puede experimentar ansiedad y temor al rechazo (López de Ayala López et al., 2022). En el contexto virtual "surgen las figuras del otro sobre el cual tengo responsabilidades éticas: el alejado, el desconocido, el extraño" (Ure, 2017, p.193). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Las redes sociales virtuales se han establecido como nuevos espacios de interacción social durante las últimas dos décadas. La interacción en contextos virtuales como Facebook e Instagram incide en diferentes dimensiones de la vida de la población adolescente. Este estudio tiene como objetivo determinar el estado de la investigación acerca del proceso de interacción social entre adolescentes en las redes sociales virtuales mediante la revisión sistemática de la literatura. Se llevó a cabo la búsqueda bibliográfica por medio del protocolo PRISMA y fueron seleccionados cuarenta estudios empíricos en las bases de datos Scopus, Web of Science, SciELO y Dialnet, los cuales fueron publicados en el periodo 2011-2022. Se establecieron cinco categorías emergentes de las investigaciones analizadas: identidad, bienestar social, competencias socioemocionales, conductas de riesgo y comunicación. Como resultado se evidencia que la población adolescente interactúa en las redes sociales virtuales con el propósito de obtener reconocimiento social, consolidar lazos de amistad y desarrollar el sentido de pertenencia a un grupo; sin embargo, es posible que se vean expuestos a riesgos como el ciberacoso. Se concluye que es necesario ampliar la investigación acerca de las interacciones online entre adolescentes; así como la necesidad de fortalecer las competencias socioemocionales de los adolescentes en el contexto familiar y escolar para que puedan hacer un uso responsable de las redes sociales virtuales. Virtual social networks have been established as new social interaction spaces during the last two decades. The interaction in virtual settings as Facebook and Instagram influences adolescents´ life on different dimensions. The objective of this study is to establish the state of research about social interaction process among adolescents into the online social networks through a systematic literature review. The bibliographic search was carried out by means of PRISMA protocol and forty empirical studies, which were published between 2011-2022, were selected in the Scopus, Web of Science, SciELO and Dialnet databases. Five categories were established from the research: identity, social well-being, socio-emotional skills, risk behaviors and communication. As a result, it is evident that the adolescent population interact in virtual social networks with the purpose of obtaining social recognition, consolidating friendship ties and developing the sense of belonging to a group; however, they can be exposed to some risks such as cyberbullying. In conclusion, it is necessary to expand the research about online interactions among adolescents; as well as the need to strengthen the socio-emotional skills of adolescents into the family environment and school context for making responsible use of virtual social networks.
Article
Full-text available
Purpose of Review: This review delves into the intricate relationship between social media and adolescent mental health, scrutinizing underlying psychological mechanisms and proposing directions for future inquiry and interventions. Recent Findings: The interaction between social media use and adolescent mental health is complex. Many studies focus on the negative outcomes, such as increased depression, anxiety, and decreased life satisfaction, with contributing factors such as social comparison, fear of missing out, cyberbullying, and impaired sleep. However, the potential for positive influences through enhanced social support and information seeking is also recognized. Summary: The review reveals a multifaceted relationship between social media use and adolescent mental health, emphasizing that effects are shaped by how, why, when, and by whom social media are used. Ongoing research must consider design features of different social media "spaces" and the psychological states of users that mold the relationship to develop a more comprehensive understanding.
Article
Amaç: Bu araştırmanın amacı, spor bilimleri fakültesi öğrencilerinin zaman yönetiminin genel erteleme davranışı ve sosyal medya bağımlılığına etkisinin incelenmesidir. Yöntem: Araştırmanın örneklemini Bingöl Üniversitesi Spor Bilimleri Fakültesinde okuyan 194 kadın ve 198 erkek olmak üzere toplam 392 üniversite öğrencisi oluşturmaktadır. Araştırmada kişisel bilgi formunun yanı sıra “Zaman Yönetimi Ölçeği”, Genel Erteleme Ölçeği” ve “Sosyal Medya Bağımlılığı Ölçeği” kullanılmıştır. Elde edilen verilerin normalliği test edildikten sonra güvenirlik sonuçları incelenmiştir. Bu çalışmanın analizlerinde, değişkenler arasındaki ilişkiyi belirlemek için korelasyon analizi kullanılmış, ayrıca bağımsız değişkenin bağımlı değişken üzerindeki etkisini değerlendirmek amacıyla regresyon analizi uygulanmıştır. Bulgular: Araştırma bulguları öğrencilerin zaman yönetimleri ile sosyal medya bağımlılığı ve erteleme davranışları arasında istatistiksel olarak anlamlı ilişkiler saptanmıştır. Buna göre, zaman planı ile sosyal medya bağımlılığı (r: -0,170; p
Article
Full-text available
Although a variety of studies have examined the predictors or outcomes of adolescents’ social networking site use, these studies did not incorporate (1) an integrated, longitudinal approach to examine these relationships longitudinally in a single comprehensive model or (2) a differential approach to distinguish between different types of social networking site use. Therefore, this two-wave panel study (N = 1,612) developed an integrated and differential model to provide a deeper understanding of the relationships among loneliness, types of Facebook use, and adolescents’ depressed mood. Using structural equation modeling, the results point to the presence of a poor-get-richer effect regarding active public Facebook use but reveal a poor-get-poorer effect regarding passive Facebook use. The discussion focuses on the explanation and understanding of these findings.
Article
Full-text available
Abstract Receiving negative peer feedback in social media may have negative consequences for adolescents' psychosocial development and well-being. Therefore, the first aim of this study was to investigate online behavior (i.e., online social exploration, risky online self-presentation) that predicts receiving negative online peer feedback. The second aim was to examine three types of precursors that may predict this online behavior and, indirectly, negative feedback: (a) developmental (i.e., sex, age), (b) dispositional (i.e., sensation seeking, inhibitory control), and (c) social precursors (i.e., peer problems, family conflict). We collected survey data among 785 Dutch adolescents (10-15 years old). Our results showed that adolescents who engaged in online social exploration and risky online self-presentation more often, were more likely to receive negative peer feedback. Online social exploration was more prevalent among the older adolescents and adolescents characterized by higher sensation seeking and more family conflict. In addition, risky online self-presentation was more prevalent among adolescents high in sensation seeking. Consequently, these adolescents' online behavior, indirectly, made them more at risk of receiving negative peer feedback in social media.
Article
Full-text available
Social networking sites (SNSs) have gained substantial popularity among youth in recent years. However, the relationship between the use of these Web-based platforms and mental health problems in children and adolescents is unclear. This study investigated the association between time spent on SNSs and unmet need for mental health support, poor self-rated mental health, and reports of psychological distress and suicidal ideation in a representative sample of middle and high school children in Ottawa, Canada. Data for this study were based on 753 students (55% female; Mage=14.1 years) in grades 7-12 derived from the 2013 Ontario Student Drug Use and Health Survey. Multinomial logistic regression was used to examine the associations between mental health variables and time spent using SNSs. Overall, 25.2% of students reported using SNSs for more than 2 hours every day, 54.3% reported using SNSs for 2 hours or less every day, and 20.5% reported infrequent or no use of SNSs. Students who reported unmet need for mental health support were more likely to report using SNSs for more than 2 hours every day than those with no identified unmet need for mental health support. Daily SNS use of more than 2 hours was also independently associated with poor self-rating of mental health and experiences of high levels of psychological distress and suicidal ideation. The findings suggest that students with poor mental health may be greater users of SNSs. These results indicate an opportunity to enhance the presence of health service providers on SNSs in order to provide support to youth.
Book
Like past editions, this ninth edition of Social Inequality: Forms, Causes, and Consequences is a user-friendly introduction to the study of social inequality. This book conveys the pervasiveness and extensiveness of social inequality in the United States within a comparative context, to show how inequality occurs, how it affects all of us, and what is being done about it. This edition benefits from a variety of changes that have significantly strengthened the text. The authors pay increased attention to disability, transgender issues, intersectionality, experiences of Muslims, Hispanic populations, and immigration. The 9th edition also includes content on the fall-out from the recession across various groups. The sections on global inequalities have been greatly updated, emphasizing comparative inequalities and the impact of the process of globalization on inequality internationally. The authors have also added material on several current social movements, including Occupy Wall Street, Black Lives Matter, and Marriage Equality.
Book
I: Background.- 1. An Introduction.- 2. Conceptualizations of Intrinsic Motivation and Self-Determination.- II: Self-Determination Theory.- 3. Cognitive Evaluation Theory: Perceived Causality and Perceived Competence.- 4. Cognitive Evaluation Theory: Interpersonal Communication and Intrapersonal Regulation.- 5. Toward an Organismic Integration Theory: Motivation and Development.- 6. Causality Orientations Theory: Personality Influences on Motivation.- III: Alternative Approaches.- 7. Operant and Attributional Theories.- 8. Information-Processing Theories.- IV: Applications and Implications.- 9. Education.- 10. Psychotherapy.- 11. Work.- 12. Sports.- References.- Author Index.
Article
Based on the uses and gratifications framework, this study examined associations between motivations for using Facebook among college students, their concern for impression management, Facebook intensity, and psychological outcomes including sense of belonging and satisfaction with campus life. Using data from an online survey (N = 246), the study found that four motivations—entertainment, relationship maintenance, self-expression, and communication—together with impression management, were significantly associated with Facebook intensity. However, the impacts of Facebook intensity on psychological consequences were relatively limited. Theoretical implications of the study were discussed.
Article
Facebook(©) is changing the way people interact and socialize. Despite great interest in psychology and sociology, little is known about Facebook behaviors in relation to physiological markers of stress. Given that the brain undergoes important development during adolescence and that glucocorticoids-a major class of stress hormones-are known to modulate its development, it is important to study psychosocial factors that may influence secretion of stress hormones during adolescence. The goal of the present study was to explore the associations between Facebook behaviors (use frequency, network size, self-presentation and peer-interaction) and basal levels of cortisol among adolescent boys and girls. Eighty-eight adolescents (41 boys, 47 girls) aged between 12 and 17 (14.5±1.8) were recruited. Participants provided four cortisol samples per day for two non-consecutive weekdays. Facebook behaviors were assessed in accordance with the existing literature. Well-validated measures of perceived stress, perceived social support, self-esteem, and depressive symptoms were also included. A hierarchical regression showed that after controlling for sex, age, time of awakening, perceived stress, and perceived social support, cortisol systemic output (area under the curve with respect to ground) was positively associated with the number of Facebook friends and negatively associated with Facebook peer-interaction. No associations were found among depressive symptoms, self-esteem, and cortisol. These results provide preliminary evidence that Facebook behaviors are associated with diurnal cortisol concentrations in adolescents.
Article
The concerns about the consequences of mental problems related to use of social media among university students have recently raised consciousness about a relatively new phenomenon termed Fear of Missing Out (FoMO). Drawing on the self-determination theory and on the assumption that low levels of basic need satisfaction may relate to FoMO and social media engagement, the aim of the present research was to examine for the first time possible links between FoMO, social media engagement, and three motivational constructs: Intrinsic, extrinsic and amotivation for learning. Data were gathered from 296 undergraduate students by using the following scales: Social Media Engagement (SME), Fear of Missing Out (FoMOs) and Academic Motivation. The SME is a new scale, specifically designed for this study to measure the extent to which students used social media in the classroom. This scale includes three categories: Social engagement, news information engagement and commercial information engagement. Path analysis results indicated that the positive links between social media engagement and two motivational factors: Extrinsic and amotivation for learning are more likely to be mediated by FoMO. Interpretation of these results, their congruence within the context of the theoretical frameworks and practical implications are discussed.