Content uploaded by Eric Neumayer
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Eric Neumayer on Sep 23, 2018
Content may be subject to copyright.
Electronic copy available at: http://ssrn.com/abstract=874965
The Gendered Nature of Natural Disasters: The Impact of Catastrophic
Events on the Gender Gap in Life Expectancy, 1981-2002
Published in:
Annals of the American Association of Geographers, 97 (3), 2007, pp. 551-566
Eric Neumayer
a
and Thomas Plümper
b
a
Department of Geography and Environment, London School of Economics and
Political Science (LSE), London WC2A 2AE, UK, e.neumayer@lse.ac.uk
b
Department of Government, University of Essex and Max-Planck Institute of
Economics, Jena, tpluem@essex.ac.uk
Electronic copy available at: http://ssrn.com/abstract=874965
1
Abstract
Natural disasters do not affect people equally. In fact, a vulnerability approach to disasters
would suggest that inequalities in exposure and sensitivity to risk as well as inequalities in
access to resources, capabilities and opportunities systematically disadvantage certain groups
of people, rendering them more vulnerable to the impact of natural disasters. In this article we
address the specific vulnerability of girls and women with respect to mortality from natural
disasters and their aftermath. Biological and physiological differences between the sexes are
unlikely to explain large-scale gender differences in mortality rates. Social norms and role
behavior provide some further explanation, but what is likely to matter most is the everyday
socio-economic status of women. We analyze the effect of disaster strength and its interaction
with the socio-economic status of women on the change in the gender gap in life expectancy
in a sample of up to 141 countries over the period 1981 to 2002. We find, first, that natural
disasters lower the life expectancy of women more than that of men. In other words, natural
disasters (and their subsequent impact) on average kill more women than men or kill women
at an earlier age than men. Since female life expectancy is generally higher than that of
males, for most countries natural disasters narrow the gender gap in life expectancy. Second,
the stronger the disaster (as approximated by the number of people killed relative to
population size), the stronger this effect on the gender gap in life expectancy. That is, major
calamities lead to more severe impacts on female life expectancy (relative to that of males)
than smaller disasters. Third, the higher women’s socio-economic status, the weaker this
effect on the gender gap in life expectancy. In other words, taken together our results show
that it is the socially constructed gender-specific vulnerability of females built into everyday
socio-economic patterns that lead to the relatively higher female disaster mortality rates
compared to men.
Key Words: natural disaster, vulnerability, gender, mortality, socio-economic status
2
Introduction
The human impact of natural disasters is never entirely determined by nature, but is
contingent on economic, cultural and social relations. In this article we address one
important, yet hitherto relatively neglected aspect (WHO 2002), namely the gendered nature
of disaster vulnerability as revealed by gender-specific disaster mortality. Anderson (2000:
86), in a World Bank publication on managing disaster risk, is adamant that “much more
research is needed to fully understand the extent to which gender plays a role in differential
casualty rates”. This article’s analysis provides an important step in that direction.
Specifically, we analyze the impact of natural disasters on the so-called gender gap in life
expectancy, which is the difference between female and male life expectancy at birth (in most
countries women live longer than men).
Our study takes seriously gender as an analytical category. We try to explain the
differential impact of natural disasters on female relative to male life expectancy not merely
by recourse to different physical exposures and biological or physiological gender
differences, but also by the different socially constructed vulnerabilities that derive from the
social roles men and women assume, voluntarily or involuntarily, as well as existing patterns
of gender discrimination. Our study adopts a vulnerability approach to natural disasters as an
analytical concept. Many disaster scholars subscribe to such an approach and have made
significant contributions to its development (see, for example, O’Keefe, Westgate and Wisner
1976; Hewitt 1983; Cuny 1983; Wisner, Blaikie, Cannon and Davis 1994, 2004; Varley
1994; Cannon 1994, 2000; Cutter 1996; Fordham 2004). As Cutter (1996: 530) has pointed
out, vulnerability “still means different things to different people”. We adopt the definition of
vulnerability given in Wisner et al. (2004). In their approach, an explanation of “the risks
involved in disasters must be connected with the vulnerability created for many people
through their normal existence”, where vulnerability is defined as “the characteristics of a
3
person or group and their situation influencing their capacity to anticipate, cope with, resist
and recover from the impact of a natural hazard” (Wisner et al. 2004: 4 and 11).
1
It follows
that the impacts of natural disasters are never merely determined by nature on its own.
Indeed, it becomes even questionable whether one can talk of “natural” disasters at all, since
Cannon (1994: 14) argues that “there are no generalized opportunities and risks in nature, but
instead there are sets of unequal access to opportunities and unequal exposures to risks
which are a consequence of the socio-economic system” (emphasis in original). In other
words, vulnerability, as used in this article, captures the differential exposure to and capacity
to cope with risks systematically attributed to people across space and time, which, together
with other attributes such as ethnicity or class, is often a function of the individual’s gender,
the focus of analysis here (see Cannon 1994; Wisner et al. 2004).
There is renewed interest in studying the social impacts of natural disasters across the
social sciences. For example, economists have recently studied how a country’s low level of
economic development, poor quality of governance institutions and high degree of inequality
increases the death toll from earthquakes (Anbarci et al. 2005) as well as other types of
natural disasters (Kahn 2005). Geographers, sociologists and other social scientists have
addressed the vulnerability of certain groups of people to natural disasters (see, among others,
Cannon 1994, 2000; Cutter 1996; Mustafa 2002; Wisner et al. 1994, 2004 and references
cited therein). Increasingly, even physical geographers and public health scholars
acknowledge that a better mitigation of negative disaster impacts is contingent on a better
understanding of the socially constructed vulnerabilities of specific groups of affected people
(Noji 1997a; Alcántara-Ayala 2002; Degg and Chester 2005a, 2005b). Within the broader
field of disaster and environmental change research, an emergent literature addresses the
specific vulnerability of women (Cutter 1995; Bolin, Jackson and Crist 1998; Enarson and
Morrow 1998; Enarson 1998, 2000; Fothergill 1998; Fordham 1999, 2000, 2004; Enarson
4
and Meyreles 2004; Bradshaw 2004). Our original contribution is to provide the first
systematic, quantitative analysis of gender differences in natural disaster mortality. This is of
course only one, but due to its far-reaching consequences (life or death) arguably the most
important aspect of the gendered impact of natural disasters. Existing studies either do not
estimate gender-specific mortality rates and patterns at all (Kahn 2005; Anbarci et al. 2005)
or are confined to single events such that no general conclusions can be drawn (Bern et al.
1993; Ikeda 1995; O’Hare 2001; Oxfam International 2005). It is only by quantitative
analysis of mortality rates or summary mortality measures such as life expectancy that we can
discern whether the anecdotal evidence captures a general trend. We believe that our
contribution buttresses Cutter’s (2003: 6) claim in her Presidential Address to the Association
of American Geographers that “geography has added a technological sophistication to
hazards research that is unrivalled among the social sciences” and that “the discipline is
rapidly becoming the driving force behind vulnerability science”.
2
In linking spatial patterns
of disaster risk to human-generated vulnerability, geography is uniquely positioned to study
the impact of natural disasters on socio-economic systems and groups of people. One of the
“most significant themes” listed by Cutter (2003: 7) is the need “to identify, delineate, and
understand those driving forces that increase or decrease vulnerability at all scales”. This
study identifies one important driving force by demonstrating how low socio-economic status
renders women more vulnerable to the mortal impact of natural disasters.
In brief, we find that natural disasters affect women more adversely than men in terms of
the effect of disasters on the life expectancy at birth. What this means is that natural disasters
on average kill more women than men or kill women at a younger age than men and the more
so the stronger the disaster. Yet, the extent to which women are more likely to die than men
or to die at a younger age from the immediate disaster impact or from post-disaster events
depends not only on disaster strength itself. Rather, the effect of catastrophic events is
5
contingent on the socio-economic status of women in the affected country’s society. The
higher women’s status, the smaller is the differential negative effect of natural disasters on
female relative to male life expectancy. What this means is that where the socio-economic
status of women is high, men and women will die in roughly equal numbers during and after
natural disasters, whereas more women than men die (or women die at a younger age) when
the socio-economic status of women is low. These results corroborate a vulnerability
approach to natural disasters since the more adverse impact of disasters on female compared
to male life expectancy is clearly contingent on the extent of socially constructed
vulnerability and there is nothing natural in the gendered impact of disasters on life
expectancy.
3
This article is structured as follows: The next section presents arguments and anecdotal
evidence suggesting that natural disasters increase female mortality more than that of men.
Two hypotheses are developed from this discussion and are put to an empirical test. The then
following section describes the sources of data and the operationalization of the relevant
variables for the econometric estimation. A discussion of the appropriate estimation
technique is followed by a presentation of results. We conclude by arguing that our study’s
findings support a vulnerability approach to natural disasters.
Natural disasters and the gender gap in life expectancy
Human beings can and in fact do influence – willingly and unwillingly – the degree to which
natural disasters harm people, reduce welfare and cost lives. This section deals with the
interaction between natural disasters and societies. For a whole range of reasons, mortality
vulnerability to natural hazards is likely to be gender-specific, with women bearing the major
burden.
6
From a conceptual perspective, it seems most fruitful to distinguish between three main
causes for gender differences in mortality vulnerability to natural disasters: First, biological
and physiological differences between men and women may at times disadvantage women in
their immediate response to the disaster. Second, social norms and role behavior may lead to
a behavior of women, which increases their vulnerability in the immediate course of the
disaster. And third, disasters may lead to shortage of resources of basic need as well as a
temporary breakdown of social order, in which case the competition between individuals
becomes fiercer and existing forms of gender discrimination become exacerbated and new
forms of discrimination can emerge. With the exception of the biological and physiological
reasons, the higher vulnerability of women is socially constructed and is due to differences in
the socio-economic status of men and women. In the following subsections, we discuss the
three main causes in turn. Needless to say, these causes are not independent but may easily
reinforce each other. However, for explanatory purposes we discuss them separately and in
turn.
Biological and physiological differences
Biological and physiological differences in disaster response capacity can lead to differential
mortality rates for three main reasons. First, men can be physiologically better equipped to
withstand a disaster’s physical impact. For instance, if a woman is less strong than male
counterparts, she will be more easily swept away by wind or water. On average, women can
run less quickly and climb posts, trees and other rescue points with greater difficulty and
lower speed. However, as an Oxfam International (2005) report on the December 2004
tsunami’s impact on women in South and South-East Asia demonstrates, differences in self-
rescue ability are partly determined by learned skills and therefore not simply down to
physiological differences: In affected regions of Sri Lanka swimming and tree climbing were
7
taught predominantly to boys and men to perform tasks “that are done nearly exclusively by
men” (Oxfam International 2005: 9), which helped them to survive the waves. Also, as Ikeda
(1995: 188) points out, one may ask why the body size of women is on average smaller and
lighter.
4
Her study of gender differences in mortality from the 1991 Bangladesh cyclone
shows how physical disadvantages interact with social norms and role behavior that put
women at a disadvantage in their rescue efforts (we discuss these issues in more depths
below). It is thus potentially misleading when a group of public health scholars attribute
higher female to male mortality from the same event uncritically “to factors such as physical
size, strength, and endurance” (Bern et al. 1993: 75).
Second, men and women have different propensities to die from various diseases, but the
implications for gender-specific disaster mortality are ambiguous. With the possible
exception of measles, for which some evidence suggests that women might be more
susceptible to die from (Garenne 1995), in general men are more prone to acquire and die
from parasitic and infectious diseases (Owens 2002). Toole (1997a) in his review of the
literature comes to the conclusion that communicable disease epidemics are rare after most
natural disasters, with the exception of droughts and famines. On the whole, there is no
reason to suspect that diseases related to natural disasters will systematically disadvantage
women. Furthermore, in principle, women are at an advantage in famines and droughts also
because they can better cope with food shortages due to their lower nutritional requirements
and higher body fat, unless they are pregnant or lactating (Rivers 1982). This can explain in
part why overall mortality rates for females are often lower in many famines, particularly the
very severe ones of the 19
th
and early 20
th
century, than they are for men (Macintyre 2002).
5
If, nevertheless, at the same time in some famines more female than male famine victims die
at a very young age or as infants, this must be due to discriminatory access to food resources
in times of famines with a bias against baby and children girls – see Mariam (1986: 57) for
8
the Ethiopian famine of the early 1970s, Kidane (1989, 1990) for the Ethiopian famine of
1984/85, Greenough (1982) and Agarwal (1990: 225f.) for the Bengal famine of 1943/44 and
Dyson (1991a, 1991b) for South Asian famines more generally.
6
There are no reliable
statistics on the great Chinese famine of the early 1960s, but the account given by a surviving
Chinese peasant woman is revealing: “Families tried to pool their rations and often the
husband would rule that any female children should be allowed to die first…” (Becker 1996:
3).
Third, women in the final stages of pregnancy are less mobile and will find it difficult to
rescue themselves. Furthermore, large-scale natural disasters can have severe detrimental
effects on the social infrastructures of affected countries, reducing access to food, hygiene,
health services and clean water (Noji 1997a). Due to their reproductive roles, women are
particularly negatively affected if the basic health care infrastructure is severely damaged or
health expenditures are reduced to re-allocate public funds for immediate disaster response
purposes. The reason is that this will reduce obstetrical care and will increase the number of
miscarriages as well as maternal and infant mortality.
Social Norms and Role Behavior
The discussion above would suggest that biologically or physiologically determined sex
differences in disaster mortality exist, but that their impact is likely to be weaker than it
appears at first sight. Social norms and role behavior might also provide reasons for gendered
disaster vulnerability in putting women at a clear disadvantage when it comes to rescue
attempts. We stress that even if women follow these social norms and role behaviors
seemingly voluntarily, the norms and roles will often derive from the unequal distribution of
power between men and women in many societies.
9
In many countries women are supposed to look after and protect children and the elderly
as well as the family’s domestic property, which hampers their own rescue efforts in almost
any type of natural disaster (Beinin 1981; Schwoebel and Menon 2004; Oxfam International
2005). Dress codes can restrict women’s ability to move quickly, while behavioral
restrictions can hinder their ability to re-locate without their husband’s, father’s or brother’s
consent. For example, in rural Bangladesh women are expected to wear a sari, a traditional
cloth that hampers running and swimming, and to remain in the bari, typically the houses of
the family and near kin. This can impede their movement and their access to information
about cyclone-induced floods (Ikeda 1995). Others report a social prejudice against women
learning to swim, drastically reducing survival chances in flooding (Cannon 2000: 52).
In many societies there is a traditional division of labor that can disadvantage women in
the event of certain natural disasters. Oxfam (2005) reports that many women in rural coastal
areas of Indonesia were at home, whereas the men were out at sea fishing or otherwise away
from home, when the Tsunami hit the coast. In India they were waiting at the seashore for the
fishermen to arrive. In both cases, many more men were spared because the waves only
gather height and strength as they approach shore and have their most fatal impact directly at
the coast. Similarly, as concerns earthquakes, if men are out in the open or work in more
robustly built factories and public buildings while women stay at home in houses and
dwellings more easily struck down by earthquakes, then this type of natural disaster is likely
to affect women more adversely, given that inadequate building structures are by far the main
cause of earthquake fatality (Noji 1997b). Even when men are at home, this does not
necessarily mean that they are equally affected as women. In earthquakes in India men
reportedly survived better events that hit by night because they would sleep outside and on
roof tops during warm nights, a behavior impossible for most women who literally became
trapped in their domestic homes (Krishnaraj 1997).
10
Yet, as with biological and physiological causes for differential mortality rates, a caveat
is in place here as well since differences in social roles and behavior need not always affect
women more adversely. The effect really depends on the type of natural disaster. In
particular, some evidence suggests that more men than women die directly from severe
weather events in the United States such as lightning, thunderstorms and flash floods
(Fothergill 1998). The same is reportedly true for immediate mortality from Hurricane Mitch
in Central America in 1998 (Bradshaw 2004: 25). A likely reason is that on average more
men are engaged in outdoors work and leisure activities during such events and are more
reckless in their behavior toward risk. While it is difficult to say whether such findings
generalize to other societies, the general point remains valid: social norms and role behavior
will often put women at greater risk of disaster mortality, but this depends on the type of
disaster and its context and at times social norms and role behavior can put men at greater
risk instead.
Discrimination in Access to Resources and the Breakdown of Social Order
We have seen so far that biological and physiological differences as well as social norms and
role behavior can disadvantage women in the event of natural disasters. Yet, we have also
seen that the evidence is ambiguous on whether these differences will affect women more
adversely to a large extent.
In this subsection, we argue that while the gender differences in casualty rates result only
partly and potentially to a small part from the immediate effects of disasters, i.e. from, for
example, collapsing buildings in earthquakes or flooded cities and villages, women are much
more likely than men to die after the disaster happened. Those indirect effects can be
explained by discrimination in access to resources and the temporary breakdown of social
order. To start with, in societies with existing patterns of gender discrimination males are
11
likely to be given preferential treatment in rescue efforts. A telling example is given by a
father who – when unable to hold on to both his son and his daughter from being swept away
by a tidal surge in the 1991 Cyclone in Bangladesh – released his daughter, because “(this)
son has to carry on the family line” (quoted in Haider, Rahman and Huq 1993: 64). Men are
also likely to access and allocate the assistance given to affected families. Even in the
absence of natural disasters, Sen (1988: 454) finds that “there is a good deal of evidence from
all over the world that food is often distributed very unequally within the family – with a
distinct sex bias (against the female) and also an age bias (against the children)”. Bairagi
(1986) reports that female children were more adversely affected by famine in rural
Bangladesh than boys. When natural disaster strikes, these pre-existing discriminatory
practices become exacerbated and their detrimental health impact on women and girls
intensified. Sen (1988: 459) reports how women and girls were systematically disadvantaged
by food relief in the aftermath of flooding in West Bengal that destroyed crops and farmland.
Enarson and Morrow (1998: 21) refer to a relief worker’s finding of discriminatory access to
relief supplies in the aftermath of the 1991 Bangladesh cyclone (Khondker (1996: 288)
reports similar experiences). Ager, Ager and Long (1995) find in their study of Mozambican
refugees in Malawi in the late 1990s that relief policies were biased in favor of refugee men.
A fact sheet by the Pan-American Health Organization (2002) would suggest that this
anecdotal evidence from a few natural disasters might be representative of a more general
trend, also suggesting unequal power structures as the underlying cause: “The majority of
relief efforts are intended for the entire population of a disaster-affected area, however, when
they rely on existing structures of resource distribution that reflects the patriarchal structure
of society, women are marginalized in their access to relief resources”.
Natural disasters, if sufficiently strong, can also have both short-term and long-term
negative effects on the affected economies (Benson and Clay 2000, 2003; Freeman 2000;
12
Hines and Jaramillo 2005). Some of these detrimental effects will be compensated for by
increased migrants’ remittances, foreign lending, aid and investment, but it takes time until
they do so (Yang 2005). Women are likely to be adversely affected by damage to economic
livelihoods as basic survival strategies such as securing water, food and wood for heating
purposes often falls on women, representing an extra burden on top of the caring and
nurturing for their family (Enarson 2000). Where natural disasters reduce the purchasing
power of households, women can be more adversely affected because in many countries men
receive preferential access to resources. When resources become scarcer, then the part of the
population suffering from discrimination beforehand will necessarily be hit even harder (see
Crow and Sultana (2002) for a study of gender-conflicts in access to water and its uses in
Bangladesh). In principle, recovery assistance could be preferentially addressed at those
groups most vulnerable to protect them from the negative effect of increased discrimination.
Yet, as mentioned above already, instead of being granted a preferential role, women are
often marginalized in their access to relief resources (Pan-American Health Organization
2002). Many disaster researchers have noted that in most countries relief efforts are almost
exclusively managed and controlled by men, systematically excluding women, their needs,
competences and experiences from contributing to these efforts (see, for example, Enarson
2000; Bradshaw 2004).
7
There is widespread agreement that the poor are more adversely hit by the impact of
natural disasters than the better-off.
8
For example, they are less likely to be able to afford
housing that can withstand seismic activity, often live in flood- and storm-prone areas as well
as on unstable slopes vulnerable to landslides and they have less access to education and
financial resources to overcome adverse impacts (Noji 1997a: 12). While some have
questioned the full extent to which existing evidence backs up the claim of a strong gender
bias in poverty (Chant 2005), there is general agreement that poor people on average are
13
more likely to be female. In combination, this implies that women will be more adversely
affected by natural disasters since they are more than proportionally represented among the
poor. For instance, O’Hare (2001) finds that the most vulnerable group affected by Hurricane
07B in the Godavari Delta in India were “migrant, scheduled (low) caste women” who
formed the major part of the landless agricultural laborers. The vulnerability resulting from
predominantly female poverty is not confined to developing countries, however. For
example, UNEP (2004) cites a study by the Japanese government stating that during the Kobe
earthquake in 1995 1.5 times as many women as men died. In Kobe, many elderly single
women died because they lived in poor residential areas, which were more heavily damaged
and more likely to catch fire.
9
Lastly, there is some evidence, if not fully conclusive, that domestic and sexual violence
against women increases due to disaster-induced stress, alcohol abuse and the (temporary)
breakdown of law and order (Bradshaw 2004). If police, military and fire brigades are unable
(or unwilling) to organize the most severely affected regions, then distributive conflicts, theft
and open violence are likely to emerge. A collapse of social order may be more likely in
countries in which the political authority is weak. However, the Post-Katrina riots in New
Orleans have demonstrated that even superpowers are not safe from disaster-related social
unrest.
What is relatively well documented is that law and order are difficult to sustain when
victims of natural disasters have to seek refuge in makeshift refugee camps often far away
from their home cities or villages (Phuong 2004). In overcrowded camps anarchy rules
leaving unaccompanied women and girls particularly vulnerable to sexual abuse and rape. In
addition, women and girls are also more negatively affected by the often appalling health and
hygienic conditions in refugee camps, as already pointed out above. This situation can
become exacerbated if culturally binding norms allow certain forms of female hygienic care
14
only under conditions of privacy and separation from men, which are often impossible to
maintain in refugee camps. Toole (1997b) reports mortality rates from several refugee camps
that are up to 100 times higher than the normal mortality rate in the country. Data
disaggregated according to gender is very rare, but Toole reports data from a Burmese
refugee camp in Bangladesh where female infants were twice as likely to die than male
infants and the mortality rates of females above the age of five was 3.5 times higher than that
of males.
Hypotheses
Biological and physiological differences between men and women, social norms and roles,
which restrict the behavior of men and women differently, emergent resource shortages and
the temporary breakdown of social order jointly suggest that more women and girls than men
and boys die during and after natural disasters or die at a younger age. Yet, the theoretical
considerations of the previous sections also suggest that this effect is conditional on the
socio-economic status of women and gender relations in the society affected by the disaster.
Therefore, we postulate two hypotheses concerning the impact of natural disasters on the
gender gap in life expectancy:
Hypothesis 1: Natural disasters reduce the life expectancy of women more than that of men
and the effect is increasing in disaster strength.
This is partly because only larger disasters will kill enough people overall to leave their mark
on our life expectancy measures but, more importantly, also because only larger disasters
lead to the breakdown of infrastructure and law and order and to the drastically intensified
competition for food and other scarce resources leaving women in societies with rampant
discrimination against females more vulnerable to disaster-induced mortality.
15
Hypothesis 2: Natural disasters reduce the life expectancy of women relative to that of men
the more the lower is the socio-economic status of women.
We expect that women are more adversely affected by natural disasters where female
discrimination is more widespread before the onset of natural disaster events. Where there is
a pro-male bias in “normal” periods, such bias will become reinforced and exacerbated in
after-disaster periods (Drèze and Sen 1989: 55; Bolin, Jackson and Crist 1998: 42). Women
are more adversely hit by natural disasters if gender inequalities in access to information and
economic resources and inequalities in personal freedom of choice before, during and after
disasters create a “gendered disaster vulnerability” (Enarson 1998).
Research Design
Following on from the formulation of testable hypotheses in the preceding section, here we
discuss how we measure and operationalize the three main concepts of our analysis.
Measuring Disaster Strength
For most countries in the world, natural disasters are a relatively common event. Our source,
the Emergency Disasters Data Base (EM-DAT), collected at the University of Louvain,
Brussels, currently includes around 9,700 natural disasters from 1900 to present. Due to
limited data availability on our measure of women’s socio-economic status, our sample is
restricted to the period 1981 to 2002. The sample still covers 4,605 natural disasters since the
coverage of natural disasters in EM-DAT is not very comprehensive for the first few decades
of the last century. EM-DAT is the only global data set of natural disasters that is publicly
available. Two other global data sets are maintained by private re-insurance companies
(Swiss Re and Munich Re), but no public access is granted (Guha-Sapir and Below 2002).
16
To be recorded in the database, an event must fulfill at least one of the following
conditions: a) ten or more people reported as killed; b) 100 people reported as affected; c) a
state of emergency has been declared; or d) the country has issued a call for international
assistance. Clearly, with the latter two criteria the inclusion of an event in the database is
partially endogenous to the response of governmental authorities in affected regions, states or
countries. Platt (1999) shows for the case of the United States how the political struggle over
who pays how much for the costs of natural disasters influences the likelihood of an event
being declared a “major disaster”. It is probable that political considerations will affect the
likelihood of declaration of state of emergency or, depending on the circumstances, a call for
international assistance, in other countries as well. We see no reason why this should bias our
results since such political considerations are unlikely to be systematically correlated with our
variables of interest. Nevertheless, we will show below that our results uphold if we restrict
the sample to observations with ten or more people killed.
Most disasters take place in large countries, with the US (442) leading the list followed
by India (293) and China (125). On the bottom end of the number of disaster ranking, we find
microstates but also Finland (1), Turkmenistan (2) and Sweden (4). Ethiopia (311,286),
Sudan (158,252) and Bangladesh (149,225) had the most victims in absolute numbers, while
the respective figure for the USA is 8,001 disaster victims. Accordingly, we find relatively
poor countries suffering relatively more from disasters in terms of people killed than
relatively rich countries. With drought- and famine-ridden countries leading the loss of
victims per disaster (Ethiopia 4716, Sudan 3297, Mozambique 2374), only about 18 people
die from the average disaster in the US.
The number of deaths per disaster offers just a poor description of the nature of our data.
In fact, while most natural disasters cost few if any lives, the three most severe disasters – the
droughts in Ethiopia and Sudan in 1984 and the flood in Bangladesh in 1991 – account for
17
almost half of all fatalities in our sample. In other words, severe disasters are rare events. As
a consequence, the distribution of the disaster strength variable is extremely skewed. It
follows that we need to carefully check the validity of our results with regards to the leverage
that certain influential observations might have on the results (see our bootstrap estimations
below).
In operationalizing the EM-DAT data we have made three important choices: First, for
the purpose of this study, we decided not to focus on a specific disaster type but to consider
all types for which EM-DAT provides information together. Our measure of natural disasters
includes droughts, earthquakes, epidemics, extreme temperatures, famines, fires, floods,
insect infestations, landslides, volcano eruptions, waves/surges, and wind storms. Table 1
provides some summary statistics on each disaster type. We recognize that famines in
particular are often triggered by both natural factors and human-made decisions, but like
Drèze and Sen (1989) we believe that these events cannot be neatly separated into “human-
made” and “nature-made” types. We exclude disasters triggered by technological hazards
such as the large-scale industrial accidents of Bhopal or Chernobyl since they are clearly
human-made. Our decision to address all natural disasters together makes it impossible to
detect differences between the effects of various disaster types on the gender gap in life
expectancy, but we believe that this is inevitable for essentially two reasons. First, the
variance of some subcategories of natural disasters is too low to allow sufficiently efficient
estimation. Second, the EM-DAT unique categorization of each natural disaster into a
specific disaster type is open to contestation. A natural disaster is thus, for example, a drought
or a famine or a flood even though in the actual event most people might die from epidemics.
< Insert Table 1 around here >
18
Second, since our theoretical considerations suggest that the impact on the gender gap in
life expectancy increases with the magnitude of disaster, we cannot simply use dummy
variables for disaster events, but need a measure of disaster strength instead. We consider the
number of people killed (rather than, for example, the number of people being affected) as
the most important information of the magnitude of a disaster. We believe that the number of
people killed is a better proxy of disaster strength because it is by far less arbitrary than the
accounts of the number of people affected. EM-DAT defines the category of affected people
as all those requiring immediate assistance. But the number of affected people, thus defined,
is much more difficult to estimate and estimates from different sources will vary much more
dramatically than the number of people killed. Guha-Sapir and Below (2002) provide some
evidence that the number of people killed can be estimated with higher accuracy than the
number of people affected. In a comparative analysis of the way disasters in four disaster-
prone countries are recorded in EM-DAT and the data sets maintained by Munich Re and
Swiss Re, they found that estimates of the number of people killed for the same disasters
were fairly close across the three data sets, whereas the estimates of the number of people
affected varied widely and sometimes by orders of magnitude. Quarantelli (2001: 326) in his
critique of disaster statistics also agrees that “figures on deaths are certainly the most
reliable”. Of course, we agree with his verdict that even estimates of fatality figures are often
subject to uncertainties and, sometimes, deliberate distortions, possibly on average tending to
overestimate true casualty figures (Quarantelli 2002: 329). The number of killed persons as
our disaster strength variable is therefore a proxy rather than an exact measure of the severity
of disaster and there is likely to be measurement error in the variable. However, as long as the
error is not systematically correlated with the gender gap in life expectancy, for which we see
no reason why this should be the case, the measurement error will make our estimates less
efficient, but will not bias them.
19
Third, we divide the number of people killed by total population size of the country hit
by the disaster. The use of per capita data is analytically warranted since the influence of
natural disasters on an affected country’s life expectancy not only depends on the magnitude
of the disaster but also on the population size of the affected country. Everything else being
equal, disasters of a given size reduce the life expectancy the more, the smaller is the
population size of the country under observation. The same should hold true when we
consider the gender gap in life expectancy rather than life expectancy itself. A disaster that
has no influence on the life expectancy can hardly affect the gender gap.
10
Ideally, we would
have life expectancy data for sub-national regions, so that we could easily estimate the
immediate and lingering consequences of a disaster on the affected population.
Unfortunately, this information does not exist. The data we have allows analyzing only the
average life expectancy at the level of the nation state. Hence, our disaster strength variable is
the cumulated number of people killed by all natural disasters in a given year divided by the
affected country’s total population.
The Gender Gap in Life Expectancy
To measure the size of the gender gap, we employ data provided by the US Census’s
International Data Base (IDB), which to our knowledge is the most reliable source for life
expectancy data in panel form. In comparison to data provided by the World Bank
(2004), the
IDP is much better maintained and has by far less missing data. The IDP was created for
scientific purposes – in response to the information requirements of International Program
Center (IPC) staff to meet the needs of organizations that sponsor research efforts. The IDB
combines data from country sources (especially censuses and surveys) with IPC’s estimates
and projections, which are based on available census data and group cohort population
projection techniques to compute data between the censuses. These projections are based on
20
country-specific fixed-slope logistic interpolations in the years between national censuses.
Moreover, recent population and socio-economic trends are taken into account “if the
projected trends are plausible” (US Census 2004: B5). For instance, projection of fertility
utilizes trends in age at marriage, the percentage of women using contraception, existence
and scope of family-planning programs and data on educational attainment are used in life
expectancy calculations.
11
The gender gap in life expectancy shows large variations across time and space. On
average, women’s life expectancy is 4.69 years higher than that of men. However, in 64 out
of 2266 country-years men actually lived longer than women. In Bangladesh, India and Nepal
this phenomenon is common and can possibly be attributed to the traditional cultural bias
against females in these countries. In all other cases a higher male than female life
expectancy is the exception rather than the rule. On the other end of the spectrum, the gender
gap is largest in post-transition Russia. The life expectancy difference between Russian
women and men peaked in 1994 reaching an extraordinary 13.74 years.
12
Noteworthy,
countries from the former Soviet Union hold 49 of the top 50 country-years in terms of
gender gap – the notable exception being Guatemala in 1981. In all these cases, women lived
about 70 to 75 years while men on average died at the age of 60 or before.
We use as our dependent variable the ratio rather than the absolute difference in years of
female to male life expectancy. The reason is that, under certain conditions
13
, changes in the
absolute difference of female to male life expectancy can be a misleading indicator of the
health effects of events. Therefore, if we measured the gender gap as the absolute difference
between the life expectancies of women and men, it is possible that even though an equal
number of men and women die, the gender gap is still decreasing. This, in turn, implies that it
is possible that male and female life expectancy can decrease by the same number of years
and yet more men had died than women. Furthermore, equal proportional decreases in male
21
and female life expectancy will lead to a larger absolute fall in the life expectancy of the
gender with the higher ex ante life expectancy – typically the female life expectancy. This
problem is accounted for if we measure the gender gap as the ratio of female to male life
expectancy. To be on the safe side, we additionally add the absolute change in population life
expectancy as a regressor. In simulations, we found that using the life expectancy ratio and
controlling for the absolute changes in population life expectancy removes the distortions that
result from the computation of life expectancies.
While at least in our view the IDB data is superior to all alternatives, it does not come
without potential drawbacks. Most importantly, the analyses of IDB data must almost
necessarily suffer from correlated errors, since models that are used to predict the values of a
certain variable can neither avoid systematic errors nor can it guarantee serial independence
of observations. The imputed data for year t+1 cannot be independent of the observation in
year t, may that year be imputed or not. Any regression analysis based on this data inherits
these systematic errors. Fortunately, since we have panel data we can use a random-effects
estimator with an assumed first-order autoregressive error that deals with the problem of
auto-correlation (see the description of the estimation procedure below).
Women’s Socio-Economic Status
To measure the socio-economic status of women in society we use a measure of women’s
economic and social rights, taken from Cingranelli and Richards’ (2004) Human Rights
Database. Using the annual United States State Department’s Country Reports on Human
Rights Practices, Cingranelli and Richards code a discrete variable for economic and social
rights that each takes on one of four values. We add both variables to create a combined
measure of women’s socio-economic rights. The appendix provides details on the range of
rights covered and the coding scheme used. Unfortunately, this variable is only available for
22
1981 onwards, which restricts our analysis to the years 1981 to 2002, after which we have no
information on other variables either.
Control Variables
Life expectancy and the gender gap therein are a product of geographical, social, economic
and political influences. Some of these influences could be correlated with the variables of
our main interest. For instance, the expected number of deaths in a disaster is negatively
related to the wealth of the country. Omitted variables correlated with the exogenous
variables of interest cause bias when they also exert an influence on the dependent variable.
To minimize bias, we follow two strategies: First, climatic and other geographical differences
as well as genetic conditions can impact the gender gap in life expectancy and are
(approximately) time-invariant (UN Population Division 1988). We therefore include
regional dummy variables in our estimations. Second, with per capita income, political
stability and the level of political suppression we add three important time-variant control
variables. Data on per capita income is taken from World Bank (2004). Regime stability is
defined as the number of years since the most recent three-point change on the so-called
Polity score, which is a measure of institutionalized democracy and autocracy popular in
political sciences, or the end of transition from a period of lack of stable political institutions
(data from www.cidcm.umd.edu/inscr/polity/). Political suppression is measured by data
provided by Freedom House (2004), which bases its scale on expert judgment of the extent of
violation of civil and political rights in countries.
Other socio-economic variables of potential additional interest, such as health
expenditures, access to food, and safe water and clean sanitation are not available for many
countries in our sample. Moreover, while the severity of disasters is partly determined by per
capita income (Kahn 2005) and (possibly) the level of democracy, the infrequent outbreak of
23
disasters eliminates the potential correlation between our control variables and disaster
strength. In fact, the correlation coefficient between the disaster strength and the control
variables does not exceed 0.06 in our sample. Thus, from an econometric point of view, the
inclusion of these variables is therefore neither recommended nor warranted.
Estimation Procedure
Our data set consists of annual observations at the country level over the years 1981 to 2002
for up to 141 countries, but the amount of information available for each country may vary. It
thus consists of what is commonly known as cross-national, time-series or unbalanced panel
data. Analysis of panel data has to deal with the two classical problems of serial correlation
and various types of unit heterogeneity. To obtain unbiased and efficient estimates of the
model at hand, the estimation procedure of choice has to resolve both problems without
causing too many unwanted side effects (Adolph et al. 2005; Plümper et al. 2005). As
mentioned already we use a random-effects estimator with an assumed first-order
autoregressive error that deals with the problem of auto-correlation. To account for some
heterogeneity across countries, we include regional dummy variables. The regions are North
America, Central America, South America, Western Europe, Eastern Europe, West Africa,
Southern Africa, Northern Africa, West Asia, South and East Asia, and Australia and
Oceania.
Estimation Results
Main Results
Our theory predicts a significantly negative effect of the disaster strength variable on the
gender gap in life expectancy, and a significantly positive interaction effect of women’s
rights and disaster strength. Table 2 reports the results from two estimates: model 1 is the
24
baseline model that merely includes disaster strength, women’s socio-economic rights and
their interaction effect, while table 3 adds the control variables. A comparison of the two
models shows that the addition of the controls makes practically no difference to the results
on our main variables of interest.
< Insert Table 2 around here >
Table 2 lends support to our hypotheses. We find that the gender gap in life expectancy
declines with disaster strength (hypothesis 1). We also find that a higher level of women’s
socio-economic rights offsets the negative effect of natural disasters on women, which
supports our second hypothesis. The coefficient has the expected positive sign and size.
14
It
suggests that the adverse effect of natural disasters on the gender gap in life expectancy is
conditioned upon the socio-economic status of women in society. In countries with better
rights for women, the adverse impact of natural disasters on women’s life expectancy relative
to men vanishes.
Robustness analysis
We conducted a number of robustness tests – see table 3, which starts by replicating our
model 1 for comparative purposes.
< Insert Table 3 around here >
We find that the negative effect of natural disasters on the gender gap in life expectancy
decreases with higher levels of women’s socio-economic rights. Western countries are
characterized by both low natural disaster intensity (in terms of people killed relative to
25
population size) and high women’s socio-economic rights. This begs the question whether
our results are perhaps driven by the inclusion of this group of countries in our sample. To
check this, in model 2 we exclude Canada, the United States, Western European countries as
well as Japan, Australia and New Zealand from the sample. The results are hardly affected.
Next, we have mentioned above that what counts as a natural disaster in our source, EM-
DAT, can be triggered by the declaration of a state of emergency or a call for international
assistance, which may be subject to political considerations. Another criterion – number of
people killed is ten or more – is far less subject to political influence, unless a country
manages to hide or artificially inflate disaster deaths. To check that declarations of state of
emergency and calls for international assistance do not bias our results, we include in model 4
only observations with ten or more people killed. The results uphold. In model 5 we exclude
droughts and famines from the definition of natural disasters, as these are events of a more
chronic nature. In model 6, we do the opposite and exclude all natural disasters other than
droughts and famines. In model 5, the disaster strength variable and its interaction effect
become marginally insignificant. However, this is due to the increase in the standard error
following the reduction in the variance of the disaster variable for the remaining disaster
types, which renders estimation less efficient. Importantly, the coefficients remain very
similar. Results from model 6 are again similar to results from model 1.
We have noted already that severe natural disasters are a rather rare event. While the
main results reported above support our theoretical expectations well, the question is whether
they are driven by a few very influential disaster observations. To check this, we now apply a
bootstrap estimation of standard errors. The purpose of this test is to see whether the
statistical significance of our main variables of interest is robust or is due to the particular
population sample at hand. To save space, we report only results of applying the bootstrap
test on results of model 1. Applying it to the results for model 2 as well makes little
26
difference. The bootstrap is a re-sampling technique, which sheds some light on the
distributional properties of statistics, but it is also useful as a means of obtaining more robust
standard errors. The bootstrap algorithm draws repeated re-samples (with replacement) from
the given population, and then estimates the model at hand. Hence, the sample that we
estimate always has the same size as model 1, but the composition of the samples varies,
because a single observation from the original dataset can be drawn repeatedly (which
implies that other observation will not be included in that estimate). Commonly used
replications are 100, 500, or 1000. The t-statistics averaged across a series of say 1000
samples necessarily have a larger standard error than the model estimated on the basis of the
total population. Table 4 reports results on our variables of interest from the bootstrap test
with 1000 replications. They suggest that the disaster strength variable and its interaction
effect with women’s economic rights remain statistically significant even if the standard
errors are bootstrapped. Moreover, we find that the bias corrected estimates of our
coefficients and the standard errors diverge no more than moderately from the results
reported in table 2. It is typically assumed that if the bias is larger than 25 percent of the
standard error of the sampling distribution, the bias corrected confidence intervals are likely
to be more appropriate than the normal confidence intervals. In our case, the bias is smaller
than 25 percent. Our interpretation of model 1 thus remains valid. In other words, we can be
fairly certain that the statistical significance of our main variables of interest do not depend
on outliers.
< Insert Table 4 around here >
27
Conclusion
Geographers and other social scientists have argued for many years that there is little natural
about the impact of natural disasters on affected people. As O’Keefe, Westgate and Wisner
(1976) have put it in the title of their early contribution to Nature, “taking the naturalness out
of natural disasters” is what is needed. Natural disasters do not affect people equally as if by
an arbitrary stroke of nature. Instead, the disaster impact is contingent on the vulnerability of
affected people, which can and often does systematically differ across economic class,
ethnicity, gender and other factors.
In this article, we addressed one specific impact of natural disasters (disaster mortality)
and how it affects women differentially from men. We observed a systematic effect of
disaster strength on the gender gap in life expectancy if the disaster affected societies, in
which the socio-economic status of women is low. In such societies, natural disasters will
kill, directly and indirectly via related post-disaster events, more women than men or will kill
women at a younger age than men. These findings support a vulnerability approach to natural
disasters. There are few reasons why female life expectancy should be systematically more
adversely affected by natural disasters than that of men were it only for reasons determined
by nature, such as biological and physiological differences that on average disadvantage
women and girls relative to men and boys. A systematic effect on the gender gap in life
expectancy is only plausible if natural disasters exacerbate previously existing patterns of
discrimination that render females more vulnerable to the fatal impact of disasters. That this
is no mere speculation is demonstrated by the fact that the adverse impact of disasters on
females relative to men vanishes with rising socio-economic status of women. We
acknowledge, however, that much more inter-disciplinary research between medical and
social scientists is needed to fully understand the interplay between mortality and gender in
the presence of natural disasters. We also need more research to fully understand why and
28
how disaster strength interacts with mortality in general and with female mortality in
particular.
Our findings require relevant stakeholders to go beyond technical fixes in dealing with
natural disasters. True, the underlying cultural, social and economic patterns that lead to a
low socio-economic status of women and thereby generate their specific vulnerability to
natural disasters are not easy to deal with. But this does not mean that nothing can be done.
Our finding that, on average, large natural disasters lower the life expectancy of women more
than that of men and particularly so where women have a lower socio-economic status
implies that policy makers, non-governmental organizations and the academic community
need to pay closer attention to the gendered nature of disaster vulnerability. Such attention
should focus on the special medical, economic and security needs of women in the aftermath
of disasters as well as on mechanisms to ensure fair and non-discriminatory allocation of
relief resources. Developing such policies will not entirely prevent the adverse impact of
large-scale natural disasters on women in societies where their everyday socio-economic
status is low. Such policies should, however, reduce the excess disaster mortality of women
compared to that of men.
Acknowledgements
Equal authorship. We would like to thank several anonymous referees and commentators for
many helpful and constructive comments. Eric Neumayer acknowledges financial support
from the Leverhulme Trust.
29
NOTES
1 We agree with Varley (1994: 4f.) that the vulnerability approach as an analytical concept is applicable
even if one does not subscribe to the critique of capitalism embraced by some of its proponents (see
Wisner 2000a, 2000b and references cited in Varley 1994: 4). Albala-Bertrand (1993) applies the
vulnerability approach within the framework of mainstream economics.
2 Cutter (2003: 7-8) herself describes a number of most significant themes for a geographical research
agenda of vulnerability science.
3 See Plümper and Neumayer (2006) for similar results on the influence of militarized conflict on the
gender gap in life expectancy.
4 Indeed, based on twin research, medical research has demonstrated that ‘environmental factors’ (year
of birth, region, childhood living conditions, and education) influenced body-height (Silventoinen et al.
2000). This research shows that biological sex differences are not independent of gender relations –
though of course gender differences fall far short of explaining physical differences across sexes in its
entirety.
5 Other reasons include the typical out-migration of men, which makes them vulnerable to accidents,
attacks and the acquisition of infectious diseases, as well as the reduced fertility rate of women during
famines compared to non-famine years (Dyson 1991b; Macintyre 2002).
6 De Waal (1989), however, finds no significant sex differences in child excess mortality in his case
study of famine mortality during 1984/85 in Darfur, Sudan, except for the age group between five and
nine where more boys seem to have died.
7 While Aquino et al. (1992) argue that unequal distribution of resources leads to less cooperation,
Kramer (1990) demonstrates that not only does an increase in resource scarcity foster cooperation, the
increase in cooperation may even partly offset the adverse effects of scarcity. More recently, Hausken
(1999) has shown that within-group competition increases if between-group competition becomes
fiercer. His findings suggest that resource scarcity may actually increase both cooperation and conflict
in a society. Our argument here is consistent with all these diverse arguments and findings, as we
simply claim that if resources become scarce, distributive issues become more important. Under this
condition, the extent of cooperation determines how many individuals will suffer; societal norms,
however, determine which individuals will suffer the most.
30
8 This is true even for highly developed countries like the United States as the televised pictures of New
Orleans in the wake of hurricane Katrina showed so vividly. The overview article by Fothergill and
Peek (2004) demonstrates that this represents a general pattern.
9 Seager (2005) in a commentary published by the Chicago Tribune presented anecdotal evidence
according to which most people trapped in New Orleans in the aftermath of hurricane Katrina were
(black) women (see also Seager 2006). She estimates that 80 percent of those who did not leave the city
within time were women and speculates that a similar ratio will apply to the sex difference in fatalities.
However, at least as concerns direct and identified victims from Katrina, the statistics published by the
State of Louisiana’s Department of Health and Hospitals would suggest that while the victims were
predominantly old and African American, an about equal number of men and women died
(http://www.dhh.louisiana.gov/offices/publications/pubs-192/Deceased%20Victims_2-23-
2006_information.pdf - last accessed 11 July 2006). The possibility remains that more women may
have died in the aftermath of and as the indirect consequence of Katrina.
10 The argument that the average life expectancy may remain constant if the female life expectancy
declines while the male life expectancy increases is not valid, because natural disasters do not increase
the life expectancy of a large subgroup of the population.
11 Recently, the World Health Organization has developed new data on Disability Adjusted Life Years
(DALYs) lost that represent a very comprehensive and data-intensive measure with less measurement
errors than the standard life expectancy measures. DALYs are calculated for individual major disease
categories and reflect the years of life lost due to death in the fatal cases as well as the expected
disability caused by a disease in non-fatal cases. The great disadvantage is that DALYs are not yet
available over a longer period of time, allowing only a cross-sectional analysis, which is a major
drawback as explained in the text. Also, while non-adjusted life expectancy is theoretically inferior to
disability-adjusted life expectancy, we note that the WHO itself has pointed out that the two are very
highly correlated (Mathers et al. 2001, figure 4).
12 Reasons were manifold. While Andreev et al. (2003) hold the Russian health care system responsible,
other sources report a steep increase in alcohol abuse and deaths from organized crime (McKee 1999).
13 Changes in mortality rates of a specific age cohort affect also person-years lived of older age cohorts
due to changes to the number of survivors to older age and this has a larger effect on life expectancy at
birth if the life expectancy of the age cohort is higher (Preston et al. 2001: 64). Natural disasters would
31
lead to relatively larger changes in the gender gap in life expectancy if the persons killed have not yet
reached age cohorts that show large gender differences in mortality rates. For example, if most
individuals killed in a disaster and its aftermath are below 30 years old and if women above 30 are less
likely to die at a certain age than men of the same age and women’s life expectancy at birth is higher,
then the change in the absolute difference between female and male life expectancy is a biased proxy
for the mortality rates of that conflict.
14 Given the estimated coefficients, theoretically there exist combinations of high values of both disaster
strength and women’s economic rights that would suggest an overall increase in the gender gap in life
expectancy. However, such combinations do not exist in our sample.
32
References
Adolph, C., D. M. Butler, and S. Wilson. 2005. Like shoes and shirt, one size does not fit all:
Evidence on time series cross-section estimators and specifications from Monte Carlo
experiments. Paper presented at the 101
st
annual conference of the American Political
Science Association, Washington.
Agarwal, B. 1990. Social security and the family: coping with seasonality and calamity in
rural India. In Social security in developing countries, eds. E. Ahmad, J. Drèze, J. Hills,
and A.K. Sen, 171-244. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Albala-Bertrand, J. M. 1993. Political Economy of Large Disasters. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Alcántara-Ayala, I. 2002. Geomorphology, natural hazards, vulnerability and prevention of
natural disasters in developing countries. Geomorphology 47:107-124.
Anbarci, N., M. Escaleras, and C. A. Register. 2005. Earthquake fatalities: the interaction of
nature and political economy. Journal of Public Economics 89 (9-10):1907-1933.
Anderson, K. M., and G. Manuel. 1994. Gender differences in reported stress response to the
Loma Prieta earthquake. Sex Roles 30 (9-10):725-733.
Anderson, W. A. 2000. Women and children facing disaster. In Managing disaster risk in
emerging economies, eds. A. Kreimer and M. Arnold, 85-90. Washington, D.C.: World
Bank.
Andreev, E., E. Nolte, V. Shkolnikov, E. Varavikova, and M. McKee. 2003. The evolving
pattern of avoidable mortality in Russia. International Journal of Epidemiology
32(3):437-46.
Aquino, K., V. Steisel and A. Kay. 1992. The effect of resource distribution, voice, and
decision framing on the provision of public goods, Journal of Conflict Resolution 36
(4):665-687.
Becker, J. 1996. Hungry ghosts: China’s secret famine. London: John Murray.
33
Beinin, L. 1981. An examination of health data following two major earthquakes in Russia.
Disasters 5 (2):142-146.
Belsley, D.A., E. Kuh, and R. E. Welsch. 1980. Regression Diagnostics. New York: John
Wiley.
Benson, C., and E. Clay. 2000. Developing countries and the economic impacts of natural
disasters. In Managing disaster risk in emerging economies, eds. A. Kreimer and M.
Arnold, 11-21. Washington, D.C.: World Bank.
———. 2003. Economic and financial impacts of natural disasters: an assessment of their
effects and options for mitigation: synthesis report. London: Overseas Development
Institute.
Bern, C., J. Sniezek, G. M. Mathbor, M. S. Siddiqi, C. Ronsmans, A. M. R. Chowdhury, A.
E. Choudhury, K. Islam, M. Bennish, E. Noji, and R. I. Glass. 1993. Risk factors for
mortality in the Bangladesh cyclone of 1991. Bulletin of the World Health Organization
71 (1): 73-78.
Bolin, R., M. Jackson, and A. Crist. 1998. Gender inequality, vulnerability and disasters:
issues in theory and research. In The gendered terrain of disaster – through women’s
eyes, eds. E. Enarson and B.H. Morrow, 27-44. Westport: Praeger.
Bradshaw, S. 2004. Socio-economic impacts of natural disasters: a gender analysis. Serie
Manuales 33. Santiago de Chile: United Nations Economic Commission for Latin
America and the Caribbean.
Cannon, T. 1994. Vulnerability analysis and the explanation of ‘natural’ disasters. In
Disasters, Development and Environment, ed. A. Varley, 13-30. Chichester: John Wiley
& Sons.
———. 2000. Vulnerability analysis and disasters. In Floods, ed. D.J. Parker, 45-55.
London: Routledge.
34
Chant, S. 2006. Re-thinking the “feminization of poverty” in relation to gender indices.
Journal of Human Development 7 (2):201-220.
Cingranelli, D. L., D. L. and Richards. 2004. Cingranelli and Richards Human Rights
Dataset. http://ciri.binghamton.edu/ (last accessed 17 August 2006).
Claeson M., E. R. Bos, T. Mawji, and I. Pathmanathan. 2000. Reducing child mortality in
India in the new millennium. Bulletin of the World Health Organization 78: 1192-1199.
Crow, B., and F. Sultana. 2002. Gender, class, and access to water: three cases in a poor and
crowded delta. Society and Natural Resources 15: 709-724.
Cuny, F. C. 1983. Disasters and development. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Cutter, S. L. 1995. The forgotten casualties: women, children, and environmental change.
Global Environmental Change 5 (3): 181-194.
———. 1996. Vulnerability to environmental hazards. Progress in Human Geography 20
(4):529-539.
———. 2003. The vulnerability of science and the science of vulnerability. Annals of the
Association of American Geographers 93 (1):1-12.
De Waal, A. 1989. Famine mortality: a case study of Darfur, Sudan 1984-5. Population
Studies 43 (1):5-24.
Degg, M. R., and D. K. Chester. 2005a. Seismic and volcanic hazards in Peru: changing
attitudes to disaster mitigation. Geographical Journal 171 (2):125-145.
———. 2005b. Earthquake vulnerability in the Middle East. Geography 90 (1):54-66.
Drèze, J., and A. K. Sen. 1989. Hunger and public action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Dyson, T. 1991a. On the demography of South Asian famines part I. Population Studies 45
(1):5-25.
———. 1991b. On the demography of South Asian famines part II. Population Studies 45
(2):279-297.
35
Enarson, E. 1998. Through women’s eyes: a gendered research agenda for disaster social
science. Disasters 22 (2):157-173.
Enarson, E. 2000. Gender and natural disasters. Working Paper No. 1 Infocus Programme on
Crisis Response and Reconstruction. Geneva: International Labour Organisation,
Recovery and Reconstruction Department.
Enarson, E., and B. H. Morrow, eds., 1998. The gendered terrain of disaster – through
women’s eyes. Westport: Praeger.
Enarson, E., and L. Meyreles. 2004. International perspectives on gender and disaster:
differences and possibilities. International Journal of Sociology and Social Policy 24
(10-11): 49-93.
Fordham, M. 2004. Gendering vulnerability analysis: towards a more nuanced approach. In
Mapping vulnerability: disasters, development and people, eds. G. Bankoff, G. Frerks,
and D. Hilhorst, 174-182. London: Earthscan.
Fothergill, A. 1998. The neglect of gender in disaster work: an overview of the literature. In
The gendered terrain of disaster – through women’s eyes, eds. E. Enarson and B.H.
Morrow, 11-25. Westport: Praeger.
Fothergill, A., and L. A. Peek. 2004. Poverty and disasters in the United States: a review of
recent sociological findings. Natural Hazards 32 (1):89-110.
Freedom House. 2004. Freedom in the World. New York: Freedom House.
www.freedomhouse.org/
Freeman, P. K. 2000. Infrastructure, natural disasters, and poverty. In Managing disaster risk
in emerging economies, eds. A. Kreimer and M. Arnold, 55-61. Washington, D.C.:
World Bank.
Garenne, M. 1995. Sex differences in measles mortality: a world review. International
Journal of Epidemiology 23 (3):632-642.
36
Greenough, P.R. 1982. Prosperity and misery in modern Bengal – the famine of 1943-1944.
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Guha-Sapir, D., and R. Below. 2002. The quality and accuracy of disaster data: a
comparative analysis of three global data sets. Brussels: WHO Centre for Research on
the Epidemiology of Disasters.
Haider, R., A. A. Rahman, and S. Huq. 1993. Cyclone ’91. An environmental and
perceptional study. Dhaka: Bangladesh Centre for Advanced Studies.
Hausken, K. 1995. The dynamics of within-group and between-group interaction. Journal of
Mathematical Economics 24 (7): 655-687.
Hewitt, K. (ed.) 1983. Interpretations of calamity from the viewpoint of human ecology.
Boston: Allen and Unwin.
Hines, J. R., and C. R. Jaramillo. 2005. The impact of large natural disasters on national
economies. Working Paper. University of Michigan: Department of Economics.
Ikeda, K. 1995. Gender differences in human loss and vulnerability in natural disasters: a
case study from Bangladesh. Indian Journal of Gender Studies 2 (2):171-193.
Kahn, M. E. 2005. The death toll from natural disasters: the role of income, geography, and
institutions. Review of Economics and Statistics 87 (2): 271-284.
Kidane, A. 1989. Demographic consequences of the 1984-1985 Ethiopian famine.
Demography 26 (3):515-522.
Kidane, A. 1990. Mortality estimates of the 1984-85 Ethiopian famine. Scandinavian Journal
of Social Medicine 18 (4):281-286.
Kramer R.M. 1990. The effect of resource scarcity on group conflict and cooperation.
Advances in Group Processes 7: 151-177.
Krishnaraj, M. 1997. Gender issues in disaster management: the Latur earthquake. Gender,
Technology and Development 1 (3):395-411.
37
Macintyre, K. 2002. Famine and the female mortality advantage. In Famine Demography –
Perspectives from the Past and Present, eds. T. Dyson and Cormac Ó Gráda, 240-259.
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Mariam, M. W. 1986. Rural vulnerability to famine in Ethiopia – 1958-1977. London:
Intermediate Technology Publications.
Mathers, C. D., R. Sadana, J. A. Salomon, C. L. J. Murray, and A. D. Lopez. 2001. Healthy
Life Expectancy in 191 Countries, 1999. The Lancet 357(9269):1685-91.
McKee, M. 1999. Alcohol in Russia. Alcohol and Alcoholism 34(6):824-29.
Mustafa, D. 2002. To each according to his power? Participation, access, and vulnerability in
irrigation and flood management in Pakistan. Environment and Planning D 20:737-752.
Noji, E.K. 1997b. Earthquakes. In The Public Health Consequences of Disasters. ed. E. K.
Noji, 135-178. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Noji. E. K. 1997a. The nature of disaster: general characteristics and public health effects. In
The Public Health Consequences of Disasters. ed. E. K. Noji, 3-20. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
O’Hare, G. 2001. Hurricane 07B in the Godavari Delta, Andhra Pradesh, India: vulnerability,
mitigation and the spatial impact. Geographical Journal 167 (1):23-38.
O’Keefe, P., K. Westgate, and B. Wisner. 1976. Taking the naturalness out of natural
disasters. Nature 260: 566-567.
Owens, I. P. F. 2002. Sex differences in mortality rate. Science 297 (20 September):2008-
2009.
Oxfam International. 2005. The tsunami’s impact on women. Briefing Note March 2005.
Oxford: Oxfam International.
Pan-American Health Organization. 2002. Gender and natural disasters. Washington D.C.:
Pan-American Health Organization.
38
Phuong, C. 2004. The international protection of internally displaced persons. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Platt, R. H. 1999. Disasters and democracy. Washington, DC: Island Press.
Plümper, T. and E. Neumayer 2006. The Unequal Burden of War. The Effect of Armed
Conflict on the Gender Gap in Life Expectancy. International Organization 60 (3): 723-
754.
Plümper, T., V.E. Troeger and P. Manow. 2005. Panel Data Analysis in Comparative Politics.
Linking Method to Theory. European Journal of Political Research 44 (2): 327-354.
Quarantelli, E. L. 2001. Statistical and conceptual problems in the study of disasters. Disaster
Prevention and Management 10 (5): 325-338.
Rivers, J. P. W. 1982. Women and children last: an essay on sex discrimination in disasters.
Disasters 6 (4):256-267.
Schwoebel, M. H., and G. Menon. 2004. Mainstreaming gender in disaster management
support project. Washington D.C.: Center for Development and Population Activities.
Seager, J. 2005. Natural disasters expose gender divides. Chicago Tribune 14 September.
Seager, J. 2006. Noticing gender (or not) in disasters. Geoforum 37 (1):2-3.
Sen, A.K. 1988. Family and food: sex bias in poverty. In Rural poverty in South Asia, eds.
T.N. Srinivasan and P.K. Bardhan, 453-472. New York: Columbia University Press.
Silventoinen, K., E. Lahelma, and O. Rahonen. 2000. Relative Effect of Genetic and
Environmental Factors on Body Height: Differences across Birth Cohorts among Finnish
Men and Women. American Journal of Public Health 90 (4): 627-630.
Toole, M. J. 1997a. Communicable diseases and disease control. In The Public Health
Consequences of Disasters. ed. E. K. Noji, 3-20. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Toole, M.J. 1997b. Displaced Persons and War. In War and Public Health, ed. Berry S. Levy
and Victor W. Sidel, 197-211. New York: Oxford University Press.
39
UN Population Division. 1988. Sex differentials in survivorship in the developing world:
levels, regional patterns and demographic determinants. Population Bulletin of the
United Nations 25:51-64.
UNEP. 2004. GEO yearbook 2004/5. Nairobi: United Nations Environment Programme.
US Census Bureau. 2004. Global Population Profile 2002, Washington D.C.
Varley, A. 1994. The exceptional and the everyday: vulnerability analysis in the international
decade for natural disaster reduction. In Disasters, Development and Environment, ed. A.
Varley, 2-11. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.
WHO. 2002. Gender and health in disasters. Geneva: World Health Organization.
Wisner, B. 2000a. From ‘acts of God’ to ‘water wars’. In Floods, ed. D.J. Parker, 89-99.
London: Routledge.
Wisner, B. 2000b. The political economy of hazards: more limits to growth? Environmental
Hazards 20: 59-61.
Wisner, B., P. Blaikie, T. Cannon, and I. Davis. 1994 and 2004. At risk: natural hazards,
people’s vulnerability and disasters. First and second edition. London: Routledge.
Wooldridge, J.M. 2002. Econometric analysis of cross section and panel data. Cambridge:
MIT Press.
World Bank. 2004. World Development Indicators on CD-Rom. Wahsington: World Bank.
Yang, D. 2005. Coping with disaster: the impact of hurricanes on international financial
flows, 1970-2001. Working Paper. University of Michigan: Department of Economics.
40
Table 1. Summary Statistics on Natural Disasters in Sample.
Disaster type Total no. of events
Total no. of deaths
Total no. of people affected
Drought 240
556,687
1,388,252,544
Earthquake 350
107,050
52,661,238
Epidemic 317
105,678
13,346,403
Extreme temp 108
16,897
6,120,497
Famine 36
11,524
57,332,711
Flood 938
119,707
1,731,081,382
Insect infestations
42
0
2,200
Landslide 182
14,228
1,122,215
Volcano 48
25,053
2,501,368
Waves/surges 12
2,724
12,919
Wild Fire 103
624
3,523,398
Wind Storm 1,121
87,029
340,100,574
41
Table 2. Natural Disasters and the Change in the Gender Gap in Life Expectancy.
model 1 model 2
change in population life expectancy 0.028 0.028
(0.009)** (0.009)**
disaster deaths per thousand people -0.732 -0.729
(0.081)** (0.081)**
women’s socio-economic rights -0.049 -0.045
(0.030) (0.030)
disaster deaths * women’s socio-economic rights 0.365 0.365
(0.052)** (0.052)**
per capita income -0.000
(0.000)
political stability -0.005
(0.004)
level of political freedom -0.072
(0.021)**
Number of observations 2266 2241
Number of countries 141 141
R² 0.29 0.28
Wald chi-square test 190.1** 206.3**
Notes: Estimations include regional dummy variables and constant (coefficients not reported)
**=p<0.01 (two-sided z-test), standard errors in brackets
42
Table 3. Extended estimation results.
model 1 model 3 model 4 model 5 model 6
change in population life expectancy 0.028 0.030 0.017 0.036 0.026
(0.009)** (0.011)** (0.033) (0.010)** (0.009)**
disaster deaths per thousand people -0.732 -0.743 -0.377 -1.005 -0.772
(0.081)** (0.095)** (0.123)** (0.692) (0.083)**
women’s socio-economic rights -0.049 -0.050 -0.135 -0.028 -0.045
(0.030) (0.039) (0.066)* (0.030) (0.030)
disaster deaths * women’s socio-economic rights 0.365 0.385 0.178 0.326 0.431
(0.052)** (0.065)** (0.075)* (0.241) (0.060)**
Number of observations 2266 1491 894 2266 2266
Number of countries 141 117 121 141 141
R² 0.29 0.28 0.29 0.29 0.29
Wald chi-square test 190.1** 146.9** 77.4** 108.9** 193.5**
Notes: Estimations include regional dummy variables and constant (coefficients not reported) **=p<0.01 *=p<0.05 (two-sided z-test), standard
errors in brackets
43
Table 4. Estimation results with bootstrapped standard errors.
observed
bias
std. error
confidence
interval
beta coefficients
disaster deaths per -0.7448
0.1342
0.6685
-2.0565
0.5670
(N)
thousand people -1.7311
0.3787
(P)
-1.7653
0.3511
(BC)
women’s economic rights -0.0484
-0.0025
0.0390
-0.1248
0.0281
(N)
-0.1319
0.0214
(P)
-0.1291
0.0215
(BC)
disaster deaths * 0.3615
-0.0730
0.3250
-0.2763
0.9993
(N)
women’s economic rights -0.1830
0.8356
(P)
-0.1595
0.8702
(BC)
standard errors
disaster deaths per 0.0762
0.0232
0.0764
-0.0737
0.2261
(N)
thousand people 0.0486
0.3538
(P)
0.0490
0.3634
(BC)
women’s economic rights 0.0284
-0.0011
0.0029
0.0227
0.0340
(N)
0.0215
0.0332
(P)
0.0236
0.0351
(BC)
disaster deaths * 0.0488
0.0119
0.0315
-0.0130
0.1105
(N)
women’s economic rights 0.0341
0.1623
(P)
0.0310
0.1176
(BC)
N = normal P = percentile BC = bias corrected
44
Appendix. Coding scheme for Cingranelli and Richards’ (2004) women’s rights
measures
The measure of economic rights covers the following:
− Equal pay for equal work
− Free choice of profession or employment without the need to obtain a husband or male
relative's consent
− The right to gainful employment without the need to obtain a husband or male relative's
consent
− Equality in hiring and promotion practices
− Job security (maternity leave, unemployment benefits, no arbitrary firing or layoffs, etc.)
− Non-discrimination by employers
− The right to be free from sexual harassment in the workplace
− The right to work at night
− The right to work in occupations classified as dangerous
− The right to work in the military and the police force
The measure of social rights covers the following:
− The right to equal inheritance
− The right to enter into marriage on a basis of equality with men
− The right to travel abroad
− The right to obtain a passport
− The right to confer citizenship to children or a husband
− The right to initiate a divorce
− The right to own, acquire, manage, and retain property brought into marriage
45
− The right to participate in social, cultural, and community activities
− The right to an education
− The freedom to choose a residence/domicile
− Freedom from female genital mutilation (FGM) of children and of adults without their
− consent
−
Freedom from forced sterilization
The coding of the variables is as follows:
(0) There are no economic (social) rights for women under law and systematic discrimination
based on sex may be built into the law. The government tolerates a high level of
discrimination against women.
(1) There are some economic (social) rights for women under law. However, in practice, the
government does not enforce the laws effectively or enforcement of laws is weak. The
government tolerates a moderate level of discrimination against women.
(2) There are some economic (social) rights for women under law. In practice, the
government DOES enforce these laws effectively. However, the government still tolerates a
low level of discrimination against women.
(3) All or nearly all of women's economic (social) rights are guaranteed by law. In practice,
the government fully and vigorously enforces these laws. The government tolerates none or
almost no discrimination against women.