ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

Values are internalized cognitive structures that guide choices by evoking a sense of basic principles of right and wrong, a sense of priorities, and a willingness to make meaning and see patterns. Like other cognitive constructs, values can be studied at the individual level or at the group level. That is societies, cultures, and other social groups have value-based norms, priorities, and guidelines, which describe what people ought to do if they are to do the ‘right,’ ‘moral,’ ‘valued’ thing. The study of values currently focuses more explicitly on the circumstances in which values predict action.
Provided for non-commercial research and educational use only.
Not for reproduction, distribution or commercial use.
This article was originally published in the International Encyclopedia of the Social
& Behavioral Sciences, 2nd edition, published by Elsevier, and the attached copy
is provided by Elsevier for the author’s benefit and for the benefit of the
author’s institution, for non-commercial research and educational use including
without limitation use in instruction at your institution, sending it to specific
colleagues who you know, and providing a copy to your institution’s administrator.
All other uses, reproduction and distribution, including
without limitation commercial reprints, selling or
licensing copies or access, or posting on open
internet sites, your personal or institution’s website or
repository, are prohibited. For exceptions, permission
may be sought for such use through Elsevier’s
permissions site at:
http://www.elsevier.com/locate/permissionusematerial
From Oyserman, D., 2015. Values, Psychology of. In: James D. Wright (editor-in-chief),
International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, 2nd edition, Vol
25. Oxford: Elsevier. pp. 36–40.
ISBN: 9780080970868
Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Ltd. unless otherwise stated. All rights reserved.
Elsevier
Author's personal copy
Values, Psychology of
Daphna Oyserman, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA
Ó2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Abstract
Values are internalized cognitive structures that guide choices by evoking a sense of basic principles of right and wrong,
a sense of priorities, and a willingness to make meaning and see patterns. Like other cognitive constructs, values can be
studied at the individual level or at the group level. That is societies, cultures, and other social groups have value-based norms,
priorities, and guidelines, which describe what people ought to do if they are to do the right,’‘moral,’‘valuedthing. The
study of values currently focuses more explicitly on the circumstances in which values predict action.
Values: Psychological Perspectives
Values are internalized cognitive structures that guide choices
by evoking a sense of basic principles of right and wrong (e.g.,
moral values), a sense of priorities (e.g., personal achievement
vs group good), and create a willingness to make meaning and
see patterns (e.g., trust vs distrust). Like other cognitive
constructs, values can be studied at the individual level.
However, while it is unusual to discuss attitudes as a group-
level phenomenon, values are amenable to analysis at the
societal or group level. That is societies, cultures, and other
social groups have value-based norms, priorities, and guide-
lines, which describe what people ought to do if they are to do
the right,’‘moral,’‘valuedthing. A society, political party, or
region can be described as having conservative or liberal values,
traditional or progressive values, religious or secular values,
and so on.
Values are supposed to inuence behavior and people are
suspicious of others if they suspect that they hold different
values in part because of the presumed link between values and
behavior. Knowing a persons value system provides a sense
that one also knows what he or she will do in a particular
situation or across situations. In spite of the strong belief that
values predict behavior, the effect of values on behavior is
subject to situational constraints and affordances. That is, if
mobilized or made salient, individual values are linked with
behavior and choices but not necessarily otherwise.
However, if values do not come to mind, they are unlikely
to inuence judgment and behavior. Cultures may be said to
provide concrete and social embodiments of values. Thus, if
cultural values are mobilized or made salient, they also
predict behavior and choices of groups and to some extent, of
individuals within these groups. Whether cultural cues (e.g.,
the national ag) serve to cue relevant values may depend on
whether their appearance in context seems unremarkable or
an obvious attempt to cue a particular value response. Some
kinds of values, those that form the basis of moral judgments,
appear to have a more visceral effect on judgment, inuencing
how people feel about a topic, situation, or choice separately
from their cognitive evaluation of the topic, situation, or
choice. Thus values can cue affective (feeling) as well as
cognitive (thinking) responses. The effect of values on judg-
ments and behavior can be seen whether the value is explicitly
part of the decision-making or not. That is, a value can be
associated with a choice without the association being
made explicit.
Overview
This article addresses the following questions. First, what are
values, how do psychologists study values, and how are values
distinguishable from related concepts motives, goals, and
attitudes? Second, are values best considered at the individual
level as located within individuals, or at the context level, as if
located in social structures, or are both levels of analyses
needed to understand values? Third, what do values imply for
behavior? With regard to the rst question, values are often
studied by asking people to rate how important certain state-
ments are to them or to people in their group. These methods
rely on explicit judgments which may be subject to the same
variety of constraints as all self-report measures (including, for
example, whether people can make explicit judgments about
what they value, and social desirability the desire to say things
that the questioner will approve of). Reliance on self-report
makes values difcult to study and persistent questions arise
as to whether they are real,that is, whether they actually can be
shown to have causal inuence on behavior.
In spite of this difculty, values have intuitive appeal as an
explanatory construct. Much of everyday life is cast in terms of
values, value judgments, or value preferences. Think of ethics,
law, religion, politics, education, art, lifestyle, child rearing, and
more. Choices in these domains are often explained in terms of
values and the value trade-offs one is willing or unwilling to
make. Abstract value judgments are embodied in seeming gut
reactions that something is right, moral, or natural verses
wrong, immoral, or unnatural. Another way to seevalues in
action is to contrast cultures or subcultures in what seems right,
natural, or moral.
Empirically, an emerging literature uses priming techniques
to show that values matter when brought to mind, not other-
wise and that what a value means depends on the context in
which it is considered. Values related to loyalty (e.g., patri-
otism) and to acceptance of authority (e.g., traditionalism), for
example, are associated with more trust in government. This
effect occurs only in contexts in which government functions
brought to mind are relevant to these (e.g., trusting the
government to make the right choices for defense spending)
36 International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, 2nd edition, Volume 25 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.24030-0
International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, Second Edition, 2015, 36–40
Author's personal copy
not otherwise (e.g., trusting the government to make the right
choices for health care). A similar argument has been made for
the role of values in acceptance of social policies, polices that
appear to come from ones own political party are preferred in
part because in-group loyalty is valued.
Another way in which values are studied is by comparison.
Comparisons in choices, behavior, social structures have all
been used to infer differences in values. One of the great
contributions of cultural and cross-cultural research is the way
that it brings Western cultural values into sharp relief by
making these values stand out in comparison to alternative
frames of core values. These and other differences are also
found in comparisons of political parties, social classes, reli-
gions, and even professions. Consider American cultural
values, Americans are said to value life, liberty, and the pursuit
of happiness. But what does valuemean? Implicitly or
explicitly we evaluate or assign value to everything regarding
things as good or bad, a truth or falsity, a virtue or a vice. How
do we know? One important means is through values. Values
can be thought of as priorities, internal compasses, or spring-
boards for action moral imperatives. In this way, values or
mores are implicit or explicit guides for action; they are general
scripts framing what is sought after and what is to be avoided.
Values can be thought of as scaffolded on universal human
needs for security, autonomy, and connection, which would
imply that since all cultures face similar problems they are
likely to overlap in values (though not necessarily in their
ranking of the importance of each value). To sustain a group,
for example, people have to believe that their place within the
group is fairly assigned, that authority is justly applied and not
used in a harmful way.
Each of these is a value judgment, while the need for these
core values may emerge from an evolutionary basis, the relative
importance of each value may emerge from socialization
within the family. Thus, saying that Americans value life,
liberty, and the pursuit of happiness does not necessarily imply
that people from other societies value these things less. Rather
it implies that Americans are likely to consider security,
autonomy, and connection as needing to t together in ways
that maximize autonomy while not undermining security and
connection. Moreover, Americans may differ in the relative
weight that they put on these core values depending on their
social class, political afliation, and profession, because each of
these provides a particular meaning to what connection, secu-
rity, and autonomy mean. Connection can focus on loyalty to
in-group and security can mean purity and sanctity to differing
degrees among different groups. This has been studied both by
cultural and cross-cultural psychologists interested in cultures
of honor and by psychologists interested in morality both
generally and as part of the study of other domains, including
religion, politics, and organizational processes.
Definitions
Modern theories of values are grounded in the work of Kohn
(class and values), Rokeach (general value systems), Merton
(social structure and values), and Kluckhohn (group-level
values). For example, Rokeach denes values as shared
proscriptive or prescriptive beliefs about ideal modes of
behavior and end states (outcomes) what is worth striving
for. Social groups couple these ideals about modes of behavior
and outcomes with regulations of allowable ways to attain
these outcomes. Thus values can focus on both the outcomes
and the procedures or process (behaviors) used to attain the
outcomes. Moral foundations theory (Haidt and Joseph)
builds on this work focusing on moral principles values one
should live by, and bridges from psychological and socio-
logical accounts to anthropological and evolutionary accounts
of morality to understand core values, which they term virtues.
Actualizing ones talents, enjoying oneself, leading
a purposeful life, caring for the next generation are all exam-
ples of operationalization of values into behaviors and
outcomes.
Values can be conceptualized on the individual and group
level. At the individual level, values are internalized social
representations or moral beliefs that people appeal to as the
ultimate rationale for their actions. Though individuals in
a society are likely to differ in the relative importance assigned
to a particular value, values are an internalization of sociocul-
tural goals that provide a means of self-regulation of impulses
that would otherwise bring individuals in conict with the
needs of the groups and structures within which they live. Thus,
discussion of values is intimately tied with social life. At the
group level, values are scripts or cultural ideals held in common
by members of a group; the groupssocial mind.Differences
in these cultural ideals, especially those with a moral compo-
nent, determine and distinguish different social systems. In
this sense Webers Protestant ethicand spiritof capitalism
describe value systems.
Values, to which individuals feel they owe an allegiance as
members of a particular group or society, are seen as the glue
that makes social life possible within groups. Yet, they also set
the stage for frictions and lack of consensual harmony in
intergroup interactions. Values are thus at the heart of the
human enterprise; embedded in social systems, they are what
makes social order both possible and resistant to change.
Values are not simply individual traits, they are social agree-
ments about what is right, good, to be cherished.
What is common to all value phenomena? At the indi-
vidual level, values contain cognitive and affective elements
and have a selective or directional quality; they are internal-
ized. Preference, judgment, and action are commonly
explained in terms of values. Values may be grounded in
evolutionary press to prefer people, contexts, and situations
which do not spread disease or harm, resulting in values of
purity, fairness, caring, loyalty to in-group, and acceptance of
authority that are differentially emphasized across time, place,
and situation. Individuals take on values as part of sociali-
zation into a family, group, and society. Once taken on, values
are assumed relatively xed over time (see Values Across
Cultures, Development of). Indeed, values that are individu-
ally endorsed and highly accessible to the individual do
predict that individuals behavior. Conversely, even person-
ally endorsed values would not inuence action when they are
not made salient to the individual at the time of action.
Moreover, in any given situation more than one personally
endorsed value may apply, and the behavioral choice appro-
priate for one value may conict with the behavioral choice
appropriate to another value.
Values, Psychology of 37
International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, Second Edition, 2015, 36–40
Author's personal copy
Values are codes or general principles guiding action; they
are not the actions themselves nor are specic checklists of what
to do and when to do it. Thus, two societies can both value
achievement but differ tremendously in their norms as to what
to achieve, how to achieve, and when pursuing achievement is
appropriate. Values underlie the sanctions for some behavioral
choices and the rewards for others. A value system presents
what is expected and what is hoped for, what is required and
what is forbidden. It is not a report of actual behavior but
a system of criteria by which behavior is judged and sanctions
applied. Values scaffold likes and dislikes, what feels pleasant
and unpleasant, and what is deemed a success or failure. Values
and value systems are often evoked as rationales for action; for
example, values of freedom and equality were evoked to elicit
American support for the Civil Rights movements. Values differ
from goals in that values provide a general rationale for more
specic goals and motivate attainment of goals through
particular methods.
History and Current Developments
Initially viewed with suspicion by Western social scientists as
too subjective for scientic study, the concept of values found
increasing use beginning with The Polish Peasant in Europe and
America (Thomas and Znaniecki, 1921). Impetus for the study
of cultural values comes from the work of Alfred Kroeber,
Clyde Kluckhohn, Talcott Parsons, Charles Morris,
Robert Redeld, Ralph Linton, Raymond Firth, A.I. Hallowell,
and more currently from Michael Bond, Jonathan Haidt,
Geerdt Hofstede, Ronald Inglehart, Milton Rokeach, and
Shalom Schwartz.
Kluckhohn and Strodtbeck (1961) proposed that cultural
value systems are variations of a set of basic value orientations
that ow from answers to ve basic questions about being.
These are rst, what is human nature evil, neutral, mixed, or
good? Second, how do we relate to nature or supernatural
subjugation, harmony, or mastery? Third, what is the nature
of time past, present, or future? Fourth, what is the nature of
human activity being, being-in-becoming, or doing? And
fth, what is the nature of our relationship to others are we
joined vertically, horizontally or are we simply separate indi-
viduals? Klukhohn and Strodtbeck also organized a system for
comparing values in terms of their level of generalization and
function in discourse and conduct, proposing that values t
into a pyramid of ascending generalization. For each society,
a few central or focal values were proposed to constitute
a mutually interdependent set of what makes for the good life.
These include the unquestioned, self-justifying premises of the
societys value system and denitions of basic and general
value terms; for example, happiness, virtue, beauty, and
morality. The basic questions raised by Klukhohn and Strodt-
beck continue to be studied, for example, by asking if societies
differ in the propensity to accept or value vertical relations in
which power differential is clear.
Since American researchers dominated values research,
much early work focused on documenting American values. As
early as 1944, need for achievement was described as an
important or even a dening American value which was in
decline (Spates, 1983). The idea that American values are in
decline continues to be a hot topic of political debate. Some
relevant research focuses on change over time in narcissism, the
extent that individuals (over) value themselves, and their own
needs and desires compared to others.
Early values studies documented the inuence of education,
age, type of employment, and socioeconomic status on value
preferences of Americans, adding to Webers thesis of the
inuence of religion (Protestantism vs Catholicism) on
achievement and work values in Europe. Kohn (1977) was
responsible for a number of important values surveys doc-
umenting that in various European countries and the
United States, parents of higher socioeconomic status value
self-direction in their children more than parents of lower
educational and occupational levels. These ndings have been
veried cross-nationally in 122 societies. The inuence of
education on values continues to be hotly debated, the concern
being that education carries with it reduced acceptance of
traditional values (e.g., submission to authority, obedience).
However, within the United States, for example, there is no
evidence that political party afliation or religiosity is causally
inuenced by obtaining a college degree.
Extending the documentation of American values,
Rokeach (1973) validated empirically 36 values related to
preferred end states and preferred ways of behaving. Using
Rokeachs scale, value differences tied to class, age, race,
subculture, and level of differences were documented in many
countries. Building on Rokeach, Schwartz (1992) delineates
values as ways of articulating universal requirements of
human existence to survive physically, have social inter-
change, and provide group continuity. For Schwartz, values
represent operationalizations of these needs as goals that t
together in meaningful clusters (achievement, self-direction,
stimulation, hedonism, universalism, benevolence, tradi-
tion, conformity, security, and power). Some clusters are
compatible (e.g., stimulation and hedonism) and others
compete (e.g., self-direction and conformity).
Using mostly data from teachers and college students in
20 primarily Western countries, Schwartz shows that, with the
exception of China, specic values mostly do clusterand
competeas expected. Thus, honest,’‘forgiving,and helpful
cluster together as benevolence,and self-direction,and
stimulationcluster far from conformity,’‘tradition,and
security.These data suggest important universality to how
values are organized cross-culturally and that societies differ in
which clusters of values predominate public life.
More recent analyses, using as its base, all literature pub-
lished between 1980 and 2000 comparing Americans either
with one another or with people from other countries on the
extent that they endorse values of individualism and collec-
tivism suggest that Americans are neither the most individu-
alistic nor the least collectivistic of people (Oyserman et al.,
2002). There was no difference between America and other
English-speaking countries and only relatively small differences
between Europe (Western and Central) and America, with
Americans being higher in individualism. The majority of
available studies (40%) focused on comparisons between East
Asians and Americans, making them the most common
comparison in the cross-national individualism literature.
Combined effect sizes for comparisons with East Asia were
moderate in size, as were combined effect sizes for Africa and
38 Values, Psychology of
International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, Second Edition, 2015, 36–40
Author's personal copy
the Middle East. Whereas these ndings corroborate conven-
tional expectations of cultural theorists, surprisingly the
combined effect size for countries in the other Asiaregion
including India and Pakistan was small. Moreover, no differ-
ence in individualism was found between the United States and
South America, with Americans in the United States being less
individualistic than Latin Americans in South America in more
than half of the country samples tested by Oyserman and her
colleagues. With regard to collectivism, Americans were found
to value collectivism less than were others from all regions of
the world, with the exception of English-speaking countries.
Surprisingly, Americans were lower in collectivism than
Europeans were, betting the idea of a uniquely American way
of being (high individualism and low collectivism) but chal-
lenging the notion of a single Westerncultural set of values. In
addition, effect sizes for Asian regions were similar to those for
European regions, with large differences in valuing collectivism
only when US and African samples were compared. Almost half
of all studies focused on comparisons between East-Asian
regions and America but effects were heterogeneous implying
that simple between group comparisons would not necessarily
yield value differences.
Priming people to consider their cultural values changes the
extent that they feel obligated to act to help others rather than
do their own thing (for a review, Oyserman and Lee, 2008).
Values frameworks have been broadened to include Confucian
values such as valuing relationships, valuing the family as the
model of relationships, valuing a basic benevolence in
engaging with others, and valuing hard work, skill develop-
ment, and thrift (education, industriousness) (Hofstede and
Bond, 1988). Cultural researchers have also shown that
changes in societies produce rapid changes in values as well as
in which personality and social interaction styles are valued.
Emotional expressivity and outgoingness are valued more as
societies shift toward individualistic values.
Controversies
Key tensions in the values literature focus on the conditions
under which they may inuence behavior, and the appropriate
level of analyses for seeing values in action. Interest in values as
a research focus has ebbed in the past as each paradigm for
studying values has been criticized for lack of specicity of
ndings as due to values as distinct from other constructs,
including social norms, attitudes, or situational constraints.
Current cultural psychology focuses attention on social struc-
tures as the repository of values such as personal freedom,
group harmony, personal happiness, and duty or lial piety.
How do we know that values exist? A number of options
are available: (1) Individual testimony people say what
values they hold. Yet, self-reports of values are subject to
pronounced context effects (see Attitudes and Behavior).
(2) Behavioral choices either in naturalistic or laboratory
settings, value differences may be imputed from behavior.
Yet, behavior is inuenced by many variables other than
values. At the individual level, values themselves are
assumedtolinktobehaviorsviatheirinuence on norms
and attitudes, but people may infer their values from their
behavior, reversing the causal relationship. (3) Cultural and
social structures expenditure of resources, time, energy, and
structuring of the natural environment; cultural products
can be seen as concrete residues of value-based choices
(see Cross-Cultural Psychology; Cultural Psychology).
(4) Social interchange observation of behavior in situations
of conict, and more generally observation of what is
rewarded or punished, praised or vilied provides data for
identifying what is socially valued. Here, too, the question of
appropriate evidence arises. To what extent is it appropriate
to assume that differences in social structures and societies
are evidence of value differences? Political and economic
inuences and simple inertia may set the stage for behaviors,
without a causal inuence of necessarily values.
Future Directions
Cross-cultural perspectives are currently becoming increas-
ingly central to values discussion. For example Inglehart
(1990) documented values and value change in a large
multinational study, and thus a large number of two-nation
comparison studies has emerged. Another important topic of
research is the connection between values of individuals,
values of subcultural groups, and values of larger cultural
systems and methods for identifying and studying each of
these. Perhaps in addition to identifying value vocabularies at
each level, it is time to begin to ask whether values appropri-
ately are studied as xed traits of individuals or as embodied in
groups and to what extent values research is synonymous with
cultural and cross-cultural research.
Given that any particular behavior importantly is inuenced
by context effects that make certain information salient at the
moment of action, it is not surprising that the effects of indi-
vidual value endorsements on behavior have a sometimes you
see it, sometimes you dontquality about them. But focusing
on individual endorsement of values may miss much of the
power of value systems to inuence everyday life. That is,
individuals may not need to personally endorse or have salient
particular values in order for their inuence to be felt. The most
profound inuence of values may be through the ways that
they inuence rules, norms, procedures within a society, and in
this way structure the everyday life choices for individuals
within a society.
Thus, whereas previous researchers have documented values
using survey techniques in which individuals rated the extent to
which various values were important to them, future assess-
ment of values may need to consider more indirect approaches
such as what services a society provides its members, what
behaviors are rewarded or sanctioned and so on.
See also: Attitudes and Behavior; Collectivism and
Individualism: Cultural and Psychological Concerns; Critical
Psychology; Cross-Cultural Psychology; Levels of Analysis in
Social Psychology; Social Cognition; Social Psychology; Values
Across Cultures, Development of; Values, Anthropology of;
Values, Sociology of; Vocational Interests, Values, and
Preferences, Psychology of.
Values, Psychology of 39
International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, Second Edition, 2015, 36–40
Author's personal copy
Bibliography
Darley, J., Shultz, T., 1990. Moral rules: their content and acquisition. Annual Review
of Psychology 41, 525556.
Grusec, J.E., 2011. Socialization processes in the family: social and emotional
development. Annual Review of Psychology 62, 243269.
Haidt, J., Joseph, C., Fall, 2004. Intuitive ethics: how innately prepared intuitions
generate culturally variable virtues. Dædalus 5566.
Hofstede, G., Bond, M.H., 1988. The confucius connection: from cultural roots to
economic growth. Organizational Dynamics 16, 521.
Inglehart, R., 1990. Culture Shift in Advanced Industrial Society. Princeton University
Press, Princeton, NJ.
Kluckhohn, F., Strodtbeck, F., 1961. Variations in Value Orientations. Row, Peterson,
Evanston, IL.
Kohn, M., 1977. Class and Conformity. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.
Merton, R.K., 1957. Social Theory and Social Structure. Free Press,
Glencoe, IL.
Oyserman, D., Coon, H.M., Kemmelmeier, M., 2002. Rethinking individualism and
collectivism: evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analyses. Psycho-
logical Bulletin 128, 373.
Oyserman, D., Lee, S.W.S., 2008. Does culture inuence what and how we think? Effects
of priming individualism and collectivism. Psychological Bulletin 134, 311342.
Rokeach, M., 1973. The Nature of Human Values. Free Press, New York.
Schooler, C., 1996. Cultural and social-structural explanations of cross-national
psychological differences. Annual Review of Sociology 22, 323349.
Schwartz, S., 1992. Universals in the content and structure of values: theoretical
advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. Advances in Experimental Social
Psychology 25, 166.
Spates, J., 1983. The sociology of values. Annual Review of Sociology 9, 2749.
Sullivan, J., Transue, J., 1999. The psychological underpinnings of democracy:
a selective review of research on political tolerance, interpersonal trust, and social
capital. Annual Review of Psychology 50, 625650.
Thomas, W., Znaniecki, K., 1921. The Polish Peasant in Europe and America. Chicago
University Press, Chicago.
40 Values, Psychology of
International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, Second Edition, 2015, 36–40
Author's personal copy
... V psychológii je to určitá vlastnosť vnímaných objektov, ktoré signalizujú motivovanému organizmu uspokojenie potreby (Nakonečný, 2009). Oyserman (2015) predstavuje hodnoty na širšom základe, než je uspokojovanie potrieb. Vníma ich ako internalizované kognitívne štruktúry, ktoré usmerňujú výber vyvolávaním pocitu základných princípov správneho a nesprávneho (napr. ...
... Underlying many prejudice reduction strategies include avoidance-oriented values via advancement of end-goals like non-prejudice and antiracism. Internalized through social norms, interactions, or organizational policies (Oyserman, 2015), they dominate the intergroup literature; yet, studies show that behavior guided by avoidant values and goals (e.g., to not discriminate) alone is experienced as impersonal (Plant & Butz, 2006;Plant et al., 2010) and leaves individuals with fewer cognitive resources to intentionally develop positive relationships (Trawalter & Richeson, 2006;Zabel et al., 2015). ...
Chapter
The mission of positive psychology is to explore and build wellbeing in the lives of individuals the world over; however, like all theoretical frameworks, its evolution propels researchers and practitioners into increasingly complex issues. Developments in the field promote a broader view on wellbeing, focusing on culture, religion, social interactions and climate action. Global issues have encouraged the need for a systemic view of wellbeing that is beyond the remit of individuals and extends to organizations, governments and societies alike. In short, the time has come for a global, contextual and systemic understanding of wellbeing. Here, we address the development of these frameworks and three emerging areas as examples of their utility; namely, climate change, egalitarianism as an aid to intergroup relations, and the need for purposeful non-Western views of wellbeing.
... Underlying many prejudice reduction strategies include avoidance-oriented values via advancement of end-goals like non-prejudice and antiracism. Internalized through social norms, interactions, or organizational policies (Oyserman, 2015), they dominate the intergroup literature; yet, studies show that behavior guided by avoidant values and goals (e.g., to not discriminate) alone is experienced as impersonal (Plant & Butz, 2006;Plant et al., 2010) and leaves individuals with fewer cognitive resources to intentionally develop positive relationships (Trawalter & Richeson, 2006;Zabel et al., 2015). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
The mission of positive psychology is to explore and build wellbeing in the lives of individuals the world over; however, like all theoretical frameworks, its evolution propels researchers and practitioners into increasingly complex issues. Developments in the field promote a broader view on wellbeing, focusing on culture, religion, social interactions, and climate action. Global issues have encouraged the need for a systemic view of wellbeing that is beyond the remit of individuals and extends to organizations, governments and societies alike. In short, the time has come for a global, contextual and systemic understanding of wellbeing. Here, we address the development of these frameworks and three emerging areas as examples of their utility; namely, climate change, egalitarianism as an aid to inter-group relations, and the need for purposeful non-Western views of wellbeing.
... Charles est considérée comme une autrice princeps concernant la prise de décision médicale partagée (Edwards & Elwyn, 2009). Son modèle est fréquemment cité dans la littérature sur le sujet et a été adapté à certains contextes médicaux particuliers (Laidsaar-Powell et al., 2016;Légaré, Stacey, Gagnon, et al., 2011;Murray, Charles, & Gafni, 2006 (Oyserman, 2015). ...
Thesis
Introduction : en France, environ 6,6 millions de personnes vivraient avec une maladie rénale et, chaque année, environ 11000 personnes commencent un traitement de suppléance rénale. Les recherches ont montré que la Maladie Rénale Chronique (MRC) peut affecter les patient.e.s mais aussi leur famille. En revanche, l'ajustement à la maladie, notamment avant ou au cours du choix de traitement, a peu été investigué. Le premier objectif de ce travail était d'explorer l'influence des proches sur la santé mentale des patient.e.s et inversement. Le deuxième était d'examiner les facteurs individuels et familiaux en jeu dans l'ajustement psychologique des patient.e.s dans la période de choix de traitement. Enfin, le troisième était d'étudier le rôle de la famille dans le choix de traitement et le vécu de la MRC chez des patient.e.s et des proches. Méthode Au sein de la cohorte de CKD-REIN, 1400 patient.e.s et 430 proches ont participé à notre recherche. Tou.te.s les participant.e.s ont rempli des questionnaires portant sur leurs relations familiales, l'influence des proches sur le choix de traitement, leur assertivité, leur anxiété et leur dépression. Les proches ont également rempli une échelle de représentations de la maladie. Certaines données médicales et sociodémographiques ont également été relevées (e.g., genre, stade de la MRC). Pour répondre à notre premier objectif, nous avons mené des modèles d'équations structurelles selon l'Actor and Partner Interaction Model auprès des 380 dyades patient.e.s-proches éligibles. Pour notre deuxième objectif, nous nous sommes centrée sur les 217 patient.e.s qui avaient assisté à des séances d'éducation thérapeutique sur les traitements. Des régressions multiples, une analyse en composantes principales (ACP) ainsi qu'une classification ascendante hiérarchique nous ont permis d'étudier l'ajustement à la période de choix de traitement. Enfin, pour répondre à notre dernier objectif, 50 patient.e.s et 56 proches ont participé à un entretien semi-directif portant sur leur expérience de la MRC et du choix de traitement. Le discours des participant.e.s a été analysé avec ALCESTE®, un logiciel de statistiques textuelles. Résultats Les résultats relatifs à notre premier objectif montrent que la cohésion familiale ainsi que l'assertivité sont associé.e.s à une moindre symptomatologie dépressive chez les patient.e.s et les proches. La réponse émotionnelle des proches à la MRC est associée aussi bien à leur propre symptomatologie anxieuse qu'à celle des patient.e.s. Le conflit familial est associé à une plus grande anxiété chez les patient.e.s et les proches. Dans notre seconde étude portant sur l'ajustement à la période de choix de traitement, l'ACP confirme le rôle des relations familiales dans la santé mentale des patient.e.s. De plus, le rôle de l'influence des proches sur l'ajustement des patient.e.s dépend du contexte dans lequel elle survient (i.e., assertivité des patient.e.s, qualité des relations familiales). Enfin, concernant notre troisième objectif, l'analyse du discours des patient.e.s pointe comment ils et elles évitent de penser à la MRC. Elle montre également que les réflexions sur la transplantation rénale et la dialyse sont des processus distincts. La famille semble avoir un rôle particulier dans le premier mais pas dans le deuxième. L'analyse du discours des proches rejoint ce constat. Toutefois, bien qu'ils et elles rapportent surtout un rôle d'écoute, les proches expriment des opinions vis-à-vis des traitements. Conclusion Ce travail montre l'intérêt d'étudier et de prendre en charge la santé mentale des patient.e.s et de leurs proches en amont du stade terminal. Ces résultats pointent également comment l'évitement cognitif au sujet de la MRC est important chez les patient.e.s. De plus, nous avons montré le rôle important de la famille dans l'ajustement à la MRC ainsi que dans le choix de traitement. Toutefois, il semble que les participant.e.s n'en aient pas conscience
... Broadly defined, values are conceptions of the desirable (Kluckhorn, 1951). They are general scripts indicating what is to be sought after and what is to be avoided and, from that perspective, they can explicitly or implicitly frame and guide behavior (Oyserman, 2015). Despite their fundamental role in human psychology, values are often disregarded on the grounds that they are too subjective or difficult to measure (Hitlin & Piliavin, 2004). ...
... Broadly defined, values are conceptions of the desirable (Kluckhorn, 1951). They are general scripts indicating what is to be sought after and what is to be avoided and, from that perspective, they can explicitly or implicitly frame and guide behavior (Oyserman, 2015). Despite their fundamental role in human psychology, values are often disregarded on the grounds that they are too subjective or difficult to measure (Hitlin & Piliavin, 2004). ...
Chapter
Full-text available
The purpose of this chapter is to outline a framework of values for Giftedness and Talent Education (GTE). We begin by discussing the functions of education and the issue of purpose in educating the gifted and talented. We then address the meaning of values and their role in guiding research and practice. Our discussion is centered around five pairs of fundamental values: wisdom and truth, beauty and aesthetics, compassion and cooperation, freedom and autonomy, and equity and justice. We conclude by proposing the integration of the different functions of education as a fundamental endeavor for understanding the purpose of education for the gifted and talented.
Article
Full-text available
Background: Work stress places a heavy economic and disease burden on society. Recent technological advances include digital health interventions for helping employees prevent and manage their stress at work effectively. Although such digital solutions come with an array of ethical risks, especially if they involve biomedical big data, the incorporation of employees' values in their design and deployment has been widely overlooked. Objective: To bridge this gap, we used the value sensitive design (VSD) framework to identify relevant values concerning a digital stress management intervention (dSMI) at the workplace, assess how users comprehend these values, and derive specific requirements for an ethics-informed design of dSMIs. VSD is a theoretically grounded framework that front-loads ethics by accounting for values throughout the design process of a technology. Methods: We conducted a literature search to identify relevant values of dSMIs at the workplace. To understand how potential users comprehend these values and derive design requirements, we conducted a web-based study that contained closed and open questions with employees of a Swiss company, allowing both quantitative and qualitative analyses. Results: The values health and well-being, privacy, autonomy, accountability, and identity were identified through our literature search. Statistical analysis of 170 responses from the web-based study revealed that the intention to use and perceived usefulness of a dSMI were moderate to high. Employees' moderate to high health and well-being concerns included worries that a dSMI would not be effective or would even amplify their stress levels. Privacy concerns were also rated on the higher end of the score range, whereas concerns regarding autonomy, accountability, and identity were rated lower. Moreover, a personalized dSMI with a monitoring system involving a machine learning-based analysis of data led to significantly higher privacy (P=.009) and accountability concerns (P=.04) than a dSMI without a monitoring system. In addition, integrability, user-friendliness, and digital independence emerged as novel values from the qualitative analysis of 85 text responses. Conclusions: Although most surveyed employees were willing to use a dSMI at the workplace, there were considerable health and well-being concerns with regard to effectiveness and problem perpetuation. For a minority of employees who value digital independence, a nondigital offer might be more suitable. In terms of the type of dSMI, privacy and accountability concerns must be particularly well addressed if a machine learning-based monitoring component is included. To help mitigate these concerns, we propose specific requirements to support the VSD of a dSMI at the workplace. The results of this work and our research protocol will inform future research on VSD-based interventions and further advance the integration of ethics in digital health.
Article
Full-text available
Every culture espouses distinct moral values systems which serve as the blueprints guiding behaviours of members of a society. Ghana as a country has a rich moral value system that shapes its citizens’ worldviews and informs the way they relate with other human beings. A reflection on, past moral values of Ghanaian societies, however, reveals a moral deficit. This situation throws up the following crucial questions. What is the future of Ghanaian societies given the current state of moral decadence? What can be done to salvage the present state of moral decadence? The paper is approached qualitatively, using interviews, and proposes a significant return to moral virtues where societies will have to teach and ensure that the young ones practice moral values through examples of virtuous living. Keywords: Ghana, Sustainable Future, Moral Decadence, Moral Values, Virtue Ethics.
Chapter
The term values designates a psychological construct that plays an important role in motivating human actions and interactions with the world throughout individuals’ life trajectories. Values operate at both individual and social group levels, acting as a sort of lens for interpreting the world—affecting one’s perception, thoughts, feelings—and as a powerful motivational force for encouraging specific actions. Diverse theoretical perspectives conceive of values differently, focusing on their different roots and qualities. Due to the immense complexity of the psychological processes implied when we refer to values, the traditional approach of psychology has been too limited with most investigations carried out from a cognitivist quantitative methodology that simply measures the frequencies of particular pre-established values in specific populations. Pre-defined categories of fixed and mutually exclusive values, however, cannot actually explain the highly complex, plural, multifaceted, and intricate quality of human motivation. In this entry, some new venues to study and theoretically conceive of the issue will be explored, opening up possible perspectives to the investigation of the ontogenesis of values and their development in specific sociocultural contexts from a systemic cultural approach in psychology. The touchless economy of the pandemic era includes a network of virtual communities in which people participate as they conduct their everyday lives in a low-exposure-risk fashion. To help understand and put these communities in context, this entry describes a typology of virtual communities adapted from Porter (2004). The five elements of the proposed typology include (1) purpose (content of interaction), (2) population (participants in the interaction), (3) platform (design of interaction), (4) place (location of interaction), and (5) profit model (return on interaction). This five-element typology facilitates recognition of essential elements and differences between the many virtual communities that are becoming part of people’s lives in the pandemic era and helps frame and put in context (a) likely new practices and technology applications and (b) new topics for future post-pandemic research in the social, business, and computing sciences. We will probably never go to the Moon, climb Mount Everest, or swim with dolphins. Virtual reality (VR), however, can allow us to do all these things using the simulative power of computers and smartphones. Specifically, what distinguishes VR from other media is the sense of presence: the feeling of “being there” inside the virtual experience produced by the technology. The feeling of presence, associated with the high level of emotional engagement allowed by virtual experiences, turns this technology into a powerful tool for exploring what is possible and engaging with it, supporting personal and clinical change.
Article
Full-text available
Employees can play a decisive role in combatting climate change by engaging in green behavior at work. Research on employee green behavior has recently gained traction, with research results pointing to the considerable influence of positive variables (e.g., personal values, positive affect) on employee green behavior. While such positive variables lie at the heart of the scholarly discipline positive organizational psychology, there is scant research at the intersection of positive organizational psychology and employee green behavior. The current manuscript aims to give impetus to such research. To this end, the manuscript presents a systematic review of the literature on positive predictors of employee green behavior and identified 94 articles that investigate such predictors. We explicitly map these investigated predictors onto a positive (organizational) psychology frame of reference. Subsequently, we use the findings of the review to identify gaps and outline concrete suggestions for future research at the intersection of positive organizational psychology and employee green behavior, addressing both theoretical and methodological suggestions.
Article
This chapter examines cross-national differences in individual values, attitudes, and behaviors. The central question raised is how social-structural and cultural factors account for the differences found. After discussing a series of theoretical issues raised by this question, the chapter reviews the findings of four quantitative sociological research programs on modern cross-national differences. The program on individual modernity led by Alex Inkeles established that social-structural conditions associated with industrialization are linked to an increase in individuals' being open to new experience, rejecting traditional authority, and taking a rational, ambitious, orderly approach to both work and human problems. The cross-national research on the Kohn-Schooler hypothesis that self-directed work increases intellectual functioning and self-directed orientations confirmed the generality of that hypothesis and established that the social status and social class differences in these psychological characte...
Article
The history of the concept of values is reviewed from its beginnings in the 19th century to the present. Work on values rose to preeminence under Talcott Parsons and his associates during 1950-1965, Nevertheless, the theory they produced was flawed: It lacked sophisticated empirical support, imposed preordained categories on reality, and was formulated at an unresearchable level of abstraction. Alternative theories of values fared only somewhat better. More vibrant is the long tradition of (nonParsonian) empirical studies in various subject areas (e.g. achievement, religion). This body of work, however, is ad hoc in nature: The data produced are essentially noncomparable and do not advance the concerns of value theory. The recent work of Kohn (on class and values) and Rokeach (on general value systems) has begun to remedy the situation. It is suggested that value analysis of the future can only solve past ditficulties if (a) it develops grounded theory and research methods—i.e, hypotheses and techniques t...