ArticlePDF Available

Effects of Drinking Cows’ Milk at Breakfast in Promoting Sleep-Health in Japanese University Athletes

Canadian Center of Science and Education
International Journal of Psychological Studies
Authors:

Abstract and Figures

This study evaluates the effects of milk intake for 20 days at breakfast on diurnal type (chronotype), sleep habits and soccer performance in Japanese university male athletes attending a university soccer club. Seventy three athletes were asked to take 200 ml of cows’ milk at breakfast for 21 consecutive days during November and December, 2014. Twenty athletes attending the same soccer club did not drink cows’ milk for the same period of time and acted a control group. An integrated questionnaire was administered twice, before the intervention period and 1 month after it to all 93 participants. The questionnaire included questions on sleep habits and diurnal type. On the 10th day and 21st days of the intervention period, a questionnaire on performance/skill was administered to all participants. The group which drank cows’ milk showed higher frequency of improvement of soccer performance than did the control group did (performance—where higher values indicate less skill: milk drinking group=29.92, control group=31.9 on day 10; milk drinking group=28.21, control group=31.9 on day 21), and also judged that their soccer performance had improved more after 21 days than 10 days of the intervention. Those participants who changed diurnal type to becoming more morning-typed were more likely to judge that their soccer performance had improved than did those who showed no change in diurnal type.
Content may be subject to copyright.
International Journal of Psychological Studies; Vol. 8, No. 3; 2016
ISSN 1918-7211 E-ISSN 1918-722X
Published by Canadian Center of Science and Education
154
Effects of Drinking Cows’ Milk at Breakfast in Promoting
Sleep-Health in Japanese University Athletes
Takahiro Kawada1,2, Yuri Takamori1, Miyo Nakade3, Fujiko Tsuji1, 4, Milada Krejci4, Teruki Noji2, Hitomi
Takeuchi1 & Tetsuo Harada1
1 Laboratory of Environmental Physiology, Graduate School of Integrated Arts and Sciences, Kochi University,
Kochi, Japan
2 Center for Regional Collaboration, Kochi University, Kochi, Japan
3 Department of Nutritional Management, Faculty of Health and Nutrition, Tokai-Gakuen University, Japan
4 University of Physical Education and Sport Palestra, Prague, Czech Republic
Correspondences: Tetsuo Harada, Laboratory of Environmental Physiology, Graduate School of Integrated Arts
and Sciences, Kochi University, Kochi, 780-8520, Japan. Tel: 81-88-844-8410. E-mail: haratets@kochi-u.ac.jp
Received: April 13, 2016 Accepted: June 1, 2016 Online Published: August 3, 2016
doi:10.5539/ijps.v8n3p154 URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.5539/ijps.v8n3p154
This study was financially supported by the Research Foundation by Japan Dairy Association (J-MILK)
(2013-2014, 2016) (To T. HARADA).
Abstract
This study evaluates the effects of milk intake for 20 days at breakfast on diurnal type (chronotype), sleep habits
and soccer performance in Japanese university male athletes attending a university soccer club. Seventy three
athletes were asked to take 200 ml of cows’ milk at breakfast for 21 consecutive days during November and
December, 2014. Twenty athletes attending the same soccer club did not drink cows’ milk for the same period of
time and acted a control group. An integrated questionnaire was administered twice, before the intervention
period and 1 month after it to all 93 participants. The questionnaire included questions on sleep habits and
diurnal type. On the 10th day and 21st days of the intervention period, a questionnaire on performance/skill was
administered to all participants. The group which drank cows’ milk showed higher frequency of improvement of
soccer performance than did the control group did (performance—where higher values indicate less skill: milk
drinking group=29.92, control group=31.9 on day 10; milk drinking group=28.21, control group=31.9 on day
21), and also judged that their soccer performance had improved more after 21 days than 10 days of the
intervention. Those participants who changed diurnal type to becoming more morning-typed were more likely to
judge that their soccer performance had improved than did those who showed no change in diurnal type.
Keywords: Japanese athletes as university soccer club members, circadian typology, sleep habits, soccer
performance
1. Introduction
Nongonierma and FitzGerald (2015) described how cows’ milk proteins promoted human health in eight ways.
The first way was “Improving satiety and weight-management”, the second was “Reducing risk of heart disease”;
the third was “Having an antimicrobial role”, the fourth was “Having anti-inflammatory effects”, the fifth was
“Showing anticancer effects”, the sixth was “Exerting antioxidant effects”, the seventh was “Affecting insulin
secretion and serum glucose regulation” and the eighth was “An action upon muscle protein synthesis”. With
regard to the relationship between sleep health and the intake of cows’ milk, Laird and Drexel (1934) reported
that a meal of cornflakes and milk strongly improved sleep quality (as judged from an uninterrupted night’s
sleep). Later, Brezinova and Oswald (1972) showed, using electroencephalography, that sleep was significantly
improved (longer and un-interrupted night sleeps) in older people when they ate a combination of cows’ milk
and cereal before going to bed. However, there have been only a few studies on the effects of drinking cows’
milk in the morning on any improvement in sleep health. For example, Takeuchi et al. (2014) reported the two
results of a questionnaire study on Japanese infants aged 2-6 years old. The first result was that infants who ate
carbohydrate (or carbohydrate and protein) plus milk at breakfast were more of a morning type and showed
ijps.ccsenet.org International Journal of Psychological Studies Vol. 8, No. 3; 2016
155
better sleep quality than those who ate only carbohydrate (or carbohydrate and protein). The second one was that
infants who drank milk at breakfast were less frequently depressed than those who did not do so.
It has been suggested that protein intake at breakfast would raise blood tryptophan levels, and that this would,
through the synthesis of serotonin (an anti-depressive agent), be effective in promoting mental health and
morning-type habits and also, following synthesis into melatonin, which promotes sleep onset at night (Harada et
al., 2007; Nakade et al., 2009, 2012; Wada et al., 2013). For Japanese children, drinking cows’ milk at breakfast
is an important source of tryptophan and could, on the basis of a questionnaire study (Takeuchi et al., 2014), be
supposed to promote the mental and sleep health, although there have been no intervention studies to test this
hypothesis. However, a group of adult athletes (Japanese university soccer team members) was given a leaflet
titled: “Three benefits: Go to bed early! Get up early! Eat a nutritionally rich breakfast!” and then encouraged to
act upon this advice for a period of one month. It was observed that their soccer performance, sleep health and
mental health all improved after the intervention (Harada et al., 2016). It is possible that an increased intake of
cows’ milk at breakfast, as a source of tryptophan, could also improve soccer performance, sleep health and
mental health. However, there have been no intervention studies to test this possibility, and so the purpose of this
study is to test this hypothesis.
2. Participants and Methods
2.1 Participants
The study involved 93 participants, 60% of whom were from Shikoku Island, the others coming from all over
Japan. Two authors of this manuscript are the general manager and the coach of the soccer club of Kochi
University, and the subjects were recruited by them by personal contact and word of mouth. The participants
were 19-25 years old.
2.2 Design-1: Experimental and Control Groups
The experimental group (n=73) drank 200 ml of cows’ milk at breakfast each morning for a period of 21 days,
from 13th November, 2014, to 4th December, 2014. The milk was provided for them. The control group (n=20)
did not drink cows’ milk for breakfast during this period. The control group consists of men who do not like to
drink cows’ milk. There were no significant differences in age, diurnal type scale and sleep habits (wake-up time,
bedtime and sleep hours both in the weekdays and at the weekend) between the two groups. After the
questionnaires and sleep diaries had been distributed, participants were allowed to answer them at home.
2.3 Design-2: Contents of an Integrated Questionnaire
An integrated questionnaire (Harada et al., 2016) was administered twice, just before the intervention and one
month later, after the intervention. The integrated questionnaire included questions to determine diurnal type (7
questions, scored from 7 to 28, higher scores show morning-type: Torsvall and Åkerstedt (1980): in this occasion,
half number of participants were defined as evening-type ones who scored 7 to 16 and another half morning type
ones who scored 17 to 28), questions on sleep habits (sleep duration, bedtime, wakeup time, interruption of a
night’s sleep, difficulty in fall asleep and difficulty in waking up) and on mental health (inability to control their
emotions, irritation, anger and depressive mood). The participants were always free to choose these. The
questions on mental health had been validated in so far as the answers showed a significant positive correlation
with those from the General Health Questionnaire (GHQ) (Harada et al., unpublished). During the intervention
period, all participants kept a sleep diary. This included information on rising time, bedtime, time asleep, sleep
latency, time of waking, and number of times when sleep was interrupted during the night.
2.4 Design-3: Self-Estimation of Change in Soccer Performance during Intervention
On the 10th and 21st day of the intervention, participants answered a questionnaire on subjective estimates of
their change in soccer performance in comparison with the performance before the intervention (Harada et al.,
2016). This questionnaire had been validated before use by two authors of this manuscript, the general manager
of the soccer team at Kochi University and the head coach of the soccer team. Participants were asked not to play
soccer on these two days. The eleven questions (Table 1) were divided into three categories which were, first,
mental or psychological issue (Questions 1, 6 and 11), second, physiological function (Questions 2, 7 and 8) and
physical techniques (Questions 3, 4, 5, 9 and 10).
ijps.ccsenet.org International Journal of Psychological Studies Vol. 8, No. 3; 2016
156
Table 1. A questionnaire on the change in the performance between pre-intervention stage and (middle) point of
one month intervention. In comparison with the performance before the intervention, how has each of the
following 11 components of your soccer performance changed from beginning to the end of intervention month?
Please put the circle (check) on the appropriated numeral. If you have the other kind of change in soccer
performance and other changes in your life, please write down in the bottom space. (Harada et al., 2016)
Question 1: Assessment of the present situation, Question 2: Visual field on playing, Question 3: Movement of foot, Question 4:
Rudimentary mistake, Question 5:First touch, Question 6: Irritation on playing, Question 7: Running out of Stamina, Question
8:Injury on playing, Question 9: Body balance, Question 10: Precision of long kick, Question 11: Motivation for the practice
(Performance has been better) ◄▬▬▬▬▬▬▬▬▬▬▬►(Performance has been worse)
Question 1: faster faster a little no change slower a little slower
Question 2: wider wider a little no change narrower a little narrower
Question 3: lighter lighter a little no change heavier a little heavier
Question 4: reduced lighter a little no change increased a few increased
Question 5: better better a little no change worse a little worse
Question 6: reduced lighter a little no change increased a few increased
Question 7: reduced lighter a little no change increased a few increased
Question 8: reduced lighter a little no change increased a few increased
Question 9: better better a little no change worse a little worse
Question 10: better better a little no change worse a little worse
Question 11: better better a little no change worse a little worse
Category 1: Mental or psychological issues (Question 1, 6 and 11).
Category 2: Physiological function (Questions 2, 7 and 8).
Category 3: Physical techniques (Questions 3. 4. 5. 9 and 10).
2.5 Ethic Treatment
Before administrating the intervention study, each participant was given a written explanation that detailed the
concepts and purposes of the study and stated that their answers to the integrated questionnaire and the
questionnaire on soccer performance (Table 1) would be used only for academic purposes. After the above
explanation, all participants agreed completely with the proposal and gave written consent. They could withdraw
at any time, and doing so would have no negative consequences for them, but there were no withdrawals. The
study followed the guidelines established by the Chronobiology International journal for the conduct of research
on human subjects (Portaluppi et al., 2010). The study was also permitted by the Kochi university soccer club
committee and committee in Laboratory of Environmental Physiology, Graduate School of Integrated Arts and
Sciences, Kochi University, which carried out ethical inspections regarding the contents of the questionnaires
and the intervention program.
2.6 Statistic Analysis
The software used for statistical analysis was SPSS 12.0 J for Windows (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA.
Mann-Whitney U-test was used for ranked variables. Friedman’s test and Wilcoxon’s test were used for paired
variables. We were sometimes interested in changes during the intervention (Friedman) and sometimes only
comparisons between the beginning and end of the intervention (Wilcoxon).
ijps.ccsenet.org International Journal of Psychological Studies Vol. 8, No. 3; 2016
157
3. Results
Figure 1. Change in sleep onset time (A) and precise night sleep hours (B) during the intervention period based
on the sleep diary data in university soccer club members
Diurnal-type scores increased during the intervention month in the athletes who were originally designated as
“evening-type” whereas, in those originally designated as “morning-type”, these scores decreased (Table 2).
There were no differences in the diurnal type scores of control group between before and after the intervention
(Wilcoxon-test: z=-0.06, p=0.95). Sleep latency was decreased from 29 min on average to 23 min in the
evening-type group, whereas it remained unchanged, at 15-16 min, in the morning-types (Table 2).
Table 2. The diurnal-type scale scores and sleep latency before and after the intervention, in the two halves of the
sample designated as “evening-type” and “morning-type” before the intervention
The Diurnal Type Scale Scores Subjective Sleep Latency (min)
Before After Before After
Evening typed group 14.08±1.81 14.63±2.57 29.34±23.86 23.44±12.21
Wilcoxon test: Z=-2.068, p=0.039 Z=-1.972, p=0.049
Morning typed group 19.13±2.05 18.17±2.98 16.05±9.62 15.41±11.08
Wilcoxon test Z=-2.696, p=0.007 Z=-1.009, p=0.272
Sleep diary data showed that sleep onset time tended to become earlier during the intervention three weeks
(Figure 1A) and other sleep parameters of sleep offset, wake up time, bedtime and sleep latency were not
changed in the milk-intake group (Table 3). All the five parameters were not change in the control group (Table
3). During the intervention period of 3 weeks, the hours from sleep onset to offset and the precise sleep hours
excluding interrupted times during night sleep, increased in the milk-intake group (Figure 1B), whereas those did
not changed in the control group (Table 4). However, the hours in bed and mid-point of sleep hours did not
ijps.ccsenet.org International Journal of Psychological Studies Vol. 8, No. 3; 2016
158
change during the intervention period in both groups (Table 4). This means that it took shorter minutes for the
fall-in-sleep in the milk intake group, although there were no significant differences in sleep latency data
between the first and third weeks of intervention (Table 3).
The total scores of 11 questions on soccer performance was decreased (it means “in-advance” of soccer
performance) in the milk-intake group, whereas that did not change in the control group (Table 5). Of the 11
questions, soccer performance was improved during the 3 weeks at No 1, 2, 6, 8, 9 and 11: (1) Assessment of the
present situation became faster; (2) Visual field on playing became wider; (6) Irritation on playing was reduced;
(8) Injury on playing was reduced; (9) Body balance became better; and (11) Motivation for the practice became
better (Table 1 and Table 6).
Table 3. Mean values for sleep habits during the 3-week intervention, based on the sleep diary data (Mean±SD)
Cow milk at breakfast Bedtime Sleep onset Sleep offset Wake up Sleep latency
The first week 23.70±0.78 24.07±0.82 7.48±0.66 7.63±0.62 0.34±0.19
The second week 23.63±0.77 23.96±0.83 7.56±0.76 7.71±0.74 0.31±0.20
The third week 23.64 ±0.68 23.97±0.80 7.41±0.75 7.58±0.71 0.33±0.21
F
readman’s test (1st 2nd and 3rd weeks)
Χ2-value 1.50 2.66 2.76 1.21 4.17
df 2222 2
p-value 0.472 0.265 0.252 0.546 0.124
Wilcoxon test (the first and third weeks)
z -1.11 -1.77 -0.99 -0.70 -0.99
p 0.266 0.077 0.322 0.482 0.320
N
o cow mil
k
Bedtime Sleep onset Sleep offset Wake up Sleep latency
The first week 23.18±0.51 23.48±0.55 7.29±0.69 7.39±0.66 0.30±0.12
The second week 23.35±0.59 23.69±0.65 7.32±0.62 7.47±0.64 0.34±0.14
The third week 23.25±0.40 23.52±0.50 7.15±0.61 7.34±0.64 0.27±0.14
F
readman’s test (1st 2nd and 3rd weeks)
Χ2-value 5.51 5.66 1.83 0.50 1.64
df 2222 2
p-value 0.064 0.059 0.401 0.779 0.441
Wilcoxon test (the first and third weeks)
z -1.42 -0.47 -1.02 -0.12 -0.12
p 0.155 0.638 0.308 0.906 0.906
At the 10th day from the start of intervention, about 70% of the soccer club members of milk intake group who
showed shorter sleep latency than that at the beginning of intervention, improved their soccer performance,
whereas only about 45% of those who showed same or longer sleep latency did so (Figure 2A). At the 21st day
from the start of intervention, about 60% of the soccer club members of milk intake group who showed shorter
ijps.ccsenet.org International Journal of Psychological Studies Vol. 8, No. 3; 2016
159
sleep latency than that at the beginning of intervention, improved their soccer performance, whereas only about
30% of those who showed same or longer sleep latency did so (Figure 2B).
Participants who drank cows’ milk, which had been distributed, on all 21 days showed higher increase in the
diurnal type scores (change to more morning-typed) (p=0.056) (Figure 3) and judged their soccer performance to
be improved with higher frequency (p=0.009) than those who took cows’ milk on only less than 21 days.
Table 4. Change in sleep habit during the intervention 3 weeks based on the sleep diary data
Cow milk at breakfast Hours in bed Onset to offset Precise hours Mid point of sleep
The first week 7.91±0.93 7.39±0.97 7.27±1.07 27.77±0.56
The second week 8.08±0.93 7.63±0.96 7.55±1.01 27.77±0.64
The third week 7.92±0.95 7.45±1.06 7.42±1.06 27.71±0.57
Freadman’s test (1st 2nd and 3rd weeks)
Χ2-value 3.16 10.19 6.47 0.64
df 2 2 2 2
p-value 0.206 0.006 0.039 0.727
Wilcoxon test (the first and third weeks)
z -0.16 -0.54 -1.19 -1.28
p 0.873 0.586 0.233 0.199
No cow milk Hours in bed Onset to offset Precise hours Mid point of sleep
The first week 8.17±0.64 7.82±0.6 7.73±0.59 27.39±0.56
The second week 8.13±0.69 7.68±0.65 7.63±0.72 27.53±0.56
The third week 8.07±0.7 7.58±0.71 7.41±0.84 27.31±0.43
Freadman’s test (1st 2nd and 3rd weeks)
Χ2-value 2.17 2.17 4.67 1.50
df 2 2 2 2
p-value 0.338 0.338 0.097 0.472
Wilcoxon test (the first and third weeks)
z -1.02 -1.33 -2.04 -1.10
p 0.308 0.182 0.041 0.272
Table 5. Index (total scores) of the change in soccer performance 10 days and 20 days later in comparison with
the beginning of intervention
10 days later* 20 days later* Wilcoxon test
Mean SD Mean SD z p
Cow milk at breakfast 29.920 3.291 28.211 4.699 -3.961 <0.001
10 days later 20 days later Wilcoxon test
Mean SD Mean Mean z p
No cow milk at breakfast 31.938 2.886 31.867 2.386 -3.961 <0.001
z p z p
Mann-Whitney U-test -2.698 0.007 -3.058 0.002
* These scores are the values as “the comparison values” between 0 day and 10 day (or 20 days). So there is no value at “0” day.
ijps.ccsenet.org International Journal of Psychological Studies Vol. 8, No. 3; 2016
160
Table 6. Comparison of the change in soccer performance (based on that at the beginning of intervention)
between 0 day and 10 days later (or 20 days later) in the three categories (Category 1: Mental or psychological
issue, Category 2: Physiological function and Category 3: Physical techniques) in soccer performances.
(Analysis with Wilcoxon’s test)
Category 1 Category 2 Category 3
z p z p z p
Cow milk at breakfast -4.21 <0.001 -3.01 0.003 -2.65 0.008
No cow milk -1.61 0.106 -1.41 0.157 -1.84 0.066
Figure 2. Relationship between the change in sleep latency from the 1st week to 2nd or 3rd week of the
intervention period and the change in performance (red: become longer; blue: same or shorter) from the start to
10th day (A) (Fisher’s exact probability test: p=0.067) or the end of intervention period (21st day); (B) (Fishers’
exact probability test: p=0.067) in university soccer club members
ijps.ccsenet.org International Journal of Psychological Studies Vol. 8, No. 3; 2016
161
Figure 3. Comparison of the change in the diurnal type scale scores during the intervention 3 weeks and
following three weeks, five weeks in total between the participants who took, every day, cow milk distributed for
21 days and those who took it for only 0-20 days
4. Discussion
The intake of cow milk at the breakfast would be possible to take the evening-typed members into more
morning-typed ones, whereas the diurnal type of the morning-typed members could shift to more evening-typed.
Morning typed members would be speculated to changed to more-evening typed ones, because the shortage of
morning sun lights occurred due to later sun rise in this season of late November and early December.
Tryptophan included in the cows’ milk could be metabolized to serotonin in the morning and serotonin again
would be synthesized to melatonin at night (Harada et al., 2013; Nakade et al., 2009, 2012; Wada et al., 2013;
Harada et al., 2016) in the pineal of brains of the participants. Therefore, the sleep latency seems to become
shorter during the intervention due to the melatonin synthesis which has been known as the natural sleep inducer
(Bruni et al., 2015) based on the cow milk intake at the breakfast. This shorter sleep latency could lead to the
longer sleep hours for the university soccer club members. In this experiment, saliva melatonin was measured at
22:00 and 23:00 (Takeuchi et al., unpublished), and only melatonin level at 22:00 was increased after the
intervention, whereas it was similar to the value before the intervention at 23:00 (Takeuchi et al., unpublished).
This result would imply that the phase of onset of melatonin synthesis was in advance by 1 hour due to drinking
cows’ milk for the three weeks.
How can we explain the improvement of soccer performance due to the intake of cow milk at the breakfast? The
serotonin synthesis promotion would get the concentration improved during play, and also serotonin could be
natural anti-depression agent (St-Pierre et al., 2016). Therefore, Category 1 as mental or psychological issue was
most effectively improved among the three categories (Category 2: Physiological function and Category 3:
Physical techniques).
Due to drinking cows’ milk, the sleep onset seemed to be earlier due to earlier melatonin synthesis. Earlier sleep
onset might promote growth hormone synthesis (Cauter & Plat, 1996) and could lead to “reduction of injury on
playing” (Question No. 8) (Harada et al., 2016). Accompanying with the longer sleep hours, the amount of REM
sleep may be increased after the intervention. Because REM sleep improves the memory-consolidation
especially including integrated memory system which consists of sensory input, information processing and
ijps.ccsenet.org International Journal of Psychological Studies Vol. 8, No. 3; 2016
162
motor output (Wieser et al., 2015; Goerke et al., 2013), a new soccer technique which was learned during a
training in the previous day could be fixed due to a sufficient REM sleep in the following night.
An advance of the circadian phase which would be caused by the intake of cow milk at breakfast might lead to
the improvement of soccer performance. Recently, a mutant mouse created by a reduction of the BMAL1 gene
that encodes proteins regulating circadian rhythm was reported to show about a 30% of reduction in muscle force
and 40% reduction in mitochondrial volume (Andrews et al., 2010). Skeletal muscle amounts and mitochondria
volume via BMAL1 gene are speculated to be related to the circadian phase of humans, although no studies have
been done on the relationship in humans which remains to be studied in the future. The longer intervention more
than 3 months might be effective for the increase of body muscles and mitochondria volume for athletes.
As a limitation of this study, the self report nature of the soccer performance scoring was adopted and also the
participants know that they might be in the intervention group and so expect an effect. In near future, a coach or
similar assess of performance instead will be adopted and also a placebo drink (without proteins) will be given
for the control group in another study as a sham experiment. which could delete the side and “psychological”
effects.
Acknowledgements
Many thanks should be due to Professor Emeritus Jim Waterhouse, Liverpool John Moores University,
Liverpool, UK, for his valuable comments from academic view point on this manuscript.
References
Andrews, J. L., Zhang, X., Mc Carthy, J. J., Mc Dearmon, E. L., Hornberger, T. A., Russell, B., … Essera, K. A.
(2010). CLOCK and BMAL1 regulate MyoD and are necessary for maintenance of skeletal muscle
phenotype and function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,
107, 19090-19095. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1014523107
Brezinova, V., & Oswald, I. (1972). Sleep after a bedtime beverage. British Medical Journal, 2, 431-433.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bmj.2.5811.431
Bruni, O., Alonso-Alconada, D., Besag, F., Biran, V., Braam, W., Cortese, S., … Curatolo, P. (2015). Current
role of melatonin in pediatric neurology: Clinical recommendations. European Journal of Paediatric
Neurology, 19, 122-133. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpn.2014.12.007
Cauter, E. V., & Plat, L. (1996). Physiology of growth hormone secretion during sleep. Journal of Pediatrics,
128, 32-37. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-3476(96)70008-2
Goerke, M., Cohrs, S., Rodenbeck, A., Grittner, U., Sommer, W., & Kunz, D. (2013). Declarative memory
consolidation during the first night in a sleep lab: The role of REM sleep and cortisol.
Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38, 1102-1111. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.10.019
Harada, T., Hirotani, M., Maeda, M., Nomura, H., & Takeuchi, H. (2007). Correlation between breakfast
tryptophan content and morningness-eveningness in Japanese infants and students aged 0-15 yrs. Journal of
Physiological Anthropology, 26, 201-207. http://dx.doi.org/10.2114/jpa2.26.201
Harada, T., Nakade, M., Wada, K., Akimitsu, O., Noji, T., Krejci, M., & Takeuchi, H. (2013). Tryptophan and
sleep: Breakfast tryptophan content and sleep. In V. R. Preedy, V. B. Patel, & L.-A. Le (Eds.), Le
Handbook of Nutrition, Diet and Sleep (Chapter 33, pp. 473-487). Wageningen Academic Publishers,
Wageningen.
Harada, T., Wada, K., Tsuji, F., Krejci, M., Kawada, T., Noji, T., … Takeuchi, H. (2016). Intervention study
using a leaflet entitled “three benefits of go to bed early! get up early! And intake nutritionally rich
breakfast! a message for athletes” to improve the soccer performance of university soccer team. Sleep and
Biological Rhythms, 14 (Suppl. 1), 65-74. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s41105-015-0035-5
Laird, D. A., & Drexel, H. (1934). Experimenting with food and sleep. I. Effects of varying types of foods in
offsetting sleep disturbances caused by hunger pangs and gastric distress children and adults. Journal of the
American Dietetic Association, 10, 89-94.
Nakade, M., Takeuchi, H., Taniwaki, N., Noji, T., & Harada, T. (2009). An integrated effect of protein intake at
breakfast and morning exposure to sunlight on the circadian typology in Japanese infants aged 2-6 years.
Journal of Physiological Anthropology, 28, 239-245. http://dx.doi.org/10.2114/jpa2.28.239
Nakade, M., Akimitsu, O., Wada, K., Krejci, M., Noji, T., Taniwaki, N., … Harada, T. (2012). Can breakfast
tryptophan and vitamin B6 intake and morning exposure to sunlight promote morning-typology in young
ijps.ccsenet.org International Journal of Psychological Studies Vol. 8, No. 3; 2016
163
children aged 2 to 6 years? Journal of Physiological Anthropology, 31, 11.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1880-6805-31-11
Nongonierma, A. B., & FitzGerald, R. J. (2015). The scientific evidence for the role of milk protein-derived
bioactive peptides in humans: A Review. Journal of Functional Foods, 17, 640-656.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jff.2015.06.021
St-Pierre, J., Laurent, L., King, S., & Vaillancourt, C. (2015). Effects of prenatal maternal stress on serotonin
and fetal development. Placenta published on 29th November 2016 on line (in press).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.placenta.2015.11.013
Portaluppi, F., Smolensky, M. H., & Touitou, Y. (2010). Effects and methods for biological rhythm research on
animals and human beings. Chronobiology International, 27, 1911-1929.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/07420528.2010.516381
Takeuchi, H., Wada, K., Kawasaki, K., Krejci, M., Noji, T., Kawada, T., … Harada, T. (2014). Effects of cow
milk intake at breakfast on the circadian typology and mental health of Japanese infants aged 1-6 years.
Psychology, 5, 172-176. http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/psych.2014.52027
Torsvall, M. D., & Åkerstedt, T. A. (1980). A diurnal type scale: Construction, consistency and validation in
shift work. Scandinavian Journal of Work & Environmental Health, 6, 283-290.
http://dx.doi.org/10.5271/sjweh.2608
Wada, K., Yata, S., Akimitsu, O., Krejci, M., Noji, T., Nakade, M., … Harada, T. (2013). A tryptophan-rich
breakfast and exposure to light with low color temperature at night improve sleep and salivary melatonin
level in Japanese students. Journal of Circadian Rhythms, 11, 4. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1740-3391-11-4
Wiesner, C. D., Pulst, J., Krause, F., Elsner, M., Baying, L., Pedersen, A., … Göder R. (2015). The effect of
selective REM-sleep deprivation on the consolidation and affective evaluation of emotional memories.
Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 122, 131-141. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2015.02.008
Copyrights
Copyright for this article is retained by the author(s), with first publication rights granted to the journal.
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution
license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
... For Japanese children, drinking cow's milk at breakfast is an important source of tryptophan and could, on the basis of a questionnaire study [47], be supposed to promote mental and sleep health. Recently, an intervention of drinking cow's milk (200 ml) at breakfast was reported to make Japanese university soccer team athletes who were originally more evening-typed than evening-typed, and this intervention also made their soccer performances in advance [49]. Another intervention study was performed for 1 month on the Japanese university soccer team members using a leaflet entitled: "Three benefits: Go to bed early! ...
... whereas the diurnal type of the morning-typed members shifted to more evening-typed [49]. Tryptophan included in the cow's milk was metabolized to serotonin in the morning and serotonin again synthesized to melatonin at night [28,29,31,48,49,59]. ...
... whereas the diurnal type of the morning-typed members shifted to more evening-typed [49]. Tryptophan included in the cow's milk was metabolized to serotonin in the morning and serotonin again synthesized to melatonin at night [28,29,31,48,49,59]. Therefore, the sleep latency seems to become shorter during the intervention [49] due to the melatonin synthesis which has been known as the natural sleep inducer [60] based on cow's milk intake at breakfast. ...
... This study aims, first, to make the second edition of the self-evaluation of soccer performance scores which was edited based on the first version of the soccer performance scores (Kawada et al., 2016), and second, to examine the reliability and validity of the second version. Each of 16 questions was evaluated by one professional soccer coach who had an university assistant professor with A-class license for soccer supervisor which was officially given by Union for European Football Association (UEFA), a university soccer team coach and a university soccer team general manager. ...
... The first edition of self-evaluated questionnaire for soccer performance was made by three under-graduate students who were university soccer club players. The first edition consisted of 11 questions and was used for the two studies on the intervention using a leaflet (Wada et al., 2012) which is to promote morning-typed life for sports athletes and using the recommendation of having milk at breakfast for university soccer club athletes (Kawada et al., 2016). The questionnaire on self-evaluation of soccer performance as the first edition (Kawada et al., 2016) consisted of the 11 components as follows: Question 1: Assessment of the present situation, Question 2: Visual field on playing, Question 3: Movement of foot, Question 4: Rudimentary mistake, Question 5: First touch, Question 6: Irritation on playing, Question 7: Running out of Stamina, Question 8: Injury on playing, Question 9: Body balance, Question 10: Precision of long kick, Question 11: Motivation for the practice. ...
... The first edition consisted of 11 questions and was used for the two studies on the intervention using a leaflet (Wada et al., 2012) which is to promote morning-typed life for sports athletes and using the recommendation of having milk at breakfast for university soccer club athletes (Kawada et al., 2016). The questionnaire on self-evaluation of soccer performance as the first edition (Kawada et al., 2016) consisted of the 11 components as follows: Question 1: Assessment of the present situation, Question 2: Visual field on playing, Question 3: Movement of foot, Question 4: Rudimentary mistake, Question 5: First touch, Question 6: Irritation on playing, Question 7: Running out of Stamina, Question 8: Injury on playing, Question 9: Body balance, Question 10: Precision of long kick, Question 11: Motivation for the practice. For the second edition, additional five components were added by one professional soccer coach who had university assistant professor with A-class license for soccer supervisor which was officially given by Union for European Football Association (UEFA), a university soccer team coach and a university soccer team general manager. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study aims, first, to make the second edition of the self-evaluation of soccer performance scores which was edited based on the first version of the soccer performance scores (Kawada et al., 2016), and second, to examine the reliability and validity of the second version. Each of 16 questions was evaluated by one professional soccer coach who had an university assistant professor with A-class license for soccer supervisor which was officially given by Union for European Football Association (UEFA), a university soccer team coach and a university soccer team general manager. This second edition which was newly evaluated was administered to 111 university soccer club players who included starting members in the first class (A) team in December 2015. One hundred and eight answers (97.8%) of 111 ones were used for the statistical analysis. After the validation analysis, this second edition can be estimated to be valuable for evaluating a valuable soccer performance scores.
... However, several randomized studies have evaluated the effect of dairy milk and caffeine intake on sleep quality among athletes. Consuming dairy milk at breakfast decreased sleep latency among Japanese soccer athletes (Kawada et al., 2016). More frequent dairy milk consumption decreased the risk for poor sleep quality among female but not male Japanese athletes (Yasuda et al., 2019). ...
... Our results are not consistent with the previous literature. For instance, Kawada et al. (2016) reported that consuming dairy milk for 20 days vs. no dairy milk resulted in decreased sleep latency among male soccer athletes. Further, Yasuda et al. (2019) found that consuming dairy milk ≤2 vs. 3 or more days/week was associated with increased risk for poor sleep quality in female but not male Japanese elite Olympic athletes. ...
Article
Full-text available
Many endurance athletes have poor sleep quality which may affect performance and health. It is unclear how dietary intake affects sleep quality among athletes. We examined if sleep quality in endurance athletes is associated with consumption of fruit, vegetables, whole grains, dairy milk, and caffeinated beverages. Two hundred thirty-four endurance athletes (39.5 ± 14.1 year) participated in a survey. Participants provided information on demographics, anthropometry, sleep behavior and quality, and dietary intake via questionnaires. Sleep quality was assessed using the Athlete Sleep Screening Questionnaire (ASSQ) with a global score (ASSQ-global) and subscales including sleep difficulty (ASSQ-SD), chronotype (ASSQ-C), and disordered breathing while sleeping (ASSQ-SDB). A general linear model (GLM), adjusted for age, body mass index, sleep discomfort, sleep behavior, gender, race, and ethnicity, showed that higher caffeinated beverage intake was related to poorer global sleep quality ( p = 0.01) and increased risk for disordered breathing while sleeping ( p = 0.03). Higher whole grain intake was associated with a morning chronotype and lower risk for sleep issues ( p = 0.01). The GLM did not reveal a relationship between sleep quality and dairy milk, fruit, and vegetable intake. In conclusion, caffeinated beverages and whole grain intake may influence sleep quality. This relationship needs to be confirmed by further research.
... The created online program, which is often applied to athletes and students, is based mainly on an intervention program compiled by a team of authors from Japan and the Czech Republic [9], [10], [11], [12]. ...
... Similarly, some studies have pointed to the beneficial effects of milk on the sleep quality [27,28]. Kawada et al. concluded that drinking cow's milk at breakfast time was improved the sleep onset time and precise sleep hours of male athletes [29]. Also, Zhou et al. observed an improvement in sleep indices due to consuming a higher protein diet using milk [30]. ...
Article
Background: Admission of patients in coronary care units can lead to sleep disorders due to advanced monitoring and interventions. Inappropriate sleep quality in cardiac patients may be influenced by their health status. So, this study was performed to detect the effect of Milk-honey mixture on sleep status of acute coronary syndrome patients in the coronary care unit. Methods: A clinical trial study (registered under IRCT.ir with identifier no. IRCT201309285134N7) was conducted with 68 hospitalized patients with the acute coronary syndrome in the coronary care unit of a referral hospital in Semnan, Iran. After hospitalization of patients and selected eligible patients, sleep status of them was measured by Richards-Campbell Sleep Questionnaire in range of 0 to 100 score. Then patients were divided into the intervention and control groups randomly. Patients in the intervention group were received milk-honey mixture twice a day for three days. The control group patients were received routine care. In the third day, sleep quality of patients in the two groups was measured again. Finally, the data were analyzed by descriptive and inferential statistics. Results: The mean and standard deviation of patients' age was 63.12 ± 32.63. There was no significant difference in sleep scores on the first day of admission between the two groups (P = 0.914). But, on the third day of admission, there was a significant difference in sleep scores between the intervention and the control groups (P = 0.001). Conclusion: The mixture of milk and honey improves the sleep status of patients. So, it can be considered as an effective and affordable intervention to enhance the sleep quality of patients with the acute coronary syndrome in coronary care units.
Article
Full-text available
Emotion boosts the consolidation of events in the declarative memory system. Rapid eye movement (REM) sleep is believed to foster the memory consolidation of emotional events. On the other hand, REM sleep is assumed to reduce the emotional tone of the memory. Here, we investigated the effect of selective REM-sleep deprivation, SWS deprivation, or wake on the affective evaluation and consolidation of emotional and neutral pictures. Prior to an 9-hour retention interval, sixty-two healthy participants (23.5 ± 2.5 years, 32 female, 30 male) learned and rated their affect to 80 neutral and 80 emotionally negative pictures. Despite rigorous deprivation of REM sleep or SWS, the residual sleep fostered the consolidation of neutral and negative pictures. Furthermore, emotional arousal helped to memorize the pictures. The better consolidation of negative pictures compared to neutral ones was most pronounced in the SWS-deprived group where a normal amount of REM sleep was present. This emotional memory bias correlated with REM sleep only in the SWS-deprived group. Furthermore, emotional arousal to the pictures decreased over time, but neither sleep nor wake had any differential effect. Neither the comparison of the affective ratings (arousal, valence) during encoding and recognition, nor the affective ratings of the recognized targets and rejected distractors supported the hypothesis that REM sleep dampens the emotional reaction to remembered stimuli. The data suggest that REM sleep fosters the consolidation of emotional memories but has no effect on the affective evaluation of the remembered contents. Copyright © 2015. Published by Elsevier Inc.
Article
Full-text available
Background Epidemiological studies in Japan have documented an association between morning type and a tryptophan-rich breakfast followed by exposure to sunlight in children. The association may be mediated by enhanced melatonin synthesis, which facilitates sleep at night. However, melatonin is inhibited by artificial light levels with high color-temperature common in Japanese homes at night. In this study, we investigated whether a combination of tryptophan-rich breakfast and light with low color-temperature at night could enhance melatonin secretion and encourage earlier sleep times. Methods The intervention included having breakfast with protein- and vitamin B6 - rich foods and exposure to sunlight after breakfast plus exposure to incandescent light (low temperature light) at night (October-November, 2010). The participants were 94 members of a university soccer club, who were divided into 3 groups for the intervention (G1: no intervention; G2: asked to have protein-rich foods such as fermented soybeans and vitamin B6-rich foods such as bananas at breakfast and sunlight exposure after breakfast; G3: the same contents as G2 and incandescent light exposure at night). Salivary melatonin was measured around 11:00 p.m. on the day before the beginning, a mid-point and on the day before the last day a mid-point and on the last day of the 1 month intervention. Results In G3, there was a significantly positive correlation between total hours the participants spent under incandescent light at night and the frequency of feeling sleepy during the last week (p = 0.034). The salivary melatonin concentration of G3 was significantly higher than that of G1 and G2 in combined salivary samplings at the mid-point and on the day before the last day of the 1 month intervention (p = 0.018), whereas no such significant differences were shown on the day just before the start of the intervention (p = 0.63). Conclusion The combined intervention on breakfast, morning sunlight and evening-lighting seems to be effective for students including athletes to keep higher melatonin secretion at night which seems to induce easy onset of the night sleep and higher quality of sleep.
Article
Full-text available
This study tried to examine, from epidemiological and physiologic anthropological (Japanese culture on breakfast) points of view, the integrated effects of the amount of tryptophan and vitamin B6 intake and the following exposure to sunlight on the circadian typology and sleep habits in young Japanese children aged 2 to 6 years, using the newly-evaluated calculating system of tryptophan (Tryptophan Index 2009) and vitamin B6 intake (VitaminB6 Index 2009) at breakfast. The positive and significant correlation was shown between the Morningness-Eveningness (M-E) score and the Tryptophan Index and also the Vitamin B6 Index. This positive correlation between M-E score and amount of tryptophan intake was shown only by children who were exposed to sunlight for longer than 10min after breakfast. These results might support the following hypothesis: higher tryptophan and vitamin B6 intake at breakfast could promote the synthesis of serotonin via light stimulation in the morning in children.
Article
A new educational material was created for athletes. It was focused on three benefits of a morning-typed lifestyle and actual strategies for promoting this lifestyle. The material is a leaflet entitled, “Three benefits from going to bed early, getting up early and taking nutritionally rich breakfast. Message to athletes!” This study tried to evaluate the educational efficacy of a month-long intervention using this leaflet on Japanese university soccer team members. Eighty-four members of university soccer team were asked to participate in a month-long intervention from mid-November, 2011. Just before and 3 months after the intervention period, an anonymous integrated questionnaire was administered to participants. Another questionnaire including questions on the change in soccer performance was administered at the middle and the end of intervention month. Responses were received from more than 95 % of the soccer team members. The participants just after the intervention tend to show higher quality of sleep (p = 0.020) and lower frequency of irritation (p = 0.006) than those before the intervention. Analysis on the sleep diary including questions on the implementation of 8 issues as contents of the leaflet showed that the sleep latency and sleep quality at nights when participants used computer game, was longer (p < 0.001) and worse (p < 0.001), respectively than those at nights when they did not. The positive and significant correlation was shown between mental health (GHQ) just after the intervention and the improvement of soccer performance (Better mental health correlates with performance improvement) (p < 0.001). In the best performance team (A+), 70 % of the team changed to more morning-typed and significant and positive correlation between the diurnal type scores (high scores mean morning-typed) just after the intervention and degree of performance improvement (morning-type correlates with performance improvement) (p < 0.001). The intervention using the leaflet could be effective for improvement of soccer performance via changing to morning-typed life.
Article
Fetuses are exposed to many environmental perturbations that can influence their development. These factors can be easily identifiable such as drugs, chronic diseases or prenatal maternal stress. Recently, it has been demonstrated that the serotonin synthetized by the placenta was crucial for fetal brain development. Moreover, many studies show the involvement of serotonin system alteration in psychiatric disease during childhood and adulthood. This review summarizes existing studies showing that prenatal maternal stress, which induces alteration of serotonin systems (placenta and fetal brain) during a critical window of early development, could lead to alteration of fetal development and increase risks of psychiatric diseases later in life. This phenomenon, termed fetal programming, could be moderated by the sex of the fetus. This review highlights the need to better understand the modification of the maternal, placental and fetal serotonin systems induced by prenatal maternal stress in order to find early biomarkers of psychiatric disorders.
Article
Milk proteins are a good source of bioactive peptides (BAPs). BAPs can positively affect various health biomarkers in vitro. The role of milk protein-derived BAPs in humans was reviewed herein. To date, a limited number of BAPs have been identified in the gastrointestinal tract of humans and in the circulation at concentrations which may be sufficient to mediate in vivo bioactivity. While the outcomes of several human intervention studies have shown promising results, some studies have failed to demonstrate additional health benefits. This may be related to a low potency and bioavailability of the BAPs evaluated as well as confounding factors (i.e. genotype, health status and other sources of inter-individual variability). Universal guidelines for the evaluation of BAPs in humans such as adequately powered double-blind randomised clinical trials are needed.
Article
Melatonin, an indoleamine secreted by the pineal gland, plays a key role in regulating circadian rhythm. It has chronobiotic, antioxidant, anti-inflammatory and free radical scavenging properties. A conference in Rome in 2014 aimed to establish consensus on the roles of melatonin in children and on treatment guidelines. The best evidence for efficacy is in sleep onset insomnia and delayed sleep phase syndrome. It is most effective when administered 3-5 h before physiological dim light melatonin onset. There is no evidence that extended-release melatonin confers advantage over immediate release. Many children with developmental disorders, such as autism spectrum disorder, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and intellectual disability have sleep disturbance and can benefit from melatonin treatment. Melatonin decreases sleep onset latency and increases total sleep time but does not decrease night awakenings. Decreased CYP 1A2 activity, genetically determined or from concomitant medication, can slow metabolism, with loss of variation in melatonin level and loss of effect. Decreasing the dose can remedy this. Animal work and limited human data suggest that melatonin does not exacerbate seizures and might decrease them. Melatonin has been used successfully in treating headache. Animal work has confirmed a neuroprotective effect of melatonin, suggesting a role in minimising neuronal damage from birth asphyxia; results from human studies are awaited. Melatonin can also be of value in the performance of sleep EEGs and as sedation for brainstem auditory evoked potential assessments. No serious adverse effects of melatonin in humans have been identified. Copyright © 2014 European Paediatric Neurology Society. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Article
While the consolidation of declarative memory is supported by slow wave sleep (SWS) in healthy subjects, it has been shown to be associated with rapid eye movement (REM) sleep in patients with insomnia. Sleep during a subject's first night in an unfamiliar environment is often disturbed, and this so-called first-night effect (FNE) has often been used as a model of transient insomnia. Additionally, sleeping for the first time in an unfamiliar environment can lead to increased cortisol secretion, and declarative memory consolidation likely depends on low cortisol levels, especially during the early part of the night. Accounting for intersubject variability in the FNE, we examined the relationship between sleep stages, cortisol secretion and declarative memory performance in 27 healthy young men. Declarative memory performance improved significantly after sleep. Whereas memory performance during the learning session and retrieval testing was strongly associated with cortisol secretion, the overnight gain was not. Post hoc analyses indicated that the overnight gain appears to be modulated by the extent of the FNE: a significant overnight improvement in memory performance was found only in subjects with a weak FNE (n=12). In these subjects, no association was found between any sleep stage and the improvement observed in their memory performance. In subjects with a strong FNE (n=12), however, the overnight change in memory performance was associated with the proportion of REM sleep and the total number of REMs. Disturbed sleep in an unfamiliar environment therefore appears to affect the memory consolidation process.