ArticlePDF Available

‘Now, he will be the leader of the house’: An equine intervention with at-risk Guatemalan youth

Taylor & Francis
International Journal of Adolescence and Youth
Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Equine-facilitated interventions have shown promise for facilitating emotional and behavioural changes in diverse groups. The current study evaluated the effectiveness of an equine workshop for vulnerable Guatemalan youth using a mixed-method approach. The 37 participants (Mage = 18.22, SD = 2.25, 14 girls) came from difficult circumstances including poverty and other risks. Using a wait-list control group design with random assignment, the effects of a 2-day equine-based workshop were evaluated. Participants completed quantitative measures of leadership, emotion regulation, aggression, and interpersonal response to threat. Mentors completed reports of aggression and prosocial behaviour. Self-reported leadership increased significantly in the group receiving the intervention; mentor reports of aggression revealed significant decreases. Focus groups with participants and family members reported multiple benefits, including improved emotion regulation. Equine-based interventions may provide at-risk Guatemalan youth with tools of leadership, reduced violence and aggression, and better emotion regulation.
Content may be subject to copyright.
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF ADOLESCENCE AND YOUTH, 2017
VOL. 22, NO. 4, 390404
https://doi.org/10.1080/02673843.2016.1202844
‘Now, he will be the leader of the house’: An equine intervention
with at-risk Guatemalan youth
Judith L. Gibbonsa, Catherine A. Cunninghamb, Leslie Paizb, Katelyn E. Poelkera and
Aracely Chajónc
aDepartment of Psychology, Saint Louis University, St. Louis, MO, USA; bLead-Up International, Jocotenango,
Guatemala; cFundación Educando a los Niños, Jocotenango, Guatemala
ABSTRACT
Equine-facilitated interventions have shown promise for facilitating emotional
and behavioural changes in diverse groups. The current study evaluated the
eectiveness of an equine workshop for vulnerable Guatemalan youth using
a mixed-method approach. The 37 participants (Mage = 18.22, SD= 2.25, 14
girls) came from dicult circumstances including poverty and other risks.
Using a wait-list control group design with random assignment, the eects
of a 2-day equine-based workshop were evaluated. Participants completed
quantitative measures of leadership, emotion regulation, aggression, and
interpersonal response to threat. Mentors completed reports of aggression
and prosocial behaviour. Self-reported leadership increased signicantly in
the group receiving the intervention; mentor reports of aggression revealed
signicant decreases. Focus groups with participants and family members
reported multiple benets, including improved emotion regulation. Equine-
based interventions may provide at-risk Guatemalan youth with tools of
leadership, reduced violence and aggression, and better emotion regulation.
Characterized as Latin America’s youngest nation due to its high percentage of youth, Guatemala is
a country in which issues of young people should be at the forefront (Population Reference Bureau,
2011). Of the country’s nearly 15 million people, nearly 60% are aged 25 or younger (Central Intelligence
Agency (CIA), 2016). Although they comprise a large percentage of the population, many Guatemalan
youth live in dicult circumstances. Poverty represents a signicant challenge to their well-being with
two-thirds of the country’s urban population living in slums (United Nations International Children’s
Emergency Fund [UNICEF, 2008]). Categorized by the World Bank (2016) as a lower middle-income
country, over 50% of the population lives below the poverty line. Many problems like malnutrition
(Guatemala has the fourth highest rate in the world) are exacerbated in rural areas and are more severe
for women and girls (World Food Programme (WFP), 2016).
Beyond poverty, physical and sexual violence are major risks for Guatemala’s young people (UNICEF,
2014b). When asked if they had been robbed in the last month, for example, 36% of Guatemalan
sixth graders said yes (UNICEF, 2014a). For victims from birth to 19years, the country has the second
highest homicide rate in the world. Furthermore, homicide is the leading cause of death among the
country’s adolescent boys (UNICEF, 2014a). In addition, approximately 25% of Guatemalan adolescent
ARTICLE HISTORY
Received 18 May 2016
Accepted 14 June 2016
KEYWORDS
At-risk youth; equine
interventions; Guatemala;
leadership
© 2016 The Author(s). Published by Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group.
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial License (http://creativecommons.
org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/), which permits unrestricted non-commercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work
is properly cited.
CONTACT Judith L. Gibbons gibbonsjl@slu.edu
* A preliminary version of these results was presented at the 45th annual meeting of the Society for Cross-Cultural Research in Portland,
OR in February, 2016.
OPEN ACCESS
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF ADOLESCENCE AND YOUTH 391
girls reported that they were physically mistreated prior to age 15 (Instituto Nacional de Estadística,
2014). The rates of sexual violence are also alarming. Nineteen per cent of adolescent girls aged 15–19
reported forced sexual intercourse and/or other involuntary sexual behaviours by their fathers alone
(UNICEF, 2014a).
Investments in adolescents can serve to combat the cycle of poverty, reduce levels of violence,
promote future societal development, and increase a nations ability to thrive (UNICEF, 2011; Wuermli,
Tubbs, Petersen, & Aber, 2015). Despite attempts to intervene with Guatemalan youth who are at risk,
there are few evidence-based programmes that foster their development and reduce their likelihood
of being perpetrators or victims of violence (Oce of Inspector General, 2014). The purpose of the
present study was to empirically investigate the eectiveness of an intervention with at-risk Guatemalan
youth, evaluating whether an equine-based programme fostered leadership and positive interpersonal
communication and decreased aggressive attitudes among Guatemalan youth.
Programmes that involve interactions with horses are used widely for addressing physical disabili-
ties such as cerebral palsy (Tseng, Chen, & Tam, 2013) and ameliorating psychological and behavioural
problems such as substance abuse, schizophrenia, mood and stress disorders, developmental disor-
ders, autism spectrum disorder, insecure attachment, depression, and posttraumatic stress disorder
(Beetz, Winkler, Julius, Uvnas-Moberg, & Kotrschal, 2015; Esbjorn, 2006; McConnell, 2010). In addition,
equine-based interventions have been proposed to improve leadership skills and foster self-awareness,
perceived social support, emotional intelligence, and well-being in individuals without diagnosed psy-
chological disorders (Adams, 2013; Chappell, 2014; Hauge, Kvalem, Berget, Enders-Slegers, & Braastad,
2014; Meola, 2016; Pendry & Roeter, 2013).
Although other animals show benets for enhancing well-being (McCardle, McCune, Grin,
Maholmes, & Freund, 2011), horses have unique qualities that make them especially well suited for
authentic interactions with humans. First, horses are extremely sensitive to human emotion and emo-
tional expression. In a recent study, horses discriminated between photographs of an angry and a happy
human face (Smith, Proops, Grounds, Wathan, & McComb, 2016). In another study, individuals who were
either leading or riding horses were told that in the next lap, an umbrella would be opened as the pair
passed; this was potentially frightening to horses (Keeling, Jonare, & Lanneborn, 2009). Although the
umbrella was not opened, the heart rates of both the horses and their human companions increased
as they passed the indicated spot. In a third study, the behaviour and heart rate of horses changed in
response to stationary humans who were either calm, afraid, or physically stressed by recent exercise
(Merkies et al., 2014). Those three studies demonstrate the extraordinary sensitivity of horses to human
feelings and emotions. Thus, the horse can serve to mirror feelings of the people surrounding them.
Secondly, by nature horses are both herd and prey animals; these qualities predispose them not only
to being sensitive to nonverbal behaviour, but to look for and expect leadership from others (Esbjorn,
2006; Roberts, 2002a, 2002b). Third, the size of horses means that they cannot readily be forced to act;
humans must use other means to elicit their cooperation and desired behaviours. Learning to direct
the behaviour of such a large animal can potentially decrease fearfulness and increase self-condence.
The authors of the many review articles evaluating the literature on equine-facilitated interventions
generally conclude that the programmes show potential, but that additional and better controlled
studies are needed (All, Loving, & Crane, 1999; Anestis, Anestis, Zawilinski, Hopkins, & Lilienfeld, 2014;
Bachi, 2012; Bates, 2002; Beebe, 2008; Brandt, 2013; Cantin & Marshall-Lucette, 2011; Frewin & Gardiner,
2005; Kendall, Maujean, Pepping, & Wright, 2014; Kruger, Trachtenberg, & Serpell, 2004; Lee, Dakin, &
McLure, 2015; Lentini & Knox, 2009, 2015; MacKinnon et al., 1995; May, Seivert, Cano, Casey, & Johnson,
2016; Notgrass, 2011; Notgrass & Petinelli, 2015; Selby & Smith-Osborne, 2013). A critical review pub-
lished in 2014 summarized the state of the art of equine interventions as lacking empirical support
and theoretical basis, with most studies exhibiting multiple threats to validity (Anestis et al., 2014). The
authors argued that equine-assisted interventions should not be oered until better research supports
their use. Since that time, additional, better controlled studies are available (Lentini & Knox, 2015). Here
we focus on the recent and well-controlled research on the eectiveness of equine interventions for
children and youth.
392 J. L. GIBBONS ET AL.
Equine-facilitated psychotherapy for children and youth varies in duration, the nature of the activ-
ities involved, and reported outcomes (Lentini & Knox, 2015). Of 47 studies published between 2008
and 2014, including a total of 672 treatment participants, the typical intervention was a 1-h session per
week for 12weeks. Only four studies met the rigorous scientic criteria of (1) including more than 10
participants, (2) implementing random-assignment to treatment and control groups, and (3) utilizing
standardized outcome measures. Two of those measured behavioural eects of an equine intervention.
Bass and colleagues found that 19 children with autism spectrum disorder, compared to a wait-list
control group showed more social motivation, as well as cognitive and sensory changes following
a therapeutic riding programme (Bass, Duchowny, & Llabre, 2009). Forty-nine typically developing
Norwegian adolescents reported signicant increases in perceived social support following a 4-month
mounted and unmounted equine programme (Hauge et al., 2014). Ten additional studies with fewer
participants and/or controls showed positive changes for at-risk adolescents (Bachi, Terkel, & Teichman,
2012; Burgon, 2011; Chaplin, 2010; Dell et al., 2011; Ecken, 2012; Maujean, Kendall, Lillan, Sharp, & Pringle,
2013; Meek, 2012; Trotter, Chandler, Goodwin-Bond, & Casey, 2008). Of those studies, the largest with
a sample of 164 child and adolescent participants, demonstrated changes in 17 behaviours, including
externalizing, internalizing, maladaptive, and adaptive behaviours (Trotter et al., 2008).
Since the studies reviewed by Lentini and Knox (2015), there has been additional research on equine
interventions with youth. A 2015 study (Frederick, Hatz, & Lanning, 2015) randomly assigned 26 at-risk
adolescents to a nonmounted treatment group (14 participants) and control group (12 participants).
Following the 5-week programme the treatment group showed lower levels of depression and higher
levels of hope. In addition, less well-controlled equine interventions have demonstrated more secure
attachment (Balluerka, Muela, Amiano, & Caldentey, 2014), less endorsement of aggressive behaviour
(Gibbons, Cunningham, Paiz, Poelker, & Montufar Cardenas, 2015), greater serenity and condence
(Hemingway, Meek, & Hill, 2015), more self-determination (Grimm, 2015), and fewer symptoms of PTSD
(McCullough, Risley-Curtiss, & Rorke, 2015) among youth.
Leadership abilities have also been fostered by equine interventions. Although the evidence is limited
and the research uncontrolled or poorly controlled, there are many accounts of the value of equine-
assisted interventions in increasing leadership abilities (Adams, 2013; Chappell, 2014; Dyk et al., 2013;
Grootveld, 2015; Meola, 2016; Pohl, 2015; West, 2015). Overall, those researchers argue that through
interacting with horses, individuals can gain self-condence and improve their emotional intelligence,
especially emotion regulation. Because those qualities are essential for eective, authentic leadership,
the equine-based programmes provide valuable experiences (e.g. Adams, 2013).
All equine-based programmes rely on experiential learning including the assumption that learning
is a continuous process grounded in transactions between the person and the environment (Kolb,
2015). Typically, in equine-based programmes, participants must solve problems in real time, such as
how to elicit behaviours in horses without the use of touch or force. Beyond those basic principles pro-
grammes vary greatly, and can include ground work, riding, and vaulting. Some inventions, especially
those aimed at producing empowered leaders use a Join-Up® exercise, developed by Monty Roberts
(About Join-Up®, 2016; Roberts, 2002a, 2002b). During the exercise that occurs in a round pen, a horse
is encouraged to choose a person as the leader, show signs of respect, and begin to follow that person.
Although few researchers use the Join-Up® designation because of its proprietary nature, many make
reference to similar exercises in the round pen (e.g. Adams, 2013; Burgon, 2011; Chappell, 2014; Dell et
al., 2011; Dyk et al., 2013; Esbjorn, 2006; Kelly, 2014; Meola, 2016; Pohl, 2015).
In a previous community study in Guatemala, the Join-Up® experience served as the centrepiece
of a 4-week equine-assisted programme, designed to reduce attitudes towards violence (Gibbons et
al., 2015). Not only did the attitudes of the participants change in the expected direction, but their
female relatives reported calmer, less aggressive behaviour and their horses were less reactive to the
participants’ approach. Although 10 of the 18 participants were adolescents, the programme was not
aimed specically at youth. The present study was designed as a follow-up to that study. First, would
the workshop be eective if delivered in two whole-day workshops and a feedback session, rather than
spread over 4weeks? Second, would it be useful for at-risk youth who, unlike the previous community
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF ADOLESCENCE AND YOUTH 393
members, had little or no experience with horses? Would the programme improve leadership skills?
Was emotion regulation a key mechanism of change? We designed the current study to address those
key issues.
Method
Participants
The original sample in the current study included 40 youth; however, because three of the participants
did not participate beyond the rst pre-test, they were dropped from the study. Thirty-seven adoles-
cents and young adults (Mage = 18.22, SD=2.25, range=15–23, 14 girls) were included in the analysis.
The average household size (including the participant) was 9.24 people (range of 2–20). Of the 32
participants reporting ethnicity, 28 identied as Ladino (mixed Indigenous and European descent) and
four identied as Indigenous. Participants were either current students (n=29) or recent graduates
(n=8) of a school in Jocotenango, Guatemala that serves economically disadvantaged youth. Families
must demonstrate severe economic need for enrolment. The school educates children in kindergarten
through middle school. The graduates who participated in this study were still given some nancial and
social support from the school while they completed their secondary education elsewhere. Participants
were chosen for this study based on the recommendation of the school psychologist. She selected
students who were coming from particularly dire circumstances (e.g. abuse and violence at home).
Materials
Data were collected from three sources: participants, participant mentors at the school, and parents.
Participants completed the self-report measures described below at three time points. Some also par-
ticipated in focus groups at the end of the study once the experimental group and wait-list controls
had completed the horse workshops. Mentors completed behavioural reports of their mentees also
detailed below. Parents participated in focus groups after the nal post-test.
Participant questionnaires
The measures, all originally developed in English, were translated to Spanish by the third author bilingual
in Spanish and English. Another native Spanish speaker, also uent in English, veried the translations
and minor discrepancies were reconciled via discussion. Because youth participants may not have had
much exposure to Likert-style scales, slight modications were made by including illustrations with
scale points to indicate the magnitude of agreement/disagreement. The range in Cronbach’s alpha
values for each questionnaire reects the internal consistency reliability estimates across the three
testing administrations.
Youth Leadership Life Skills Development (YLLSD; Ricochet Leadership Skills Survey, n.d.; Smith, Gentry,
& Ketrig, 2005; adapted from Seevers, Dormody, & Clason, 1995). The YLLSD is a 30-item self-report
measure of leadership ability (see Ricochet Leadership Skills Survey, n.d.; Smith et al., 2005). Participants
responded to each item on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (no ability) to 4 (a lot of ability). Circles
of various sizes were presented along with the scale anchors to illustrate the magnitude of agreement/
disagreement. Sample items included ‘I feel I can set realistic goals’ and ‘I feel I can listen eectively’. In
the current study, alphas were excellent ranging from .92 to .96.
Transgression-Related Interpersonal Motivations Inventory (TRIM; McCullough et al., 1998). The TRIM
is a 12-item scale that assesses reactions to interpersonal transgressions. Responses are rated on a
5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Response options were
illustrated as follows: NO/no/maybe/yes/YES (see Chaplin & John, 2010). Higher scores indicate higher
tendency for retaliation and ill will for the transgressor. Before answering the items, participants were
394 J. L. GIBBONS ET AL.
prompted to think of someone who recently hurt them and answered the questions with the person in
mind. Sample items included ‘I cut o the relationship with him/her’ (avoidance) and ‘I am going to get
even’ (revenge). Items are divided into two subscales: Avoidance (α=.85–.91) and Revenge (α=.86–.90).
Normative Beliefs about Aggression Scale (NBAS; Huesmann & Guerra, 1997). The NBAS is a 20-item
measure that addresses approval of physical and verbal aggression in youth. For nine items, the scale
ranged from 1 (it’s perfectly okay) to 4 (it’s really wrong) and for the remainder, the scale was reversed
and lower scores indicated disagreement (i.e. lower acceptance of aggressive responses). For analysis,
the 11 items were reversed such that higher scores indicated greater acceptance of aggression. Smiling
or frowning faces that varied in size to illustrate magnitude of agreement/disagreement accompanied
scale points. Participants were given eight brief scenarios (e.g. ‘Suppose a boy says something bad to
a girl’ and ‘Suppose a girl hits another girl, Maria’) and answered questions about the acceptability of
responses to those situations. A question posed in response to the rst example is ‘Do you think it’s
wrong for the girl to scream at home’ and in response to the second, ‘Do you think it is wrong for Maria
to hit her back?’ The remaining seven questions addressed more general views on aggression (e.g. ‘In
general, it is okay to yell at others and say bad things’.) Alphas ranged from .69 to .85.
Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ; Gross & John, 2003). The ERQ is a 10-item scale of emotion
regulation with two subscales. The Reappraisal subscale includes six items (e.g. ‘I control my emotions
by changing the way I think about the situation I’m in’) and the Suppression subscale includes four (e.g. ‘I
control my emotions by not expressing them’). All items are rated on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from
1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). Participants were presented with these response options: NO/
NO/no/maybe/yes/YES/YES (Chaplin & John, 2010). Higher scores indicate greater emotion regulation.
Alphas for the Reappraisal subscale ranged from .64 to .81 and from .22 to .60 for the Suppression
subscale.
Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire – Blaming Others (CERQ; Garnefski, Kraaij, & Spinhoven,
2001). Participants also completed the Blaming Others subscale of the CERQ. This subscale consists
of four items and participants responded using same 7-item Likert scale that was used for the ERQ
(Gross & John, 2003). These items emphasized that others were often responsible when something bad
happens. For example, ‘When something bad happens to me, I think about the mistakes others have
made in this matter’. Cronbach’s alphas were good and ranged from .79 to .84.
Mentor evaluations
Mentors rated prosocial and aggressive behaviours of the participants at three time points.
Child Behaviour Checklist (CBS; Ladd & Prolet, 1996). Participant mentors completed the Aggressive
(seven items, e.g. ‘taunts, teases’) and Prosocial behaviours (seven items, e.g. ‘cooperative with peers’)
subscales of the CBS (Ladd & Prolet, 1996). Although originally developed for use with younger
children, this measure has been used to evaluate the behaviour of adolescents (see Kataoka & Vandell,
2013). Mentors responded on a 3-point scale ranging from 1 (doesn’t applystudent seldom displays the
behaviour) to 3 (certainly appliesstudent often displays the behaviour). Alphas for the prosocial items
ranged from .85 to .92 and from .79 to .90 for the aggressive items.
Demographic questionnaire
In addition to basic demographic information (e.g. age, ethnicity), participants also provided a proxy
for socio-economic status by indicating whether or not their household owned or had access to certain
objects (e.g. stove, book shelf with books) and amenities (e.g. electricity, hot water).
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF ADOLESCENCE AND YOUTH 395
Participant focus groups
Seventeen of the youth participants attended one of two focus groups 2weeks after the post-test
assessment at Time 3. They were asked to describe what they had learned from the workshop and how
they have applied that knowledge to their lives.
Focus groups with family
Eighteen relatives participated in one of two focus groups after the Time 3 post-test. Participants
included two fathers, three sisters, and 13 mothers. Family members were asked to describe changes
in their relative’s behaviour since participating in the workshop. Specically, they were asked if they
had noticed any changes in how the participant was behaving with family, friends and/or classmates.
Coding
Focus groups were audio recorded and later transcribed. Responses were coded using thematic anal-
ysis (Braun & Clarke, 2006). Each of the authors rst read the focus group transcripts independently
in Spanish. The team then came together and developed codes related to the youth and family focus
groups. Once the codes were created, each team member read through the focus group transcripts
again, organizing the responses into the established codes. Any disagreements were resolved through
discussion. Then the ve team members grouped the lower level codes into higher level themes.
Workshop curriculum
The workshops centred on a Join-Up® exercise developed by natural horsemanship trainer Monty
Roberts (About Join-Up®, 2016; Roberts, 2002a, 2002b). The Join-Up® experience is designed to foster
trust and leadership between the equine and his human handler (here, the youth participants) through
non-violent communication (About Join-Up®, 2016; Roberts, 2002a, 2002b). By utilizing the horse’s natu-
ral herd instinct and sensitivity to human nonverbal behaviour, the Join-Up® experience fosters trust and
respect between the horse and handler. This condence then allows the handler to eectively handle
the horse without the use of violence or physical force and a human–equine partnership is formed. In
the end, the equine and the handler form a partnership, with the human emerging as the horse’s leader.
The 2-day workshops included PowerPoint and video presentations as well as question-
and-answer sessions between the participants and workshop facilitators. After observing a Join-Up®,
participants performed a mock-join up with a person substituting for a horse, followed by a Join-Up®
with a horse. In addition, youth participants groomed horses and led them around obstacles. Participants
also engaged in a breathing exercise; in close contact with a horse, they concentrated on breathing
and self-awareness for 20–30min, recording the horse’s hear t rate before and after the contact. A short
session 2weeks later included participants’ feedback on how the new skills had been applied to their
lives and a role-playing exercise.
Procedure
This study utilized a wait-list control design. Participants were randomly assigned to one of two groups:
experimental (n=18) or wait-list control (n=19). The experimental group received the 2-day workshop
rst, followed by the wait-list control group approximately one week later. For each group, there were
approximately ve days between Day 1 and Day 2 of the workshop. Data were collected at three time
points from participants and mentors. Time 1 was the pre-test for both groups. Time 2 was the rst
post-test for the experimental group and the second pre-test for the wait-list controls. Time 3 served as
the second post-test for the experimental group and the sole post-test for the wait-list controls. There
were 4weeks between testing at Time 1 and Testing at Time 2 and 2weeks between Times 2 and 3. The
pre-testing at Time 1 occurred several weeks prior to the experimental group receiving the workshop.
The experimental group received the workshop between Time 1 and Time 2, while the wait-list control
396 J. L. GIBBONS ET AL.
received the workshop between Time 2 and Time 3. Focus groups with the family members and with
the participants themselves occurred following the post-test at Time 3, and the feedback session.
Prior to the start of data collection, written parental consent was obtained for youth participants
under age 18. Adolescent assent was also sought in those cases. For youth participants over age 18, for
parents, and for teachers, written consent was obtained at the start of the study. Participants completed
their questionnaire packets in a classroom of the school. Each of the focus groups lasted approximately
two hours.
Results
The mixed-method approach resulted in quantitative and qualitative data from three sources. We have
both types of data from the workshop participants, quantitative data from mentors, and qualitative
data from participants’ relatives. The quantitative analyses are presented rst, followed by the coding
analyses of the focus groups.
To assess the eectiveness of the workshop, a series of one-way analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs)
with condition (experimental vs. wait-list control group) as the between-subjects independent variable
were run to compare experimental group participants with wait-list control participants at Time 2 on
the measures described above. Time 1 scores on those same measures served as the covariate. Recall
that at Time 2 the experimental group had received the programme while the wait-list control group
had not. Adjusted means, after taking the covariate into account, are presented for signicant analyses
only. When the ANCOVA was signicant, follow-up paired-samples t-tests compared experimental group
participants only at Time 2 and Time 3 to test whether the eects held for approximately two weeks
between Time 2 and Time 3 assessments.
Participants’ quantitative self-reports
Participants’ self-reported leadership ability was signicantly higher for the experimental group
(M= 103.85) at Time 2 compared to the wait-list control group (M=94.61), F(1, 34) = 6.39, p= .016,
partial η2=.156. The paired-samples t-test indicated that those in the experimental group maintained
those advances in leadership from Time 2 to Time 3 because there was no signicant dierence between
the two time points, t(17) = −.67, p>.05. See Figure 1.
Dierences in TRIM scores between the experimental and wait-list control groups were not signi-
cant at Time 2, F(1, 34) = 1.35, p>.05. Self-reported attitudes towards aggression on the NBAS did not
reveal a signicant between-condition dierence at Time 2, F(1, 34) = .15, p> .05. See Figure 2. With
respect to emotion regulation, only the Repression subscale of the ERQ and the Blaming Others CERQ
were analysed due to the poor alpha of the ERQ Suppression subscale. Neither measure of emotion
80
85
90
95
100
105
110
Time 1 Time 2 Time 3
Total Leadership Score
Experimental
Wait-List Control
Figure 1.Participants’ self-reported leadership ability across time points.
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF ADOLESCENCE AND YOUTH 397
regulation revealed signicant dierences between conditions at Time 2, F(1, 34) = .56, p>.05 (ERQ
Repression) and F(1, 34) = 2.50, p>.05 (CERQ Blaming Others).
Mentors’ reports
Mentor reports of aggressive behaviour as measured by the aggression subscale of the ABS indicated
that there were signicant dierences between groups at Time 2 for aggressive behaviour, F(1, 34) = 4.82,
p<.035, partial η2=.12. Means indicated that experimental group participants (M=1.26) were lower
on aggression at Time 2 than wait-list control group participants (M=1.43). A paired-samples t-test
suggested that experimental group participants retained those lower aggression scores from Time
2 to Time 3 as the analysis was not signicant, t(17) = .74, p>.05. The dierence between groups on
prosocial behaviour at Time 2 was not signicant, F(1, 34) = .92, p>.05
Participant focus groups
Three central themes emerged from participant focus groups: Emotion Regulation, Empowerment, and
Positive Emotion. All themes were guided by the overarching idea that participants learned valuable
lessons from the workshop. See Figure 3. With respect to Emotion Regulation, participants spoke often
about applying the breathing exercises during conicts or challenges as a tool to remain calm. They
also described how participation in the workshop helped them to be less angry and to ght less with
others. For example, a boy indicated that, ‘Before, I used to beat my brothers because they used to tease
me but now I’ve calmed down a lot and I just ask them not to bother me’.
Participants spoke often of Empowerment that encouraged them to assume leadership roles at home
and to share what they learned in the workshop with other family members. When talking about the
breathing exercises, one girl oered, … so, I taught my mother the breathing technique’. Furthermore,
this sense of empowerment promoted better interpersonal interactions and encouraged future per-
sonal growth. In one case, a boy explained, ‘I can grow more as a person. To have condence in myself
is what will help me to succeed’. Similarly, when discussing Positive Emotions, youth mentioned how
they were happier and laughing more, while feeling less scared and more condent. One girl shared,
‘Now I am calmer and happier’.
Family focus groups
The focus groups with parents and siblings yielded four themes: Emotion Regulation, Better Interpersonal
Interactions, Empowered Leaders, and Learning from Horses. Those themes were guided by an overall
0.9
1
1.1
1.2
1.3
1.4
1.5
1.6
1.7
1.8
1.9
2
2.1
2.2
Time 1Time 2Time 3
Mean Aggressive Behavior Scor
e
Experimental
Wait-List Control
Figure 2.Mentors’ reports of participants’ aggression across time points.
398 J. L. GIBBONS ET AL.
observation of change in participants following the workshops. Family members often spoke of ‘cambió
bastante’ in Spanish, which means ‘he (she) changed a lot’. See Figure 4.
Family members described increased Emotion Regulation much like participants did. They spoke
often of participants being calmer and more peaceful (mas tranquilo/a) and using less aggression.
Describing her brother, a sister explained, ‘As my father was aggressive, he learned to behave aggres-
sively, but [after the workshops] he decided to make a change and not be aggressive like my father’.
When describing her daughter’s improvement in managing conicts with her sister, one mother
recounted, ‘She tries to control herself as she was taught and she tells me how it is done’.
Examples and narratives suggested that the workshops allowed participants to become Empowered
Leaders. Family members spoke of budding leadership skills and condence, ‘You told him, you are
the leader of the horse”, and now he will be the leader of the house’. At home participants were more
responsible and also readily shared what they had learned in the workshops with siblings and parents.
When commenting on changes she has noticed in her son, one mother said, ‘Now he is responsible.
Before [the workshop] he was irresponsible’. A mother shared that her daughter, ‘is sharing what she
learned … exactly’.
Given the positive changes in the participants themselves post-workshop, it is not surprising that
family members noticed improved interpersonal interactions. Specically, family members mentioned
better communication skills and greater evidence of compassion with others. A mother shared about
her son, ‘In his way of communicating he was very timid and distant, he didn’t trust people, now he
Figure 3.Themes from participants’ focus groups.
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF ADOLESCENCE AND YOUTH 399
chats more and comes to tell me personal things’. Finally, family members noted that participants had
learned directly from the horses, (e.g. ‘… then she [the daughter] says to me [the mother] “calm down”
and says how beautiful it feels to be with the horses and how much they taught her’, suggesting that
the opportunity for human–equine interaction was an integral component of the workshop’s success.
Discussion
Using random assignment to treatment and control groups, multiple sources of information, and psy-
chometrically sound instruments, we found evidence for the eectiveness of an equine-facilitated
workshop in changing the attitudes and behaviour of at-risk Guatemalan adolescents. Compared to
a wait-list control group, youth who experienced the programme reported more condence in their
leadership abilities. In addition, their mentors indicated that they showed less aggressive behaviour.
During focus groups family members also reported more leadership among the participants, as well
as better interpersonal interactions and emotion regulation. Participants themselves reported better
emotion regulation, a sense of empowerment, and more positive emotions. That is, we were able to
answer the rst two guiding questions armatively: the equine-facilitated workshop was eective
with adolescents who had little or no experience with horses. The answer to the third question – was
emotion regulation a key mechanism of change – was less denitive. Although quantitative measures
revealed no changes, focus groups with both the participants themselves and family members sug-
gested that emotion regulation was a critical feature. Both the youth participants and family mem-
bers narrated examples of improved emotion regulation. Participants talked about implementing the
breathing exercises when in dicult situations (e.g. when having an argument with a sibling). Family
members added that they witnessed less aggression and a sense of calmness among the youth who
had received the intervention.
This discrepancy between the quantitative and qualitative results may be attributed to diculties
with the quantitative measures of emotion regulation (i.e. the ERQ, Gross & John, 2013 and the CERQ,
Figure 4.Themes from family members’ focus groups.
400 J. L. GIBBONS ET AL.
Garnefski et al., 2001). Those measures were developed in the USA and the EQR had relatively poor
reliability in this sample of youth. School professionals thought that the participants might have had dif
-
culty understanding the items. Signicant instrument adaptation may be necessary to not only ensure
that the wording of the items is appropriate for Guatemalan youth, but also to ensure that the breadth
and depth of the construct is adequately addressed for this sample (e.g. Ægisdóttir & Einarsdóttir, 2012).
Another implication of the ndings from the focus groups is that there may be a ripple eect, the
diusion of the use of nonviolent conict resolution to other family members. For example, one partic-
ipant reported, ‘I taught [my mother] the breathing exercises and that is helping her a lot; now I want
to teach my sister to do the same’. Family members also noted this spread of knowledge as described
above. The spread of behaviours to other situations and settings had been reported in a previous study
with young prisoners (Hemingway et al., 2015).
Still unknown is the exact mechanism by which the intervention changed youth’s attitudes and
behaviour. What experiences are essential – the Join-Up® itself, other interactions with the horse, the
breathing exercise, the group discussions, or nonspecic aspects such as attention and support? In
focus groups, participants often mentioned the breathing exercise – a task based on slow, controlled
breathing and awareness of the present moment, a technique very similar to mindfulness exercises
(e.g. Bluth et al., 2016; Broderick & Metz, 2009). A ‘Learning to BREATHE’ mindfulness programme has
had good success in relieving depression and stress and increasing calmness and self-acceptance in
at-risk adolescents (Bluth et al., 2016; Broderick & Metz, 2009). Identifying the critical elements of the
programme is an important next step.
Limitations and future directions
Although this study has many strengths, it is not without limitations. First, the sample size is relatively
small and rather homogeneous. Participants were enrolled in school and were from families with very
limited economic resources. This was the target group of interest in the current study, but those sample
characteristics should be taken into consideration before generalizing these results to all Guatemalan
youth or all at-risk youth more broadly.
Second, the participant focus groups were conducted by the programme director, immediately
following the feedback session, allowing for the possibility of demand characteristics in the responses.
Third, as noted the very low alpha levels of some subscales of the emotion regulation measures indicated
the questions might not have been appropriate for our participants. Therefore, the evidence of quanti-
tative non-signicant changes in emotion regulation should not yet be interpreted as an inadequacy
of the workshops to address this issue, but instead that the measures were not adequately adapted for
the sample. The focus group data support this conclusion.
Despite these limitations, the ndings strengthen the results of previous research that used less
rigorous methods. In the present study, an equine-based intervention, based on the Join-Up® procedure
was eective in promoting leadership and emotion regulation and in reducing aggressive behaviour.
It successfully fostered empowered leadership among at-risk youth in Guatemala. The programme can
provide tools for youth to address the many problems that they face. Those tools may not only promote
success for the individual participants, but also for their family members, classmates, and Guatemalan
society at large.
Notes on contributors
Judith L. Gibbons is Professor Emerita of Psychology at Saint Louis University. She is the founding editor of the American
Psychological Association Division 52 journal International Perspectives in Psychology: Research, Practice, Consultation,
the past-president of the Interamerican Society of Psychology (SIP) and a former Fulbright scholar at the Universidad del
Valle de Guatemala. Her research interests include adolescent development in the majority world, especially in Guatemala,
intercountry adoption, gender roles and violence prevention. In collaboration with other researchers, she has published
over 100 journal articles and chapters and 2 books, The Thoughts of Youth and Intercountry Adoption: Policies, Practices,
and Outcomes.
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF ADOLESCENCE AND YOUTH 401
Catherine A. Cunningham is the founder of Lead-Up International. She is an advanced student of Monty Roberts and an
advocate of Join-Up and non-violence. In 2014, Catherine collaborated with fellow researchers to conduct a scientic study
of a Join-Up themed horse-handling program. The research produced evidence of reduced violence and abuse towards
horses and humans following the program (Gibbons, Cunningham, Paiz, Poelker, Montufar, HAIB 2015). These eorts led
to the creation of Lead-Up International, Aliate Program of Join-Up International.
Leslie Paiz is an equine enthusiast and advocate for non-violence. She is a member of the Lead-Up International team.
Leslie continues to work with programmes addressing the challenges faced by at-risk youth and communities with high
rates of interpersonal violence. In 2015, she was a co-author of an article demonstrating the eectiveness of a non-vio-
lent horse training program to reduce violence in a rural Guatemalan community (Gibbons, Cunningham, Paiz, Poelker,
& Montufar, 2015).
Katelyn E. Poelker is a doctoral candidate in Developmental Psychology at Saint Louis University. Currently, her research
is focused on Guatemalan adolescents' socio-emotional development and how gratitude and envy relate to well-being.
She has published articles on adolescents’ social and emotional development in the US and Guatemala as well as inter-
generational change in adolescents’ ideals.
Aracely Chajón is a psychologist working with at-risk children and adolescents at Fundación Educando a los Niños in
Guatemala. She provides social support services to youth and their families facing challenges due to living with limited
economic resources. In her role, Aracely ensures that in addition to their education in the classroom, students are receiving
the mental health and social support services that they need to thrive.
ORCID
Judith L. Gibbons http://orcid.org/0000-0002-9263-3096
Katelyn E. Poelker http://orcid.org/0000-0002-1139-7213
References
About Join-Up®. (2016). Retrieved from http://www.montyroberts.com/ab_about_monty/ju_about/
Adams, B. L. (2013). Using horses to teach authentic leadership skills to at-risk youth (Unpublished doctoral dissertation).
University of Florida, Gainesville.
Ægisdóttir, S., & Einarsdóttir, S. (2012). Cross-cultural adaptation of the Icelandic Beliefs About Psychological Services Scale
(I-BAPS). International Perspectives in Psychology: Research, Practice, and Consultation, 1, 236–251. Retrieved from http://
dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0030854
All, A. C., Loving, G. L., & Crane, L. L. (1999). Animals, horseback riding, and implications for rehabilitation therapy. Journal
of Rehabilitation, 65, 49–57.
Anestis, M. D., Anestis, J. C., Zawilinski, L. L., Hopkins, T. A., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2014). Equine-related treatments for mental
disorders lack empirical support: A systematic review of empirical investigations. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 70,
1115–1132. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jclp.22113
Bachi, K. (2012). Equine-facilitated psychotherapy: The gap between practice and knowledge. Society & Animals, 20, 364–380.
doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/15685306-12341242
Bachi, K., Terkel, J., & Teichman, M. (2012). Equine-facilitated psychotherapy for at-risk adolescents: The inuence
on self-image, self-control and trust. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 17, 298–312. doi:http://dx.doi.
org/10.1177/1359104511404177
Balluerka, N., Muela, A., Amiano, N., & Caldentey, M.A. (2014). Inuence of animal-assisted therapy (AAT) on the attachment
representations of youth in residential care. Children and Youth Services Review, 42, 103–109. doi:http://dx.doi.
org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2014.04.007
Bass, M. M., Duchowny, C. A., & Llabre, M. M. (2009). The eect of therapeutic horseback riding on social functioning in
children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 1261–1267. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/
s10803-009-0734-3
Bates, A. (2002). Of patients and horses: Equine-facilitated psychotherapy. Journal of Psychosocial Nursing and Mental Health
Services, 40, 16–19. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.3928/0279-3695-20020501-14
Beebe, B. J. (2008). The use of equine-assisted psychotherapy in youth chemical dependency: A new modality for nurse
practitioners (Unpublished manuscript). Washington State University, Pullman.
Beetz, A. B., Winkler, N., Julius, H., Uvnas-Moberg, K., & Kotrschal, K. (2015). A comparison of equine-assisted intervention and
conventional play-based early intervention for mother-child dyads with insecure attachment. Jouranl of Occupational
Therapy, Schools, & Early Intervention, 8, 17–39. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/19411243.2015.1026017
Bluth, K., Campo, R. A., Pruteanu-Malinici, S., Reams, A., Mullarkey, M., & Broderick, P. C. (2016). A school-based mindfulness
pilot study for ethnically diverse at-risk adolescents. Mindfulness, 7, 90–104. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12671-014-
0376-1
402 J. L. GIBBONS ET AL.
Brandt, C. (2013). Equine-facilitated psychotherapy as a complementary treatment intervention. The Practitioner Scholar:
Journal of Counseling and Professional Psychology, 20, 23–42.
Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 3, 77–101. doi:http://
dx.doi.org/10.1191/1478088706qp063oa
Broderick, P. C., & Metz, S. (2009). Learning to BREATHE: A pilot trial of a mindfulness curriculum for adolescents. Advances
in School Mental Health Promotion, 2, 35–46. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1754730X.2009.9715696
Burgon, H. L. (2011). ‘Queen of the world’: Experiences of ‘at-risk’ young people participating in equine-assisted learning/
therapy. Journal of Social Work Practice, 25, 165–183. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02650533.2011.561304
Cantin, A., & Marshall-Lucette, S. (2011). Examining the literature on the ecacy of equine assisted therapy for people
with mental health and behavioural disorders. Mental Health and Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 8, 51–61.
doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.5920/mhldrp.2011.8151
Central Intelligence Agency (CIA). (2016). The world factbook: Guatemala. Retrieved from https://www.cia.gov/library/
publications/the-world-factbook/geos/gt.html
Chappell, K. K. (2014). Equine-facilitated life coaching for youth leadership (Unpublished Master’s thesis). Prescott College,
Gardnerville, NV.
Chaplin, D. (2010). The therapeutic eects of working with Dartmoor ponies: A pathnding project. The Plymouth Student
Scientist, 3, 60–91.
Chaplin, L. N., & John, D. R. (2010). Interpersonal inuences on adolescent materialism: A new look at the role of parents
and peers. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 20, 176–184. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2010.02.002
Dell, C. A., Chalmers, D., Bresette, N., Swain, S., Rankin, D., & Hopkins, C. (2011). A healing space: The experiences of
First Nations and Inuit youth with equine-assisted learning. Child Youth Care Forum, 40, 319–336. doi:http://dx.doi.
org/10.1007/s10566-011-9140-z
Dyk, P., Cheung, R., Pohl, L., Noriega, C., Lindgreen, J., & Hayden, C. (2013). The eectiveness of equine guided leadership education
to develop emotional intelligence in expert nurses: A pilot research study. Lexington, KY: Center for Leadership Development,
University of Kentucky. Retrieved from http://www2.ca.uky.edu/cd/content/EGLEResearchPilotReportFinal5-20-13.pdf
Ecken, T. S. (2012). An investigation of adolescents’ experiences of EAT: A qualitative approach (Unpublished doctoral
dissertation). Middlesex University, London.
Esbjorn, R. J. (2006). When horses heal: A qualitative inquiry into equine facilitated Psychotherapy (Unpublished doctoral
dissertation). Institute of Transpersonal Psychology, Palo Alto, CA.
Frederick, K. E., Hatz, J. I., & Lanning, B. (2015). Not just horsing around: The impact of equine-assisted learning on levels of
hope and depression in at-risk adolescents. Community Mental Health Journal, 51, 809–817. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/
s10597-015-9836-x
Frewin, K., & Gardiner, B. (2005). New age or old sage? A review of equine assisted therapy. The Australian Journal of
Counseling Psychology, 6, 13–16.
Garnefski, N., Kraaij, V., & Spinhoven, P. (2001). Negative life events, cognitive emotion regulation, and emotional problems.
Personality and Individual Dierences, 30, 1311–1327. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00113-6
Gibbons, J. L., Cunningham, C. A., Paiz, L., Poelker, K. E., & Montufar Cardenas, M. A. (2015). “Before, he fought every day
with the horse and with me”: Reducing violence in a Guatemalan community through a horse-handling program.
Human-Animal Interaction Bulletin, 3, 37–55.
Grimm, E. J. (2015). Non-traditional therapeutic interventions for at-risk youth & students with complex support needs: Equine
therapy. Pittsburgh, PA: Duquesne University.
Grootveld, N. N. (2015). Eective leadership: An explorative study to the active aspects of equine-assisted learning regarding
emotional intelligence (Unpublished Master’s thesis). University of Twente, Enschede, Netherlands.
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual dierences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for aect,
relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 348–362. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.
org/0.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348
Hauge, H., Kvalem, I. L., Berget, B., Enders-Slegers, M. J., & Braastad, B. O. (2014). Equine-assisted activities and the impact
on perceived social support, self-esteem and self-ecacy among adolescents – an intervention study. International
Journal of Adolescence and Youth, 19(1), 1–21. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02673843.2013.779587
Hemingway, A., Meek, R., & Hill, C. E. (2015). An exploration of an equine-facilitated learning intervention with young
oenders. Society & Animals, 23, 544–568. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/15685306-12341382
Huesmann, L. R., & Guerra, N. G. (1997). Children’s normative beliefs about aggression and aggressive behavior. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 408–419. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.72.2.408
Instituto Nacional de Estadística. (2014). Guatemala - Encuesta Nacional de Salud Materno Infantil 2008-2009 [National
Survey of Maternal and Child Health 2008-2009]. Retrieved from http://microdata.worldbank.org/index.php/catalog/983
Kataoka, S., & Vandell, D. L. (2013). Quality of after school activities and relative change in adolescent functioning over two
years. Applied Developmental Science, 17, 123–134. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10888691.2013.804375
Keeling, L. J., Jonare, L., & Lanneborn, L. (2009). Investigating horse-human interactions: The eect of a nervous human.
The Veterinary Journal, 181, 70–71. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tvjl.2009.03.013
Kelly, S. (2014). Horses for courses. Exploring the limits of leadership development through equine-assisted learning. Journal
of Management Education, 38, 216–223. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1052562913489027
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF ADOLESCENCE AND YOUTH 403
Kendall, E., Maujean, A., Pepping, C. A., & Wright, J. J. (2014). Hypotheses about the psychological benets of horses. Explore,
10, 81–87. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.explore.2013.12.001
Kolb, D. A. (2015). Experiential learning: Experience as the source of learning and development (2nd ed.). Englewood Clis,
NJ: Pearson.
Kruger, K. A., Trachtenberg, S. W., & Serpell, J. A. (2004). Can animals help humans heal? Animal assisted interventions in
adolescent mental health. Retrieved from https://www.researchgate.net/prole/James_Serpell/publication/234839772_
Can_Animals_Help_Humans_Heal_Animal-Assisted_Interventions_in_Adolescent_Mental_Health/
links/0fcfd5101857ca16bd000000.pdf
Ladd, G. W., & Prolet, S. M. (1996). The Child Behavior Scale: A teacher-report measure of young children’s aggressive,
withdrawn, and prosocial behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 32, 1008–1024. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-
1649.32.6.1008
Lee, P.T., Dakin, E., & McLure, M. (2015). Narrative synthesis of equine-assisted psychotherapy literature: Current knowledge
and future research directions. Health and Social Care in the Community, 24, 226–246. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/
hsc.12201
Lentini, J. A., & Knox, M. (2009). A qualitative and quantitative review of equine facilitated psychotherapy (EFP) with
children and adolescents. The Open Complementary Medicine Journal, 1, 51–57. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.2174/18763
91X00901010051
Lentini, J. A., & Knox, M. S. (2015). Equine-facilitated psychotherapy with children and adolescents: An update and literature
review. Journal of Creativity in Mental Health, 10, 278–305. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15401383.2015.1023916
MacKinnon, J. R., Noh, S., Laliberte, J., Lariviere, A., Allan, D. E., & Laliberte, D. (1995). A study of therapeutic eects of
horseback riding for children with cerebal palsy. Physical & Occupational Therapy in Pediatrics, 15, 17–34. doi:http://
dx.doi.org/10.1080/J006v15n01_02
Maujean, A., Kendall, E., Lillan, R., Sharp, T., & Pringle, G. (2013). Connecting for health: Playing with horses as a therapeutic
tool. Journal of Community Psychology, 41, 515–522. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jcop.21547
May, D. K., Seivert, N. P., Cano, A., Casey, R. J., & Johnson, A. (2016). Animal-assisted therapy for youth: A systematic
methodological critique. Human-Animal Interaction Bulletin, 4(1), 1–18.
McCardle, P., McCune, S., Grin, L., Maholmes, V., & Freund, L. (2011). Afterword: An agenda for future research. In
P. M cCardle, S. McCune, J.A. Grin, & V. Maholmes (Eds.), How animals aect us: Examining the inuences of human–animal
interaction on child development and human health (pp. 193–202). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
McConnell, P. J. (2010). National survey on equine assisted therapy: An exploratory study of current practitioners and programs
(Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Walden University, Minneapolis, MN.
McCullough, M. E., Rachal, K. C., Sandage, S. J., Worthington, E. L., Brown, S. W., & Hight, T. L. (1998). Interpersonal forgiving
in close relationships: II. Theoretical elaboration and measurement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75,
1586–1603. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.75.6.1586
McCullough, L., Risley-Curtiss, C., & Rorke, J. (2015). Equine facilitated psychotherapy: A pilot study of eect on post-
traumatic stress symptoms in maltreated youth. Journal of Infant, Child, and Adolescent Psychotherapy, 14, 158–173.
doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15289168.2015.1021658
Meek, R. (2012). TheHorseCourse at HMP/YOI Portland: Interim evaluation ndings. Middlesbrough: Teesside University.
Retrieved from http://hopereinstherapy.com/pdfs/Resources%20-%20Eval-THC.pdf
Meola, C. C. (2016). Addressing the needs of the Millennial workforce through equine assisted learning. Journal of
Management Development, 35, 294–303. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/JMD-08-2015-0110
Merkies, K., Sievers, A., Zakrajsek, E., MacGregor, H., Bergeron, R., & von Borstel, U. (2014). Preliminary results suggest an
inuence of psychological and physiological stress in humans on horse heart rate and behavior. Journal of Veterinary
Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research., 9, 242–247. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2014.06.003
Notgrass, C. G. (2011). Equine assisted psychotherapy: A study of US practitioners trained by the equine assisted growth and
learning association (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Saint Louis University, Saint Louis, MO.
Notgrass, C. G., & Petinelli, J. D. (2015). Equine assisted psychotherapy: The Equine Assisted Growth and Learning
Association’s Model Overview of Equine-Based Modalities. Journal of Experiential Education, 38, 162–174. doi:http://
dx.doi.org/10.1177/1053825914528472
Oce of Inspector General, U.S. Agency for International Development. (2014). Audit of USAID/Guatemala’s violence
prevention project. Retrieved from http://oig.usaid.gov/sites/default/les/audit-reports/1-520-14-009-p.pdf
Pendry, P., & Roeter, S. (2013). Experimental trial demonstrates positive eects of equine facilitated learning on child social
competence. Human-Animal Interaction Bulletin, 1(1), 1–19.
Pohl, L. D. (2015). Equine guided leadership education: Leveraging somatic intelligence in learning leadership competencies.
Lexington, KY: Center for Leadership Development, University of Kentucky. Retrieved from https://www.researchgate.
net/prole/Lissa_Pohl_Ma/publications
Population Reference Bureau. (2011). Guatemala: Beyond the early phase of the demographic transition. Retrieved from
http://www.prb.org/publications/datasheets/2011/world-population-data-sheet/guatemala.aspx
Ricochet Leadership Skills Survey (n.d.). Retrieved from https://www.extension.iastate.edu/4h/sites/www.extension.iastate.
edu/les/4h/ricochet/resources/EvaluationModiedLeadershipLifeSkillsScalePostPro.doc
404 J. L. GIBBONS ET AL.
Roberts, M. (2002a). From my hands to yours: Lessons from a lifetime of training championship horses. Solvang, CA: Monty
and Pat Roberts Inc.
Roberts, M. (2002b). Horse sense for people. New York, NY: Penguin.
Seevers, B. S., Dormody, T. J., & Clason, D. L. (1995). Developing a scale to research and evaluate youth leadership life skills
development. Journal of Agricultural Education, 36, 28–35. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.5032/jae.1995.02028.
Selby, A., & Smith-Osborne, A. (2013). A systematic review of eectiveness of complementary and adjunct therapies and
interventions involving equines. Health Psychology, 32, 418–432. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0029188
Smith, T. A., Gentry, L. S., & Ketrig, S. A.. (2005). Evaluating a youth leadership life skills development program. Journal of
Extension, 43. Retrieved from http://www.joe.org/joe/2005april/rb3.php
Smith, A.V., Proops, L., Grounds, K., Wathan, J., & McComb, K. (2016). Functionally relevant responses to human facial
expressions of emotion in the domestic horse (Equus caballus). Biology Letters, 12, 20150907. doi:http://dx.doi.
org/10.1098/rsbl.2015.0907
Trotter, K. S., Chandler, C. K., Goodwin-Bond, D., & Casey, J. (2008). A comparative study of the ecacy of group equine
assisted counseling with at-risk children and adolescents. Journal of Creativity in Mental Health, 3, 254–284. doi:http://
dx.doi.org/10.1080/15401380802356880
Tseng, S. H., Chen, H. C., & Tam, K. W. (2013). Systematic review and meta-analysis of the eect of equine assisted activities
and therapies on gross motor outcome in children with cerebral palsy. Disability and Rehabilitation, 35, 89–99.
UNICEF. (2008). The state of Latin American and Caribbean children 2008. Retrieved from http://www.unicef.org/publications/
les/SOLAC_2008_EN_041408.pdf
UNICEF. (2011). The state of the world’s children 2011: Adolescence an age of opportunity. Retrieved from http://www.unicef.
org/sowc2011/pdfs/SOWC-2011-Main-Report_EN_02092011.pdf
UNICEF. (2014a). Hidden in plain sight: A statistical analysis of violence against children. Retrieved from http://les.unicef.org/
publications/les/Hidden_in_plain_sight_statistical_analysis_EN_3_Sept_2014.pdf
UNICEF. (2014b). UNICEF annual report 2014 Guatemala. Retrieved from http://www.unicef.org/about/annualreport/les/
Guatemala_Annual_Report_2014.pdf
West, R. A. (2015). The silent language of emotions: Equine inspired leadership development (Unpublished doctoral dissertation).
Wilmington University, New Castle, DE.
World Bank. (2016). Guatemala. Retrieved from http://data.worldbank.org/country/guatemala
World Food Programme (WFP). (2016). Guatemala. Retrieved from https://www.wfp.org/countries/guatemala/overview
Wuermli, A. J., Tubbs, C. C., Petersen, A. C., & Aber, L. J. (2015). Children and youth in low- and middle-income countries:
toward an integrated developmental and intervention science. Child Development Perspectives, 9, 61–66. doi:http://
dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12108
... However, gender information was not specified in the report by Bachi et al. [57]. All studies were conducted with horses (100%), and in almost all studies the young participants have been self-selected, not randomly assigned, except for Mueller and McCullough's [15] who quasi-randomized participants to treatment, and Gibbons et al. [58] study, whose participants were randomly assigned to experimental or waitlist control groups. ...
... The most frequent period of intervention was 2 to 3 months [12,15,56,59]. The number of individual sessions varied from 8 to 26 (MEAN= 11.5 ± 4.6), except for Gibbons et al. [58] study, in which full 2-day sessions were applied. In Schultz et al. [60] study, the variation in the number of sessions each child received was enormous, so they only considered those clients receiving at least 6 sessions for analysis. ...
... After the 8-week Gestalt Program ended, the young clients reported less frequent sleepless nights due to worrying, from baseline and fewer incidences of anger. Gibbons et al. [58] reported several significant benefits of using Join-Up® Model, for two days, in young Guatemalan victims of abuse or violence, including improvements in emotion regulation and decrease in aggression, and self-reported leadership (p= 0.016). Within this study, the young participants were randomly assigned to treatment or control groups, and besides using multiple and complete evaluation instruments, their results were based on participants' self-reports, and on mentors and parents' perceptions. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Childhood abuse can have long-lasting consequences and be a risk for mental health. This review aimed to explore the recent studies on equine-assisted psychotherapy (EAP), which uses equines in therapy to help young people recover from adverse childhood experiences. Five relevant data-bases were utilized in the search of full-text articles published between January 2000 and August 2024 - addressing the incorporation of equines in therapeutic activities of young people (between 6 and 25 years old) who have experienced abuse, violence, or psychological trauma. The search excluded articles that lacked well-defined treatment programs or focused on physical treatments and limited the results to scholarly journals. The initial search yielded 537 articles, of which 27 were extracted. Following a detailed examination, 18 were excluded, leaving 9 works for further analysis. All authors agree that EAP holds much promise for youth with a history of trauma or abuse, however, a lack of methodological rigor was found across the publications collected, including inconsistencies in the concepts used; the variability in participants, designs, and settings of the programs; minimal use of randomization or control groups; and lack of reliability. The implications for future research are discussed, including the need to address discrepancies in the program’s descriptions and reporting outcomes.
... However, a randomized-controlled trial (RCT) by Gibbons et al. (2017) examining whether emotion regulation would be a possible mechanism of change for reducing aggression when undergoing EAP found differing results. The authors found non-significant pre-to post-intervention changes on both the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ; Gross & John, 2003) ...
... As mentioned earlier in the paper, such physical interactions have been found to reduce psychological stress (Kendall et al., 2015;Shen et al., 2018) through the activation of the oxytocinergic system (Beetz et al., 2012). Similarly, qualitative findings of emotion regulation also described that undergoing EAP made youths feel calmer and less likely to behave aggressively (Gibbons et al., 2017). Thus, there was reduced activation of automatic emotional suppression when the effects of accumulated stressors were mitigated when undergoing EAP or EAA. ...
... Lastly, the differing results reported by Gibbons et al. (2017) can be reconciled with the findings of the current study. Firstly, Gibbons et al. (2017) posited that non-significance of the ERQ in their study could be due to the scale being unsuitable for their predominant sample of youths (age; mean = 18.22, ...
Thesis
While there is increased involvement of animals in the clinical setting, studies investigating this form of complementary therapy suffer from methodological limitations that diminish the veracity of their findings. The involvement of horses in clinical settings for the improvement of mental health outcomes is known as Equine Assisted Psychotherapy (EAP), whereas participating in horsemanship activities with the horse without the involvement of a mental health professional is known as Equine-Assisted Activities (EAA). This study investigated the effectiveness of EAP and EAA on two strategies of emotion regulation, cognitive reappraisal and emotional suppression. In a single-blind, placebo-controlled, randomized trial, 90 young adults (21 males, 69 females) were randomly assigned to the EAP (N = 31), EAA (N = 26), or Placebo-Control (N = 33) condition, and then followed up 1 week after the experimental session. While results found no significant differences between conditions for cognitive reappraisal, both EAP and EAA revealed significant reductions in emotional suppression compared to the Placebo-Control condition. Results provide experimental evidence for the human-horse bond component present in both EAP and EAA, and theoretical implications are discussed.
... Principal among these is that horses are recognised as being extremely sensitive to human emotion, and to subtle expressions of emotions. 4,11 Several studies, using empirical behavioural and physiological markers of horses, support this 11 ; a recent study, for example, revealed that horses are able to spontaneously discriminate between positive (happy) and negative (angry) human facial expressions in photographs 13 . A corollary is that horses are able to mirror or reflect these emotional states, through interactional responses, and in this way provide feedback, which is central to the therapeutic aspect of the process. ...
... A corollary is that horses are able to mirror or reflect these emotional states, through interactional responses, and in this way provide feedback, which is central to the therapeutic aspect of the process. 4,11 Such responses and interactions are spontaneous, and impartial (or non-judgemental), and are therefore authentic, and this is a further defining feature of EAP. 4 The sensitivity of horses to their surroundings is in part due to the fact that they are prey (as opposed to predator) animals. They are naturally hyper-vigilant to danger, will flee from it, and will communicate their fear instantly to the rest of the herd. ...
... 14 Successful completion of tasks with the horses leads to increased confidence, and an overcoming of fear. 11 Because of their size, horses cannot be physically coerced to act in predetermined ways during therapeutic sessions, and other strategies have to ORIGINAL be utilised in order to enable their cooperation. 11 Several apparent contradictions emerge from the juxtaposition of horses as large and powerful animals on the one hand, and as therapeutic facilitators on the other. ...
Article
Full-text available
Equine Assisted Psychotherapy (EAP) is gaining much popularity as an intervention for a range of mental health problems. Although evidence for its efficacy has to date been low, this may largely be due to poorly conducted research. This article examines the evidence for EAP, and the indications for its use. It also explores its theoretical foundations and discusses an EAP project that has been conducted for four years at Valkenberg Hospital’s Forensic Mental Health Unit.
... Accordingly, the concept of using horses for leadership development might be regarded as a mere novelty, yet another fad or fashion in the corporate training field (Kelly, 2014;Stock & Kolb, 2016). However previous research suggests that equine-assisted interventions provide the opportunity to foster the development of emotional intelligence (Bouchard, 2014;Dyk et al., 2012;Fransson, 2015;Gibbons, Cunningham, Paiz, Poelker, & Chajón, 2017;Grootveld, 2015;Perkins, 2018;Rauen, 2017), development of life skills (Evans et al., 2009;Perkins, 2018;Slocum, 2004), social skills (Bouchard, 2014;Iwachiw, 2017), problem solving skills (Murphy, Wilson, & Greenberg, 2017) and interpersonal skills (de Beer, 2016;Hesse & Capitao, 2012), to build assertiveness (Fransson, 2015), social competence (Pendry et al., 2014), social-awareness (Rauen, 2017), perceived social support (Hauge, Kvalem, Berget, Enders-Slegers, & Braastad, 2014), work ethic, time management, balance, collaboration, teamwork (de Beer, 2016;Duff, 2010;Mikulec & McKinney, 2014), communication (de Beer, 2016Mikulec & McKinney, 2014;Murphy et al., 2017;Notschaele, 2011;Pentecost, 2017;Perkins, 2018;Saggers & Strachan, 2016), self-awareness (Bouchard, 2014;de Beer, 2016;Grootveld, 2015;Notschaele, 2011;Rauen, 2017), self-efficacy (Bouchard, 2014); self-confidence (All, Loving & Crane, 1999;Bouchard, 2014;de Beer, 2016;Perkins, 2018;Saggers & Strachan, 2016), motivation (All et al., 1999;Pentecost, 2017), selfesteem, cooperation, enthusiasm (All et al., 1999), which are critical traits to being an influential leader. These findings verify that horse-assisted activities can help individuals develop leadership skills and thus contribute to leadership development. ...
... These findings verify that horse-assisted activities can help individuals develop leadership skills and thus contribute to leadership development. Previous research also suggests that participating in equine-assisted learning helps to teach leadership skills (Benson, 2012;Duff, 2010;Gibbons et al., 2017;Gunter et al., 2017;Kolzarek, 2001;Lyle & Schlamb, 2017, Mikulec & McKinney, 2014Pohl, 2006;Ridding, 2005) and authentic leadership skills (Adams, 2013;Adams-Pope & Stedman, 2014;Grootveld, 2015). ...
Article
Full-text available
In today's fast-paced business world, where there is a need to develop divergent thinking and a wider range of skills, organizations seek original ways to be successful. Accordingly, leadership development has seen several uncommon approaches. Equine-assisted learning programs which involve innovative, underutilized, and motivating techniques and strategies, provide a dynamic process of building and developing leadership skills through horse-human interactions. In this paper, after the history of horse-human relationships and interactions are examined, a literature review is conducted on developing research and using equine-assisted leadership development programs. Although the extant research shows that equine-assisted leadership development programs promote skills critical to being an influential leader, the present paper provides an insight into equine-assisted experiential learning on leadership development, provides an overview on what happens in a typical equine-assisted experiential learning session, and suggests practical implications for researchers and organizations.
... Scientific research on horses is multiple and fascinating research field which ranges from riding sports to the field of dealing with emotional challenges, such as in equine-assisted psychotherapy. Studies (Gibbons, Cunningham, Paiz, Poelker, & Chajón, 2016;Lentini & Knox, 2015) have demonstrated the extraordinary sensitivity of horses to human feelings and emotions. A horse can serve to mirror the feelings of the people surrounding them. ...
... Third, the size of a horse means that they cannot readily be forced to act; humans must use other means to elicit their cooperation and desired behaviour. Learning to direct the behaviour of such a large animal can potentially decrease fearfulness and increase self-confidence (Gibbons et al., 2016). In horse-assisted psychotherapy, horses are equal members of the team: there are two-and four-legged team members instead of humans and horses. ...
Article
The focus of this article is a triad of creative writing (CW), equine, and poetry therapy (PT) on a five-month course titled “Writing with horses.” The researcher and writer (W2) has experience in implementing expressive literature and art at the stable where two ponies are kept in Northern Finland. In this case study of how CW and horses may benefit self-expression, we apply CW and reading in contact with horses and in the context of PT. What is CW like when done in the presence of a horse? How an animal is present in a PT triad between literature, facilitator and participant. The theory behind this study is based on the literature and theory behind CW (expressive writing by Pennebaker, 2004 (Writing to heal: A guided journal for recovering from trauma and emotional upheaval. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger Press)), and post-humanist theories, such as human–animal studies, as well as Hynes and Hynes-Berry (1986/1994. Biblio/poetry therapy. The interactive process. A handbook. East Peoria, IL: Versa Press. North Star Press of St Cloud) as well as biblio/poetry therapy. The method applied is a literature-based process of CW. PT, CW and equine and these elements as a combination are a rewarding tool for increasing self-expression and thus becoming sensitive toward self-knowledge and self-esteem. New skills are activated, emotions and the enjoyment of self-expression through CW methods and contents are shown. The CW process among horses allows the study of one’s feelings and concepts.
... Findings from existing scoping and systematic reviews across the broader Equine-Assisted Services space suggest that Equine-Assisted Learning approaches (EAL) can successfully engage Aboriginal children and young people with additional needs who have not previously responded to traditional therapeutic interventions or those who experienced sexual abuse or were exposed to problematic parental substance use [74,[87][88][89][90]. Reported outcomes include increased prosocial behaviours [91,92], confidence and self-esteem [51,93,94], self-awareness [66], self-control and emotional regulation [82,93,95] and reduced anxiety [92,94]. Participants with a history of trauma have commented on the safety and comfort horses provide [79]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Australian Aboriginal people experience stressors from inequalities across crucial social determinants, including deep and entrenched disadvantage and exclusion. The impact of unaddressed historical issues is pervasive and intergenerational. The disproportionate rates of Aboriginal youth suicide, juvenile detention and imprisonment highlight the inadequacy of existing social and emotional wellbeing programs and services for Aboriginal children and young people. There is increasing recognition in Australia that aligning social and emotional wellbeing interventions with Western values and conceptions of mental health is one of the main barriers to service uptake among Aboriginal people. This suggests fundamental questions remain unanswered about what type of services effectively address the complex constellation of social-emotional and wellbeing challenges arising from intergenerational poverty and trauma. Yawardani Jan-ga is an Aboriginal-led, operated, culturally secure, Equine-Assisted Learning (EAL) project designed by and with local Aboriginal young people, community Elders, members, and experts to address the complex constellation of social-emotional, spiritual and wellbeing needs of Aboriginal children and young people, aged 6–26 years, across multiple communities in the Kimberley region of Western Australia. EAL is a strengths-based learning approach where participants work with horses’ inherent characteristics to learn transferable life skills, such as communication skills, self-awareness, and emotional regulation, to promote social and emotional growth and wellbeing. Although EAL has been previously used with Aboriginal children and young people internationally, they are yet to be widely used with Aboriginal people in Australia. Here, we describe the three subcomponents of the Yawardani Jan-ga implementation science project and the planned Participatory Action Research and phenomenological approaches to capture the distinctive experiences of participants and the local communities where the intervention is implemented. We anticipate that findings will build an evidence base that informs policy and practice by understanding key intervention elements of social and emotional wellbeing support for Aboriginal youth, how to incorporate Aboriginal worldviews across different stages of interventions, and how to capture impact best using culturally secure methods.
... To understand the economic and financial situations of the participants, we provided them with a list of 14 household items (e.g., television, refrigerator, computer, bookshelf with books) and asked them to indicate whether they had each of these items in their homes. This is a method for measuring SES that has been used various times in Guatemala (Ashdown et al., 2017;Gibbons et al., 2016;Poelker et al., 2019;Poelker & Gibbons, 2018). Collecting an estimate of SES in this way allows researchers to understand the participants' financial situations relative to other participants in the sample and does not require the participants to provide an estimate of household incomewhich many adolescents would not know. ...
Article
Full-text available
Utilizing adolescents' drawings to investigate their perceptions of self has a long history in psychology. The methodological approach is particularly useful in places, such as Guatemala, where the population may have relatively lower levels of literacy and/or less experience engaging with Western-style research surveys. By asking adolescents to draw a picture of themselves and list five characteristics that they hope to have in 15 years, we were able to collect valuable data on issues such as the students' desires for the future and what they see as possible for themselves. Participants (N = 81, Mage = 14.56 years, 49.4% cisgender girls, all from Jocotenango, Guatemala) provided five characteristics that they hoped to have 15 years in the future and then drew a self-portrait of themselves 15 years in the future. The drawings and characteristics underwent a process of thematic analysis to determine patterns and themes that are prevalent in the data. Common themes that emerged include jobs or specific professions they hope to have, owning a home or property such as a car, and having a family. In the drawings, the common themes are similar: mentioning specific careers, owning a home or property, and having a family. These data can help us determine what type of future these students hope for to ensure that schools and other institutions are providing the tools students will need for those futures.
Article
Full-text available
Youth have experienced an increase in mental health concerns and can be challenging to work with using traditional talk-based prevention and treatment options. This meta-analysis aimed to synthesize existing studies on the effectiveness of equine-assisted interventions with youth for psychosocial outcomes, such as internalizing and externalizing problems, adaptive efficacy, self-esteem, and depressive symptoms. Search and selection procedures involved screening 3525 records to yield 16 controlled studies published between 2009 and 2021 with 1009 participants. The results showed a statistically significant, homogenous, and medium effect for the overall effectiveness of equine interventions for improving overall psychosocial outcomes for youth (n = 16, d = .535, 95% CI [.345, .726], p < .001, I² = 0.39). The results also showed similar statistically significant effects for externalizing problems, internalizing problems, and adaptive efficacy. However, the effectiveness of equine-assisted interventions for the self-esteem and depressive symptoms (when measured separately from internalizing problems) of youth was statistically non-significant. For self-esteem, the effects were heterogeneous, suggesting the studies may not be measuring the same effect. Future research on equine-assisted interventions for the mental health of youth should utilize designs with larger sample sizes, randomization and/or clear equivalence of comparison groups, a credible comparison treatment, complete and analyzable follow-up measurements, and adequate statistical analyses and reporting.
Article
Equine Facilitated Programs (EFPs) appear to improve wellbeing in young people, however, empirical research is in its infancy. This study examined the experiences of young people participating in an EFP, and explored the mechanisms of change. Eight Irish socio-economically disadvantaged, adolescent males (aged 12–14 years; M = 13.86), with social and emotional difficulties took part in 31 semi-structured interviews and two focus groups over an eight week EFP. Thematic analysis generated three themes: Initial Engagement; Engagement with the Program; and Personal Growth. Potential mechanisms of change included curiosity, reciprocity, having a goal, and mastery of skills. EFPs may therefore improve wellbeing in young people through mechanisms that encourage both initial and ongoing engagement, and that support personal growth.
Article
Full-text available
This paper throws light on equine-assisted learning or which may also be called "horse whispering" and considers the results of human-animal interactions which can act as a tool for the development and improvement of one self. Through this human-animal interaction, the paper presents three Lacanian concepts of subjectivity, desire, and fantasy in determining the fractured relationship between self and other which may also be portrayed as the leader-follower relations. Organizations are struggling hard to meet the needs of the Millennials. Managers are thus facing the problems in building relationships with Millennial employees and fully understanding which motivating factors affect them and their work. Special training focusing on the Millennials can help the leaders to work with Millennials in a more effective manner which in turn can increase retention rates for organizations, reducing the costs as well as increasing the loyalty among them. Throughout history we have observed that the horses have been remained the symbols of power and leadership. They have been the deciding factor in the fall and rise of various empires for thousands of years. But can we imagine that horses can still be influential in the corporate world today also through EAL programs that is Equine Assisted Learning.
Article
Full-text available
Animal Assisted Therapy (AAT) for youth has the potential to benefit both physical and mental health outcomes. Yet little is known about the extent to which study designs in this area are aligned with established standards of intervention research. This critical review assesses current research methodologies focusing on AATs for youth with physical and mental health concerns. The main aims of this review are to advance the knowledge base of empirically supported treatments and identify next steps that researchers can take to secure the place of AATs as sound and valid interventions for youth.
Article
Full-text available
Whether non-human animals can recognize human signals, including emotions, has both scientific and applied importance, and is particularly relevant for domesticated species. This study presents the first evidence of horses’ abilities to spontaneously discriminate between positive (happy) and negative (angry) human facial expressions in photographs. Our results showed that the angry faces induced responses indicative of a functional under- standing of the stimuli: horses displayed a left-gaze bias (a lateralization generally associated with stimuli perceived as negative) and a quicker increase in heart rate (HR) towards these photographs. Such lateralized responses towards human emotion have previously only been documented in dogs, and effects of facial expressions on HR have not been shown in any heterospe- cific studies. Alongside the insights that these findings provide into interspecific communication, they raise interesting questions about the generality and adaptiveness of emotional expression and perception across species.
Article
Full-text available
This research reports a qualitative study to explore the behavioral responses and reflections from Young Offenders undertaking an Equine-Facilitated Learning ( efl) Intervention in prison in the United Kingdom. Learning was facilitated by an instructor, and the participants were taught introductory natural horsemanship skills. Establishments holding young adult prisoners are typically characterized by increased disruption to the regime, and by greater incidents of violence, bullying, and conflict than in other types of prison. A resulting challenge for those working with young prisoners is the need to respond to increased levels of social isolation, and difficulties in managing impulsivity, problem solving, temper, and conduct. It is hoped that this research will provide some initial evidence to contribute to ideas around the nature of learning practical, positive skills and knowledge through inter-species interactions.
Article
Full-text available
Community and family violence are endemic in Guatemala. We evaluated the effectiveness of a horse-handling program to reduce violent attitudes and aggressive behavior. Eighteen community members who worked with horses in their daily lives (16 men, 2 women, ages 15 to 58) participated in four weekly sessions of embodied experiences with horses. The program taught Monty Roberts’ Join-Up®, a method of non-violent handling, as well as desensitization of horses to feared objects. Compared to the pretest, on a posttest participants were less likely to endorse violent attitudes with respect to wife-beating, corporal punishment of children, and physical punishment of horses. Their horses also were less reactive to the owner’s approach. Female relatives reported that following the program participants provided better care for horses and were calmer and less aggressive with other people. These findings imply that an equine-facilitated program shows promise for transforming communities in which violence is prevalent.
Research
Full-text available
Abstract: The Effectiveness of Equine Guided Leadership Education to Develop Emotional Intelligence in Expert Nurses Purpose: The purpose of this pilot study was to identify, the feasibility and effectiveness of Equine Guided Leadership Education (EGLE), a form of Equine Assisted Learning (EAL), in developing emotional intelligence competencies in expert nurses. Our hypothesis was that Equine Guided Leadership Education would be effective at developing the emotional intelligence competencies being measured by the assessment instrument. Background and Significance: There is growing interest and research being conducted on the impact of developing the emotional intelligence (EQ) competencies of nurses and other healthcare professionals. Recent findings suggest that emotional intelligence may be a useful concept for exploring innovations that target improved clinical staff performance and retention. Anecdotal evidence, from the field of equine assisted learning, finds that experiential learning programs that utilize ground exercises with horses helps to develop leadership skills, including emotional intelligence in humans. The intention behind this collaboration between The Center for Leadership Development and UK Healthcare at the University of Kentucky was to further research efforts in both fields. Methods: A voluntary sample of 21 expert nurses was recruited to both an intervention group (n=11) and a control group (n=10); assignment to these groups was determined by the hospital unit service line. Both groups completed a pre-intervention online Emotional Intelligence Appraisal assessment measuring the following emotional intelligence dimensions: Self-Awareness, Self-Management, Social Awareness, and Relationship Management. The intervention consisted of a 1-day EGLE workshop. The nurses in the intervention group also filled out a qualitative survey on their experience at the end of their day with the horses and again three months later. At six months post-intervention, both groups were re-assessed using the online emotional intelligence assessment. Results: The comparative analysis between the change scores, as measured by the online EQ assessment, of the intervention group to the control group showed higher EQ scores in all dimensions measured with the largest increase in the Relationship Management dimension. The thematic analysis of the pre and post qualitative surveys completed by the intervention participants indicated an increase in the following emotional intelligence competencies: self-awareness, body language, and social awareness. Conclusions: This pilot proved successful as a feasibility study. The encouraging results gained can be used as a starting point for conducting a similar but much larger longitudinal study looking at the effectiveness of working with horses to increase emotional intelligence.
Article
Purpose – Organizations are striving to stay abreast of the transitions occurring from the influx of Millennials into the workforce. Managers are having trouble building relationships with Millennial employees and understanding what motivates them to be effective workers. Trainings focussed on helping managers work with Millennials can increase retention rates for organizations, saving money as well as building loyalty and buy-in to organizational goals and values. Equine assisted learning (EAL) programs are an effective training tool that many organizations are currently using to bridge this gap between older managers and Millennial employees. These programs would all fulfill the requirements Millennials seek in on-the-job soft skills training. The paper aims to discuss these issues. Design/methodology/approach – A literature review was conducted on ongoing research and use of EAL in the field of management training and development. Findings – Current research shows that EAL promotes competency development in leadership, communication, body language awareness, relationship building, and teamwork building. Originality/value – EAL is an up-and-coming management training trend that is available on a wider basis than ever before. There is a greater evidence-based research base that is continuing to grow as well in this field.
Article
Domestication of animals began over 12,000 years ago and continues today. Animals and humans have been developing special relationships for centuries. Despite descriptive and anecdotal reports, research focused on the use of animals as therapeutic interventions and the unique relationship that often exist between animals and humans has been sparse and frequently not based on rigorous methodology. The following article reviews interventions and outcomes of animals commonly considered companions or pets and horses. Issues and implications for rehabilitation professionals in regards to their awareness and use of these less than traditional forms of interventions are explored.
Article
During the past several years, there has been a large increase in the number of studies of equine-facilitated psychotherapy (EFP) with children and adolescents. However, due to the diversity of methods, samples, and publication types represented in the literature, it may be difficult for EFP practitioners to synthesize the existing research into useful guidelines and treatment recommendations for patients. This article summarizes and tabulates investigation findings by methods, theoretical underpinnings, and treatment populations. This review of 47 recent publications suggests that EFP is a useful modality with children and adolescents. In the large majority of reviewed papers, benefits were found for a variety of presenting problems and disorders. The most often studied populations were “at-risk” youth and children with an Autism Spectrum Disorder diagnosis. Recommendations for counselors and directions of future EFP research are made.