Content uploaded by Marissa A. Sharif
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Marissa A. Sharif on May 30, 2022
Content may be subject to copyright.
MARISSA A. SHARIF and SUZANNE B. SHU*
Marketers of programs that are designed to help consumers reach goals
face dual challenges of making the program attractive enough to encourage
consumer signup while still motivating consumers to reach desirable goals
and thus stay satisfied with the program. The authors offer a possible
solution to this challenge: the emergency reserve, or slack with a cost. They
demonstrate how an explicitly defined emergency reserve not only is
preferred over other options for goal-related programs but can also lead
to increased persistence. Study 1 demonstrates that consumers prefer
programs with emergency reserves to programs that do not have them, and
Study 2 further clarifies that consumers’preference for an emergency
reserve depends on the presence of a superordinate goal. Study 3 reveals
that consumers prefer goals with emergency reserves because they perceive
them to have both higher attainability and value than other goals. Study 4
demonstrates that reserves can lead to increased goal persistence in a
realistic task that involves persistence over time. Finally, Studies 5 and 6
reveal that consumers persist more with reserve goals because they want to
avoid using the “emergency”reserve.
Keywords: emergency reserve, slack, goals, motivation, preference
Online Supplement : http://dx.doi.org/10.1509/15.0231
The Benefits of Emergency Reserves:
Greater Preference and Persistence for
Goals That Have Slack with a Cost
Students cheer when they learn that they have extra credit
available; dieters love having a possible “cheat day”in their
diet; consumers greatly appreciate having a buffer in case they
exceed their minutes in their mobile plans. People seem to love
flexibility, specifically a “just-in-case”type of flexibility. From
the marketer’s perspective, offering this “just-in-case”type of
flexibility may encourage consumers to sign up initially for
various programs, such as dieting programs, personal training
programs, or phone plans. However, not only do companies
want to attract consumers to sign up, they also want to ensure
that consumers perform well and meet desirable goals (e.g.,
lose more weight, exercise more, use fewer minutes) so that
they will stay satisfied with the program and continue to use it.
How can marketers offer consumers some kind of “just-in-
case”flexibility while still encouraging them to reach their
original, difficult goal? In this article, we offer a solution to this
challenge based on the notion of flexibility with a cost: the
emergency reserve. We define an emergency reserve as pre-
defined slack around a goal that can be used if needed but at a
small cost. For example, a reserve can be 500 extra emergency
calories available in a diet for the week, anemergency skipday
for the gym, or anemergency buffer range in a phone program.
Small costs associated with these reserves might be purely
psychological costs (consumers try to not use the emergency
reserve unless absolutely necessary), opportunity costs (if
consumers use it today, they cannot use it tomorrow),or future
costs (if consumers use the reserve today, they might have to
do something tomorrow to make up for it).
In this article, to test how emergency reserves affect both
preference and persistence, we examine three types of goals:
reserve goals, hard reference point goals (hard goals), and easy
reference point goals (easy goals). Reserve goals have a difficult
*Marisa A. Sharif is a doctoral candidate in marketing (e-mail: marissa.
sharif.1@anderson.ucla.edu). Suzanne Shu is Associate Professor of Marketing
(e-mail: suzanne.shu@anderson.ucla.edu). Anderson School of Management, Uni-
versity of California, Los Angeles. This article is based on part of the first author's
dissertation. Coeditor: Rebecca Ratner; Associate Editor: Anirban Mukhopadhyay.
© 2017, American Marketing Association Journal of Marketing Research
ISSN: 0022-2437 (print) Vol. LIV (June 2017), 495–509
1547-7193 (electronic) DOI: 10.1509/jmr.15.0231495
reference point plus an additional emergency reserve (e.g.,
a goal of going to the gym seven days a week plus two
“emergency skip”days). Hard goals have the same difficult
reference point but without the additional emergency reserve
(e.g., a goal of going to the gym seven days a week). Easy
goals have an easier reference point, with the additional
emergency reserve already incorporated into the goal with no
additional cost (e.g., a goal of going to the gym five days a
week). We propose that including an explicitly defined emer-
gency reserve within a goal will (1) be preferred to both hard
and easy goals when the consumer has a superordinate goal
(e.g., lose weight) because of both the higher expectancy and
value of the reserve goal and (2) result in greater goal per-
sistence than hard and easy goals as a result of a resistance to
break into the emergency reserve.
In this research, we investigate the influence of emergency
reserves on preference and persistence in six studies. The first
three studies explore when and why people prefer emergency
reserves, and the last three studies investigate how and why
emergency reserves influence goal persistence. We first pro-
pose the theoretical basis for why consumers prefer reserve
goals and then discuss why these goals better enable consumers
to persist.
PREFERENCE FOR RESERVES
According to expectancy value theories, a consumer’s
motivation to reach a goal depends on both the expectancy of
reaching the goal and the value of the goal (Atkinson 1957;
Tolman 1955). However, because there are distinct phases
within goal pursuit, consumers are motivated differently de-
pending on how much progress they have made toward their
goal (Gollwitzer 1990; Heckhausen 1977; Heckhausen and
Gollwitzer 1987). In the initial stages of goal pursuit, con-
sumers primarily focus on the attainability of the goal rather
than the value of the goal (Zhang and Huang 2010). Both the
sociocognitive model (Bandura 1997) and goal-setting theory
(Locke and Latham 1990) suggest that consumers’willingness
to adopt a goal is largely influenced by how attainable the goal
seems. Consistent with this notion, consumers are more mo-
tivated with high perceived velocity (vs. low perceived ve-
locity) toward reaching a goal in initial goal pursuit (Huang
and Zhang 2011). In addition, because consumers perceive
goals that can be achieved through multiple means as more
attainable (Kruglanski, Pierro, and Sheveland 2011), they are
more motivated when there are multiple means to achieve a
goal (Huang and Zhang 2013) or when there is more variety
among these means (Etkin and Ratner 2012). Therefore, this
stream of research suggests that making a goal more attainable
(or making it seem more attainable) motivates consumers in
the initial stages of goal pursuit.
In line with this theorizing, before adopting a goal (or
signing up for a program), consumers’preferences for reserve
goals compared with hard or easy goals may depend on the
attainability of these goals. Consumers may initially prefer
reserve goals to hard goals because they are more attainable
than hard goals. Reserve goals have the same difficult refer-
ence point (e.g., going to the gym seven days a week) as hard
goals but provide additional flexibility (e.g., two emergency
skip days), making them more attainable than hard goals.
However, consumers may not initially prefer reserve goals to
easy goals because they are equally attainable as easy goals
(e.g., consumers with both goals have technically not violated
their goal if they go to the gym only five days a week). In some
cases, reserve goals may even be perceived as less attainable
than easy goals because they focus on a more difficult ref-
erence point than easy goals (e.g., going to the gym seven
days a week vs. going to the gym five days a week). In general,
these perceptions of attainability may determine preferences
between hard, easy, and reserve goals, with hard goals viewed
as the least attainable.
However, we expect preferences for these goals to be af-
fected by more than just attainability if there is a salient su-
perordinate goal. We hypothesize that the value of a goal (in
addition to the attainability of the goal) becomes relevant in
goal preference if there is a salient superordinate goal. Prior
research has demonstrated that people can perform better
in tasks with more difficult reference points/goals (Heath,
Larrick, and Wu 1999; Locke and Latham 1990). When there
is a superordinate goal, more difficult goals may be perceived
as more attractive (valuable) because they increase the chance
of obtaining the superordinate goal. For example, if a person
achieves a more difficult goal of going to the gym seven days a
week instead of an easier goal of going to the gym five days a
week, (s)he will also be more likely to achieve the superor-
dinate goal of becoming more fitorlosingweight.
In line with this theorizing, how does the existence of a
superordinate goal affect preference for reserve goals over
other goals? A reserve goal focuses consumers on a more dif-
ficult reference point than an easy goal, increasing the chance
of performing better at the superordinate goal and thus also
increasing the attractiveness (value) of the goal. However, the
attainability of both goals will remain the same; people with a
reserve goal(e.g., going tothe gym seven daysa week plus two
“emergency skip”days) and people with an easy goal (e.g.,
five days a week) can both meet their goals by going to the gym
five days a week. Because the goals are equally attainable but
the reserve goal has a higher value, we expect that consumers
will prefer reserve goals toeasy goals if there is a superordinate
goal.
We hypothesize that consumers will still prefer reserve goals
to hard goals when there is a superordinate goal. Both reserve
and hard goals have more difficult reference points (e.g., going
to the gym seven days a week) and thus are equally valuable.
However, the reserve goal has added flexibility (e.g., two
emergency skip days) that makes it more attainable. Because
the goals have equal value but the reserve goal is more at-
tainable, we expect that consumers will prefer reserve goals to
hard goals.
H
1
: Consumers prefer reserve goals to both easy goals and hard goals
if there is a superordinate goal. Consumers prefer reserve goals
to hard goals, but not to easy goals, if there is no superordinate
goal.
PERSISTENCE WITH RESERVES
We propose that not only will consumers predictably prefer
reserve goals to easy and hard goals when there is a super-
ordinate goal, but they will persist more with reserve goals than
easy goals, hard goals, and other goals with flexibility. Al-
though we posit that the higher expectancy and value of the
reserve drives preference for the reserve goal, we expect that
when actually pursuing a reserve goal, consumers will persist
more because of an additional element of the reserve: the cost
of using it.
496 JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, JUNE 2017
Unlike other types of slack,1the emergency reserve is
unique because there is (1) a small cost of using the available
flexibility and (2) a predefined finite amount available. It is
designed to be used in “just-in-case”scenarios. We predict that
this small cost of using the emergency reserve causes con-
sumers to resist using it, leading to more persistence than those
with other goals.
Costs of the Reserve
Costs of using the reserve may include psychological, op-
portunity, and/or future costs. We examine cases in which the
reserve has only a psychological cost, only an opportunity
cost, or both. Although we suggest that the emergency re-
serve could be effective with other costs (e.g., future costs),
by examining only psychological and opportunity costs, we
do not affect the external incentives for achieving the goal.
To impose a psychological cost for the reserves in our
studies, we simply label the reserve an “emergency”reserve,
inducing people to avoid using the reserve unless absolutely
necessary. Prior research has suggested that people are sen-
sitive to similar labels and the psychological costs for vio-
lating them. More specifically, research in mental accounting/
budgeting has indicated that people use their resources dif-
ferently depending on how they are labeled (Heath and Soll
1996; Henderson and Peterson 1992; Kahneman and Tversky
1984; Thaler 1980, 1985). Most relevant to the “emergency
reserve,”people have “rainy day”funds, in which they set
aside money in case ofan emergency. They restrict themselves
to use that money only if absolutely necessary (Shefrin and
Thaler 1988; Thaler 1990). In this case, the cost of using the
funds is purely psychological as a result of the “rainy day”
labeling. In addition, as a more tangible example of salient
psychological costs, poor residents in India who were given
part of their salary in a separate, specially designated sealed
envelope were highly resistant to opening it and spending it
relative to those who receivedthe salary all at once (Soman and
Cheema 2011). Thus, drawing on this literature, by labeling the
reserve as “emergency-only use,”we induce a psychological
cost of breaking into the emergency reserve. We hypothesize
that consumers will try to resist using their reserve unless they
absolutely need it, avoiding the psychological cost associated
with using the emergency reserve.
In addition, to mimic real-life goal environments, we ex-
amine goal pursuit “over time”or in situations in which there
are multiple instances to apply the emergency reserve. In these
scenarios,the framing of the emergency reserve goal (e.g., goal
of going to the gym seven days a week with two “emergency”
skip days) makes the limited amount of flexibility (e.g., only
two “emergency”skip days) very salient to consumers. Con-
sumers are more sensitive to their opportunity costs when their
resources are constrained (Spiller 2011) and also more resistant
to use resources with smaller budgets (Heath and Soll 1996;
Krishnamurthy and Prokopec 2010; Morewedge, Holtzman,
and Epley 2007; Shefrin and Thaler 1988; Stilley, Inman, and
Wakefield 2010). Thus, because the reserve goal is framed
with a limited number of emergency reserves available (e.g.,
two emergency skip days), it is very salient to consumers that if
they use their emergency reserves earlier, they will not be able
to use them later.As a result, because of the salient opportunity
cost of using their emergency reserves earlier, consumers may
resist using their emergency reserves, waiting for a time when
they might need to use them more.
We thus hypothesize that consumers will resist using their
emergency reserve because of the (1) psychological cost
(induced by the “emergency”labeling) and (2) opportunity
cost of using their emergency reserve too early, leading to more
persistence than those with other goals. We predict that the
underlying process for why consumers persist more with reserve
goals compared with other goal types is the same: resistance to
use the reserve because of the cost. Next, we formally describe
our hypotheses and explain why consumers with reserve goals
will be more likely to persist than those with easygoals,hard
goals, and other goals with flexibility.
Reserve Versus Easy Goals
In addition to the cost of using the reserve, reserve goals
focus on a more difficult reference point (e.g., goal of going to
the gym seven days a week plus two “emergency skip”days)
than easy goals (e.g., goal of going to the gym five days a
week). Prior research has suggested that people perform better
with more difficult goals (Locke et al. 1981). Using prospect
theory as a framework (Kahneman and Tversky 1979), Heath,
Larrick, and Wu (1999) demonstrate that people expect to
work much harder if they have not yet reached their goal (in
the losses domain) than if they have already succeeded at their
goal (in the gains domain).
Thus, after succeeding (or being able to succeed later) at
reaching this easier reference point (e.g., going to the gym five
days a week), we expect those with easy goals to be less likely
to persist. However, we expect consumers with reserve goals
to try to avoid using their reserve and thus strive for a more
difficult reference point. Therefore, we expect consumers with
reserve goals to be more likely to persist than those with easy
goals because of the following dual influence: (1) easy-goal
consumers will be less likely to persist because they have an
easier reference point and (2) reserve-goal consumers will be
more likely to persist as a result of a resistance to use the
emergency reserve.
H
2
: Consumers with reserve goals are more likely to persist than
those with easy goals.
Reserve Versus Flexible Goals
To ensure that the beneficial effects of the reserve are not
due to simply being a flexible goal, we also examine how
reserve goals compare with other flexible goals. Prior research
in both the mental accounting and goal literature has found that
flexible goals and flexible mental accounts generally do not
result in better performance and can even result in worse per-
formance (compared with less flexible goals and accounts).
Consumers exploit malleability and ambiguity in their mental
accounting rules to justify indulging in temptations, thus re-
ducing self-control performance (Ainslie 2001; Cheema and
Soman 2006). They perform worse with goals that have
multiple means of achieving them, once they are past the initial
stages of goal pursuit (Huang and Zhang 2013), and consumers
with high–low goals (e.g., a goal toscore between two and four
points) are more likely to pursue their goal again but are not
more likely to perform better than those with single goals
(e.g., a goal to score two points or a goal to score four points)
1Because of their cost, emergency reserves differ from Zauberman and
Lynch’s (2005) definition of slack (the perceived future surplus of a given
resource).
The Benefits of Emergency Reserves 497
(Scott and Nowlis 2013). Furthermore, consumers with goals
with backup plans for the superordinate goal perform worse on
their primary goal than those without backup plans (Shin and
Milkman 2016). Finally, goal specificity has different effects
on consumers depending on the construal level. Nonspecific
goals under high construal lead to a lower perception of im-
portance and, in turn, lower success rates compared with specific
goals; however, goals under low construal lead to a reduced
sense of difficulty and, thus, higher success rates ( ¨
Ulk¨umen and
Cheema 2011).
To verify that goals with emergency reserves act dif-
ferently from other flexible goals, we compare the effects of
reserve goals with the effects of other goals with flexibility
(what we term “range goals”). We examine two types of range
goals: range-easy goals, which focus on the easier reference
point (e.g., “Your goal is to go to the gym five days a week;
however, you should aim to go to the gym seven days a
week”), and range-hard goals, which focus on the more dif-
ficult reference point (e.g., “Your goal is to go to the gym seven
days a week; however, it is okay if you go to the gym five
days a week”). We hypothesize that consumers with range
goals will be less likely than those with reserve goals to reach
the more difficult reference point of this flexibility region
because there is no cost (psychological or otherwise) associ-
ated with using the flexibility inherent in those goals.
H
3
: Consumers with reserve goals are more likely to persist than
those with range goals.
Reserve Versus Hard Goals
Reserve goals and hard goals both focus on the same dif-
ficult reference point (e.g., go to the gym seven days a week).
However, reserve goals have some flexibility, with a cost,
available (e.g., two emergency skip days). We hypothesize that
consumers with hard goals will make an effort to reach their
goal, but those with reserve goals will make an even greater
effort than those with hard goals to avoid using their reserve
because of its associated cost. As we have mentioned, the
psychological cost of the reserve is amplified by terming it an
“emergency”reserve, and people are sensitive to such label-
ing (Heath and Soll 1996; Henderson and Peterson 1992;
Kahneman and Tversky 1984; Shefrin and Thaler 1988; Thaler
1980, 1985, 1990). Therefore, in mere goals (i.e., there is no
external incentive for reaching the goal), the cost of using the
reserve may be perceived as greater than the cost of violating
the hard goal, leading to more persistence with reserve goals
than hard goals. While participants with hard goals will try
their best to reach their goal, those with reserve goals will try to
avoid using their “emergency”reserve unless they absolutely
need it.
H
4
: Consumers with reserve goals are more likely to persist than
those with hard goals.
This article demonstrates that consumers prefer reserve
goals to hard and easy goals and also persist more with reserve
goals than with other goals. Studies 1–3 test initial preferences
for emergency reserves. Studies 1a and 1b demonstrate that
consumers prefer reserve goals to hard and easy goals in a
weight loss program, and Study 2 further clarifies this pref-
erence by showing that reserve goals are preferred to both easy
and hard goals when they are part of a superordinate goal.
Study 3 reveals that consumers prefer reserve goals to easy and
hard goals because reserve goals have a higher perceived value
and attainability than many other types of goals. The final
three studies examine consumers’persistence with reserve
goals compared with those with easy, hard, and range goals
in tasks with superordinate goals. Study 4 demonstrates the
beneficial effects of an emergency reserve in a realistic work
situation that requires persistence in a task over multiple
days. Finally, Studies 5 and 6 reveal that consumers with
reserve goals persist more because they try to resist using
their emergency reserve.
STUDIES 1A AND 1B: WEIGHT LOSS STUDIES
In Studies 1a and 1b, we explore consumers’preference
foranoptionwithanemergencyreserveinadomainwitha
superordinate goal: a hypothetical point-based weight loss
program. We expected that participants would prefer the reserve
option to the easy and hard options because there is a salient
superordinate goal within this domain (i.e., to lose weight).
Method
One hundred Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk) par-
ticipants completed each study (Study 1a: M
age
=34.33
years, age range: 19–65 years; 56 men; Study 1b: M
age
=
32.91 years, age range: 18–62 years; 12 men). Participants
were told to imagine that they wanted to lose weight and that
they were considering three different point-based weight
loss programs. Participants were asked to indicate which
program they would be most likely to sign up for. The easy
option offered 32 points per day, and the hard option offered
30 points per day. In Study 1a, the reserve option offered 30
points per day with 2 optional emergency points per day.
The cost of using the reserve in this scenario was purely
psychological by labeling the points as “emergency”(for
details, see Web Appendix A). In Study 1b, the reserve
option offered 30 points per day plus 14 optional emergency
points per week. This increases the flexibility of the reserve
relative to Study 1a, but it also generates an opportunity
cost, in addition to a psychological cost, for early use of the
optional points.
Results
For both studies, we conducted a multinomial logistic re-
gression with the reserve option as the reference group. In
Study 1a, participants were marginally significantly more
likely to choose the reserve option (46%) over the easy option
(29%; p= .052) and significantly more likely to choose the
reserve option over the hard option (25%; p= .014). In Study
1b, participants were significantly more likely to choose the
reserve option (55%) over the easy option (22%; p<.001) and
the hard option (23%; p<.001).
Discussion
In support of H
1
, we found in Studies 1a and 1b that
participants preferred reserve goals to hard goals and easy
goals when there was a superordinate goal (e.g., to lose
weight). In Study 1a, the only cost of using the reserve was
psychological; in other words, simply labeling some of the
points for emergency use was enough to affect preferences. In
Study 1b, participants also preferred the reserve if it had an
additional cost, an opportunity cost, such that use of the
emergency points today reduces the opportunity to use them
tomorrow.
498 JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, JUNE 2017
In Study 2, we aim to explore the boundaries of pref-
erence for the reserve goal by testing how it is affected by the
salience of a superordinate goal. We use a scenario about
taking a class and manipulate the presence of a superor-
dinate goal (a career exam). We predict that participants will
prefer the reserve option to the easy option only if the class is
needed for a larger career exam (superordinate goal) but not
if the material would not be needed again in the future (no
superordinate goal).
STUDY 2: CLASS STUDY
In this study, we explore consumers’preference for a
reserveoptioncomparedwithaneasyoptionandahard
option within an education scenario in the presence and
absence of a superordinate goal. Participants indicated their
preference on a Likert scale between only two of the options
(either reserve vs. hard or reserve vs. easy).2We also begin
to explore why consumers may prefer the reserve option to
easy and hard options.
Method
Two hundred paid MTurk participants (M
age
= 31.07 years,
age range: 18–59 years; 126 men) took part in this study.
Participants were asked to imagine that they were required to
take a class. Participants were randomly assigned to the
career exam condition and the no–career exam condition. In
the career exam condition, participants were told that the
required class was a preparatory class for a separate larger
exam they had to take for their future career (a salient su-
perordinate goal). In the no–career exam condition, partic-
ipants were told that the class material would not be used in
their career and that they would not be required to take a
future related class (no superordinate goal). Therefore, in the
career exam condition, studying hard for the class would
increase students’likelihood of passing the class and also
help them better prepare for their career (i.e., have a better
chance of obtaining their superordinate goal). In the no–
career exam condition, studying hard for the class would
only increase their chances of passing the class.
Participants were told that two teachers taught the same
exact class in a similar fashion and that they used the exact
same final exam at the end of the class. One of the teachers
offered an emergency reserve for the test (i.e., the reserve
option): students would have to receive a score of 20 (out of
25) on the exam to pass the class, but they also could earn 5
“emergency”extra credit points if they received below a 20.
The easy-option teacher required students to receive a score
of 15 (out of 25) on the exam, and the hard-option teacher
required a score of 20 (out of 25) to pass the class, without any
extra credit available.
Participants were randomly assigned to read about and
compare the reserve option and the easy option or the reserve
option and the hard option. They were asked, “Which teacher
would you be more likely to choose?”on a Likert scale with
1=“Definitely more likely to choose Teacher A,”4=“Equally
likely to choose Teacher A or Teacher B,”and 7 = “Definitely
more likely to choose Teacher B.”Teacher A was either the
hard option or the easy option and Teacher B was the reserve
option (for details, see Web Appendix B).
After indicating their preference, participants were asked
with which teacher they would study harder, would have
learned more after the final, and would be more likely to pass
the class on a similar seven-point Likert scale. Participants
then completed the brief self-control measure (Tangney,
Baumeister, and Boone 2004).
Results
Because half of the participants indicated a preference
between the reserve and easy option and the other half in-
dicated a preference between the reserve and hard option, we
analyze these data separately (as combining them would not
be meaningful; see Figure 1). We used independent sam-
ple t-tests and one-sample t-tests versus the midpoint of 4
(being indifferent between the teachers) to analyze the data.
Preference for reserve goals to easy goals. Our superor-
dinate goal salience manipulation (career exam vs. no career
exam) had a significant effect on participant’s preferences for
the reserve option versus the easy option. In support of H
1
,
participants were significantly more likely to prefer the reserve
option to the easy option in the career exam condition than in
the no–career exam condition (M
CE
=5.20vs.M
NCE
=3.89;
t(100) = −3.04, p= .003). Within the career exam condition,
participants were significantly more likely to prefer the reserve
option compared with preferring both options equally
(M
reserve
=5.20vs.M
both
=4.0;t(48)=4.24,p<.001).
However, within the no career exam condition, participants
were not significantly more likely to prefer the reserve option
to preferring both options equally (M
reserve
=3.88vs.M
both
=
4.0; t(52) = −.35, p=.73).
Preference for reserve goals over hard goals. There was no
significant effect of our superordinate goal salience manipu-
lation on participants’preference between the hard option and
the reserve option (M
CE
=6.29vs.M
NCE
= 6.14; t(96) = −.58,
p= .57). Within both the career exam condition and the
no–career exam condition, participants were significantly more
Figure 1
PREFERENCE FOR THE RESERVE VERSUS EASY AND HARD
GOALS, SPLIT BY CONDITION
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
Versus Easy Versus Hard
Preference
Superordinate goal No superordinate goal
Notes: For preference, 7 = “reserve goal,”and 1 = “other goal.”
2One could argue from Studies 1a and 1b that participants were simply
choosing at random between the reserve and nonreserve goals (easy and
hard). We test this alternative explanation in Study 2 by using a seven-point
Likert scale.
The Benefits of Emergency Reserves 499
likelytopreferthereserveoptioncomparedwithpreferringboth
options equally (career exam condition: M
reserve
=6.29vs.
M
both
= 4.0; t(48) = 16.34, p<.001; no–career exam condition:
M
reserve
=6.14vs.M
both
= 4.0; t(48) = 10.50, p<.001).
Motivation (easy vs. reserve). There was no significant
effect of our superordinate goal salience manipulation on par-
ticipants’intuition about their likelihood of passing the class
and their motivation to study between the easy option and the
reserve option. In both conditions, participants thought
they would study harder with the reserve option (career
exam condition: M
reserve
= 5.25 vs. M
both
= 4.0; t(48) = 5.20,
p<.001; no–career exam condition: M
reserve
= 4.91 vs.
M
both
= 4.0; t(52) = 3.36, p= .001) and would learn more with
the reserve option (career exam condition: M
reserve
= 5.23 vs.
M
both
= 4.0; t(48) = 6.14, p<.001; no–career exam condition:
M
reserve
= 4.79 vs. M
both
= 4.0; t(52) = 3.79, p<.001).
Furthermore, participants thought they would have similar
chances of passing with the reserve option and easy option in
both conditions.
Motivation (hard vs. reserve). There was no significant
effect of our superordinate goal salience manipulation on
participants’intuition about their likelihood of passing/
learning from the class and their motivation to study be-
tween the hard option and the reserve option. In both con-
ditions, participants thought they would study similarly as
much and learn a similar amount with the reserve option and
the hard option but would be significantly more likely to pass
the class with the reserve option than the hard option (career
exam condition: M
reserve
=5.57vs.M
both
= 4.0; t(48) = 6.63,
p<.001; no–career exam condition: M
reserve
=5.94vs.M
both
=
4.0; t(48) = 10.73, p<.001).
Self-control. We used ordinary least squares regression to
examine the effect of individual levels of self-control on
choice. We found that participants with high self-control were
overall more likely to choose the reserve option over the easy
option (b=.77,p= .014). Self-control did not significantly
affect participants’likelihood of choosing the reserve option
over the hard option (b=.10,p= .66). We also did not find a
significant two-way interaction between individual-level self-
control and the superordinate goal manipulation (easy vs.
reserve: career exam vs. no career exam × self-control in-
teraction: b=.43,p= .49; hard vs. reserve: career exam vs. no
career exam × self-control interaction: b=.10,p=.76).
Discussion
Study 2 demonstrated that consumers prefer the reserve
option to the easy option only if they have a superordinate
goal and prefer the reserve option to the hard option in-
dependent of having a superordinate goal (H
1
). This study
thus reveals the importance of a contextual factor, the sa-
lience of superordinate goal, on preferences for emergency
reserves.
Study 2 also enabled us to test the importance of indi-
vidual factors, such as self-control, in predicting preference
for reserves. We found that participants with higher self-
control were more likely to prefer the reserve option to the
easy option overall. Participants may consider the emer-
gency reserve a safe self-control precommitment strategy
(Milkman, Minson, and Volpp 2013; Wertenbroch 1998);
those with high self-control are more likely to want to
precommit themselves to studying and, thus, to learning
more from the course. Consequently, both situational and
individual factors are important for predicting preference for
reserves.
Finally, Study 2 revealed why reserve options might be
preferred to easy and hard options. Participants thought they
would be more motivated to study harder and would learn
more with the reserve option than the easy option, suggesting
that reserve goals are perceived to have greater value than
easy goals. They also thought they would be equally likely to
pass the class with both teachers, which indicates that they
perceived the attainability of both goals to be equal.
Participants seemed to prefer the reserve option to the hard
option for a different reason. Participants thought they would
be more likely to pass the class with the reserve option than
with the hard option, suggesting that the reserve option is
perceived as more attainable. In addition, they thought they
would study equally as hard with both options, suggesting
that both goals are equally as valuable. In the next study, we
aim to replicate these effects and also examine consumers’
preference for other flexible goals within a domain with a
salient superordinate goal.
STUDY 3: WORD SEARCH PREFERENCE STUDY
In Study 3, we further examine consumers’preference
for the reserve goal and intuitions about the attainability
(likelihood of succeeding at their goal) and value (likelihood
of performing better on the superordinate goal) of reserve
goals, hard goals, and easy goals when there is a superor-
dinate goal. In addition, we introduce two range goals. One
range goal focuses on the more difficult, aspirational ref-
erence point; we refer to this range goal as range-hard. The
other range goal focuses on the easier, more attainable
reference point; we refer to this range goal as range-easy.
However, both goals are equally attainable as easy goals,
similar to reserve goals.
Method
Two hundred paid MTurk participants (M
age
= 35.43 years,
age range: 18–70 years; 79 men) completed this survey.
Participants were asked to imagine that they were com-
pletingtrainingforadifficult word search test and that they
would receive a bonus if they performed very well on this
test. They were asked to complete a series of training word
searches in preparation for the test. The more word searches
they practiced, the more likely it was that they would do
better on the difficult word search test at the end. Therefore,
the superordinate goal in this scenario is to perform well on
the word search test. Only participants who successfully
completed their training goal could try the difficult word
search test. Participants were then able to choose which
training goal they would prefer.
For each training word search they completed, they were
told they would score one point in this hypothetical game.
Participants were randomly assigned to choose between the
reserve goal (“Goal to score 3 points. However, you also
have one emergency point that you can apply if you fail one
word search test”) and one of the other goals: easy (“Goal to
score 2 points”), hard (“Goal to score 3 points”), range-easy
(“Goal to score 2 points. However, you should aim to score 3
points. You will be able to try the word search test if you
score 2 points”), or range-hard (“Goal to score 3 points, but
it’s okay if you score 2 points. You will be able to try the
word search test if you score 2 points”).
500 JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, JUNE 2017
Afterward, they were asked to choose with which goal
they thought they would be more likely to qualify for the
word search test (i.e., expectancy) and with which goal they
thought they would be more likely to perform better on the
word search test (i.e., value). Participants saw graphical
representations and descriptions of each goal when making
their choices. Range-hard, reserve, and hard goals all focus
on the more difficult goal of completing all three training
word searches, reinforced by both text and a graphical
representation. However, the range-hard and reserve goals
offer flexibility, making them more attainable than the hard
goal. Range-easy and easy goals focus on the easier goal of
completing two word searches, also reinforced by text and
graphical representation (for visual representations of all
goals, see Web Appendix C).
Results
Easy versus reserve. Participants were significantly more
likely to choose a reserve goal over an easy goal (66.67%
reserve
vs. 33.34%
easy
;c
2
(1,N=48)=5.33,
3p= .021. Participants
thought they would be equally likely to qualify to try the word
search test with both goals (52.1%
reserve
vs. 47.9%
easy
)
.
Lastly,
participants thought they would perform significantly better
with the reserve goal than the easy goal on the word search test
(64.6%
reserve
vs. 35.4%
easy
;c
2
(1, N = 48) = 4.08, p=.043).
Hard versus reserve. Participants were significantly more
likely to choosea reserve goal over a hard goal (72.5%
reserve
vs.
26.5%
hard
;c
2
(1, N = 49) = 10.80, p= .001). Participants
thought they would be significantly more likely to qualify
to try the word search test with the reserve goal than the
hard goal (68.1%
reserve
vs. 31.9%
hard;
c
2
(1,N=47)=6.15,
p=.013)
.
Lastly, participants thought they would perform
approximately equally well on the word search test with the
reserve goal and the hard goal (56.3%
reserve
vs. 43.8%
hard
).
Range-easy versus reserve. Participants were equally likely
to choose the range-easy and easy goals (51%
reserve
vs.
49%
range-easy
). In addition, they thought they would be equally
likely to qualify for the word search test with both goals
(51%
reserve
vs. 49%
range-easy
) and thought both goals would
help them perform equally well (56%
reserve
vs. 44%
range-easy
).
Range-hard versus reserve. Participants were significantly
more likely to choose a range-hard goal over a reserve goal
(34%
reserve
vs. 66%
range-hard
;c
2
(1,N=47)=4.79,p=.029).
Participants thought they would be equally likely to qualify for
the word search test (45.1%
reserve
vs. 54.9%
range-hard
)and
would perform equally well (48.1%
reserve
vs. 51.9%
range-hard
)
under both goals.
Discussion
This study replicates the effects of Studies 1 and 2, such that
participants were more likely to choose a reserve goal over
both an easy goal and a hard goal in the presence of a su-
perordinate goal. In addition, replicating Study 2, participants
seem to prefer reserve goals over easy goals because they
perceive reserve goals as having a higher value (greater
likelihood of performing well on the superordinate goal) than
easy goals, but with the same attainability as easy goals
(equally likely to qualify for the final word search). Con-
versely, participants seem to prefer reserve goals over hard
goals because they perceive reserve goals as being more
attainable than hard goals, but with the same value as hard
goals (see Figure 2).
Furthermore, the results of this study suggest that con-
sumers overall prefer flexible goals. Participants had no pref-
erence between reserve and range-easy goals, perceiving
them to have similar value and attainability. Notably, par-
ticipants preferred range-hard goals to reserve goals but
thought they had similar value and attainability. Consumers
may prefer range-hard goals to avoid the psychological cost
associated with the reserve. However, we demonstrate that
this cost they may be trying to avoid ends up being a key
motivating factor in goal persistence.
Studies 1–3 demonstrated that participants prefer reserve
goals to other goals. In the remaining three studies, we examine
how participants persist with these different types of goals.
STUDY 4: DAILY CHALLENGE STUDY
In Study 4, we use an incentive-compatible task with real
behavior and real consequences to examine goal persistence.
This task requires persistence over multiple days because it
asks that participants set aside time each day to complete an
annoying task to reach their goals, much like daily gym at-
tendance or other long-term goals. In addition to comparing
participants’persistence under hard and easy goals with par-
ticipants’persistence under reserve goals, we begin by ex-
ploring how the persistence of participants with one type of
flexible goal, range-easy, compares with the persistence of
those with reserve goals. While our prediction is that it is the
cost associated with the emergency reserve that encourages
persistence, it may simply be that introducing flexibility is
enough to encourage more persistence; the range-easy goal
thus provides an early test of this alternate explanation. In
Studies 5 and 6, we examine the persistence of participants
with other types of flexible goals. Drawing on H
2
–H
4
,we
predicted that participants with reserve goals would persist
more than those with other goal types, such as easy, range-
easy, and hard goals.
Method
Two hundred twenty-six paid participants (M
age
=22.12
years, age range: 18–53 years; 60 men) from a large public
university in the southwestern United States participated in this
study. Every morning for seven days, a different set of 35
CAPTCHAs was posted online for participants to complete.
Participants were randomly assigned to one of four conditions
in which we manipulated the goal for the number of days they
should complete the CAPTCHAs (easy, range-easy, reserve,
and hard).
Procedure
Participants in this study were on their university’swinter
break. A different set of 35 CAPTCHAS was posted every
morning and then removed at midnight. Participants received
$1 for every day that they completed each set of 35
CAPTCHAs. Participants were informed that there would be
no penalty for not completing the task daily.
Participants were randomly assigned to one of four con-
ditions, in which the goal of the task was manipulated. In
addition to receiving $1 per day for completing the task,
3All chi-square tests compare the number of participants who chose the
reserve option versus the other option (easy, hard, range-hard, or range-easy,
depending on condition) with what would be expected by chance (50% vs.
50% of observations).
The Benefits of Emergency Reserves 501
participants received an additional $5 bonus if they com-
pleted their goal. In the easy condition, participants’goal was
to complete the task five days a week. In the range-easy
condition, participants’goal was to complete the task five
days a week, but they were also told that they “should aim to
complete the task every day of the week.”In the reserve
condition, participants’goal was to complete the task every
day (seven days) of the week. They were also told, “In case
you need it, up to two days will be excused,”and that they
would still receive their bonus if they missed up to two days
(but they would not receive the $1 per day payment for the
days they missed). In the hard condition, participants’goal
was to complete the task every day of the week (seven days).
In this study, there is both a psychological and an op-
portunity cost associated with using the reserve. If a par-
ticipant decides to skip the task on Monday and Tuesday,
(s)he cannot skip the task on Wednesday and still make the
goal (thus, there is an opportunity cost for early use of the
emergency reserve). In addition, there is a psychological cost
in the framing of the emergency reserve because participants
aretoldtouseitonly“in case they need it.”We predicted that
participants with reserve goals would be more likely to
complete their goal than participants with easy, hard, and
range-easy goals.
Results
Goal attainment. Reserve, range-easy, and easy goal
participants all received their bonus if they completed the
task five days a week. Hard-goal participants received their
bonus if they completed the task seven days a week. We used
logistic regression predicting successful goal attainment
(1 = successfully attained their goal, 0 = otherwise) from
three dummy variables representing the conditions (easy,
range-easy, and hard), with the reserve condition as the
reference group.4
A test of the full model against a constant-only model was
statistically significant, indicating that our predictors as a set
(indicators of different goal types) had a significanteffecton
goal attainment (c
2
(4, N = 226) = 14.76, p<.001). Reserve
participants (52.5%) were significantly more likely to re-
ceive their bonus than easy participants (25.9%; b=−1.15,
c
2
(1) = 8.07, p= .005), range-easy participants (33.9%;
b=−.77, c
2
(1) = 4.00, p= .046), and hard participants
(21.1%; b=−.1.42, c
2
(1) = 11.68, p= .001). Because hard-
goal participants did not receive their bonus unless they
completed the task seven days a week (more than all of the
other conditions), we ran another logistic regression pre-
dicting achievement of the easier reference point goal of five
days a week from the same three dummy variables repre-
senting each condition. Reserve participants were still sig-
nificantly more likely to complete this lower threshold than
hard participants (52.5%
reserve
vs. 31.6%
hard
;b=−.88,
c
2
(1) = 5.13, p= .023; see Figure 3).
Preference. On the last day of the task, all of the conditions
were described, and participants (84 total) were asked to in-
dicate which condition they would prefer if they had to
complete the task again. Using a multinomial logistic re-
gression, we found that participants chose the reserve option
Figure 2
PROPORTION OF PARTICIPANTS WHO PREFERRED THE RESERVE GOAL, CONSIDERED IT TO BE MORE ATTAINABLE, AND
CONSIDERED IT TO HAVE MORE VALUE VERSUS THE OTHER GOAL OPTION
0
.1
.2
.3
.4
.5
.6
.7
.8
.9
Versus Easy Versus Range-Easy Versus Range-Hard Versus Hard
Proportion of Participants Who
Chose the Reserve Goal
Preference Attainability Value
4The effects reported next are the betas and p-values for each of these
dummy variables from one regression model (not individual pairwise
comparisons).
502 JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, JUNE 2017
significantly more than each of the other options (62%
reserve
vs. 14%
easy
, 14%
hard
, and 10%
range-easy
;ps<.001).
Discussion
This study provides evidence that consumers with emer-
gency reserves persist more than those with other goals (easy,
range-easy, and hard). Reserve participants were more likely
to receive their bonus and reach the easier goal compared
with participants in all other conditions.
We also found that participants prefer the reserve con-
dition to all other conditions after actually experiencing the
task. There is a superordinate goal in this study to make as
much money as possible because participants could make $1
per day in all conditions. Therefore, this result is consistent
with the preferences expressed in Studies 1–3 and with the
predictions in H
1
.5
Although this study provides evidence for the basic effect
(i.e., consumers with reserve goals persist more thanthose with
other goals), it does not demonstrate why consumers with
reserve goals persist more. According to expectancy-value
theories, reserve goals may provide more motivation by fo-
cusing consumers on the value (i.e., the more difficult goal;
e.g., complete the task for seven days) while maintaining the
expectancy (by providing a reserve); under this explanation,
the cost of the reserve is an unnecessary part of the process.
To explore whether the cost of the reserve is a critical compo-
nent of the process, we next examine participants’persistence
with a range-hard goal, which similarly focuses on a more
difficult, aspirational goal while keeping the same attain-
ability as an easy goal. We expect that if the cost of the reserve
is an unnecessary part of the motivational process, partici-
pants with range-hard goals will persist equally as much as
those with reserve goals. However, if participants with re-
serve goals persist more, we can conclude that there is an
additional motivational benefit of the cost of the reserve
beyond what expectancy-value theories would predict.
In addition, we examine participants’persistence with a
psychological cost-free reserve by terming it a “bonus”reserve
rather than an “emergency”reserve. Although “bonus”re-
serves may still have an opportunity cost associated with using
them, the psychological cost of the reserve is reduced.
Therefore, if cost is an important motivational component of
the reserve, participants with “emergency reserves”should
persist more than those with “bonus reserves”as a result of
the increased cost (emergency reserve: psychological cost +
opportunity cost vs. bonus reserve: opportunity cost).
STUDY 5: SPOT-THE-DIFFERENCE STUDY
In Study 5, participants were asked to complete three spot-
the-difference “training”gamestohelpthempreparefora
more difficult spot-the-difference test at the end of the survey.
Participants were randomly assigned to one of six training
goals to beat a particular number of the three spot-the-difference
training games: easy, range-easy, range-hard, emergency re-
serve, bonus reserve, or hard.
The first game was very easy (leading all participants to
score one point), but the second game in the training set was
very difficult. The number of differences participants found
before giving up on this difficult game was our dependent
variable of interest and measure of persistence. We expected
that participants with an emergency reserve would find more
differences before giving up than those with other types of
goals, as a result of the cost of reserve. As in Study 4, in this
study, there were two costs of using their emergency reserve:
a psychological cost (i.e., labeling it “emergency”)andan
opportunity cost (i.e., if participants use their emergency re-
serve on the second game, they cannot use it on the third
game).
Procedure
Six hundred one paid MTurk participants (M
age
=35.71
years, age range: 18–78 years; 211 men) completed this study.
The experiment began by explaining to participants that
theywouldbecompletingtrainingforadifficult spot-the-
difference test at the end of the study. If they performed
well on this test, they could qualify for a potential survey in
the future. To train for this test, they would be asked to
complete a series of training spot-the-difference games. The
more spot-the-difference games they practiced, the more
likely it is that they would do better on the test. Thus,
performing well on this difficult spot-the-difference test was
the superordinate goal. Only participants who successfully
completed the training could try to take the difficult spot-the-
difference test and thus be eligible for a potential survey in
the future. Before participants were assigned their goal, they
received instructions and completed a practice spot-the-
difference task.
Participants were then randomly assigned to one of the
training goal conditions (easy, range-easy, range-hard, hard,
emergency reserve, or bonus reserve), in which we manipu-
lated the goal for the number of spot-the-difference games they
should complete (out of three). They were told that they would
receive one point for every spot-the-difference game that they
won. In the easy condition, participants’goal was to score two
points. In the range-easy condition, participants were told,
“Your goal is to score 2 points. However, you should aim to
score 3 points. You will be able to try the spot-the-difference
test if you score 2 points.”In the range-hard condition,
Figure 3
PROPORTION OF PARTICIPANTS WHO REACHED THE FIVE-DAY
GOAL IN EACH CONDITION
0
.1
.2
.3
.4
.5
.6
.7
Easy Ran
g
e-Easy Reserve Hard
Proportion Who Completed
Five-Day Goal
5Only those participants who completed the seventh day completed the
preference measure. Thus, our preference results from this study may be not
representative of the full sample of participants.
The Benefits of Emergency Reserves 503
participants were told, “Your goal is to score 3 points.
However, it's okay if you score 2 points instead. You will be
able to try the spot-the-difference test if you score 2 points.”
In the hard condition, participants’goal was to score three
points. In the emergency reserve and bonus reserve condi-
tions, participants’goal was also to score three points.
However, participants in the emergency reserve condition
were also told, “Throughout these games, you will have one
optional ‘emergency’point available just in case you need it.
If you fail one spot the difference game, you can apply this
emergency point and receive a point for that failed game.”
Afterward, they saw a graphical representation of the emer-
gency reserve with a red “Apply Emergency Point”button.
They were informed that if they failed a game, they would
click on this button to apply their emergency point. This
description was exactly the same for the bonus reserve
condition, except the word “emergency”was replaced with
the word “bonus.”In addition, the graphical representation
of the reserve button was green rather than red and said
“Apply Bonus Point”rather than “Apply Emergency Point”
(see figure 4).”
After every spot-the-difference game, participants were
shown a graphical depiction of their progress and their goal.
To focus attention on the easier goal in the range-easy and
easy conditions and the harder goal in the range-hard,
emergency reserve, bonus reserve, and hard conditions, the
graphical representation for these goals showed a goal of
scoring two points and three points, respectively.
All participants then completed the very easy first spot-the-
difference game. Participants were asked to find two differ-
ences using as much time as they wanted; all participants were
informed that they had beaten the game.
For the second spot-the-difference game, participants
were told that there were between 10 and 12 differences
(there were actually only 10 differences) between the two
pictures (see Figure 5). They had as much time as they
needed, but they were informed that they could give up at any
time. The number of differences that participants found be-
fore giving up was our dependent variable of interest. After
participants gave up (or thought they found all the differ-
ences), they were informed that they either beat the game or
failed the game and thus did/did not receive a point. If they
failed this game, participants in the reserve conditions were
asked if they wanted to apply their “emergency point”or
“bonus point.”
After the second game, participants completed the third
spot-the-difference game. All participants then took the dif-
ficult spot-the-difference test.6They were given two minutes to
find as many differences as they could. After taking this test,
participants were asked to imagine that they had to complete a
similar set oftraining tasks again. They werepresented withall
of the different training goals with matching graphical de-
pictions and were asked to choose between them.
Results
Number of differences found. We used ordinary least
squares regression to predict the number of differences found
from five dummy variables representing each condition
(easy, range-easy, range-hard, hard, and bonus reserve), with
the emergency reserve condition as the reference group (see
Figure 6).7A test of the full model against a constant-only
model was marginally statistically significant (F(5, 595) =
2.209, p= .052). Participants in the emergency reserve
condition (M = 6.98) found significantly more differences
than those in the easy condition (M = 5.93; b=−1.05, p=
.004), the range-easy condition (M = 6.13; b=−.85, p= .020),
the range-hard condition (M = 6.21; b=−.77, p= .034), and
the hard condition (M = 5.98; b=−1.00, p= .006); emer-
gency reserve participants found marginally significantly
more differences than those in the bonus reserve condition
(M
e-reserve
= 6.98 vs. M
b-reserve
= 6.34; b=−.64, p= .076).
Preference. We conducted a multinomial logistic re-
gression with the emergency reserve option as the reference
group and found that participants preferred the emergency
reserve goal (14.3%) significantly more than the hard (8.2%,
p= .002) and easy (7.8%, p= .001) goals and significantly
less than the range-easy goal (21.1%, p= .005). Participants
had no significant preference between the emergency reserve
goal and the range-hard goal (17.3%, p= .19). In addition,
we conducted a multinomial logistic regression with the
bonus reserve goal as the reference group and found that
participants preferred the bonus reserve goal significantly
more than all of the other goals (31.3%
b-reserve
vs. 8.2%
hard
,
p<.001; 7.8%
easy
,p<.001; 21.1%
range-easy
,p= .001;
17.3%
range-hard
,p<.001; 14.3%
e-reserve
,p<.001).
Discussion
Participants with an emergency reserve goal found more
differences (i.e., tried harder) on the second spot-the-difference
game than those with all other goal types. Participants in all
conditions (except the hard condition) could try the final spot-
the-difference test if they scored two points. All participants
scored one point on the first game, so if participants with these
goals gave up on the second difficult game, they could still
qualify to take the spot-the-difference test by completing the
third game. Despite this similarity among the goals, partici-
pants with an emergency reserve goal tried the hardest on this
second game, resisting using their emergency reserve. In ad-
dition, participants with emergency reserves persisted more on
the second game than even those with a hard goal.
In this study, we also examined the persistence of partici-
pants with a range-hard goal, a range goal that focused on the
aspirational goal, to investigate how crucial the cost of the
reserve is for increased persistence. We found that participants
with an emergency reserve goal persisted more than those
with a range-hard goal. Relatedly, we introduced a psycho-
logical cost-free bonus reserve goal. We also found that par-
ticipants persisted more with an emergency reserve goal, a
reserve with more cost (psychological + opportunity costs),
than with a psychological cost–free bonus reserve, a reserve
with only an opportunity cost. Both of these comparisons
suggest that the cost of the reserve is a crucial component of the
mechanism of the emergency reserve.
After experiencing the task, participants preferred the
emergency reserve goal to the hard goal and easy goals and
6Even participants who failed their training goal were asked to take the
spot-the-difference test. However, participants who failed their training goal
were told that they would not qualify for future surveys regardless of their
performance.
7The effects reported next are the betas and p-values for each of these
dummy variables from one regression model (not individual pairwise
comparisons).
504 JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, JUNE 2017
preferred the bonus reserve goal the most out of all goals.
Because participants had to choose between a bonus reserve
goal and an emergency reserve goal, we expect that they may
have shied away from the greater psychological cost of the
emergency reserve goal. Nonetheless, this study provides
overall evidence that there is strong preference for having a
reserve of some kind as part of a difficult goal.
In this study, we demonstrated that participants try to avoid
using their reserve on game 2 when they have an opportunity to
use it later (e.g., on game 3). In Study 6, we examine whether
participants try to avoid using their reserve even if there is no
opportunity to use it later. Thus, this final study examines how
effective the reserve is when it has only a psychological cost
(by labeling it “emergency”) rather than when it has both a
psychological cost and an opportunity cost, as in Studies 4 and
5. Specifically, we examine how likely participants with a
reserve are to persist on a third game even after succeeding at
the easier goal (e.g., scoring two points out of three).
In addition, in Study 5, participants had to successfully
complete a training goal to take a test at the end of the survey.
In the real world, there is not always a strict qualification for
achieving a superordinate goal. People often set goals (e.g., go
to the gym seven days a week) to help them be more likely
to reach their superordinate goal (e.g., become more fit).
However, people can often still reach the superordinate goal
even if they fail the short-term goal (e.g., they can still lose
some weight if they go to the gym only six days a week
instead of seven). Therefore, in Study 6, we relax the linkage
between the goals to be more aligned with these types of real-
world scenarios.
STUDY 6: WORD SEARCH PERSISTENCE STUDY
In Study 6, participants were asked to complete three word
search “training”games to help them prepare for a more
difficult word search test at the end of the survey. Participants
were randomly assigned to one of five training goals to beat a
particular number of the three word search training games:
easy, range-easy, range-hard, emergency reserve, or hard.
Participants completed two word search practice games that
they were likely to succeed at and then were asked whether
they would like to try the third word search practice game or
move on to the word search test; this is our dependent variable
and measure of persistence. We expected that participants with
reserve goals would be more likely to try the third word search
practice game than those with other goals.
Procedure
Five hundred ten participants (M
age
= 35.85 years, age
range: 18–77 years; 212 men) completed this survey on
MTurk. The experiment began by explaining to participants
that they would be completing training for a difficult word
search test at the end of the study. If they performed well on
this word search test, they could be eligible for a potential
survey in the future (the superordinate goal). To train for this
word search test, they would be asked to complete a series of
training word searches. They were told that the more word
searches they practiced, the more likely it is that they would
do better on the difficult word search test. Participants did not
have to successfully complete their training goal to try the
word search test. However, they were told that completing
their goal was a good indication that they were prepared for
the test. Participants received instructions and completed a
practice word search.
Participants were then randomly assigned to one of the
training goalconditions (easy, range-easy, range-hard, hard, or
reserve), in which the goal for how many word searches they
should complete (out of three) was manipulated. They were
told they would receive one point for every word search that
they beat. In the easy condition, participants’goal was to score
Figure 5
SECOND SPOT-THE-DIFFERENCE GAME
Figure 4
IMAGE FOR BONUS RESERVE VERSUS EMERGENCY RESERVE
Your Progress Your Goal
Apply
Emergency Point
Your Progress Your Goal
Apply Bonus
Point
Emergency Reserve Condition Bonus Reserve Condition
The Benefits of Emergency Reserves 505
two points. In the range-easy condition, participants were
told, “Your goal is to score 2 points. However, you should
aim to score 3 points.”In the range-hard condition, participants
were told, “Your goal is to score 3 points. However, it's okay if
you score 2 points instead.”In the hard and reserve conditions,
participants’goal was to score three points. However, partic-
ipants in the reserve condition were also told, “Throughout
these games, you will have one ‘emergency’point available. If
you fail to complete one word search, you can apply this
emergency point. This emergency point does not need to be
used. It should only be used if you need it.”As in Study 5, after
every word search, participants were shown a graphical de-
piction of their progress and their goal.
All participants then completed the very easy first word
search. Participants were asked to find four words in three
minutes; all participants were informed that they beat the word
search. Participants next completed a slightly more difficult (but
still easy) word search, in which they had to find five words in
three and a half minutes. Most participants succeeded at both of
these timed word search practice games and thus had scored two
points by this point in the study.
After succeeding at the second word search, participants
were then given a description of the third word search. The
third word search did not have a time limit and required
participants to find ten words. Without seeing the exact word
search, they were asked, “Do you want to try the 3rd word
search game described or would you like to move on to the
word search test?”If participants chose to move on to the
word search test, they were asked to confirm that they indeed
wanted to move on and, in the reserve condition, that they
wanted to use their emergency point.
All participants then took the difficult word search test.
They were given three minutes to find as many of the ten
words listed as possible. Immediately before and after taking
this test, participants were asked to imagine that they had to
complete a similar training session again and were asked to
choose between the different training goals.
Results
Twenty-nine participants failed one of the first two word
searches and were excluded from further analyses. We
conducted a logistic regression predicting choice toattempt the
third word search (1 = Tried the third word search, 0 = Did not
try the third word search) from four dummy variables repre-
senting each of the conditions (easy, range-easy, range-hard,
and hard), with the reserve condition serving as the reference
group. We also conducted an additional logistic regression
predicting successful completion of the third word search
(1 = Successfully completedthe third word search, 0 = Did not
successfully complete the third word search) from the same
four dummy variables.8
Try third word search. A test of the full model against a
constant-only model was statistically significant (c
2
(4, N =
481) = 29.61, p<.001). We found that participants with re-
serves (86.3%) were more likely to try the third word search
compared with those in the easy condition (51.5%; b=−1.78,
c
2
(1) = 24.30, p<.001), the range-easy condition (73.4%;
b=−.83, c
2
(1) = 4.76, p= .029), the range-hard condition
(68%; b=−1.09, c
2
(1) = 8.76, p= .003), and the hard
condition (73.7%; b=−.81, c
2
(1) = 4.60, p= .032) (see
Figure 7).
Figure 6
NUMBER OF DIFFERENCES FOUND IN THE SECOND SPOT-THE-DIFFERENCE GAME, SPLIT BY CONDITION
0
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
Easy Range-Easy Range-Hard Emergency
Reserve
Bonus Reserve Hard
Total Differences Found
8The effects reported next are the betas and p-values for each of these
dummy variables from one regression model (not individual pairwise
comparisons).
506 JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, JUNE 2017
Score three points. A test of the full model against a
constant-only model was statistically significant (c
2
(4, N =
481) = 25.74, p<.001). We found that participants with
reserves (81.1%) were more likely to score three points
(complete the third word search game) than those in the easy
condition (46.4%; b=−1.60, c
2
(1) = 23.21, p<.001), the
range-easy condition (77.1%; b=−.89, c
2
(1) = 6.84, p=
.009), the range-hard condition (63%; b=−.92, c
2
(1) = 7.62,
p= .006), and the hard condition (64.2%; b=−.87, c
2
(1) =
6.61, p= .010).
Preference. We used a multinomial regression to analyze
the preference data, with the reserve option as the refer-
ence group. Participants preferred the reserve goal signifi-
cantly more than the easy goal before the word search test
(22.2%
reserve
vs. 10.1%
easy
;b=−.76, c
2
(1) = 19.72, p<.001)
and marginally significantly more than the easy goal after
the word search test (19.3%
reserve
vs. 14.8%
easy
;b=−.27,
c
2
(1) = 2.93, p= .087). However, they preferred the reserve
goal less than the hard goal (22.2%
reserve
vs. 29.5%
hard
;
b= .28, c
2
(1) = 4.89, p= .027), and there were no differences
in preference for the reserve goal and the other range goals.
Discussion
We found that participants with reserve goals were more
likely to persist to try the third word search compared with
participants with other goals when there was only a psycho-
logical cost associated with using the reserve. Participants tried
to resist using the emergency reserve even when there was no
future benefit to saving it, leading them to persist after reach-
ing the easier goal (scoring two points). Replicating Study 5,
participants with reserve goals again persisted more than those
with range-hard goals, suggesting that the psychological cost
of the reserve goal is a crucial component of its ability to
motivate. They also persisted more than those with hard goals.
After experiencing the task, we found that participants
preferred the reserve goal to the easy goal but not to the hard
goal. In Study 5, we found that participants preferred reserve
goals to both easy and hard goals. This difference may be due
to the fact that the training goals overall were much easier in
Study 6 than in Study 5. In this study, most participants
succeeded at the second word search and did not apply their
emergency reserve, whereas in Study 5, most participants
failed to complete the second spot-the-difference game,
making the emergency reserve more necessary and valuable.
Thus, preferences for the reserve goal to other goals may
depend on how difficult the task is, with reserves becoming
more preferred when the task involves some level of per-
ceived difficulty.
GENERAL DISCUSSION
This article demonstrates that the emergency reserve is
both preferred in goal pursuit and highly motivating, leading
to more persistence than other goals. Our results suggest that
offering emergency reserves can not only encourage con-
sumers to sign up for a program initially but also help them
reach desirable goals and keep them satisfied with their
outcomes.
We support these claims through six studies. Study 1
demonstrated that consumers prefer weight loss programs (a
program with a superordinate goal) with emergency reserves,
and Study 2 revealed that the emergency reserve is preferred to
hard and easy options when there is a superordinate goal and is
preferred to hard options (but not easy options) when there is
no superordinate goal. Study 3 further showed that consumers
prefer reserve goals to easy and hard goals when there is a
superordinate goal because they perceive both the attainabil-
ity and value to be greater. Study 4 demonstrated that con-
sumers persist more than those with easy and hard goals in an
incentive-compatible real-life task. Finally, Studies 5 and 6
demonstrated that consumers persist more with reserve goals
compared with other goal types because they try to avoid using
the reserve. This research contributes to the literature on goals
by suggesting an innovative strategy to not only initiate goal
pursuit but also improve goal persistence. Our findings suggest
that predefined flexibility with a cost can actually be beneficial,
rather than maladaptive, in goals and mental budgets.
We note that programs outside of the lab environment that
attempt to help people reach superordinate goals use concepts
similar to the emergency reserve. For example, Weight
Watchers, a very successful point-based weight loss program,9
gives its participants weekly “optional points”and “activity
points”that they can use any day throughout the week, which
they can earn by doing physical activity. Although the points
are available if needed, there is an opportunity cost associated
with using the optional points and a future/past cost associated
with the activity points. In a separate domain, giving children a
“bedtime pass”that allows them to leave their bedroom only
once per night has been shown to be an effective way to get
children to go to sleep at bedtime (Friman et al. 1999; Moore
et al. 2007). The success of these programs suggests that
including emergency reserves can be both a preferred and a
successful strategy to help consumers reach their goals. Our
research also suggests that labeling the optional points and
Figure 7
PROPORTION OF PARTICIPANTS WHO TRIED THE THIRD WORD
SEARCH PRACTICE GAME, SPLIT BY CONDITION
0
.1
.2
.3
.4
.5
.6
.7
.8
.9
1
Easy Range-Easy Range-Hard Reserve Hard
Proportion Who Tried Third Word-Search Game
9According to U.S. News and World Report, in 2015 Weight Watchers was
ranked number one for the fifth consecutive year for “Best Weight Loss Diet”
and ranked number one as “Easiest Diet to Follow”for the fourth consecutive
year.
The Benefits of Emergency Reserves 507
bedtime pass as for “emergency use”only may further increase
their effectiveness.
Implications for Marketers
This set of studies suggests that marketers can be more
successful in recruiting consumers to sign up for their
programs by offering an emergency reserve. Programs fo-
cusing on helping consumers reach superordinate goals (e.g.,
weight loss programs) can more easily encourage consumers
to sign up for their programs by offering a reserve. However,
even companies that may not necessarily focus on super-
ordinate goals can encourage consumers to choose programs
with a reserve if they make a superordinate goal more salient.
For example, a phone company could emphasize the costs
of exceeding a consumer’s allocated data per month (a su-
perordinate goal). Making the overage costs salient should
highlight the importance of having a plan with less data plus
an emergency buffer compared with simply having more data
(with the buffer already included).
Our results also reveal that people persist more if they
have reserve goals compared with other goal types. This
increased persistence will lead to better long-term results
and, thus, more satisfied consumers. Furthermore, Studies 4
and 5 revealed that participants prefer reserve goals to both
easy goals and hard goals after experiencing a difficult task,
suggesting that consumers are likely to be more satisfied with
programs with emergency reserves. Therefore, emergency
reserves have direct implications for marketers because they
are a method to encourage consumers to sign up for a pro-
gram, help them reach their long-term goals, and lead to their
higher satisfaction and return.
Further Research
It is worth noting that we explored a specific type of
persistence in this research. We focused on situations in
which participants succeeded at their goal or could still
succeed at their goal in the future (i.e., persistence before
failure). Thus, this article did not explore how goal pursuit is
affected if consumers fail at a subgoal and actually need to
use their emergency reserves. Prior research has shown that
violating a goal or subgoal can have negative consequences
(Cochran and Tesser 1996; Fishbach, Dhar, and Zhang
2006; Heath, Larrick, and Wu 1999; Polivy 1976; Soman
and Cheema 2004; Wilcox, Block, and Eisenstein 2011),
such as a deterioration of subsequent performance or complete
abandonment of the goal. In addition to helping consumers
persist to reach a more difficult goal/reference point, emer-
gency reserves may have another distinct benefit not explored
in this article: greater persistence after a subgoal or goal
violation. By applying emergency reserves after a subgoal
or goal violation, some negative consequences of goal and/or
subgoal violation may be reduced, leading consumers with
goals with emergency reserves to continue to persist even
after small failures.
In this article, we focused only on persistence in tasks
with imposed extrinsic superordinate goals (e.g., perform
well on the final word search test). Research has shown that
success versus failure of a subgoal has different effects de-
pending on whether the person focuses on a superordinate
goal (Fishbach, Dhar, and Zhang 2006). Success of a sub-
goal highlights commitment rather than progress when the
superordinate goal is primed, leading people to continue to
pursue the goal, rather than shifting their attention to other
goals (Fishbach and Dhar 2005; Fishbach, Dhar, and Zhang
2006). Furthermore, consumers may have more motivation to
resist using their emergency reserves when doing so helps
them reach their superordinate goal (e.g., not using two
“emergency skip”days from the gym helps a person become
more fit.) Relatedly, superordinate goals that are intrinsically,
rather than extrinsically, motivated have been shown to affect
people’s behavior in pursuing goals (Wang and Mukhopadhyay
2012). Further research should explore whether reserve goals
are as effective with an intrinsic superordinate goal or in the
absence of a superordinate goal.
Emergency reserves may differ in size and type, which
may affect how people use them. If the reserve is too small, it
may not provide as much of a needed buffer. However, if it
is too big, people may no longer mentally encode it as a
reserve and instead start incorporating it into their goal,
encoding it as an easy goal instead. In addition, rather than
being consumed all at once or not at all (as in our studies),
emergency reserves can also be continuous. For example,
in a dieting context, people could have a supply of 1,000
emergency calories. Unlike the all-or-nothing reserves, peo-
ple may choose to use some of the reserve, but not all of it, at
a given time. Exploring and understanding more about these
intricacies of emergency reserves will help us design the
most optimal emergency reserves.
Conclusion
This article provides an innovative strategy of structuring
goals to provide a sense of flexibility while still maintaining
stringency through the addition of an emergency reserve. The
emergency reserve can be applied to a variety of different
goals, such as saving money in the financial domain, studying
for a test in the education domain, or trying to lose weight in
the food/exercise domain. These are long-term goals that
people consistently have trouble achieving. By exploring
more about the mechanisms and applications of emergency
reserves, we can understand more generally about how to
help consumers initially pursue their goals as well as succeed
at many of the long-term goals that they have been struggling
to achieve.
REFERENCES
Ainslie, George (2001), Breakdown of Will. Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Atkinson, John W. (1957), “Motivational Determinants of Risk-
Taking Behavior,”Psychological Review, 64 (6, Part 1), 359–72.
Bandura, Albert (1997), Self-Efficacy: The Exercise of Control. New
York: Freeman.
Cheema, Amar, and Dilip Soman (2006), “Malleable Mental Ac-
counting: The Effect of Flexibility on the Justification of
Attractive Spending and Consumption Decisions,”Journal of
Consumer Psychology,16(1),33–44.
Cochran, Winona, and Abraham Tesser (1996), “The ‘What the
Hell’Effect: Some Effects of Goal Proximity and Goal Framing
on Performance,”in Striving and Feeling: Interactions Among
Goals, Affect, and Self-Regulation, Leonard L. Martin and
Abraham Tesser, eds. New York: Patent, 99–120.
Etkin, Jordan, and Rebeca K. Ratner (2012), “The Dynamic Impact
of Variety Among Means on Motivation,”Journal of Consumer
Research, 38 (6), 1076–92.
508 JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, JUNE 2017
Fishbach, Ayelet, and Ravi Dhar (2005), “Goals as Excuses or
Guides: The Liberating Effect of Perceived Goal Progress on
Choice,”Journal of Consumer Research, 32 (3), 370–77.
Fishbach, Ayelet, Ravi Dhar, and Ying Zhang (2006), “Subgoals
as Substitutes or Complements: The Role of Goal Accessibil-
ity,”Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,91(2),
232–42.
Friman, Patrick C., Kathryn E. Hoff, Connie Schnoes, Kurt A.
Freeman, Douglas W. Woods, and Nathan Blum (1999), “The
Bedtime Pass: An Approach to Bedtime Crying and Leaving the
Room,”Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 153 (10),
1027–29.
Gollwitzer, Peter M. (1990), “Action Phases and Mind-Sets,”in
Handbook of Motivation and Cognition: Foundations of Social
Behavior, Vol. 2, E. Tory Higgins and Richard M. Sorrentino,
eds. New York: Guilford Press, 53–92.
Heath, Chip, Richard P. Larrick, and George Wu (1999), “Goals as
Reference Points,”Cognitive Psychology, 38 (1), 79–109.
Heath, Chip, and Jack B. Soll (1996), “Mental Budgeting and
Consumer Decisions,”Journal of Consumer Research, 23 (1),
40–52.
Heckhausen, Heinz (1977), “Achievement Motivation and Its
Constructs: A Cognitive Model,”Motivation and Emotion, 1 (4),
283–329.
Heckhausen, Heinz, and Peter M. Gollwitzer (1987), “Thought
Contents and Cognitive Functioning in Motivational Versus
Volitional States of Mind,”Motivation and Emotion, 11 (2),
101–20.
Henderson, Pamela W., and Robert A. Peterson (1992), “Mental
Accounting and Categorization,”Organizational Behavior and
Human Decision Processes, 51 (1), 92–117.
Huang, Szu-Chi, and Ying Zhang (2011), “Motivational Conse-
quences of Perceived Velocity in Consumer Goal Pursuit,”
Journal of Marketing Research, 48 (December), 1045–56.
Huang, Szu-Chi, and Ying Zhang (2013), “All Roads Lead to Rome:
The Impact of Multiple Attainment Means on Motivation,”
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104 (2), 236–48.
Kahneman, Daniel, and Amos Tversky (1979), “Prospect Theory:
An Analysis of Decisions Under Risk,”Econometrica, 47 (2),
263–91.
Kahneman, Daniel, and Amos Tversky (1984), “Choices, Values,
and Frames,”American Psychologist, 39 (4), 341–50.
Krishnamurthy, Parthasarathy, and Sonja Prokopec (2010), “Re-
sisting That Triple-Chocolate Cake: Mental Budgets and Self-
Control,”Journal of Consumer Research, 37 (1), 68–79.
Kruglanski, Arie W., Antonio Pierro, and Anna Sheveland (2011),
“How Many Roads Lead to Rome? Equifinality Set-Size and
Commitment to Goals and Means,”European Journal of Social
Psychology, 41 (3), 344–52.
Locke, Edwin A., and Gary P. Latham (1990), A Theory of Goal
Setting & Task Performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice
Hall.
Locke, Edwin A., Karyll N. Shaw, Lise M. Saari, and Gary P.
Latham (1981), “Goal Setting and Task Performance: 1969–1980,”
Psychological Bulletin,90(1),125–52.
Milkman, Katherine L., Julia A. Minson, and Kevin G.M. Volpp
(2013), “Holding the Hunger Games Hostage at the Gym: An
Evaluation of Temptation Bundling,”Management Science,
60 (2), 283–99.
Moore, Brie A., Patrick C. Friman, Alan E. Fruzzetti, and Ken
MacAleese (2007), “Brief Report: Evaluating the Bedtime Pass
Program for Child Resistance to Bedtime—A Randomized,
Controlled Trial,”Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 32 (3),
283–87.
Morewedge, Carey K., Leif Holtzman, and Nicholas Epley (2007),
“Unfixed Resources: Perceived Costs, Consumption, and the
Accessible Account Effect,”Journal of Consumer Research,
34 (4), 459–67.
Polivy, Janet (1976), “Perception of Calories and Regulation of
Intake in Restrained and Unrestrained Subjects,”Addictive Be-
haviors, 1 (3), 237–43.
Scott, Maura L., and Scott M. Nowlis (2013), “The Effect of Goal
Specificity on Consumer Goal Reengagement,”Journal of Con-
sumer Research, 40 (3), 444–59.
Shefrin, Hersh M., and Richard H. Thaler (1988), “The Behavioral
Life Cycle Hypothesis,”Economic Inquiry, 26 (4), 609–44.
Shin, Jihae, and Katherine L. Milkman (2016), “How Backup Plans
Can Harm Goal Pursuit: The Unexpected Downside of Being
Prepared for Failure,”Organizational Behavior and Human
Decision Processes, 135, 1–9.
Soman, Dilip, and Amar Cheema (2004), “When Goals Are
Counterproductive: The Effects of Violation of a Behavioral Goal
on Subsequent Performance,”Journal of Consumer Research,
31 (1), 52–62.
Soman, Dilip, and Amar Cheema (2011), “Earmarking and Parti-
tioning: Increasing Saving by Low-Income Households,”Journal
of Marketing Research, 48 (Special Issue), S14–22.
Spiller, Stephen A. (2011), “Opportunity Cost Consideration,”
Journal of Consumer Research, 38 (4), 595–610.
Stilley, Karen M., J. Jeffrey Inman, and Kirk L. Wakefield (2010),
“Planning to Make Unplanned Purchases? The Role of In-Store
Slack in Budget Deviation,”Journal of Consumer Research,
37 (2), 264–78.
Tangney, June P., Roy F. Baumeister, and Angie L. Boone (2004),
“High Self-Control Predicts Good Adjustment, Less Pathology,
Better Grades, and Interpersonal Success,”Journal of Person-
ality, 72 (2), 271–324.
Thaler, Richard (1980), “Toward a Positive Theory of Consumer
Choice,”Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 1 (1),
39–60.
Thaler, Richard (1985), “Mental Accounting and Consumer Choice,”
Marketing Science, 4 (3), 199–214.
Thaler, Richard (1990), “Anomalies: Saving, Fungibility, and
Mental Accounts,”Journal of Economic Perspectives,4(1),
193–205.
Tolman, Edward C. (1955), “Principles of Performance,”Psycho-
logical Review, 62 (5), 315–26.
¨
Ulk¨umen, Gulden, and Amar Cheema (2011), “Framing Goals to
Influence Personal Savings: The Role of Specificity and Construal
Level,”Journal of Marketing Research, 48 (December), 958–69.
Wang, Chen, and Anirban Mukhopadhyay (2012), “The Dynamics
of Goal Revision: A Cybernetic Multiperiod Test-Operate-Test-
Adjust-Loop (Total) Model of Self-Regulation,”Journal of
Consumer Research, 38 (5), 815–32.
Wertenbroch, Klaus (1998), “Consumption Self-Control by Ra-
tioning Purchase Quantities of Virtue and Vice,”Marketing
Science, 17 (4), 317–37.
Wilcox, Keith, Lauren G. Block, and Eric M. Eisenstein (2011),
“Leave Home Without It? The Effects of Credit Card Debt and
Available Credit on Spending,”Journal of Marketing Research,
48 (Special Issue), S78–90.
Zauberman, Gal, and John G. Lynch Jr. (2005), “Resource Slack
and Propensity to Discount Delayed Investments of Time Versus
Money,”Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 134 (1),
23–37.
Zhang, Ying, and Szu-Chi Huang (2010), “How Endowed Versus
Earned Progress Affects Consumer Goal Commitment and
Motivation,”Journal of Consumer Research, 37 (4), 641–54.
The Benefits of Emergency Reserves 509