ArticlePDF AvailableLiterature Review

A Citation-Based Analysis and Review of Significant Papers on Timing and Time Perception

Frontiers
Frontiers in Neuroscience
Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Time is an important dimension of brain function, but little is yet known about the underlying cognitive principles and neurobiological mechanisms. The field of timing and time perception has witnessed tremendous growth and multidisciplinary interest in the recent years with the advent of modern neuroimaging and neurophysiological approaches. In this article, I used a data mining approach to analyze the timing literature published by a select group of researchers (n = 202) during the period 2000–2015 and highlight important reviews as well as empirical articles that meet the criterion of a minimum of 100 citations. The qualifying articles (n = 150) are listed in a table along with key details such as number of citations, names of authors, year and journal of publication as well as a short summary of the findings of each study. The results of such a data-driven approach to literature review not only serve as a useful resource to any researcher interested in timing, but also provides a means to evaluate key papers that have significantly influenced the field and summarize recent progress and popular research trends in the field. Additionally, such analyses provides food for thought about future scientific directions and raises important questions about improving organizational structures to boost open science and progress in the field. I discuss exciting avenues for future research that have the potential to significantly advance our understanding of the neurobiology of timing, and propose the establishment of a new society, the Timing Research Forum, to promote open science and collaborative work within the highly diverse and multidisciplinary community of researchers in the field of timing and time perception.
Content may be subject to copyright.
MINI REVIEW
published: 15 July 2016
doi: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00330
Frontiers in Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 1July 2016 | Volume 10 | Article 330
Edited by:
Andrea Ravignani,
Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Belgium
Reviewed by:
Warren H. Meck,
Duke University, USA
Marshall Gilmer Hussain Shuler,
Johns Hopkins University, USA
*Correspondence:
Sundeep Teki
sundeep.teki@gmail.com
Specialty section:
This article was submitted to
Auditory Cognitive Neuroscience,
a section of the journal
Frontiers in Neuroscience
Received: 12 April 2016
Accepted: 30 June 2016
Published: 15 July 2016
Citation:
Teki S (2016) A Citation-Based
Analysis and Review of Significant
Papers on Timing and Time
Perception. Front. Neurosci. 10:330.
doi: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00330
A Citation-Based Analysis and
Review of Significant Papers on
Timing and Time Perception
Sundeep Teki *
Department of Physiology, Anatomy and Genetics, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
Time is an important dimension of brain function, but little is yet known about the
underlying cognitive principles and neurobiological mechanisms. The field of timing
and time perception has witnessed tremendous growth and multidisciplinary interest
in the recent years with the advent of modern neuroimaging and neurophysiological
approaches. In this article, I used a data mining approach to analyze the timing literature
published by a select group of researchers (n=202) during the period 2000–2015
and highlight important reviews as well as empirical articles that meet the criterion of
a minimum of 100 citations. The qualifying articles (n=150) are listed in a table along
with key details such as number of citations, names of authors, year and journal of
publication as well as a short summary of the findings of each study. The results of
such a data-driven approach to literature review not only serve as a useful resource to
any researcher interested in timing, but also provides a means to evaluate key papers
that have significantly influenced the field and summarize recent progress and popular
research trends in the field. Additionally, such analyses provides food for thought about
future scientific directions and raises important questions about improving organizational
structures to boost open science and progress in the field. I discuss exciting avenues
for future research that have the potential to significantly advance our understanding of
the neurobiology of timing, and propose the establishment of a new society, the Timing
Research Forum, to promote open science and collaborative work within the highly
diverse and multidisciplinary community of researchers in the field of timing and time
perception.
Keywords: timing, time perception, rhythm perception, music perception, interval timing, temporal processing,
citations, bibliometrics
INTRODUCTION
Natural sounds have a rich temporal structure, in the form of sequences of sounds that rapidly
change over time and result in dynamic states of perceptual organization. Natural sound sequences
like speech and music form sequences of temporal intervals, often evoking the percept of a rhythm.
How the brain processes time intervals and rhythmic sound sequences is an unresolved and
challenging problem, given the absence of dedicated neural systems for encoding time.
William James was one of the first psychologists to recognize time as a “sensation, and
heralded a longstanding interest and debate on the nature of time perception and its underlying
representation in the brain (James, 1890). William Gooddy, recognized the importance of motor
structures for timing from a neurological perspective and suggested that they act as “observers”
Teki A Citation-Based Review of Papers on Timing
of time (Gooddy, 1958). Braitenberg (1967) proposed the
cerebellum as an internal timekeeper and hypothesized that
parallel fibers act as delay lines and provide a means to represent
temporal patterns. In the 1970 and 1980s, electrophysiological
studies led by Llinas, Cohen and colleagues revealed the
specialization of the olivocerebellar circuits for temporal
representation (Llinas et al., 1974; Llinás and Yarom, 1981; Welsh
et al., 1995; see Yarom and Cohen, 2002 for a review). At the
same time, fundamental properties of timing behavior like scalar
property provided a theoretical foundation that formal models
of an internal clock must address (Church, 1984; Gibbon et al.,
1984). In the 1980s and 1990s, neuropsychological work in
patients with disorders of the cerebellum and basal ganglia (e.g.,
Ataxia, Parkinson’s) began to provide causal evidence for a role
of these brain regions in perceptual and motor timing (Ivry et al.,
1988; Ivry and Keele, 1989; Artieda et al., 1992; Pastor et al., 1992;
Ivry, 1993; Nichelli et al., 1996).
In the last two decades, however, scientific interest
and progress in understanding the neural codes and
mechanisms underlying temporal processing has advanced
rapidly, aided by technological developments in functional
neuroimaging techniques like magnetic resonance imaging
and magnetoencephalography; brain stimulation techniques
like transcranial magnetic stimulation and transcranial current
stimulation; as well as progress in neural recording methods with
the development of dense multi-electrode arrays, two-photon
calcium imaging, genetic and molecular biology tools including
the use of novel experimental animals models and optogenetic
targeting of specific cell-types for causal investigations amongst
others. Our understanding of the neural mechanisms and circuits
involved in temporal computations has significantly advanced
through the use of these new technologies and continues to shed
light on their underlying brain bases.
However, paralleling the recent advancements in the field is an
exponential growth in research output in terms of more research
articles, conference proceedings, and new journals. Therefore,
unlike in the previous decades, a synthesis of the research
advances in the field poses a significant challenge. Discovery of
knowledge represents an acute problem with a low “signal-to-
noise” threshold, and it is a veritable challenge for a new or
even a current investigator in the field to assimilate new ideas
and apply these concepts for designing innovative experimental
paradigms.
In order to make sense of the progress in the field of timing
and time perception in the last fifteen years, I have adopted
a data-mining approach to identify key review articles and
empirical papers, from a select group of authors that have
significantly impacted research on the cognitive and neural
principles of time perception. The process involved shortlisting
a group of established researchers in the field of timing, and
identifying articles published during the period 2000–2015 that
have received a minimum of 100 citations. Each qualifying article
(n=150) from this group of authors (n=202) is listed in Table 1
along with the number of citations, the rank of each article in
terms of number of citations as well as number of citations
normalized by time since publication, the names of the authors,
the name of the journal, the year of publication, whether the
article was an empirical study or a review, and a short summary
of each article.
KEY PAPERS ON TIMING AND TIME
PERCEPTION
To obtain a representative picture of the field, I examined
research articles by a select group of experts on timing and
time perception. These authors were selected on the basis
of their contribution to the recent special issue on “Interval
timing and skill learning: the multi sensory representation
of Time and Action” published in the Current Opinion
of Behavioral Sciences (Meck and Ivry, 2016; 75 authors)
as well as on the basis of membership of the recently
concluded European COST Action—Timely (http://www.timely-
cost.eu/?q=members_list; 127 authors). These 202 authors
represented research group all over the world (see Supplementary
material B for the complete list of authors), and covered
various aspects of timing research including psychophysics,
neuroimaging, modeling, and electrophysiology in both humans
and experimental animal models.
A number of metrics are commonly used to evaluate
the quality and impact of research articles including impact
factor, h-index, i-10 index amongst others. Although none of
these bibliometrics represent an unbiased estimate of research
impact nor are they accepted as standard across the scientific
community, the number of citations represents a useful metric
as it indicates the impact of a paper and how well the reported
findings are accepted and circulated in the field. It is not an
ideal measure, for the number of citations an article receives is
often skewed by the impact factor of the journal. In order to
draw reasonable conclusions about recent progress in the field,
articles that were published from 2000 to 2015 and indexed in
Google Scholar were considered eligible. Furthermore, to identify
the most impactful papers (ideas), a threshold of a minimum
of 100 citations was applied. As such a metric may be biased
toward older papers than more recent articles, a measure based
on the number of citations normalized by the number of years
since publication was also considered. Although it is possible to
design a more optimal multi-variate measure of research impact
(based on number of citations, impact factor of journal or novel
altmetrics including number of downloads, number of views and
circulation in social media amongst other variables), that is not
the motivation of the paper.
Using the above criteria, 150 papers were identified as
listed in Table 1 (references of these papers in Supplementary
material A; up-to-date as of April 10, 2016). These papers
covered topics related to perception of time, rhythm, music,
inter-sensory synchrony amongst others and used techniques
including psychophysics, neuroimaging, electrophysiology and
modeling. Out of the 150 papers, 52 papers were review articles
(34.7% of all articles; marked with an asterisk next to the number
of citations) that received an average of 271.7 citations (median:
183), i.e., one out of three prominent articles on timing in the
last ten years were review articles that either summarized the
current state of research or presented new hypotheses to drive
Frontiers in Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 2July 2016 | Volume 10 | Article 330
Teki A Citation-Based Review of Papers on Timing
TABLE 1 | List of 150 papers on timing and time perception from 2000 to present sorted according to the number of citations (minimum of 100 citations)
in Google Scholar collated on 10 April, 2016 (see Section Key Papers on Timing and Time Perception for More Details).
Citation and rank Reference Year Journal Summary
1305*[1, 1] Patel 2008 Oxford Uni Press A book that analyses music cognition in relation to language from the
standpoint of cognitive neuroscience.
1192*, [2, 2] Buhusi and Meck 2005 Nat. Rev. Neurosci. Time is represented in a distributed manner through coincidental
activation of cortico-striatal neuronal populations.
1054, [3, 4] Boroditsky 2001 Cogn. Psychol. Native language shapes how we think about time.
1036, [4, 6] Boroditsky 2000 Cognition Time structure is shaped by metaphorical mapping from experiential
domains like space.
719, [5, 13] Rao et al. 2001 Nat. Neurosci. Cortical-subcortical network mediates different components of
temporal processing.
623, [6, 3] Casasanto and Boroditsky 2008 Cognition Spatial information affects judgments about duration but not vice versa.
622*, [7, 14] Lewis and Miall 2003 Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. Timing is measured by automatic (motor) system and cognitive
(prefrontal and parietal) systems.
587*, [8, 12] Mauk and Buonomano 2004 Ann. Rev. Neurosci. Temporal processing depends on state-dependent changes in network
dynamics.
569*, [9, 15] Matell and Meck 2004 Cogn. Brain. Res. Striatal beat frequency model proposes basal ganglia as coincidence
detector of cortical and thalamic input.
551*, [10, 16] Ivry and Spencer 2004 Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. Cerebellum mediates precise timing and basal ganglia mediates
decisions for longer intervals.
512, [11, 11] Wittmann et al. 2006 Chronobiol. Int. Social jetlag, i.e., the discrepancy between social and biological timing
affects wellbeing and stimulant consumption.
469, [12, 10] Grahn et al. 2007 J. Cogn. Neurosci. Basal ganglia and Supplementary Motor Areas mediate beat
perception, in addition to motor production.
450, [13, 23] Coull et al. 2004 Science Attention to time is mediated by a corticostriatal network.
410*, [14, 45] Matell and Meck 2000 Bioessays Coincidence detection of neural activity represents a fundamental
mechanism of timing.
379*, [15, 47] Grondin 2001 Psychol. Bull. Weber’s law provides a framework for psychological models of time.
364*, [16, 25] Ivry et al. 2006 Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. Cerebellum provides an explicit representation of time.
364, [17, 50] Coull et al. 2000 Neuropsychologia Temporal orienting depends on sensory events and top-down
expectations.
360*, [18, 8] Grondin 2010 Att. Percept. Psychophys. Review of recent behavioral and neuroscientific studies of timing.
346, [19, 41] Spencer et al. 2003 Science Cerebellar patients can produce continuous rhythmic movements but
not discontinuous movements.
338*, [20, 19] Ivry and Schlerf 2008 Trends Cogn. Sci. Dedicated models of timing are preferred over intrinsic models.
333, [21, 24] Karmarkar and Buonomano 2007 Neuron Cortical networks can read out time as a result of intrinsic network
dynamics.
332*, [22, 5] Coull et al. 2011 Neuropsychopharmacology Review of neuroimaging, neuropsychological and
psychopharmacological aspects of timing.
320, [23, 21] Chen et al. 2008 Cereb. Cortex Passively listening to rhythms recruits motor regions of the brain.
318*, [24, 28] Droit-Volet and Meck 2007 Trends Cogn. Sci. Review of how emotional arousal and valence modulates attentional
time-sharing and clock speed.
318, [25, 29] Shuler and Bear 2006 Science Primary sensory cortex, like V1, mediates reward-timing activity.
315*, [26, 62] Lewkowicz 2000 Psychol. Bull. Temporal relations emerge in a hierarchical and sequential fashion.
306, [27, 17] Patel et al. 2009 Curr. Biol. Snowball, a cuckatoo, can spontaneously synchronize its movements
to a musical beat.
296, [28, 39] Morrone et al. 2005 Nat. Neurosci. Short intervals of time between two successive perisaccadic visual
stimuli (but not auditory) are underestimated.
289, [29, 51] Lewis and Miall 2003 Neuropsychologia Distinct brain areas encode time in the sub- and supra-second range.
287*, [30, 26] Wittmann and Paulus 2008 Trends Cogn. Sci. Review of how impulsivity affects perception of time and decision
making.
283, [31, 77] Penney et al. 2000 J. Exp. Psychol. Hum.
Perc. Perf.
Attention modulates the internal clock at different rates for auditory and
visual signals.
268, [32, 22] Winkler et al. 2009 Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
U.S.A.
Newborn infants show beat perception.
267, [33, 9] MacDonald et al. 2011 Neuron Hippocampal time cells encode successive moments during a
sequence of events.
(Continued)
Frontiers in Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 3July 2016 | Volume 10 | Article 330
Teki A Citation-Based Review of Papers on Timing
TABLE 1 | Continued
Citation and rank Reference Year Journal Summary
257*, [34, 18] Wiener et al. 2010 Neuroimage Meta analysis that suggests distinct for perceptual vs. motor timing;
SMA and right IFG are most commonly activated in various timing
tasks.
256*, [35, 71] Meck and Benson 2002 Brain Cogn. Frontostriatal circuits are involved in interval timing and shifting
attention between contexts.
241*, [36, 32] Meck et al. 2008 Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. Review that proposes striatum serves as a core timer, as part of a
distributed timing system.
241*, [37, 40] Nobre et al. 2007 Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. Review that describes how temporal expectations modulate perception
and action, and the underlying neural mechanisms.
241*, [38, 52] Meck 2005 Brain Cogn. Review of timing that suggests a distributed representation of time
across multiple neural systems.
240, [39, 70] Patel et al. 2003 Cognition Rhythms of French and English speech and music are different.
237*, [40, 35] Coull and Nobre 2008 Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. Review that suggests basal ganglia is key for explicit timing while
parietal and premotor areas mediate implicit timing.
235*, [41, 92] Nobre 2001 Neuropsychologia Optimization of behavior by temporal orienting is reflected in latency
and amplitude of ERPs.
234, [42, 99] Buonomano 2000 J. Neurosci. Neural circuits possess intrinsic synaptic mechanisms for timing.
231, [43, 83] Gentner et al. 2002 Lang. Cogn. Proc. Humans use spatial metaphors in temporal reasoning and language.
230, [44, 64] Vroomen et al. 2004 Cogn. Brain Res. Perception of temporal order is shaped by exposure to audio-visual
asynchronies.
222, [45, 89] Janata et al. 2002 Cogn. Aff. Behav. Neurosci. Attentive listening to music is mediated by domain-general areas.
220, [46, 38] Chen et al. 2008 J. Cogn. Neurosci. Musicians show greater prefrontal cortex activity vs. non-musicians
while tapping to complex auditory rhythms.
218, [47, 82] Matell et al. 2003 Behav. Neurosci. Striatal and cortical neurons encode time intervals in their firing rates.
213, [48, 109] Medina et al. 2000 J. Neurosci. Computer simulations show that cerebellum can learn adaptively timed
responses.
212*, [49, 42] Eagleman 2008 Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. Review summarizing illusions of time perception in humans.
208, [50, 65] Patel et al. 2005 Exp. Brain. Res. Beat perception and synchronization show modality specific benefits
for auditory vs. visual beat patterns.
207, [51, 36] Grahn and Rowe 2009 J. Neurosci. Putamen, SMA and premotor cortex are important for internal
generation of the beat and auditory motor coupling during beat
perception.
197, [52, 61] Meck 2006 Brain Res. Dopamine depleting lesions in different parts of the basal ganglia
shows dissociable effects on duration discrimination.
197, [53, 122] Cemgil et al. 2000 J. New Mus. Res. Kalman filter based approach can be used to track tempo.
187*, [54, 67] Lewis and Miall 2006 Trends Cogn. Sci. Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex mediates working memory as well as
timing.
186*, [55, 110] Buonomano and Karmarkar 2002 Neuroscientist Review that argues that time is coded by the population activity of a
large group of neurons.
185, [56, 43] Arvaniti 2009 Phonetica Review of work on rhythmic categorization which argues that timing is
distinct from rhythm.
185, [57, 112] Buhusi and Meck 2002 Behav. Neurosci. Dopamine modulates attentional components of interval timing.
184*, [58, 7] Merchant et al. 2013 Ann. Rev. Neurosci. Review that highlights the role of a core timing mechanism in the basal
ganglia and its interaction with context dependent areas.
183, [59, 56] Noesselt et al. 2007 J. Neurosci. Temporal correspondence between auditory and visual streams
modulates activity of multisensory STS as well as unisensory cortices.
182*, [60, 31] Kotz and Schwartze 2010 Trends Cogn. Sci. Review which suggests that temporal and speech processing is
processed by cortical and subcortical systems associated with motor
control.
182*, [61, 72] Patel 2006 Music Percept. Review that focuses on the evolutionary aspects of musical rhythm.
179*, [62, 34] Vroomen and Kreetels 2010 Att. Percept. Psychophys. Review that focuses on intersensory timing and mechanisms that
encode intersensory lags.
179, [63, 58] Burr et al. 2007 Nat. Neurosci. Short visual events are encoded by visual neural mechanisms with
localized receptive fields rather than by a centralized supramodal clock.
178, [64, 59] Wittmann et al. 2007 Exp. Brain Res. Posterior insula mediates delayed gratification of reward while striatum
encodes time delay.
(Continued)
Frontiers in Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 4July 2016 | Volume 10 | Article 330
Teki A Citation-Based Review of Papers on Timing
TABLE 1 | Continued
Citation and rank Reference Year Journal Summary
178, [65, 74] McAuley et al. 2006 J. Exp. Psychol. General Event timing profiles for a battery of perceptual-motor timing tasks vary
across the life span (4–95 years old).
177, [66, 27] Boroditsky et al. 2011 Cognition English and Mandarin speakers think about time differently.
177*, [67, 46] Wittmann 2009 Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B Review that discusses different models of time perception with a
particular focus on the insula as a core timer.
177, [68, 76] Zelaznik et al. 2006 J. Exp. Psychol. Hum.
Perc. Perf.
Repetitive tapping and drawing movements highlight explicit vs. implicit
timing.
175, [69, 101] Harrington et al. 2004 Brain Motor vs. clock variability in time reproduction and perception tasks
does not support a role for cerebellum in timekeeping.
175, [70, 128] Yarrow et al. 2001 Nature Perceptual fill-in during saccadic suppression underlies the illusion of
chronostasis.
174*, [71, 37] Block et al. 2010 Acta Psychol. Meta analysis that focuses on the effects of cognitive load on
prospective and retrospective duration judgments.
174, [72, 80] Chen et al. 2006 Neuroimage Metrical structure of musical rhythms modulates functional connectivity
between auditory and dorsal premotor cortex.
174, [73, 102] Droit-Volet et al. 2004 Cogn. Emot. The duration of emotional faces is overestimated compared to neutral
ones.
174, [74, 108] Nenadic et al. 2003 Exp. Brain Res. fMRI during a time estimation task shows activation in right putamen.
172*, [75, 123] Ivry and Richardson 2002 Brain Cogn. A multiple timer model accounts for timing and coordination of
repetitive movements.
169*, [76, 20] Allman and Meck 2012 Brain Review that focuses on distortions of time perception and timed
performance in various neurological and psychiatric conditions.
167*, [77, 68] Taatgen et al. 2007 Psychol. Rev. A time perception model based on adaptive control of thought-rational
can explain effects of attention and learning during time estimation.
165, [78, 133] Burle and Casini 2001 J. Exp. Psychol. Hum.
Perc. Perf.
Activation and attention have independent effects on timing
performance.
163, [79, 118] McAuley and Jones 2003 J. Exp. Psychol. Hum.
Perc. Perf.
Timing performance is enhanced when intervals fall on vs. off the beat.
162, [80, 107] Lewis et al. 2004 Neuropsychologia Brain activity during over-learned tapping varies with temporal
complexity of the sequence.
159, [81, 111] Harrington et al. 2004 Cogn. Brain Res. Event-related fMRI reveals brain areas subserving different aspects of
timing.
158, [82, 60] O’Reilly et al. 2008 J. Neurosci. Posterior cerebellum provides a temporal signal to cortical networks for
spatial orienting.
158, [83, 103] Matlock et al. 2005 Cogn. Sci. Fictive motion influences temporal reasoning.
156, [84, 104] Doherty et al. 2005 J. Neurosci. Combined spatial and temporal attention lead to enhanced P1
response.
156, [85, 132] Droit-Volet and Wearden 2002 Q. J. Exp. Psychol. Visual flicker increases the internal clock speed in young children.
155, [86, 125] Rubia et al. 2003 J. Abn. Child Psychol. Motor timing is impaired in children with ADHD and hyperactivity.
154*, [87, 94] Correa et al. 2006 Brain Res. Review that focuses on how temporal attention modulates the
amplitude and latency of ERPs like N2 and P300 components.
154*, [88, 114] Rubia and Smith 2004 Acta Neurobiol. Motor timing and time estimation is mediated by common brain
networks.
153, [89, 30] Nozaradan et al. 2011 J. Neurosci. EEG frequency tagging reveals neural entrainment to beat and meter.
152*, [90, 53] Rubia et al. 2009 Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B Review that suggests that impulsivity in ADHD is related to
compromised timing functions and dopamine dysregulation.
151*, [91, 54] Droit-Volet and Gil 2009 Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B Review that addresses the role of emotional context on timing.
151, [92, 81] Pariyadath and Eagleman 2007 PLoS ONE Repetition suppression underlies duration distortion.
151*, [94, 117] Coull 2004 Cogn. Brain Res. Frontal operculum is key for mediating attentional aspects of time
estimation.
151, [93, 129] Desain and Honing 2003 Perception Musical metro primes the perception of rhythmic categories.
150, [95, 33] Teki et al. 2011 J. Neurosci. Perception of relative and absolute time is mediated by distinct
networks based in the basal ganglia and the cerebellum, respectively.
148, [96, 84] Noulhiane et al. 2007 Emotion Emotional stimuli are judged longer than neutral stimuli, when balanced
for the levels of arousal.
(Continued)
Frontiers in Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 5July 2016 | Volume 10 | Article 330
Teki A Citation-Based Review of Papers on Timing
TABLE 1 | Continued
Citation and rank Reference Year Journal Summary
147, [97, 98] Kanai et al. 2006 J. Vis. Temporal frequency of a stimulus serves as the clock for perceived
duration.
145, [98, 55] Kotz et al. 2009 Cortex Review that focuses on the non-motor functions of basal ganglia with
particular emphasis on prediction in speech and language.
143, [99, 87] Styns et al. 2007 Hum. Mov. Sci. Walking speed is modulated by the tempo of musical and metronome
stimuli.
140, [100, 49] Fuhrman and Boroditsky 2010 Cogn. Sci. Temporal judgments in nonlinguistic tasks are influenced by culturally
specific spatial representations.
140*, [101, 115] Eagleman et al. 2005 J. Neurosci. Review of timing based on psychophysics, electrophysiology, imaging
and computational modeling.
139, [102, 105] Lewis and Miall 2006 Behav. Proc. Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex mediates working memory and posterior
parietal cortex and anterior cingulate attentional aspects of timing.
137, [103, 106] Rammsayer and Altenmuller 2006 Music Percept. Musicians perform better than non-musicians in temporal
discrimination but not temporal generalization tasks.
136, [104, 63] Grahn and Brett 2009 Cortex Parkinson’s patients show selective deficits in discrimination of
beat-based rhythms.
134, [105, 97] Keller et al. 2007 Consc. Cogn. Action simulation in ensemble musicians like pianists underlies
synchronization and self-recognition.
132*, [106, 66] Eagleman and Pariyadath 2009 Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B Energy expended in coding a stimulus represents its duration.
132, [107, 124] Navarra et al. 2005 Cogn. Brain Res. Temporal window for audiovisual integration is extended for
asynchronous speech and music.
131*, [108, 126] Lustig et al. 2005 Memory Striatum may detect oscillatory cortical firing in a coincident manner to
time brief intervals.
131*, [109, 145] Mauk et al. 2000 Curr. Biol. Cerebellum is key for movement through feedforward use of sensory
information via temporally specific learning.
130, [110, 113] Patel et al. 2006 J. Acoust. Soc. Am. Music reflects durational patterns in speech as well as patterns of
variability in pitch.
130, [111, 135] Hinton and Meck 2004 Cogn. Brain Res. fMRI activations show involvement of fronto-striatal circuits in interval
timing.
127, [112, 88] Ishihara et al. 2008 Cortex A mental time line exists from left to right along the horizontal axis in
space.
127, [113, 116] Matell et al. 2006 Psychopharm Methamphetamine produces a dose-dependent overestimation of time.
126, [114, 90] van Eijk et al. 2008 Att. Percept. Psychophys. Synchrony and temporal order judgment tasks produce different PSS
estimates.
126, [115, 91] Wassenhove et al. 2008 PLoS ONE Multisensory interactions influence perception of time: vision can
impact auditory temporal perception.
126, [116, 131] Correa et al. 2005 Psychon. Bull. Rev. Temporal orienting enhances perceptual processing.
125*, [117, 119] Ivry 2006 Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. Review that analyzes the role of the cerebellum as an internal clock.
124, [118, 75] Iversen et al. 2009 Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. Beta-band activity influences auditory rhythm perception.
124, [119, 93] Wearden et al. 2008 J. Exp. Psychol. Hum.
Perc. Perf.
Decreasing arousal affects performance on time perception tasks.
123*, [120, 95] Wearden and Lejeune 2008 Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A review of the conformity and violations of the scalar property in
human timing tasks.
123, [121, 138] Smith et al. 2003 Neuroimage Right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is involved in time perception, and
may serve as an accumulator.
122, [122, 78] Grahn and McAuley 2009 Neuroimage Individual differences in beat perception exist and modulate activity in
auditory and motor areas.
122, [123, 79] Zarco et al. 2009 J. Neurophys. Performance of rhesus monkeys and humans is compared on a
number of sub-second interval reproduction tasks.
121, [124, 136] Matell et al. 2004 Behav. Neurosci. Intermittent but not continuous administration of cocaine increases the
speed of internal clock.
121*, [125, 139] Wearden 2003 Time and Mind II Book chapter that reviews timing in the light of scalar expectancy
theory.
120, [126, 57] Boroditsky and Gaby 2010 Psychol. Sci. Pormpuraaw, an Australian Aboriginal community represent time
according to cardinal directions.
(Continued)
Frontiers in Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 6July 2016 | Volume 10 | Article 330
Teki A Citation-Based Review of Papers on Timing
TABLE 1 | Continued
Citation and rank Reference Year Journal Summary
119, [127, 48] Simen et al. 2011 J. Neurosci. A temporal integration model yields a firing-rate based representation
of time.
118, [128, 137] Correa et al. 2004 Percept. Psychophys. Temporal orienting effects are larger when temporal expectancy is
varied between and not within blocks.
118, [129, 144] Griffin et al. 2002 Neuropsychologia Spatial and temporal orienting optimize behavior through distinct
attentional processes.
118, [130, 146] Droit-Volet and Wearden 2001 J. Exp. Child Psychol. 8 year old children show higher temporal sensitivity than 3 and 5 year
old children.
117*, [131, 100] Keller 2008 Emerg. Comm. Review that addresses cognitive processes underlying joint action in
music performance.
117, [132, 127] Vatakis and Spence 2006 Brain Res. Cross-modal temporal discrimination performance is better for
audiovisual stimuli of lower complexity.
115, [133, 130] Effron et al. 2006 Emotion Embodiment plays a role in the emotional modulation of time.
114, [134, 140] Muller-Gethmann et al. 2003 Psychophysiol Temporal preparation enhances the processing speed of early evoked
potentials.
114, [135, 148] Lustig and Meck 2001 Psychol. Sci. Age-related changes in attentional resources affects interval timing.
113*, [136, 85] Buhusi and Meck 2009 Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B Attentional and memory resources for timing are shared between timed
and intruder events.
112*, [137, 86] Balsam and Gallistel 2009 Trends Neurosci. Review which suggests that associative learning depends on temporal
contiguity.
112, [138, 120] Droit-Volet et al. 2007 Behav. Proc. 5- and 8-year old children underestimate the duration of visual vs.
auditory signals.
112, [139, 121] Stetson et al. 2007 PLoS ONE Slowing of time during threatening events is a function of episodic
recollection, not perception.
111, [140, 69] Casasanto et al. 2010 Cogn. Sci. Spatial information influences temporal judgments more than time
affects spatial judgments in children as well as adults.
110, [141, 143] Lange et al. 2003 Psychophysiol Stimuli presented at attended vs. unattended moments in time yield an
enhanced N1 response.
109, [142, 134] Jahanshahi et al. 2006 J. Neurosci. Basal ganglia and cerebellum are involved in reproduction of both short
and long intervals.
108*, [143, 147] Wing 2002 Brain Cogn. Review that presents an information processing perspective on human
voluntary timing.
107, [144, 73] Jahanshahi et al. 2010 Brain Dopamine increases connectivity between caudate nucleus and
prefrontal cortex during motor timing.
106, [145, 96] Cummins 2009 J. Phonetics Rhythm affords synchronization among two speakers.
104, [146, 44] Arvaniti 2012 J. Phonetics Rhythm metrics for classification and cross-linguistic comparisons
should be used with caution.
104, [147, 141] Repp and Keller 2004 Q. J. Exp. Psychol. Period correction depends on intention, attention and awareness of
tempo changes whilst phase correction depends on intention.
104, [148, 149] Volz et al. 2001 Neuroreport Schizophrenic patients show hypo-activation in putamen and prefrontal
cortex during time estimation.
103*, [149, 142] MacDonald and Meck 2004 Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. A review that assesses the close correspondence between reaction
time and interval timing.
101*, [150, 150] Meck 2001 CRC Press A book that reviews functional and neural mechanisms of interval
timing in humans and animals.
Asterisks next to the number of citations denote review articles as opposed to empirical papers. The number of citations, name(s) of authors, year and journal of publication as well as
a brief summary is presented for each qualifying article. The authors’ names are hyperlinked to the corresponding article’s web page on Google Scholar. The numbers in the square
brackets next to the number of citations denote the rank of each article in terms of overall number of citations and the rank according to the number of citations normalized by years
since publication, respectively. References of all articles in this table are provided in supplementary information.
the field forward. The remaining empirical papers, 98 in all
(65.3% of all articles), received an average of 208 citations per
paper (median: 157). Normalizing the number of citations by
the number of years since publication to remove the bias due to
the “age” of each article revealed a similar trend—review articles
receive more citations (mean: 30.0; median: 21.8) than empirical
papers (mean: 20.7; median: 16.5). A brief one-sentence summary
of each study is also presented in the last column of Table 1, to
provide the reader an informed basis to select relevant papers for
more in-depth review.
There are several conclusions to be drawn from Table 1,
for instance—review articles tend to dominate the field in
Frontiers in Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 7July 2016 | Volume 10 | Article 330
Teki A Citation-Based Review of Papers on Timing
terms of number of citations while only an average of six
significant empirical papers are published every year (also see
Supplementary material C, D, and E). Although many of these
reviews are now “classic” in the field, even the most recent article
in the table is a review (Merchant et al., 2013a; 184 citations).
Among other things, this suggests that either the field is still in an
embryonic stage where review articles by established researchers
are needed to set the precedent on certain topics, or that the field
of timing is too diverse, and represents the intersection of various
sub-fields including time perception, rhythm perception, music
perception, temporal coding, inter sensory asynchrony, motor
timing and coordination, that is reflected in the diversity of topics
covered by the review articles.
It is not clear whether a similar analysis of the most recent
and highly cited papers in other prominent fields like memory,
vision, or decision-making will yield similar trends, e.g., ratio of
reviews to empirical studies but one could make a null hypothesis
that such a ratio may be smaller than for the highly diverse
and multidisciplinary field of timing. Alternatively, compared to
research topics like vision and memory that have been intensely
studied for several decades, the field of timing is still in a nascent
stage and does not boast of a large research community as
evidenced by the number of specialist journals on timing, or
number of exclusive workshops and meetings dedicated to timing
research.
FUTURE DIRECTIONS—SCIENTIFIC
Apart from organizational considerations, there are several new
scientific directions that the field can and should embrace
to achieve a more comprehensive understanding of the
neurobiology of natural timing behavior. Animal models of
timing focused on core timing networks including the basal
ganglia, cerebellum, premotor and parietal cortex (Grahn, 2012;
Schneider and Ghose, 2012; Teki et al., 2012; Merchant et al.,
2013a; Allman et al., 2014; Hayashi et al., 2015) will be key to
understanding the encoding of time by neuronal ensembles. Such
a line of work has been recently pioneered by Merchant and
colleagues in rhesus macaques that combines timing behaviors
and the examination of the underlying neuronal code in the
basal ganglia (Merchant et al., 2011, 2013b; Bartolo et al., 2014;
Bartolo and Merchant, 2015). Recent work by Mello et al. (2015)
and Gouvêa et al. (2015) further demonstrated that a population
code for time exists in the striatum that scales with the interval
being timed and multiplexes information about action as well as
time. Optogenetic approaches in specific identified cells in animal
models will yield crucial insights into the causal role of such
mechanisms and their impact on timing behavior (Grosenick
et al., 2015). For instance, a recent study by Chen et al. (2014)
reported rapid modulation of striatal activity by the cerebellum
via a disynaptic pathway that has implications for the coordinated
processing of temporal information in these two core timing
areas.
The other dominant view of timing is that time is not
based on the computations in dedicated circuits but rather
represents the output of intrinsic neuronal dynamics (Karmarkar
and Buonomano, 2007). In this respect, the activity of sensory
areas including auditory, visual, and somatosensory cortices
merits further attention. Combining optogenetics and single-
unit recordings in primary visual cortex (V1), Hussain Shuler
and colleagues have recently provided novel insights into how
basal forebrain cholinergic input to V1 provides a teaching
signal to modulate the response dynamics of V1 so that cues
predictive of given delays to future reward produce responses
that express those learned delays (Chubykin et al., 2013; Liu
et al., 2015), that those responses reflect learned reward timing
(Shuler and Bear, 2006; Zold and Hussain Shuler, 2015) and
inform visually-cued timing (Namboodiri et al., 2015). Similar
work in other sensory domains such as audition will enable
us to decipher the multi-sensory representation of time and
action during adaptive behaviors such as speech and movement.
Further neurophysiological work using high channel-count
electrophysiology (n400–1000) based on new Silicon probes
based on CMOS technology (e.g., Berényi et al., 2014; Lopez et al.,
2016) or mesoscopic analysis of timing behavior across different
cortical layers and multiple brain areas using multi-plane calcium
imaging may further shed new light on the underlying circuit-
level cortical computations (Yang et al., 2016).
Apart from adopting the latest technological tools and genetic
probes, a fundamental understanding of timing can be obtained
by designing more naturalistic tasks that use ecological stimuli
that are meaningful to the experimental subject in the real
world. Naturalistic sequences with variable temporal structure
(Teki et al., 2011; Teki and Griffiths, 2014, 2016) that go
beyond the traditional use of single intervals may yield novel
insights into the encoding of time as well as associated motor
behaviors (Kornysheva and Diedrichsen, 2014). Table 1 and the
reviews therein highlight that timing is not mediated by a single
brain area but rather involves a distributed network (Meck,
2005) in cortical and subcortical areas including prefrontal,
parietal, premotor and sensory cortices, insula, basal ganglia,
cerebellum, inferior olive amongst others. To formulate a unified
theory of how timing is mediated by these structures, it is
also important to understand the core functions of these areas
and what particular aspect of timing they mediate, whether
it is related to perception, attention, or memory. The use of
comparative paradigms in healthy human volunteers as well as
clinical populations that show timing deficits such as patients
with Parkinson’s, Huntington’s, Schizophrenia amongst others
will provide a more uniform understanding of timing functions
and dysfunctions in health and disease (Allman and Meck, 2012).
An identical approach (and even the use of similar paradigms)
in animal models via use of control animals as well as lesion
or knock-out models will complement findings from the human
literature and provide a more generic understanding of the neural
computations and circuits that underlie timing.
FUTURE DIRECTIONS—ORGANIZATIONAL
In order to drive more impactful experimental work, the field of
timing needs to attract young researchers which would require
more concerted efforts from the entire timing community.
Frontiers in Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 8July 2016 | Volume 10 | Article 330
Teki A Citation-Based Review of Papers on Timing
A recent positive step in this direction was marked by the
launch of a specialist journal for timing, Timing and Time
Perception (Meck et al., 2013) as well as its corresponding
review journal, Timing and Time Perception Reviews. Another
step forward would be the launch of an academic society
exclusively for researchers in timing that would promote
interdisciplinary exchange of ideas amongst researchers
with diverse interests in timing via annual conferences that
draw on a range of methods from purely behavioral to
neurophysiological and neuroanatomical measures; share
pertinent news and information like grant funding calls,
new papers, job opportunities for doctoral and postdoctoral
candidates, workshops and training opportunities; and promote
the career development of young researchers through grants for
short cross-disciplinary collaborations or exchange visits and
funding for attending conferences and mentoring support.
Although there already exist a few scientific societies and
communities relevant to timing like the Society for Music
Perception and Cognition (SPMC: http://www.musicperception.
org), Rhythm Perception and Production Workshop (RPPW:
http://rppw.org), European Society for Cognitive Sciences of
Music (ESCOM: http://escom2015.org), Society for Education,
Music and Psychology Research (SEMPRE: http://www.sempre.
org.uk), Deutsche Gesellschaft fur Musikpsychologie (DGM:
http://www.music-psychology.de), Asia-Pacific Society for
the Cognitive Sciences of Music, Fondazione Mariani (http://
fondazione-mariani.org/) that organizes the NeuroMusic
conferences, their scope is limited to music perception and
psychology, and do not cover all aspects of timing and time
perception. Society for Neuroscience (SfN) represents the
primary venue where timing researchers gather for structured
symposia on human and animal timing research but the
scientific discussions are limited given the busy nature of SfN
meetings. A recent example of such a successful academic
organization for a diverse topic of research is the Society for
the Neurobiology of Language (http://www.neurolang.org/)
funded by the National Institutes of Health, which since its
inception in 2009, attracts more than 400 researchers for its
annual conferences. To address the absence of an association of
researchers working on all aspects of timing, Argiro Vatakis and
I have established a new timing society to promote open science
and collaboration—the “Timing Research Forum” (http://
timingforum.org).
Irrespective of the present state of affairs, the field of timing
and time perception represents a promising and exciting field
of research that is growing every year in terms of number of
researchers and scientific output, and one where new students
and researchers may find a relatively unexplored topic of research
and make a significant impact on the field.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and
approved it for publication.
FUNDING
ST is funded by the Wellcome Trust (WT106084/Z/14/Z; Sir
Henry Wellcome Postdoctoral Fellowship).
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
I thank Anu Chowdhry for help with compiling the list of papers
in Table 1.
SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found
online at: http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fnins.
2016.00330
DATA
Metrics data presented in Table 1 are available to download as
a .mat file from Figshare:
Link : https://figshare.com/s/0fb93a59927786300644;
DOI : https://dx.doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.3153124.
REFERENCES
Allman, M. J., and Meck, W. H. (2012). Pathophysiological distortions
in time perception and timed performance. Brain 135, 656–677. doi:
10.1093/brain/awr210
Allman, M. J., Teki, S., Griffiths, T. D., and Meck, W. H. (2014). Properties of the
internal clock: first- and second-order principles of subjective time. Annu. Rev.
Psychol. 65, 743–771. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115117
Artieda, J., Pastor, M. A., Lacruz, F., and Obeso, J. A. (1992). Temporal
discrimination is abnormal in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 115, 199–210. doi:
10.1093/brain/115.1.199
Bartolo, R., and Merchant, H. (2015). βoscillations are linked to the
initiation of sensory-cued movement sequences and the internal guidance
of regular tapping in the monkey. J. Neurosci. 35, 4635–4640. doi: 10.1523/
JNEUROSCI.4570-14.2015
Bartolo, R., Prado, L., and Merchant, H. (2014). Information processing in the
primate basal ganglia during sensory-guided and internally driven rhythmic
tapping. J. Neurosci. 34, 3910–3923. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2679-13.2014
Berényi, A., Somogyvári, Z., Nagy, A. J., Roux, L., Long, J. D., Fujisawa, S.,
et al. (2014). Large-scale, high-density (up to 512 channels) recording of local
circuits in behaving animals. J. Neurophysiol. 111, 1132–1149. doi: 10.1152/
jn.00785.2013
Braitenberg, V. (1967). Is the cerebellar cortex a biological clock in the millisecond
range. Prog. Brain Res. 25, 334–346. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)60971-1
Chen, C. H., Fremont, R., Arteaga-Bracho, E. E., and Khodakhah, K. (2014).
Short latency cerebellar modulation of the basal ganglia. Nat. Neurosci. 17,
1767–1775. doi: 10.1038/nn.3868
Chubykin, A. A., Roach, E. B., Bear, M. F., and Shuler, M. G. H. (2013). A
cholinergic mechanism for reward timing within primary visual cortex. Neuron
77, 723–735. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.12.039
Church, R. M. (1984). Properties of the internal clock. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 423,
566–582. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1984.tb23459.x
Gibbon, J., Church, R. M., and Meck, W. H. (1984). Scalar timing in memory. Ann.
N. Y. Acad. Sci. 423, 52–77. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1984.tb23417.x
Gooddy, W. (1958). Time and the nervous system: the brain as a clock. Lancet 1,
1139–1144. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(58)91948-2
Frontiers in Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 9July 2016 | Volume 10 | Article 330
Teki A Citation-Based Review of Papers on Timing
Gouvêa, T. S., Monteiro, T., Motiwala, A., Soares, S., Machens, C., and Paton, J.
J. (2015). Striatal dynamics explain duration judgments. Elife 4:e11386. doi:
10.7554/eLife.11386
Grahn, J. A. (2012). Neural mechanisms of rhythm perception: current findings
and future perspectives. Top. Cogn. Sci. 4, 585–606. doi: 10.1111/j.1756-
8765.2012.01213.x
Grosenick, L., Marshel, J. H., and Deisseroth, K. (2015). Closed-loop and
activity-guided optogenetic control. Neuron 86, 106–139. doi: 10.1016/
j.neuron.2015.03.034
Hayashi, M. J., Ditye, T., Harada, T., Hashiguchi, M., Sadato, N., Carlson,
S., et al. (2015). Time adaptation shows duration selectivity in the
human parietal cortex. PLoS Biol. 13:e1002262. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.
1002262
Ivry, R. (1993). Cerebellar involvement in the explicit representation of temporal
information. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 682, 214–230. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-
6632.1993.tb22970.x
Ivry, R. B., and Keele, S. W. (1989). Timing functions of the cerebellum. J. Cogn.
Neurosci. 1, 136–152. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1989.1.2.136
Ivry, R. B., Keele, S. W., and Diener, H. C. (1988). Dissociation of the lateral and
medial cerebellum in movement timing and movement execution. Exp. Brain
Res. 73, 167–180. doi: 10.1007/BF00279670
James, W. (1890). The Principles of Psychology, Vol. 1. New York, NY: Henry Holt.
Karmarkar, U. R., and Buonomano, D. V. (2007). Timing in the absence of clocks:
encoding time in neural network states. Neuron 53, 427–438. doi: 10.1016/
j.neuron.2007.01.006
Kornysheva, K., and Diedrichsen, J. (2014). Human premotor areas parse
sequences into their spatial and temporal features. Elife 3:e03043. doi:
10.7554/eLife.03043
Liu, C.-H., Coleman, J. E., Davoudi, H., Zhang, K., and Hussain Shuler, M.
G. (2015). Selective activation of a putative reinforcement signal conditions
cued interval timing in primary visual cortex. Curr. Biol. 25, 1551–1561. doi:
10.1016/j.cub.2015.04.028
Llinas, R., Baker, R., and Sotelo, C. (1974). Electrotonic coupling between neurons
in cat inferior olive. J. Neurophysiol. 37, 560–571.
Llinás, R., and Yarom, Y. (1981). Electrophysiology of mammalian inferior olivary
neurons in vitro. Different types of voltage-dependent ionic conductances.
J. Physiol. 315, 549–567. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1981.sp013763
Lopez, C. M., Mitra, S., Putzeys, J., Raducanu, B., Ballini, M., Andrei, A., et al.
(2016). 22.7 A 966-Electrode Neural Probe with 384 Configurable Channels in
0.13µm SOI CMOS (IEEE publication), 392–393.
Meck, W. H. (2005). Neuropsychology of timing and time perception. Brain Cogn.
58, 1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.09.004
Meck, W. H., and Ivry, R. B. (2016). Editorial overview: Time in perception
and action. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 8, vi–x. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.
03.001
Meck, W. H., Vatakis, A., and van Rijn, H. (2013). Timing & Time Perception
enters a new dimension. Timing Time Percept. 1, 1–2. doi: 10.1163/22134468-
00002014
Mello, G. B. M., Soares, S., and Paton, J. J. (2015). A scalable population code
for time in the striatum. Curr. Biol. 25, 1113–1122. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.
02.036
Merchant, H., Harrington, D. L., and Meck, W. H. (2013a). Neural basis of the
perception and estimation of time. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 36, 313–336. doi:
10.1146/annurev-neuro-062012-170349
Merchant, H., Pérez, O., Zarco, W., and Gámez, J. (2013b). Interval tuning in the
primate medial premotor cortex as a general timing mechanism. J. Neurosci. 33,
9082–9096. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5513-12.2013
Merchant, H., Zarco, W., Pérez, O., Prado, L., and Bartolo, R. (2011).
Measuring time with different neural chronometers during a synchronization-
continuation task. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 19784–19789. doi: 10.1073/
pnas.1112933108
Namboodiri, V. M. K., Huertas, M. A., Monk, K. J., Shouval, H. Z., and Hussain
Shuler, M. G. (2015). Visually cued action timing in the primary visual cortex.
Neuron 86, 319–330. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.02.043
Nichelli, P., Alway, D., and Grafman, J. (1996). Perceptual timing in cerebellar
degeneration. Neuropsychologia 34, 863–871.
Pastor, M. A., Artieda, J., Jahanshahi, M., and Obeso, J. A. (1992). Time estimation
and reproduction is abnormal in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 115, 211–225. doi:
10.1093/brain/115.1.211
Schneider, B. A., and Ghose, G. M. (2012). Temporal production signals in parietal
cortex. PLoS Biol. 10:e1001413. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001413
Shuler, M. G., and Bear, M. F. (2006). Reward timing in the primary visual cortex.
Science 311, 1606–1609. doi: 10.1126/science.1123513
Teki, S., and Griffiths, T. D. (2014). Working memory for time intervals in auditory
rhythmic sequences. Front. Psychol. 5:1329. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01329
Teki, S., and Griffiths, T. D. (2016). Brain bases of working memory
for time intervals in rhythmic sequences. Front. Neurosci. 10:239. doi:
10.3389/fnins.2016.00239
Teki, S., Grube, M., and Griffiths, T. D. (2012). A unified model of time perception
accounts for duration-based and beat-based timing mechanisms. Front. Integr.
Neurosci. 5:90. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2011.00090
Teki, S., Grube, M., Kumar, S., and Griffiths, T. D. (2011). Distinct neural substrates
of duration-based and beat-based auditory timing. J. Neurosci. 31, 3805–3812.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5561-10.2011
Welsh, J. P., Lang, E., Sugihara, I., and Llinás, R. (1995). Dynamic organization
of motor control within the olivocerebellar system. Nature 374, 453–457. doi:
10.1038/374453a0
Yang, W., Miller, J.-E. K., Carrillo-Reid, L., Pnevmatikakis, E., Paninski, L., Yuste,
R., et al. (2016). Simultaneous multi-plane imaging of neural circuits. Neuron
89, 269–284. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.12.012
Yarom, Y., and Cohen, D. (2002). The olivocerebellar system as a generator of
temporal patterns. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 978, 122–134. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-
6632.2002.tb07561.x
Zold, C. L., and Hussain Shuler, M. G. (2015). Theta oscillations in visual cortex
emerge with experience to convey expected reward time and experienced
reward rate. J. Neurosci. 35, 9603–9614. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0296-
15.2015
Conflict of Interest Statement: The author declares that the research was
conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could
be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Copyright © 2016 Teki. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms
of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or
reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor
are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance
with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted
which does not comply with these terms.
Frontiers in Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 10 July 2016 | Volume 10 | Article 330

Supplementary resource (1)

... Об актуальности этой темы свидетельству ют многочисленные экспериментальные данные и предложенные на их основе теории. В настоя щее время обсуждаются несколько подходов к по ниманию этой проблемы [43, 71,113]. В обзоре [113] анализируются 150 наиболее цитируемых работ по этой тематике, проведенных в период 2000-2015 гг. ...
... В настоя щее время обсуждаются несколько подходов к по ниманию этой проблемы [43, 71,113]. В обзоре [113] анализируются 150 наиболее цитируемых работ по этой тематике, проведенных в период 2000-2015 гг. Отмечено, что способность точно оценивать и вос производить длительность стимулов и интервалов между ними зависит от многих факторов, включая физические свойства стимула, процессы внимания [14,33,38,71], новизну [92], мотивацию и эмоции [37,45]. ...
... К настоящему време ни мнение о том, что оценка коротких интерва лов времени зависит от взаимодействия БГ, преф ронтальной коры (ПфК) и дофаминергических влияний, разделяют многие исследователи [13]. В пользу того, что нейроны новой коры и стриату ма играют важную роль в оценке времени в субсе кундной шкале, свидетельствуют результаты экс периментов, в которых регистрировали активность отдельных нейронов или групп клеток, либо опре деляли нарушения восприятия времени при по вреждении разных мозговых структур [79,113]. ...
Article
Full-text available
A mechanism of time perception in subsecond scale, which we proposed earlier [Sil'kis I. UFN. 2011. 42:41–56] now is supplemented in view of that various factors influence the processing of sensory information. This mechanism is based on the assumption that since there is no necessity to determine time parameters of sensory stimuli regardless of their physical properties and there is no special organ for time perception, a processing of all incoming information is performed in the same parallel associative and limbic neural circuits. These circuits are: neocortex (basolateral amygdala) – basal ganglia – thalamus – neocortex (basolateral amygdala), and neocortex – subthalamic nucleus – pedunculopontine nucleus – thalamus – neocortex. The time parameters of a stimulus are determined based on the clock rate of information processing. This rate is inversely proportional to the duration of one cycle of repeated excitation of the neocortex (about 20–25 ms). Such excitation is provided by circulation of activity in mentioned neural circuits. The duration of circulation depends on such factors as the strength of a stimulus, its emotional significance, attention, and current concentrations of neuromodulators. It follows from proposed unified mechanism that any factor reinforcing (weakening) the initial neural representation of physical properties of a stimulus in the neocortex and therefore leading to increase (decrease) in the efficacy of cortico-striatal inputs, should facilitate (hinder) the disinhibition of the thalamus through the basal ganglia and its excitation by the pedunculopontine nucleus. Subsequent decrease (increase) in duration of activity circulation in considered neural loops will cause a rise (fall) in clock rate, and overestimation (underestimation) of perceived time. This mechanism can serve the neurophysiological basis for recently proposed
... Because motivation is based on time perception, the spatial relationships of material systems resurface as temporal connections in psychology (Farah, 2017). Time regulation emerges from the brain's inner clock (Caro-Martin et al., 2015;Teki, 2016;Tsao et al., 2018), which originates in midbrain dopamine neurons (Soares et al., 2016). Populations of lateral entorhinal cortex neurons represent time inherently through the encoding of experience (Tsao et al., 2018). ...
... The brain shows a cunning ability to maintain the stability of the resting state against relentless bombardment by outside stimuli (Deli et al., 2018;Lin et al., 2017;Mantini & Vanduffel, 2013;Raichle & Snyder, 2007). The brain's electric activities formulate a harmonic motion of energy (or information) between the cortex and the limbic brain with substantial autonomy (Teki, 2016). ...
Article
Full-text available
The recognition that the brain's electric activities correlate with psychophysiology has encouraged the application of physical principles in the analysis of cognitive processes. Perception is an orthogonal transformation of the spatial organization of the physical world into a temporally organized system-the discrete processing of stimulus centers on the resting-state. Information overload formulates a time pressure of stress and temporal excess spurs satisfaction. Both conditions enhance time perception but correspond to opposite emotions and attitudes. Emotions underlie all cognitive and processes by forming the fundamental motivations, attachments, and personal boundaries. Emotions have irresistible power over our behavior; by operating behind conscious awareness, they modulate what we see, hear, and think. The fermionic mind hypothesis (FMH) establishes consciousness as the smallest unit of intellect that operates via the principles of string theory, general relativity, and quantum mechanics. Adopting physical laws ensures survival by integrating the mind into the environment.
... Although the neural mechanisms underlying time perception are still matter of investigation 19 , previous studies suggest that time is processed in different manners depending on the duration of the time intervals and the typology of the tasks employed in the experimental paradigms 20,21 . For example, the regulation of circadian cycles (with a 24-h scale) involves subcortical structures, in particular the suprachiasmatic nucleus of the hypothalamus, while very fast processes on milli-second timing scales are strongly associated with cerebellar and motor systems 22 . ...
Article
Full-text available
Sense of time is a complex construct, and its neural correlates remain to date in most part unknown. To complicate the frame, physical attributes of the stimulus, such as its intensity or movement, influence temporal perception. Although previous studies have shown that time perception can be compromised after a brain lesion, the evidence on the role of the left and right hemispheres are meager. In two experiments, the study explores the ability of temporal estimation of multi-second actions and non-biological movements in 33 patients suffering from unilateral brain lesion. Furthermore, the modulatory role of induced embodiment processes is investigated. The results reveal a joint contribution of the two hemispheres depending not only on different durations but also on the presence of actions. Indeed, the left hemisphere damaged patients find it difficult to estimate 4500 ms or longer durations, while the right hemisphere damaged patients fail in 3000 ms durations. Furthermore, the former fail when a biological action is shown, while the latter fail in non-biological movement. Embodiment processes have a modulatory effect only after right hemisphere lesions. Among neuropsychological variables, only spatial neglect influences estimation of non-biological movement.
... Time fundamentally shapes our experience of the world around us (Wittmann, 2016), and distortions of time perception have been found to be related to pathological conditions such as schizophrenia, Parkinson's disease, and attention deficit disorders (Grondin, 2010). Despite this, the cognitive and neural mechanisms of human time perception remain largely unknown and debated (Teki, 2016;Wittmann, 1999). One influential model of time perception is that of the internal clock (Treisman, 1963;Wang & Wöllner, 2019), which proposes that humans possess an inner pacemaker that keeps track of time through the accumulation of pulses. ...
Article
Full-text available
While the effects of synthesised visual stimuli on time perception processes are well documented, very little research on time estimation in human movement stimuli exists. This study investigated the effects of movement speed and agency on duration estimation of human motion. Participants were recorded using optical motion capture while they performed dance-like movements at three different speeds. They later returned for a perceptual experiment in which they watched point-light displays of themselves and one other participant. Participants were asked to identify themselves, to estimate the duration of the recordings, and to rate expressivity and quality of the movements. Results indicate that speed of movement affected duration estimations such that faster speeds were rated longer, in accordance with previous findings in non-biological motion. The biasing effects of speed were stronger for watching others’ movements than for watching one’s own point-light movements. Duration estimations were longer after acting out the movement compared with watching it, and speed differentially affected ratings of expressivity and quality. Findings suggest that aspects of temporal processing of visual stimuli may be modulated by inner motor representations of previously performed movements, and by physically carrying out an action compared with just watching it. Results also support the inner clock and change theories of time perception for the processing of human motion stimuli, which can inform the temporal mechanisms of the hypothesised separate processor for human movement information.
... Although a comprehensive understanding of the cellular and molecular basis of interval timing awaits further investigation, the development of the striatal beat-frequency (SBF) model of interval timing Meck, 2000, 2004) continues to serve as an important guidepost for directing future research (e.g., Farrell, 2011;Oprisan and Buhusi, 2011;Soares et al., 2016;Teki, 2016;Dallérac et al., 2017;Toda et al., 2017;Gu et al., 2018). The main reason for this is that the SBF model provides a neurobiologically plausible account of interval timing within cortico-striatal circuits that can be extended and revised as additional information becomes available. ...
Article
Full-text available
The majority of studies in the field of timing and time perception have generally focused on sub- and supra-second time scales, specific behavioral processes, and/or discrete neuronal circuits. In an attempt to find common elements of interval timing from a broader perspective, we review the literature and highlight the need for cell and molecular studies that can delineate the neural mechanisms underlying temporal processing. Moreover, given the recent attention to the function of microtubule proteins and their potential contributions to learning and memory consolidation/re-consolidation, we propose that these proteins play key roles in coding temporal information in cerebellar Purkinje cells (PCs) and striatal medium spiny neurons (MSNs). The presence of microtubules at relevant neuronal sites, as well as their adaptability, dynamic structure, and longevity, makes them a suitable candidate for neural plasticity at both intra- and inter-cellular levels. As a consequence, microtubules appear capable of maintaining a temporal code or engram and thereby regulate the firing patterns of PCs and MSNs known to be involved in interval timing. This proposed mechanism would control the storage of temporal information triggered by postsynaptic activation of mGluR7. This, in turn, leads to alterations in microtubule dynamics through a “read-write” memory process involving alterations in microtubule dynamics and their hexagonal lattice structures involved in the molecular basis of temporal memory.
... Similarly, the following searches were performed to locate papers related to syntax processing: "(fMRI OR functional magnetic resonance imaging OR PET OR positron emission tomography) AND grammatical", "(fMRI OR functional magnetic resonance imaging OR PET OR positron emission tomography) AND syntactic", "(fMRI OR functional magnetic resonance imaging OR PET OR positron emission tomography) AND grammar", "(fMRI OR functional magnetic resonance imaging OR PET OR positron emission tomography) AND syntax". Additional rhythm papers were located by reviewing the citations of these nine review papers: (Fitch, 2013;Geiser et al., 2014;Grahn, 2012;Kotz et al., 2018;Merchant et al., 2015;Pearce and Rohrmeier, 2012;Repp and Su, 2013;Teki, 2016;Teki et al., 2012). The full process of elimination is summarized according to PRISMA standards within Supplementary Materials S1 and S2 (Moher et al., 2009). ...
Article
A growing body of evidence has highlighted behavioral connections between musical rhythm and linguistic syntax, suggesting that these may be mediated by common neural resources. Here, we performed a quantitative meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies using activation likelihood estimate (ALE) to localize the shared neural structures engaged in a representative set of musical rhythm (rhythm, beat, and meter) and linguistic syntax (merge movement, and reanalysis). Rhythm engaged a bilateral sensorimotor network throughout the brain consisting of the inferior frontal gyri, supplementary motor area, superior temporal gyri/temporoparietal junction, insula, the intraparietal lobule, and putamen. By contrast, syntax mostly recruited the left sensorimotor network including the inferior frontal gyrus, posterior superior temporal gyrus, premotor cortex, and supplementary motor area. Intersections between rhythm and syntax maps yielded overlapping regions in the left inferior frontal gyrus, left supplementary motor area, and bilateral insula-neural substrates involved in temporal hierarchy processing and predictive coding. Together, this is the first neuroimaging meta-analysis providing detailed anatomical overlap of sensorimotor regions recruited for musical rhythm and linguistic syntax.
... Similarly, the following searches were performed to locate papers related to syntax processing: "(fMRI OR functional magnetic resonance imaging OR PET OR positron emission tomography) AND grammatical", "(fMRI OR functional magnetic resonance imaging OR PET OR positron emission tomography) AND syntactic", "(fMRI OR functional magnetic resonance imaging OR PET OR positron emission tomography) AND grammar", "(fMRI OR functional magnetic resonance imaging OR PET OR positron emission tomography) AND syntax". Additional rhythm papers were located by reviewing the citations of these nine review papers: (Fitch, 2013;Geiser et al., 2014;Grahn, 2012;Kotz et al., 2018;Merchant et al., 2015;Pearce and Rohrmeier, 2012;Repp and Su, 2013;Teki, 2016;Teki et al., 2012). The full process of elimination is summarized according to PRISMA standards within Supplementary Materials S1 and S2 (Moher et al., 2009). ...
Chapter
Our ability to perceive event duration and order is critical in every aspect of our lives, from everyday tasks like coordinating our limbs to walk safely, to uniquely human activities like planning our children’s future. Many theoretical accounts of timing have been proposed to explain the mechanisms underlying our ability to estimate time and unify events in time. Continuous progress is being met in further refining and extending current theories, with the aim not only to advance our understanding of timing and time perception, but also to make timing more accessible and applicable to daily life. For this to be possible, cross-disciplinary thinking is required, which is something one cannot easily attain in a scientific conference, rather it requires a community. Having a community with an interest and/or expertise in timing can allow for cross-fertilization of ideas. This chapter introduced the story of the Timing Research Forum or else TRF.
Article
This article addresses the nature and neurobiological mechanisms of subjective time. Subjective time is shown to be a consequence of the functioning of consciousness. Subjective time is generated by consciousness and is one of its natural properties. A formal model is constructed providing explanations of the psychophysiological mechanisms of subjective time and the still mysterious phenomena which it generates. The qualitative characteristics of subjective time are formulated and are in good correspondence with extensive experimental data on the perception of short time intervals (stimulus durations and interstimulus intervals) and unusual phenomena occurring only episodically. Subjective time is a separate entity not directly dependent on physical time, though it may be associated with it via the intensity of the subject’s mental activity.
Article
The term ‘timing’ is interchangeably used to convey processing of the order or the duration of events. Yet, whereas temporal order processing means judging when one event happens relative to another (first or second), duration estimation means measuring how long the event lasts. In this Review, we show that the functional distinction between these two temporal features is reflected in their discrete neural substrates. Temporal order processing preferentially engages the left inferior parietal cortex, whereas duration estimation recruits the supplementary motor area, basal ganglia and cerebellum. The functional distinction between temporal order processing and duration estimation also enables better characterization of temporal perturbations present in clinical disorders. For instance, individuals with schizophrenia have trouble individuating and ordering consecutive events in time and show atypical responses to stimuli that do not appear when expected. Therefore, individuals with schizophrenia might have a fundamental impairment in processing when a stimulus occurs relative to another event, rather than in estimating how long it lasts. These neural and clinical dissociations demonstrate that the phenomenological sensation of a unitary and cohesive flow of time (‘time’s arrow’) can be separated into two distinct, though intertwined, components. Subjective time perception involves processing when an event happens relative to another event versus how long an event lasts. In this Review, Coull and Giersch describe the functional and neural differences between temporal order processing and duration estimation by exploring perturbations in individuals with schizophrenia.
Article
Full-text available
Skilled performance is characterized by precise and flexible control of movement sequences in space and time. Recent theories suggest that integrated spatio-temporal trajectories are generated by intrinsic dynamics of motor and premotor networks. This contrasts with behavioural advantages that emerge when a trained spatial or temporal feature of sequences is transferred to a new spatio-temporal combination arguing for independent neural representations of these sequence features. We used a new fMRI pattern classification approach to identify brain regions with independent vs integrated representations. A distinct regional dissociation within motor areas was revealed: whereas only the contralateral primary motor cortex exhibited unique patterns for each spatio-temporal sequence combination, bilateral premotor areas represented spatial and temporal features independently of each other. These findings advocate a unique function of higher motor areas for flexible recombination and efficient encoding of complex motor behaviours.
Article
Full-text available
[This corrects the article DOI: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002262.].
Article
Full-text available
Perception of auditory time intervals is critical for accurate comprehension of natural sounds like speech and music. However, the neural substrates and mechanisms underlying the representation of time intervals in working memory are poorly understood. In this study, we investigate the brain bases of working memory for time intervals in rhythmic sequences using functional magnetic resonance imaging. We used a novel behavioral paradigm to investigate time-interval representation in working memory as a function of the temporal jitter and memory load of the sequences containing those time intervals. Human participants were presented with a sequence of intervals and required to reproduce the duration of a particular probed interval. We found that perceptual timing areas including the cerebellum and the striatum were more or less active as a function of increasing and decreasing jitter of the intervals held in working memory respectively whilst the activity of the inferior parietal cortex is modulated as a function of memory load. Additionally, we also analyzed structural correlations between gray and white matter density and behavior and found significant correlations in the cerebellum and the striatum, mirroring the functional results. Our data demonstrate neural substrates of working memory for time intervals and suggest that the cerebellum and the striatum represent core areas for representing temporal information in working memory.
Article
Full-text available
Author Summary The human brain has the ability to estimate the passage of time, which allows us to perform complex cognitive tasks such as playing music, dancing, and understanding speech. Scientists have just begun to understand which brain areas become active when we estimate time. However, it still remains a mystery how exactly the information about time is represented in the brain. In this study, we hypothesized that time might be represented by neurons that are specifically tuned to a specific duration, as has been known for simple visual features such as the orientation and the motion direction in the visual cortex. To test this idea, we performed multiple functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) adaptation experiments in which we sought evidence of neuronal adaptation, that is, a reduction in the responsiveness of neurons to repeated presentations of similar durations. Our experiments revealed that the level of brain activity in the right inferior parietal lobule (IPL) was strongly reduced when a stimulus of the same duration was repeatedly presented. This finding was reproduced for a range of subsecond durations. Our results indicate that neurons in the human IPL are tuned to specific preferred durations.
Article
Full-text available
As a consequence of conditioning visual cues with delayed reward, cue-evoked neural activity that predicts the time of expected future reward emerges in the primary visual cortex (V1). We hypothesized that this reward-timing activity is engendered by a reinforcement signal conveying reward acquisition to V1. In lieu of behavioral conditioning, we assessed in vivo whether selective activation of either basal forebrain (BF) or cholinergic innervation is sufficient to condition cued interval-timing activity. Substituting for actual reward, optogenetic activation of BF or cholinergic input within V1 at fixed delays following visual stimulation entrains neural responses that mimic behaviorally conditioned reward-timing activity. Optogenetically conditioned neural responses express cue-evoked temporal intervals that correspond to the conditioning intervals, are bidirectionally modifiable, display experience-dependent refinement, and exhibit a scale invariance to the encoded delay. Our results demonstrate that the activation of BF or cholinergic input within V1 is sufficient to encode cued interval-timing activity and indicate that V1 itself is a substrate for associative learning that may inform the timing of visually cued behaviors. Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Article
Recording the activity of large populations of neurons is an important step toward understanding the emergent function of neural circuits. Here we present a simple holographic method to simultaneously perform two-photon calcium imaging of neuronal populations across multiple areas and layers of mouse cortex in vivo. We use prior knowledge of neuronal locations, activity sparsity, and a constrained nonnegative matrix factorization algorithm to extract signals from neurons imaged simultaneously and located in different focal planes or fields of view. Our laser multiplexing approach is simple and fast, and could be used as a general method to image the activity of neural circuits in three dimensions across multiple areas in the brain. Yang et al. demonstrate a novel approach for simultaneously imaging multiple layers of the mouse cortex. They combine holographic two-photon microscopy with advanced computational source extraction to create a flexible platform for studying mesoscale neural circuits at multiple depths of the brain with cellular resolution.
Article
The primary visual cortex (V1) is widely regarded as faithfully conveying the physical properties of visual stimuli. Thus, experience-induced changes in V1 are often interpreted as improving visual perception (i.e., perceptual learning). Here we describe how, with experience, cue-evoked oscillations emerge in V1 to convey expected reward time as well as to relate experienced reward rate. We show, in chronic multisite local field potential recordings from rat V1, that repeated presentation of visual cues induces the emergence of visually evoked oscillatory activity. Early in training, the visually evoked oscillations relate to the physical parameters of the stimuli. However, with training, the oscillations evolve to relate the time in which those stimuli foretell expected reward. Moreover, the oscillation prevalence reflects the reward rate recently experienced by the animal. Thus, training induces experience-dependent changes in V1 activity that relate to what those stimuli have come to signify behaviorally: when to expect future reward and at what rate. Copyright © 2015 the authors 0270-6474/15/359603-12$15.00/0.