Content uploaded by Mikael Andéhn
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Mikael Andéhn on Aug 02, 2016
Content may be subject to copyright.
1
The country of origin effect – Key issues and future direction
Mikael Andéhn[1], Alexei Gloukhovtsev & John Schouten
Aalto University School of Business
[1]Corresponding Author
Summary
The country of origin effect (COE) has been a central topic in scholarly international marketing
literature for over half a century, but the concept seems to have stubbornly resisted all attempts at
providing an encompassing account of how it comes to affect consumers in practice. Through an
approach which treats COE as a perceptual phenomenon that is contingent on various
psychological mechanisms this conceptual work revisits some three central theoretical issues of
COE research and attempt to ferret out tentative means of addressing some of these long lived
problems that have been identified in the literature to date.
Keywords: Country of origin, place, memory, judgement and decision making, international
marketing.
Introduction
The COE effect is generally thought of as a phenomenon that occurs when consumers infer that
the characteristics of a country transfers onto a product (Bilkey and Nes, 1982; Verlegh and
Steenkamp, 1999; Pharr, 2005), a brand (Keller, 1993; Holt et al., 2004) or even a service
(Javalgi et al., 2001; Thelen et al., 2010). In other words, if a particular country is associated
with, a particular trait, the consumer will be inclined to think that the products, brands or services
from that country will, if not share the trait, at least somehow be affected by it. This phenomenon
is generally thought of as having been the object of scientific inquiry since the 1960s, with either
Ernst Dichter (1962) or Robert D. Schooler (1965) typically being credited with having started
the systematic study of the effect in the literature. The practical relevance of this phenomenon is
not difficult to fathom, as it would indicate that marketers could use the country-of-origin as a
pre-packaged quality indicator. In a world where differentiation has become increasingly difficult
to achieve (Baker and Ballington, 2002) and where the consequent costs of brand development
have skyrocketed (Bluemelhuber et al. 2007) the appeal of the country-of-origin effect is rather
2
clear. Yet in spite of its inquiry and apparent practical managerial relevance and resounding
proliferation as a field of scholarly inquiry, having been referred to as the most studied aspect of
international marketing (Tan and Farley, 1987), with numerous scholars vouching for its pivotal
role as a factor in international marketing efforts (Johansson, 1993; Peterson and Jolibert, 1995;
Batra et al., 2000; Papadopoulos and Heslop, 2002; Magnusson et al., 2011) and having
generated in the excess of 800 scholarly articles to date (Usunier, 2006), the COE is often
claimed to be undertheorized (Nebenzahl et al., 1997) and its practical relevance is sometimes
described as uncertain (Samiee, 1994; O’Shaughnessy and O’Shaughnessy, 2000; Samiee et al.,
2005; Samiee 2011; Usunier, 2006; 2011). In the present work we will explore some key issues
that constitute a frontier of the understanding of the effect, in the hope of arriving at a clearer
understanding of how the COE could be more precisely understood. These issues, in the end of
our argument, will lead to a proposal for a redefinition of the effect. The questions the present
work seeks to address are listed below.
How is the effect sourced, and what is an appropriate delineation of this construct?
What is “origin” in the context of the COE?
How are consumers influenced by the COE in their decision making from a
psychological perspective?
Arguments pertaining to these three issues will be presented in three short chapters, followed by a
concluding note. We begin with the issue of how to make sense of the construct from which the
COE is generally believed to be sourced – the country image, and the means of this “sourcing”
origin itself.
Country image and origin – facets and delineation
It seems like an inevitable conclusion that a phenomenon labelled the country-of-origin effect
would be highly contingent to country image. One of the historically most central contributions to
this concept is that of Akira Nagashima who described image as “ideas, emotional background
and connotations associated to a concept” (1970; 68), and explained how these ideas come to
transfer to products through the vehicle of being “made in” a specific country. Notable
developments of how the construct of country image has been used in the literature to date
include among other contributions the development of a scale to measure it (Martin and Eroglu,
1992). Its division into country image and product country image (Zeugner-Roth and
Diamantopolous, 2009), to distinguish between notions of a country writ-large and those that are
more directly relevant to product evaluation. The further division of the concept into basic origin
image, product origin image and category origin image (Josiassen et al. 2013) to account for the
potent moderating effect evoked by product categories.
Product categories is a crucial problem for understanding the COE. There is a very large number
of scholarly publications that deal with this issue at length (Wang and Lamb, 1983; Roth and
Romeo, 1992; Verlegh and Steenkamp, 1999; O’Shaughnessy and O’Shaughnessy, 2000;
3
Usunier and Cestré, 2007; Thanasuta et al., 2009), and it appears as if recent studies that compare
the effect evoked by a general country image versus the effect related to some interrelation
between country image and categories conclude that product categories generally play a central
role in determining the influence evoked by a COE (Josiassen et al., 2013; Andéhn et al., 2015).
Country image, it seems, operates along a continuum of how it applies to the evaluation at hand,
influencing consumer judgment through a simultaneous effect of its attributed meaning ranging
from what is general to what is pertinent only for the specific context of the evaluation.
This appears to be a feasible explanation of how a country image could affect evaluation. But to
what does country image in particular owe this ability to influence consumer evaluation? Are
countries really a category of constructs that hold some particular power in the context of
evaluation? The answer to that question appears to be no. Indeed there has been several studies
that have found that COEs can indeed be sourced from regions (Orth et al., 2005; Van Ittersum et
al., 2003; Rasmussen and Lockshin, 1999) or cities (Lentz et al., 2007). Some authors have made
the argument that it is not countries but places that represents the appropriate level of abstraction
in the context of the country of origin effect (Josiassen et al., 2013; Andéhn, 2013). Given these
observations it seems like the time has come to forgo the use of the term “country” in country of
origin in favor of the more encompassing and appropriate term “place”.
“Country” may not be the only term that has appears to less than optimal when describing the
situation in which a COE occurs. Arguably “origin” is a potent indicator of linkage between place
and some consumable object, but this origin is not contingent on some objective means of
establishing this linkage beyond what the consumer perceives it to be (Thakor and Kohli, 1996;
Thakor and Lavack, 2003; Josiassen and Harzing, 2008; Magnusson et al. 2011). Indeed “origin”,
as it pertains to the situation of a COE, may be better labeled “association” as even a
misidentified origin may result in a potent COE (Magnusson et al., 2011). In turn this would
collapse any division of origin into for instance country-of-assembly, country-of-design or
country-of-manufacture (cf. Insch and McBride, 1998) into a simple matter of how well these
various aspects would represent a proxy for which country consumers actually associate to a
given product, good or service.
Judgment, decision making and the COE
In their seminal review of the literature on COE Verlegh and Steenkamp (1999) made a central
observation; The overwhelming majority (albeit with a few exceptions (cf. Liu and Johnsson,
2005)), of studies on the country-of-origin effect have focused on consumption situations were
cognitive elaboration, or where the consumer is made to consciously consider the implications of
a certain origin, occurs. This observation is corroborated by Bloemer et al. (2009) who clearly
distinguish between consumption situations which may trigger cognitive elaboration and those
that do not, expressively stating that the extant theories of COE encapsulates only the former of
these two consumption situations.
If consumers are thought to always consciously deliberate their consumption decisions the
argument that country-of-origin effects are irrelevant, based on poor origin recognition accuracy
4
(Liefeld, 2004; Samiee et al. 2005), or that consumers do not report using country-of-origin as an
important cue in product evaluation (Balabanis and Diamantopoulos, 2008) appears entirely
reasonable. However, conscious elaboration may not be the norm in consumption situations; in
fact, mounting evidence suggests that most consumer decision making occurs without conscious
deliberation. Decision making, it seems, should be thought of as the result of primarily
unconscious processes (Fitzsimons et al., 2002).
The subject of how humans make decisions has been a core concern of psychologists for a very
long time. Within the context of economics the decision maker was, in the early days of the study
of decision making, thought of as being a rational actor, or as a natural statistician concerned with
utility maximization (Peterson and Beach, 1967), who would remain rational unless mislead by
his base instincts and emotions. This conceptualization of the “rational man” was put to rest in
favor of a “bounded rational man” by Herbert Simon (1955). He identified several constraints in
human decision capabilities that would render absolute rationality unfeasible. Building on the
works of Simon, scholars from different fields continued to identify situations in which human
decision-making would be guided by heuristics, or shortcuts in reasoning, which would prevent
rationality geared towards utility maximization (Kahneman and Tversky, 1979; Tversky and
Kahneman, 1981). This rather pessimistic outlook on human decision making capabilities did not
remain unopposed, as some scholars have noted that the heuristics employed in human decision
making situations actually serve to help make rather good decisions if the time constrains and
other factors that surround decision making are considered (Gigerenzer and Goldstein, 1996;
Gigerenzer and Brighton, 2009).
Parallel to this development, the idea of a rational mind and an irrational emotional body, came
under criticism in favor of a view of emotions as playing a vital role in the decision making
process (Zajonc, 1968; Damasio, 1994; Bechara et al., 1996; LeDoux, 1997). One important role
of emotions appears to be to allow the our conscience to access the vast processing capabilities of
the unconscious systems in the form of emotional feedback derived from an unconscious analysis
(Bechara et al., 1996; Lehrer, 2010) it has also been suggested that even these unconscious
analyses make use of so called heuristics (Gigerenzer, 2007) or logical shortcuts like halo effects
which is a common means of conceptualizing how a COE influences behavior (Han, 1989).
This proposed role of emotions also offers a tentative explanation of how humans can make
complex decisions given the constraints of our conscious processing capabilities. One early study
of the processing capabilities of the human conscience indicated that humans are only capable of
handling about seven pieces of data simultaneously (Miller, 1955), and more recent studies have
suggested that this modest capability may be even more limited than Miller proposed
(Dijksterhuis and Nordgren, 2006). In contrast, the unconscious processing system has been
shown to have the capacity to process enormous quantities of data (Dijksterhuis et al., 2000;
Dijksterhuis and Nordgren 2006), which suggests that decisions, to large extent, occur beyond
our conscious awareness (Bechara et al., 1996; Bargh, 2002).
It would seem then, that human learning and subsequent decision making is an integrative
process between the conscious and the unconscious systems, with one sometimes guiding the
other (Whittlesea and Wright, 1997; Baumeister et al., 2011). Thus, human decisions are not only
the results of conscious deliberative processes but rather far more complex. In the context of
Verlegh and Steenkamp’s (1999) as well as Bloemer et al.’s (2009) observations that COE
5
research has focused almost exclusively on consumption situations in which cognitive elaboration
is likely to occur, the brief overview on the advances in the understanding of human decision
making based on contemporary psychological research presented above, exposes a palpable gap
in the country-of-origin effect literature.
Conclusions
The present paper argues that it is ill-advised to assume that the country-of-origin effect is
irrelevant as a marketing phenomenon based solely on the observation that consumers generally
perform poorly in unassisted brand origin recognition tests (Samiee et al., 2005). First, as
Magnusson et al. (2011) showed, misidentifying a brand´s origin does not necessarily mean that a
country of origin effect will not occur; but, rather that the determining factor of a brand’s origin,
as it pertains to the country of origin effect, is the consumer’s perception of it. If, for instance, a
consumer believes that Samsung is Japanese, it is possible that this consumer’s judgments of
Samsung will be affected by how the consumer feels about Japan. Magnusson et al. (2011) did
not only show that even a “misidentified” origin matters but, by extrapolation, also provided
strong support of a view of the country of origin effect that is derived entirely from the perceptual
world of consumers. This is in opposition to the actual geographical location of corporate
headquarters, manufacturing facilities or any other factor in the world beyond the consumer’s
perception. These findings have far reaching implications for the conceptualization of the country
of origin effect, as well as calling for a re-evaluation of the meaning of the generally poor
performance on BORA tests for the relevance of the country of origin effect phenomenon.
The second requirement, about consumer’s conscious deliberation, is also questionable. Indeed, it
appears to be incongruent with the general direction of the findings of recent psychological
studies, which have emphasized the importance of unconscious processes in the formation of
consumer attitude and decision making (Fitzsimons et al., 2002). An extrapolation of the results
of these studies would indicate that consumers can, not only unconsciously attribute meanings to
entities such as countries, but can also be affected by these attributed meanings whilst making
decisions or forming attitudes towards entities such as brands. As a result, the COE may be seen
as a phenomenon that bears a great similarity to a stereotype-induced bias in the sense that it is
learned, activated and influences decisions without the knowledge or control of the decision
maker.
COE indeed has the potential, as suggested in earlier studies, to influence the formation of
consumer attitude and their decision making, the question that arises then is two-fold: what
makes countries (or perhaps places, writ large, as recent studies on region- and city-of-origin
effects suggest (Rasmussen and Lockshin, 1999; van Ittersum et al., 2003; Orth et al., 2005;
Lentz et al., 2007)) such potent purveyors of meaning and, how can the commercial relevance of
place image be understood? Future studies on the topic of country-of-origin effect would do well
to attempt to ground their propositions in theory in relevant fields and not shy away from drawing
on advances made in, for instance, psychology and geography as a means of making sense of the
COO effect phenomenon. The “self-referential” trend in COO research, as identified by Usunier
(2006), is a hindrance in the advancement of understanding this phenomenon and a widening of
the scope seems most appropriate if the field is to make meaningful progress in the near future.
6
References
Andéhn, M., Nordin, F., & Nilsson, M. E. (2015). Facets of country image and brand equity:
Revisiting the role of product categories in country‐of‐origin effect research. Journal of
Consumer Behaviour. DOI: 10.1002/cb.1550
Andéhn, M. (2013). Place-of-origin effects on brand equity: explicating the evaluative pertinence
of product categories and association strength. Stockholm University Press: Stockholm.
Baker, M.J. & Ballington, L. (2002) "Country of origin as a source of competitive advantage"
Journal of Strategic Marketing, 10 (2002): 157-168.
Balabanis, G. and Diamantopoulos, A. (2008) “Brand Origin Identification by Consumers: a
Classification Perspective”. Journal of International Marketing, Vol. 16 No. 1, pp. 39-71.
Batra, R., Ramaswamy, V., Alden, D.L., Steenkamp, J-B.E.M. and Ramachander, S. (2000)
“Effects of Brand Local and Non-local Origin on Consumer Attitudes in Developing Countries”.
Journal of Consumer Psychology, Vol. 9 No. 2, pp. 83-95.
Bargh, J.A. (2002) “Losing Consciousness: Automatic Influences on Consumer Judgment,
Behavior, and Motivation” Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 29, No. 2, pp. 280-285
Baumeister, R.F., Masicampo, E.J. and Vohs, K.D. (2011) “Do Conscious Thoughts Cause
Behavior” The Annual Review of Psychology, Vol. 62 No 1, pp. 331-361.
Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D. and Damasio, A.R. (1997) “Deciding Advantageously
Before Knowing the Advantageous Strategy” Science, Vol. 275, pp. 1293-1295.
Bilkey, W.J. and Nes, E. (1982) “Country-of-Origin Effects on Product Evaluation”. Journal of
International Business Studies, Vol. 13 No. 1, pp. 89-99.
Bloemer, J., Brijs, K. and Kasper, H. (2009) “The CoO-ELM model – A theoretical framework
for the cognitive processes underlying country of origin-effects” European Journal of Marketing,
Vol. 43, No. 1/2, pp. 62-89.
Bluemelhuber C., Carter, L.L. and Lambe, J.C. (2007) “Extending the view of brand alliance
effects” International Marketing Review, Vol. 24 No. 4, pp. 427-443.
Damasio A, 1994. Descartes’s Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain. New York:
Gosset/Putnam
Dichter, E. (1962) “The world customer” Harvard Business Review, Vol. 40 No 4, pp. 113-122.
Dijksterhuis, A., Bos, M.W., Nordgren, F.L. and van Baaren, R.B. (2000) “On Making the Right
Choice: The Deliberation-Without-Attention Effect” Science, Vol. 133 No. 5763, pp. 1005-1007.
7
Dijksterhuis, A., Smith, P.K., van Baaren, R.B. and Wigboldus, D.H.J. (2005) “The Unconscious
Consumer: Effects of Environment on Consumer Behaviour” Journal of Consumer Psychology,
Vol. 15 No. 3, pp. 193-202.
Dijksterhuis, A., and Nordgren, F.L. (2006) “A Theory of Unconscious Thought” Perspectives on
Psychological Science, Vol. 1 No. 2, pp. 95-109.
Fitzsimons et al, (2002) “Non-Conscious Influences on Consumer Choice”, Marketing Letters,
Vol. 13 No. 3, pp. 269-279.
Gigerenzer, G., and Goldstein, D.G. (1996) “Reasoning the fast and frugal way: Models of
bounded rationality” Psychological Review. Vol. 103 No. 4, pp. 650-669.
Gigerenzer, G. (2007) Gut feelings: The intelligence of the unconscious. New York: Viking.
Gigerenzer, G. and Brighton, H. (2009) “Homo Heuristicus: Why Biased Minds Make Better
Inferences” Topics in Cognitive Science, Vol. 1, No. 1, pp. 107-143.
Han, C. M. (1989). Country image: halo or summary construct?. Journal of marketing research,
Vol. 26 No. 2, pp. 222-229.
Holt, D.B., Quelch, J. and Taylor, E. (2004) “How Global Brands Compete” Harvard Business
Review, Vol. 82 No. 9, pp. 68-75.
Insch, G.S. and McBride, J.B. (1998) “Decomposing the country-of-origin construct: An
empirical test of country do design, country of parts and country of assembly” Journal of
International Consumer Marketing, Vol. 10, No. 4, pp. 69-91
Javalgi, R.G., Cutler, B.D. & Winans, W.A. (2001) "At your service! Does country of origin
apply to services” Journal of Service Marketing, Vol. 15 No. 7, pp. 565-582.
Johansson (1993). Missing a strategic opportunity: Managers' denial of country-of-origin effects'.
London, Haworth Press.
Josiassen, A., Lukas, B. A., Whitwell, G. J., & Assaf, A. G. (2013). The halo model of origin
images: conceptualisation and initial empirical test. Journal of Consumer Behaviour, Vol. 12 No.
4, pp. 253-266.
Josiassen, A., & Harzing, A. W. (2008). Comment: Descending from the ivory tower: reflections
on the relevance and future of country‐of‐origin research. European Management Review, Vol. 5
No. 4, pp. 264-270.
Kahneman, D and Tversky, A. (1979) “Prospect Theory: An Analysis of Decision Under Risk”
Econometrica, Vol. 47 No. 2 pp. 263-292.
Keller, K.L. (1993) “Conceptualizing, measuring and managing customer-based brand equity”.
Journal of Marketing. Vol. 57, No. 1, pp. 1-22.
8
LeDoux, J. (1997) The emotional brain. New York: Weidenfeld.
Lehrer, J. (2009), The Decisive Moment: How the Brain Makes up Its Mind, Canongate books
Ltd, Ediburough.
Lentz, P., Holzmüller, H. and Schirrmann, E. (2007) “City of origin effects in the German beer
market: transferring an international construct to a local context‟, Advances in International
Marketing, Vol. 17 No 3, pp. 251-274.
Liefeld, J.P. (2004) “Consumer Knowledge and use of country-of-origin information at the point
of purchase” Journal of Consumer Behavior, Vol. 4 No. 2, pp. 85-96.
Liu, S.S. and Johnson, K.F. (2005) “The Automatic Country-of-Origin Effects on Brand
Judgments” Journal of Advertising, Vol. 34, No. 1 pp. 87-97.
Magnusson, P., Westjohn, S.A. and Zdravkovic, S. (2011) “‘What, I thought Samsung was
Japanese’: Accurate or not, perceived country of origin matters”, International Marketing
Review, Vol. 28 No. 5, pp. 454-72.
Martin, I. M., and Eroglu, S. (1993). Measuring a multi-dimensional construct: country image.
Journal of business research, Vol. 28 No. 3, pp. 191-210.
Miller, (1956), "The Magical Number Seven, Plus or Minus Two: Some Limits on Our Capacity
For Processing Information" The Psychological Review, Vol. 63 No. 2 pp. 81-97.
Nagashima, A. (1970) "A Comparison of Japanese and U.S. Attitudes Towards Foreign
Products" Journal of Marketing, Vol. 34 (January): pp. 68-74.
Nebenzahl, I.D., Jaffé, E.D. and Lampert, S.I. (1997) “Towards a theory of country image effect
on product evaluation” Management International Review, Vol. 37 No. 1, pp. 27-49.
O’Shaughnessy, J. and O’Shaughnessy, N.J. (2000) “Treating the Nation as a Brand: Some
Neglected Issues” Journal of Makromarketing, Vol. 20 No. 56, pp. 56-64.
Orth, U.R., Wolf, M.M.G. and Dodd, T.H. (2005) “Dimensions of wine region equity and their
impact on consumer preference” Journal of Product & Brand Management, Vol. 14 No. 2, pp.
88-97
Papadopoulos, N. (1993). What product and country images are and are not in Product-country
images: Impact and role in international marketing, Edited by N. Papadopoulos, and L.A. Heslop.
New York: International Business Press, pp. 3-38.
Peterson, C.R. and Beach, L.R. (1967) “Man as an intuitive statistician” Psychological Bulletin,
Vol. 68 No, 1, pp. 29–46.
9
Peterson, R.A. and Jolibert, J.P.J (1995) “A Meta-Analysis of Country-of-Origin Effects” Journal
of International Business Studies, Vol. 26 No. 4 pp. 83-101.
Pharr, J.M. (2005) “Synthesizing Country-of-Origin Research from the Last Decade: Is the
Concept still Salient in an Era of Global Brands”. Journal of Marketing, Vol. 13 No. 4, pp. 34-
45.
Rasmussen, M. and Lockshin, L. (1999) "Wine Choice Behaviour: The Effect of Regional
Branding", International Journal of Wine Marketing, Vol. 11 No. 1, pp. 36-46.
Roth, M.S. and Romeo, J.B. (1992) “Matching Product Category and Country Image Perception:
A Framework for Managing Country-of-Origin Effects” Journal of International Business
Studies, Vol. 23 No. 3, pp. 477-497.
Samiee, S. (1994) “Consumer Evaluation of Products in a Global Market” International Business
Studies, Vol. 25 No. 3, pp. 579-604.
Samiee, S., Shimp, T.A. and Sharma, S. (2005) “Brand Origin Recognition Accuracy: Its
Antecedents and Consumers' Cognitive Limitations” Journal of International Business Studies.
Vol. 36 No. 4, pp. 379-397.
Samiee, S. (2011) “Resolving the impasse regarding research on the origins of products and
brands” International Marketing Review, Vol. 28 No. 5 pp. 473-485.
Schooler, R.D. (1965) “Product Bias in the Central American Common Market” Journal of
Marketing Research, Vol. 2 No. 4, pp. 394-397.
Simon, H. A. (1955) “A behavioral model of rational choice”. Quarterly Journal of Economics.
Vol. 69 No.1 pp. 99–118.
Tan, C.T. and Farley, J.U. (1987) “The Impact of Cultural Patterns on Cognition and Intension”
Journal of Consumer Marketing, Vol. 13 No. 3 pp. 27-42.
Thakor, M.V. and Kohli, C.S. (1996) “Brand origin: conceptualization and review” Journal of
Consumer Marketing, Vol. 13 No. 3, pp. 27-42.
Thakor, V.M. and Lavack, A.M. (2003) “Effect of perceived brand origin associations on
consumer perceptions of quality”. Journal of Product and Brand Management, Vol. 12 No. 6, pp.
394-407.
Thanasuta, K., Patoomsuwan, T., Chaimahawong, V. and Chiaravutthi, Y. (2009) “Brand and
country of origin valuations of automobiles” Asia Pacific Journal of Marketing and Logistics,
Vol. 21 No. 3, pp. 355-375.
Thelen, S.T., Honeycutt, E.D. Murphy, T.P. (2010) “Services offshoring: Does perceived service
quality affect country-of-service origin preference?” Managing Service Quality, Vol. 20 No. 3,
pp. 196-212
10
Tversky, A. Kahneman, D., (1981), "The Framing of Decisions and the Psychology of Choice"
Science, Vol. 211 No. 30, pp. 453-458.
Usunier, J-C. (2006) “Relevance in business research: the case of country-of-origin research in
marketing.” European Management Review, Vol. 3 No. 2006, pp. 60-73.
Usunier, J. C., and Cestre, G. (2007) “Product ethnicity: Revisiting the match between products
and countries.” Journal of international marketing, Vol. 15 No. 3, pp. 32-72.
Usunier, J-C. (2011) “The shift from manufacturing to brand origin: suggestions for improving
COO relevance” International Marketing Review, Vol. 28 No. 5, pp. 486-496.
Van Ittersum, K., Candel, M. J., and Meulenberg, M. T. (2003). The influence of the image of a
product's region of origin on product evaluation. Journal of Business research, Vol. 56 No. 3, pp.
215-226.
Verlegh, P.W.J. and Steenkamp, J-B.E.M. (1999) “A Review and Meta-Analysis of Country of
Origin Research” Journal of Economic Psychology, Vol. 20 No. 5, pp. 521-546.
Wang, C-K. and Lamb, C.W.Jr. (1983) “The Impact of Selected Environmental Forces Upon
Consumers' Willingness to Buy Foreign Products” Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science,
Vol. 11 No. 2, pp. 71-84.
Whittlesea, B.W.A. and Wright, R.L. (1997) “Implicit (and explicit) learning: Acting adaptively
without knowing the consequences” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, &
Cognition, Vol. 23 No. 1, pp. 181-200.
Zeugner-Roth, K.P. and Diamantopoulos, A. (2010) “Advancing the country image construct”
Journal of Business Research, Vol. 63 No. 4, pp. 446-449.
Zajonc, R.B. (1968) “Attitudinal Effects of Mere Exposure” Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, Vol. 9 No 2, pp. 1-27.