Conference PaperPDF Available

Does chess instruction enhance mathematical ability in children? A three-group design to control for placebo effects

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Pupils’ poor achievement in mathematics has recently been a concern in many Western countries. In order to address this is- sue, it has been proposed to teach chess in schools. However, in spite of optimistic claims, no convincing evidence of the ac- ademic benefits of chess instruction has ever been provided, because no study has ever controlled for possible placebo ef- fects. In this experimental study, a three-group design (i.e., ex- perimental, placebo, and control groups) was implemented to control for possible placebo effects. Measures of mathematical ability and metacognitive skills were taken before and after the treatment. We were interested in metacognitive skills because they have been claimed to be boosted by chess instruction, in turn positively influencing the enhancement of mathematical ability. The results show that the experimental group (partici- pants attending a chess course) achieved better scores in math- ematics than the placebo group (participants attending a Go course) but not than the control group (participants attending regular school lessons). With regard to metacognition, no dif- ferences were found between the three groups. These results suggest that some chess-related skills generalize to the mathe- matical domain, because the chess lessons compensated for the hours of school lessons lost, whereas the Go lessons did not. However, this transfer does not seem to be mediated by meta- cognitive skills, and thus appears to be too limited to offer ed- ucational advantages.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Does Chess Instruction Enhance Mathematical Ability in Children?
A Three-Group Design to Control for Placebo Effects
Giovanni Sala (giovanni.sala@liv.ac.uk)
Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Liverpool, Bedford Street South, Liverpool, UK
Fernand Gobet (fernand.gobet@liv.ac.uk)
Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Liverpool, Bedford Street South, Liverpool, UK
Roberto Trinchero (roberto.trinchero@unito.it)
Department of Philosophy and Education, University of Turin, Via Gaudenzio Ferrari 9, Turin, ITA
Salvatore Ventura (salvatore.ventura@unimi.it)
University of Milan, ITA
Abstract
Pupils’ poor achievement in mathematics has recently been a
concern in many Western countries. In order to address this is-
sue, it has been proposed to teach chess in schools. However,
in spite of optimistic claims, no convincing evidence of the ac-
ademic benefits of chess instruction has ever been provided,
because no study has ever controlled for possible placebo ef-
fects. In this experimental study, a three-group design (i.e., ex-
perimental, placebo, and control groups) was implemented to
control for possible placebo effects. Measures of mathematical
ability and metacognitive skills were taken before and after the
treatment. We were interested in metacognitive skills because
they have been claimed to be boosted by chess instruction, in
turn positively influencing the enhancement of mathematical
ability. The results show that the experimental group (partici-
pants attending a chess course) achieved better scores in math-
ematics than the placebo group (participants attending a Go
course) but not than the control group (participants attending
regular school lessons). With regard to metacognition, no dif-
ferences were found between the three groups. These results
suggest that some chess-related skills generalize to the mathe-
matical domain, because the chess lessons compensated for the
hours of school lessons lost, whereas the Go lessons did not.
However, this transfer does not seem to be mediated by meta-
cognitive skills, and thus appears to be too limited to offer ed-
ucational advantages.
Keywords: chess; mathematics; transfer; education.
Introduction
Recently, pupils’ poor achievement in mathematics has been
the subject of debate both in the United States (Hanushek,
Peterson & Woessmann, 2012; Richland, Stigler, & Holyoak,
2012) and in Europe (Grek, 2009). Policy makers and re-
searchers have investigated several alternative methods and
activities with the aim of improving the effectiveness of
mathematical teaching. Teaching chess in schools is one of
these activities. Chess has recently become part of the school
curriculum in several countries, and several large studies and
educational projects involving chess are currently ongoing in
Germany, Italy, Spain, Turkey, the United Kingdom, and the
United States. Moreover, the European Parliament has ex-
pressed its positive opinion on using chess courses in schools
as an educational tool (Binev, Attard-Montalto, Deva,
Mauro, & Takkula, 2011).
Chess as Educational Tool: The available Evidence
Several studies have tried to demonstrate the potential bene-
fits of chess training on various cognitive abilities such as at-
tention (Scholz et al., 2008), development of spatial concepts
(Sigirtmac, 2012), general intelligence (Hong & Bart, 2007),
and metacognition (Kazemi, Yektayar, & Abad, 2012). Other
studies focused on academic variables, such as reading and
mathematics (Christiaen & Verhofstadt-Denève, 1981).
Recently, several studies have investigated the positive in-
fluence that chess could exert on children’s mathematical
abilities (Barrett & Fish, 2011; Kazemi et al., 2012; Sala,
Gorini, & Pravettoni, 2015; Scholz et al., 2008; Trinchero,
2012; Trinchero & Sala, in press). Barrett and Fish (2011)
examined the effect of chess instruction on adolescents re-
ceiving special education services, using TAKS (Texas As-
sessment of Knowledge and Skills) scores in mathematics.
The chess-treatment group showed an overall better perfor-
mance than the control group. In Kazemi et al. (2012), the
same pattern occurred with a sample of typically developing
male students from primary and secondary schools. Scholz et
al. (2008) examined the possible benefits of chess instruction
on a sample of primary school children with learning disabil-
ities (IQ between 70 and 85), with disappointing results. Fi-
nally, Sala’s and Trinchero’s studies (Sala, Gorini, & Pra-
vettoni, 2015; Trinchero, 2012; Trinchero & Sala, in press)
focused on the effect of chess instruction on primary school
children’s mathematical problem-solving ability. In these
studies the chess-treatment groups systematically outper-
formed the control groups.
The Lack of a Placebo Group
Following the above overall positive results, the view of the
chess community has been that chess practice increases aca-
demic performance because chess is an intellectually de-
manding and stimulating game (Bart, 2014; Garner, 2012;
Root, 2006). Such optimistic view has been challenged, ante
litteram, by Gobet and Campitelli’s (2006) review of the lit-
Sala, G., Gobet, F., Trinchero, R., & Ventura, S. (2016). Does chess instruction
enhance mathematical ability in children? A three-group design to control for placebo
effects. Proceedings of the 38th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society.
erature regarding the effectiveness of chess instruction in en-
hancing children’s academic and cognitive abilities. Gobet
and Campitelli highlighted that – in spite of some the prom-
ising results reported in the reviewed experiments – the ben-
efits of chess practice were yet to be clearly established, be-
cause of the poor experimental design used in most studies.
In particular, most studies did not control for placebo effects
– that is, effects that are not due to the treatment per se but
that are due to other, uncontrolled aspects of the experimental
design. Potential mechanisms behind placebo effects include
instructors’ motivation, the state of excitement and attention
induced by a novel activity, and teachers’ expectations.
More recently, a meta-analysis (Sala & Gobet, 2016) has
lent further support to Gobet and Campitelli’s conclusions. In
that meta-analysis, 24 studies passed the selection criteria,
with 40 effect sizes. The overall effect size of chess instruc-
tion was moderate, with g = 0.34. It was also found that stud-
ies with at least 25 hours of chess instruction were more likely
to have a positive effect on mathematics, reading, and cogni-
tive abilities (g = 0.43) than studies with less than 25 hours
treatment (g = 0.30). Only one study (Fried & Ginsburg, n.d.),
which was interested in perceptual and visuo-spatial skills,
included a placebo control group. This study showed no dif-
ference between the chess group and the placebo group,
which consisted of attending counselling sessions. Unfortu-
nately, Fried and Ginsburg’s (n.d.) study did not investigate
the effect of chess instruction on children’s mathematical
ability. Therefore, this study cannot corroborate or disprove
any claims about the effectiveness of chess in enhancing
mathematical abilities.
The lack of a placebo control group – i.e., a group attending
other activities non-related to chess – has thus been identified
as the most serious flaw of the previous studies in the field.
These studies often compared the practice of the game of
chess to regular school activities. However, chess practice
may exert a moderate positive influence on children’s aca-
demic just because it is a novel activity in their curriculum
that is fun, and this may induce a state of increased attention
and motivation.
From an educational (and practical) perspective, this might
be irrelevant. Policy makers and educators might be inter-
ested in the effectiveness of chess instruction, regardless of
any chess-specific or non-chess-specific element causing the
improvement in pupils’ academic skills. Assuming this
“whatever works” perspective, the only element of interest
would be the size of the effect – i.e., how much children at-
tending chess courses improve their academic skills, such as
mathematical literacy.
Nonetheless, evaluating whether the benefits of chess prac-
tice on children’s academic skills are due to elements specific
or non-specific to the game of chess is an important theoreti-
cal question. In fact, assuming that skills acquired in chess
lead to benefits in domains such as mathematics clearly im-
plies the presence of far transfer. Far transfer occurs when a
set of skills acquired in one domain (e.g., chess) generalizes
to other domains (e.g., mathematics) that are only loosely re-
lated to the source domain.
The Problem of Transfer in Chess
Since transfer is a function of the extent to which two do-
mains share common features (Thorndike & Woodworth,
1901), far transfer rarely occurs, as shown by substantial re-
search in education and psychology (Donovan, Bransford, &
Pellegrino, 1999; Gobet, 2015a,b). Moreover, in line with
Thorndike and Woodworth’s (1901) hypothesis, several stud-
ies have shown that chess players’ skills tend to be context-
bound, suggesting that it is difficult to achieve far transfer
from chess to mathematics. In her classic study, Chi (1978)
demonstrated that chess players’ (both adults and children)
memory for chess positions did not extend to the recall of
digits. Chess players outperformed non-chess players in re-
membering chess positions, but no difference occurred with
lists of digits. The same result was obtained in a study carried
out by Schneider, Gruber, Gold, and Opwis (1993). Waters,
Gobet, and Leyden (2002) found that chess players’ percep-
tual skills did not generalize to visual memory of shapes.
More recently, Bühren and Frank (2010) found that chess
grandmasters did not outperform chess amateurs in the eco-
nomic game known as beauty contest. Finally, Unterrainer,
Kaller, Leonhart, and Rahm (2011) found that chess players’
planning abilities did not transfer to the Tower of London, a
test assessing executive function and planning skills.
The Present Study
The research on the transferability of chess players’ skills to
other domains offers a complementary perspective to the re-
search on the benefits of chess instruction on children’s aca-
demic (e.g., mathematics) achievement. Whilst the latter has
provided encouraging results, the former has offered results
that seem to preclude any generalizability of chess-specific
skills.
A possible explanation may be that chess instruction shares
with the domain of mathematics some general features, such
as quantitative relationships (e.g., the value of the chess
pieces) and problem-solving situations (e.g., tactics), which
can in turn generalize to mathematics. However, this transfer
can occur only when these skills are at the beginning of their
development, and hence still generalizable. Put simply, chil-
dren studying the four operations may take advantage of an
activity (i.e., chess) dealing with the calculation, for example,
of the pieces’ values. By contrast, it is hard to believe that
knowing advanced chess techniques, such as Lucena’s
method in Rook endgames, might be beneficial for college
students’ skills in calculus or combinatorics. In the former
case, there may be an overlap between the domain of chess
and mathematics, in the latter there is none. This may explain
why the studies dealing with the effect of chess instructions
on children’s mathematical ability have provided positive re-
sults, whilst studies regarding the generalizability of chess
masters’ skills have showed no effect.
An alternative explanation has been suggested by Kazemi
et al. (2012), according to which chess instruction improves
children’s metacognition i.e., the ability to monitor one’s
own cognitive processing (Brown, 1987; Flavell, 1979)
which, in turn, enhance their mathematical ability. Kazemi et
al. (2012) found that youngsters who attended a chess course
outperformed the control group not only in mathematical
ability, but also in metacognitive skills. Once again, unfortu-
nately, this study did not control for possible placebo effects.
In any case, since metacognitive skills have been claimed
to be one of the most important predictors of mathematical
ability (Desoete & Roeyers, 2003; Veenman, Van Hout-
Wolters, & Afflerbach, 2006), and since playing chess is an
activity for which the self-monitoring of one’s thinking pro-
cesses is essential (De Groot, 1965), Kazemi et al.’s (2012)
hypothesis is plausible. Obviously, the null hypothesis – i.e.,
chess instruction has no effect on children’s mathematical
and/or cognitive abilities, and the observed benefits are mar-
ginal and only due to placebo effects – may be valid too.
Since there is a need, in this field of research, to clarify
whether the observed positive influence of chess practice is
due to placebo effects or to chess training itself, we ran a
three-group design study. Beyond the usual experimental and
control groups – attending a chess course and regular school
activities, respectively – an active control group was added to
control for potential placebo effects. Moreover, the partici-
pants of this study were givenalong with a test of mathe-
matical ability – a questionnaire assessing metacognitive
skills, in order to evaluate whether metacognition is the link
connecting chess instruction to the improvements in mathe-
matical ability, as proposed by several authors in the field.
This work thus investigates (a) whether chess instruction
enhances children’s mathematical ability, (b) whether this ef-
fect is mediated by metacognition, and (c) whether the effect
of chess instruction on the above two variables is genuine or
due to placebo effects.
Method
Participants
Fifty-two fourth graders in three classes of a primary school
in Italy took part in this experiment. The mean age of the par-
ticipants was 9.32 years (SD = 0.32). Parental consent was
asked and obtained for all the participants.
Material
A six-item test was designed to test participants’ mathemati-
cal ability (range score 0 – 6). The items used were all from
the Italian version of the IEA-TIMSS international survey
among fourth graders (Mullis & Martin, 2013), a test with
good psychometric properties. These items were chosen be-
cause they measure not only procedural mathematical
knowledge (e.g., 3 + 7 = ?), but also problem-solving ability.
In fact, all the items required solving a mathematical problem
1
Randomizing participants instead of classes is unpractical in
schools, especially when interventions replace part of the curricular
hours. The participants were assigned to the three classes according
to accidental sampling – i.e., the teachers did not adopt any particu-
lar didactic criterion (e.g., the students struggling with math in a par-
ticular class).
starting from a given set of data. An example of such items is
provided in Figure 1.
Figure 1. An example of the items used in the test of mathe-
matical ability.
To assess participants’ metacognitive skills, we used the
Italian version of Panaoura and Philippou’s (2003) question-
naire (15-item version; range score 15 – 75). Participants
were given 45 minutes for completing the battery of tests.
Design
The three classes were randomly
1
assigned to three groups:
One class attended 15 hours of chess lessons during
school hours, along with regular school activities
(experimental group).
One class attended regular school activities only
(control group).
One class attended 15 hours of Go
2
lessons during
school hours, along with regular school activities
(placebo group).
Importantly, the two interventions – i.e., chess and Go
courses replaced part of the hours originally dedicated to
2
Go (or Baduk) is an Asian strategic board game for two players.
The objective of the game is to surround a larger portion of the board
(called Goban) than the opponent. To pursue this aim, the two play-
ers place stones (the game pieces) on the board.
mathematics and sciences. This way, we could confront the
effectiveness of chess (and Go) instruction with the tradi-
tional way of teaching mathematics or mathematics-related
subjects – such as sciences.
The chess and Go lessons followed a pre-arranged teaching
protocol, which consisted of the basic rules of the games, tac-
tical exercises, and playing complete games. All these activ-
ities focused mainly on problem-solving situations, such as
finding the correct move and evaluating the ad-
vantages/weaknesses in a particular position. It must be no-
ticed that the two courses (chess or Go) did not deal with or
introduce any mathematics-related topics, unless these were
part of the respective games (e.g., in chess, a Bishop is worth
three Pawns). That is, the two courses had only chess- and
Go-related contents. In order to rule out possible extraneous
effects related to instructor personality (e.g., Pygmalion ef-
fect), the chess and Go interventions were carried out by the
same instructor, who is an experienced teacher and both a
chess and Go trainer. The tests of mathematical ability and
metacognition were administered twice, once before the be-
ginning of the course and once after the end. The experi-
mental design is summarized in Figure 2.
Figure 2. The experimental design.
Finally, the chess/Go trainer, the teachers, the parents, and
the children involved reported no unpleasant issues. On the
contrary, the people who took part in this project expressed a
positive feedback.
Results
Mathematical Ability
No significant differences between the three groups were
found in the pretest scores (F(2, 51) = 1.030, p = .365). A
univariate analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was used to
evaluate the role of group (independent variable) and mathe-
matics pretest scores (covariate) in affecting mathematics
post-intervention scores (dependent variable).
The results showed a significant effect of the covariate
(F(1, 48) = 21.834, p < .001), and a significant effect of group
(F(2, 48) = 3.371, p = .043). The pairwise comparisons
showed that the control group outperformed the Go group (p
= .017), the chess group marginally outperformed the Go
group (p = .088), whereas no significant difference was found
between the control and the chess group (p = .487). The re-
sults are summarized in Table 1.
Table 1. Mathematical ability scores in the three groups.
Note. Standard deviations are shown in brackets.
Metacognitive Skills
No significant differences between the three groups were
found in the pretest scores (F(2, 51) = 0.487, p = .617). A
univariate analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was used to
evaluate the role of group (independent variable) and meta-
cognition pre test scores (covariate) in affecting metacogni-
tion post-intervention scores (dependent variable).
The results showed a significant effect of the covariate
(F(1, 48) = 47.809, p < .001), and no significant effect of
group (F(2, 48) = 0.367, p = .694). The pairwise comparisons
showed no differences between the three groups. The scores
are summarized in Table 2.
Table 2. Metacognitive-skill scores in the three groups.
Note. Standard deviations are shown in brackets.
Discussion
According to the results presented in this paper, chess seems
to be more effective in enhancing children’s mathematical
skills than Go, but not than regular school activities. This out-
come – which is consistent with the aforementioned reviews
(Gobet & Campitelli, 2006; Sala & Gobet, 2016) – might be
discouraging for researchers and teachers who have upheld
the implementation of chess instruction in school curricula.
However, the fact that the placebo group (Go instruction) un-
derperformed in this experiment – whilst the chess group
equalled the performance of the control groups suggests
that some chess-related skills generalized to the domain of
mathematics (Scholz et al., 2008), and therefore that the ben-
efits of chess instruction, albeit limited, are not only the mere
by-product of placebo effects.
With regard to metacognitive skills, children do not seem
to benefit from any advantage from chess instruction. In fact,
the participants of the three groups performed equally, both
Group Pretest Posttest Adjusted mean
Chess 2.13 (1.26) 2.50 (1.41) 2.30
Go 1.81 (1.08) 1.62 (1.20) 1.63
Control 1.53 (1.13) 2.40 (1.55) 2.60
Group Pretest Posttest Adjusted mean
Chess 55.2 (11.0) 57.0 (10.5) 56.3
Go 52.7 (9.2) 54.8 (8.6) 55.8
Control 55.3 (6.5) 58.3 (6.0) 57.6
in the pretest and in the posttest, suggesting that metacogni-
tion does not represent the cognitive link between chess in-
struction and mathematical ability.
Strengths and Limitations of the Study
In spite of the long history of research on the benefits of chess
instruction on mathematical ability, which started more than
thirty years ago with Christiaen and Verhofstadt-Denève
(1981), the current study is the first to use a placebo group.
In addition to its design, it has several strengths. First, the
same instructor taught the experimental and placebo groups.
Second, the interventions were implemented during school
hours, and replaced part of the lessons dedicated to the teach-
ing of mathematics or mathematics-related subjects (i.e., sci-
ences), in order to compare directly chess and Go instruction
to ordinary school teaching. Finally, the design included both
a cognitive (i.e., metacognition) and an academic (i.e., math-
ematics) variable, in order to search for a possible causal link
between chess instruction and enhancement of children’s
mathematical ability.
The study also suffers from a few weaknesses. The sample
size was relatively small, which affected statistical power.
Randomization was done at the class level rather than the in-
dividual level. (However, as remarked in footnote 1, random-
ization at the individual level is nearly impossible in the con-
text of real schools.) Finally, only one measure of mathemat-
ical ability and metacognitive skills, respectively, was used.
Recommendations for Future Research
The present study supports the hypothesis according to which
chess skill transfers to mathematical ability. Importantly, be-
cause the effect was not observed in the Go condition, this
generalization of chess skill does not depend on placebo ef-
fects. However, this far transfer seems – when it occurs – to
be limited in size, which is in line with substantial previous
research in the field (e.g., Donovan et al., 1999). Further-
more, the results of this study do not corroborate Kazemi’s et
al. (2012) idea that chess instruction fosters children’s math-
ematical ability by enhancing their metacognitive skills.
Given the near-absence of studies controlling for placebo
effects in this line of research, it is essential to replicate and
extend this work. First, since the duration of treatment seems
to be positively related to the effect of chess instruction on
cognitive and academic outcomes (Sala & Gobet, 2016), fu-
ture studies should directly manipulate this variable, in order
to understand the optimal duration of chess courses. Second,
given that the positive effect of chess instruction does not ap-
pear superior to the regular curricular activities, it would be
interesting to compare the effect of chess interventions held
during school hours with the effect of chess interventions
held during extra-curricular hours. Third, there has been little
research that has explicitly tried to teach mathematics using
chess. Possible examples include illustrating the Cartesian
graph with the chess board and introducing the concept of
block distance – as opposed to Euclidean distance – with the
movement of the King. As it is known that awareness makes
transfer more likely (Gick & Holyoak, 1980; Salomon & Per-
kins, 1989), it is plausible that making explicit the links be-
tween chess and mathematics could facilitate transfer. Fi-
nally, other activities could be used with the placebo groups,
such as other board games (e.g., checkers) and music.
The chess-in-school field of research has been nearly ex-
clusively interested in establishing the presence of benefits of
chess instruction for curricular topics (mostly mathematics
and reading) and general cognitive abilities (e.g. intelligence
and creativity). However, very little research has been carried
out on the mechanisms (presumably) leading to such benefits.
A crucial aim for this field of research, then, is to develop a
detailed causal model explaining the cognitive processes that
mediate learning and transfer. With such a model, more pre-
cise hypotheses could be tested than it is currently the case.
Acknowledgments
The authors gratefully thank the principal, the teachers, the
parents, and the children involved in this study.
References
Barrett, D. C., & Fish, W. W. (2011). Our move. Using chess
to improve math achievement for students who receive spe-
cial education services. International Journal of Special
Education, 26, 181–193.
Bart, W. M. (2014). On the effect of chess training on scho-
lastic achievement. Frontiers in Psychology, 5:762.
Binev, S., Attard-Montalto, J., Deva, N., Mauro, M., & Tak-
kula, H. (2011). Declaration of the European Parliament,
0050/2011.
Brown, A. (1987). Metacognition, executive control, self-
regulation and other more mysterious mechanisms. In F. E.
Weinert & R. H. Kluwe (Eds.), Metacognition, motivation
and understanding (pp. 65–116). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bühren, C., & Frank, B. (2010). Chess players’ performance
beyond 64 squares: A case study on the limitations of cog-
nitive abilities transfer. Joint Discussion Paper Series in
Economics, 19-2010.
Chi, M. T. H. (1978). Knowledge structures and memory de-
velopment. In R. S. Siegler (Ed.), Children’s thinking:
What develops? (pp. 73–96). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Christiaen, J., & Verhofstadt-Denève, L. (1981). Schaken en
cognitieve ontwikkeling [Chess and cognitive develop-
ment]. Nederlands Tijdschrift voor de Psychologie en haar
Grensgebieden, 36, 561–582.
De Groot, A. D. (1965). Thought and choice in chess. The
Hague: Mouton.
Desoete, A., & Roeyers, H. (2003). Can off-line metacogni-
tion enhance mathematical problem solving? Journal of
Educational Psychology, 95, 188–200.
Donovan, M. S., Bransford, J. D., & Pellegrino, J. W. (1999).
How people learn: Bridging research and practice. Wash-
ington, DC: National Academies Press.
Flavell, J. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring.
American Psychologist, 34, 906–911.
Fried, S., & Ginsburg, N., (undated). The effect of learning to
play chess on cognitive, perceptual and emotional devel-
opment in children.
Garner, R. (2012). Chess makes a dramatic comeback in pri-
mary schools. Retrieved from: http://www.independ-
ent.co.uk/news/education/education-news/chess-makes-a-
dramatic-comeback-in-primary-schools-8301313.html
Gick, M. L., & Holyoak, K. J. (1980). Analogical problem
solving. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 306–355.
Gobet, F. (2015a). Cognitive aspects of learning in formal
and non-formal contexts: Lessons from expertise research.
British Journal of Educational Psychology, Monograph
Series II: Number 11, Learning beyond the Classroom, 23–
37.
Gobet, F. (2015b). Understanding expertise: A multi-discipli-
nary approach. London: Palgrave.
Gobet, F., & Campitelli, G. (2006). Educational benefits of
chess instruction. A critical review. In T. Redman (Ed.),
Chess and education. Selected essays from the Koltanowski
Conference (pp. 124–143). Dallas, TX: University of
Texas at Dallas.
Grek, S. (2009). Governing by numbers: The PISA ‘effect’ in
Europe. Journal of Education Policy, 24, 23–37.
Hanushek, E. A., Peterson, P. E., & Woessmann, L.
(2012). Achievement growth: International and US state
trends in student performance. Harvard’s Program on Ed-
ucation Policy and Governance.
Hong, S., & Bart, W. M. (2007). Cognitive effects of chess
instruction on students at risk for academic failure. Inter-
national Journal of Special Education, 22, 89–96.
Kazemi, F., Yektayar, M., & Abad, A. M. B. (2012). Investi-
gation the impact of chess play on developing meta-cogni-
tive ability and math problem-solving power of students at
different levels of education. Procedia - Social and Behav-
ioral Sciences, 32, 372–379.
Mullis, I. V. S., & Martin, M. O. (2013). TIMSS 2015 assess-
ment frameworks. Chestnut Hill, MA: TIMSS & PIRLS In-
ternational Study Center, Boston College.
Panaoura, A., & Philippou, G. (2003). The construct validity
of an inventory for the measurement of young pupils’ met-
acognitive abilities in mathematics. Paper presented at the
International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics
Education Conference, Honolulu, HI.
Perkins, D. N., & Salomon, G. (1994). Transfer of learning.
In T. N. Postlethwaite & T. Husen (Eds.), International
Encyclopedia of Education (pp. 6452–6457). Oxford:
Elsevier.
Richland, L. E., Stigler, J. W., & Holyoak, K. J. (2012).
Teaching the conceptual structure of mathematics. Educa-
tional Psychologist, 47, 189–203.
Root, A. W. (2006). Children and chess: A guide for educa-
tors. Westport, Ct: Teacher Ideas Press.
Sala, G., & Gobet, F. (2016). Do the benefits of chess instruc-
tion transfer to academic and cognitive skills? A meta-anal-
ysis. Educational Research Review, 18, 46–57.
Sala, G., Gorini, A., & Pravettoni, G. (2015). Mathematical
problem solving abilities and chess: An experimental study
on young pupils. SAGE Open, July-September, 1–9.
Schneider, W., Gruber, H., Gold, A., & Opwis, K. (1993).
Chess expertise and memory for chess positions in children
and adults. Journal of Experimenal Child Psychology, 56,
328–349.
Scholz, M., Niesch, H., Steffen, O., Ernst, B., Loeffler, M.,
Witruk, E., & Schwarz, H. (2008). Impact of chess training
on mathematics performance and concentration ability of
children with learning disabilities. International Journal of
Special Education, 23, 138–148.
Sigirtmac, A. D. (2012). Does chess training affect concep-
tual development of six-year-old children in Turkey? Early
Child Development and Care, 182, 797–806.
Thorndike, E. L., & Woodworth, R. S. (1901). The influence
of improvement in one mental function upon the efficiency
of other functions. Psychological Review, 9, 374–382.
Trinchero, R. (2012). Gli scacchi, un gioco per crescere. Sei
anni di sperimentazione nella scuola primaria [Chess, a
game to grow up with. Six year of research in primary
school]. Milan: Franco Angeli.
Trinchero, R., & Sala, G. (2016). Can chess training improve
Pisa scores in Mathematics? An experiment in Italian pri-
mary schools. Eurasia Journal of Mathematics, Science &
Technology Education, 12, 655–668.
Unterrainer, J. M., Kaller, C. P., Leonhart, R., & Rahm, B.
(2011). Revising superior planning performance in chess
players: The impact of time restriction and motivation as-
pects. American Journal of Psychology, 124, 213–225.
Veenman, M. V. J., Van Hout-Wolters, B., & Afflerbach, P.
(2006). Metacognition and learning: Conceptual and meth-
odological considerations. Metacognition and Learning, 1,
3–14.
Waters, A. J., Gobet, F., & Leyden, G. (2002). Visuo-spatial
abilities in chess players. British Journal of Psychology,
30, 303–311.
... At the same time, the results of some research conducted in the field document that using chess in the educational system can improve children's academic abilities, especially in mathematics. A systematic review and metaanalyses by Sala (2016) document the positive effects of chess on academic achievement. ...
... However, although some studies document the positive effects of chess learning on children's mathematical and academic abilities, research on the transferability of chess skills suggests some limitations to generalization (Sala et al., 2016). For example, generalization between chess and mathematics, especially in problem-solving and quantitative relationships, may simplify and clarify these findings. ...
Article
Full-text available
This article presents a comprehensive study of the methodological foundations that have been carried out on the selection of intellectual games, especially chess, aimed at developing meta-abilities in older preschool children. This research, conducted in the context of the Republic of Armenia, addresses a significant knowledge gap regarding the development of meta-abilities of preschool children, with particular emphasis on the use of game-based learning methodologies, particularly intellectual games. Based on the work of Bethel-Fox (1992), which clearly distinguishes types of meta-abilities, the study offers a solid methodological framework aimed at optimizing the selection of intellectual games, mainly focusing on the development of personal meta-abilities such as self-orientation and initiative (Brown R. B, 2002). During the research, the gaps and features related to the field were thoroughly studied, and specific methods of their correction were developed in the context of the selection of intellectual games. At the end of the research, theoretical analyses were presented with empirical evidence collected from experimental research in two groups of 5-6-year-olds. The first group comprised 20 children, and the second group comprised 6. The first group was in Hrazdan, and the second was in Yerevan. The article examines in detail the content, implementation strategies, and resulting outcomes of using Chess as a means of building meta-skills.
... Studies have shown that the frontal and posterior parietal areas, which are known to be involved in the orientation of attention, perception and working memory, are engaged in the game of chess [13]. Systematical chess practice develops several important skills in solving mathematical problems [14][15][16], such as maintaining a high level of attention [17] and focusing on tasks [18], perseverance in pursuing goals, creativity [19], recognizing strategic information in situations and using it in planning strategies, critical reflection on one's actions and predicting the course of events [20]. There is a statistically significant correlation between intelligence and chess performance [21][22][23][24][25][26][27], thus the child who excels at a certain school subject has the chance to achieve better performance in chess. ...
... According to Sala [16], experimental design with a control group is not effective. To prevent the placebo effect, a third group is needed that has the status of the one who practices chess. ...
Article
Full-text available
The role of intelligence in chess is crucial because the game involves a situation of adversity between two players whose goal is to checkmate the opponent’s king. Due to the complex nature of the game and the huge amount of information needed to become a professional chess player, the ability to receive, analyze, sort and use abstract notions is essential. A total of 67 children from the third grade were selected and tested twice, initially and finally, to establish the level of body schema and intelligence. The Raven test was used to numerically quantify their intelligence and the Goodenough test was conducted for the body schema. We used the paired samples T-test to highlight the statistical difference between the results and performed a simple linear regression to see if the level of intelligence is a predictor of the body schema. There is a linear relationship between intelligence and body schema, and we can use the first one to predict the evolution of the second. In conclusion, body schema can be educated through chess lessons, and this will lead to better psychomotor development.
... Perversely, game-based learning principles reach more than a dozen domains or parts of learning and are indicated to provide a holistic approach to the learning process to a student and expand from traditional academic disciplines (Plass et al. 2015). which tends to improve fare pattern recognition, decision-making, and other cognitive skills (Sala et al., 2016). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study examines the effect of integrating minigames into chess training on the interest and engagement of new players compared to traditional teaching methods. The results show that the enthusiasm of most new players increased significantly when exposed to the minigames. The experimental group that used the curriculum enriched with minigames showed substantially higher interest than the control group that received regular instruction. Current research supports these findings by highlighting the exciting nature of minigames, making learning more playful and exploratory. Minigames provide a structured yet accessible way to break down the complexity of chess, improve motivation, and promote a positive attitude toward learning. The novelty and variety of minigames encourage continued interest and engagement, potentially reinvigorating chess education and fostering engagement in the chess community. Research of long-term effects on different populations and individual learning preferences is warranted to fully understand the potential of minigames to improve chess training.
... Šachmatų sportas į vaikų ugdymo procesą yra integruojamas įvairiomis formomis daugelyje pasaulio šalių (Sala et al., 2016). Atlikti moksliniai tyrimai rodo, kad vaikų akademiniai pasiekimai yra susiję su mokymusi žaisti šachmatais: pagerėja jų kognityviniai bei socialiniai įgūdžiai. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background. Children who play chess improve their academic results, cognitive and social skills, however, both cognitive and physical abilities are important for healthy development. Recent evidence suggests that physical activity has a positive impact not only on children’s physical abilities but cognitive functions as well. Yet it is not clear the effect of physical activity and chess on children’s physical condition and cognitive functions. The aim. To identify the influence of physical activity and chess sports on children’s physical condition and cognitive functions. Methods. The study involved 30 children (8-9 years). They were equally divided into three groups: chess-playing (CHESS), physical activity and chess-playing (PA+CHESS), and physical activity (PA). The fnger reaching test was used to assess flexibility, 10x5 m shuttlecock test was used to assess dexterity, handgrip strength was measured using the dynamometer, cognitive functions were evaluated using “MoCA-LT” and Bourdon-Anfmov tests. Results. Flexibility and handgrip strength did not differ signifcantly between groups (p>0.05). The dexterity in CHESS group was worse than in CHESS+PA and PA groups (p<0.05). The dexterity results of the CHESS+PA and PA groups did not differ (p>0.05). The cognitive functions of the CHESS+PA group, as assessed by the MoCA test, were better than those of the PA group (p<0.05). No difference was found between other groups (p>0.05). Conclusions. Children who play chess and participate in sport activity have better cognitive functions than children who only do sport. The worst results of dexterity test were in children who play chess only. Keywords: physical condition, cognitive functions, chess sports, children.
... Based on the results of these and similar researches, the use of chess in the curriculum of some countries, including Canada, Sweden, Cuba, Turkey, and America, became formal and sometimes unofficial. Moreover, many research projects were underway to investigate the effects of this game or have been taken place (Sala, Gobet, Trinchero, & Ventura, 2016). For example, in the state of New Brunswick, Canada, there is a textbook called "Challenging Mathematics" in which chess is used to teach mathematics (Isabella, 2006;quoted by Razvani, Fadaie, & Goya, 2014). ...
... In recent studies, it has been reported that teaching chess yields benefits in school 7,9,11,13,14 . These benefits have been detected, particularly in children's math 12,[15][16][17][18][19][20][21] (Table 1) and reading comprehension scores [22][23][24][25] (Table 2), though the effect is not the same between the two subjects. According to a recent meta-analysis evaluating which of the two subject areas (math or reading) is most benefitted, the area most positively impacted was math 26 . ...
Article
Full-text available
In this work, we report the cognitive benefits of playing chess for school-aged children. The most benefitted areas appear to be math and reading. To validate these results, a diversity of scientific studies are described, in which brain activation is demonstrated through magnetic resonance imaging when novice, intermediate, and advance chess players play the game. Given this evidence, it is suggested that chess be used as a tool to improve academic performance in boys and girls. In addition, it is concluded that studying the use of chess could lead to new lines of research that could validate the neural mechanisms that occur when boys and girls play chess.
Article
Full-text available
The placebo effect is the belief that a product is giving someone effective results, but in reality, it is not producing any results. However, in this paper, the placebo effect is related to prestige and how a university with a stronger reputation/brand seems to be more prestigious than other universities. To test this effect, I created a survey that has junior and senior high school students choose what criteria they would want in a university. That criterion was taken into account and in the last question, participants had to choose between two universities, one more prestigious than the other, and explain their choice. The data showed that prestige/familiarity was not as important a factor as I hypothesized. Instead, location and school size were the deciding factors for students.
Article
Full-text available
In accordance with the outcomes from a number of reports, there are cognitive and academic improvements derived from chess learning and chess playing. This evidence, however, endures three key limitations: (a) ignoring theoretical premises about the concept of transfer, (b) several shortcomings regarding ideal experiment guidelines, and (c) an uncritical faith in null hypothesis significance testing (NHST) statistical analyses. The present review scrutinized the NHST outcomes from 45 studies describing chess instruction interventions (n = 12,705) in nineteen countries that targeted cognitive ability (100 tests) and academic performance (108 tests), with a mean Hedge’s effect size g = 572 (95% CI = [0.127, 1.062]). There was a lower average statistical power, a higher proportion of false positive outcomes, larger publication biases, and lower replication rates for the studies in the academic performance domain than in the cognitive ability domain. These findings raised reasonable concerns over the evidence about the benefits of chess instruction, which was particularly problematic regarding academic achievement outcomes. Chess should perhaps be regularly taught, however, regardless of whether it has a direct impact or not in cognitive abilities and academic performance, because these are far transfer targets. The more likely impact of chess on near transfer outcomes from higher quality studies remains at present unexplored.
Article
Full-text available
Introduction: One of the most common childhood disorders considered by psychologists and psychiatrists is Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder, which leads to attention deficit, hyperactivity and impulsivity in affected people. Aim: The purpose of this study was to investigate the impact of Chess training in students with ADHD. Method: The statistical community of the present quasi-experimental study was 76 students with ADHD in Khorramabad city in the academic year of 2019-2020. 32 students were selected by convenience sampling and randomly divided into two groups of 16 controls and experiments based on age, sex and pre-test results. And the experimental group received chess training for 11 consecutive weeks and a weekly session of 60 to 90 minutes. Subjects were evaluated three times (pre-test, post-test, two-week follow-up) with Connors and Swanson questionnaires and data were analyzed by ANOVA and Bonferroni test and Spss 21 software. Results: The results of the study showed a direct and high correlation between the results obtained from both Swanson and Connors measuring instruments and the positive effect of chess training on students with ADHD. (P=0.001, F=20.17). Conclusions: Due to the positive effect of Chess training on the symptoms of ADHD, it is suggested that Chess training be used to reduce the symptoms of ADHD.
Article
Full-text available
The study aimed to analyze the influence of learning chess on mathematical, attention, and concentration abilities in school-aged children of two Schools in Peruvian Amazon. First-grade secondary school students were selected from two schools in Puerto Maldonado city, public and private. The design of the study was quasi-experimental, pretest-posttest evaluations and acontrol group. Each group had between 18-30 students, and the experimental group was given chess classes twice a week for three months. To analyze the effect of the intervention a Generalized Linear Model was used. In the two abilities evaluated, the posttest results of the experimental group were higher than the control group. A significant effect of the application of sports chess was found in the development of abilities in mathematical reasoning, attention, and concentration (P< 0.001). In the public school, the effect of the program on mathematical reasoning was higher in boys than in girls (P< 0.05). Our results provide relevant information for schools to develop strategies to promote chess as a teaching strategy. We concluded that the implementation of a chess program influences the development of the intellectual abilitiesin school-aged children of Peruvian Amazon.
Article
Full-text available
What are the cognitive mechanisms involved in learning in environments beyond the formal context of the classroom? A possible answer to this question lies in the body of literature relating to the nature of expertise. This is arguably a good starting point, as most of this research has studied individuals who have acquired their expertise outside of a classroom environment. This paper addresses the role of practice, feedback, strategies and memory in acquiring expert performance. It considers the extent to which we can apply an understanding of expert behaviour to support beginners and intermediates in formal and non-formal contexts. While there are differences between the processes and behaviours required to attain expertise compared to less intensive forms of learning, there are also important commonalities. Finally, a discussion on the principles based on radical constructivism and situated learning in the light of expertise research shows that, while these approaches correctly emphasise the role of learning by doing and of the social environment, they may be limited by ignoring the role of practice and of teachers’ guidance.
Article
Full-text available
In recent years, pupils' poor achievement in mathematics has been a concern in many Western countries. Chess instruction has been proposed as one way to remedy this state of affairs, as well as improving other academic topics such as reading and general cognitive abilities such as intelligence. The aim of this paper is to quantitatively evaluate the available empirical evidence that skills acquired during chess instruction in schools positively transfer to mathematics, reading and general cognitive skills. The selection criteria were satisfied by 24 studies (40 effect sizes), with 2,788 young people in the chess condition and 2,433 in the control groups. The results show (a) a moderate overall effect size (g = 0.338); (b) a tendency for a stronger effect on mathematical (g = 0.382) than reading skill (g = 0.248), and (c) a significant and positive effect of duration of treatment (Q(1) = 3.89, b = 0.0038, p < .05). However, no study used an "ideal design" including pre- and post-test, full random allocation of participants to conditions and, most importantly, both a do-nothing control group and an active control group - a problem common in education research. Directions for further research are discussed.
Book
Full-text available
What makes an expert? What strategies do they use? If you are an expert in one domain, are you more likely to become an expert in a second? This book provides a comprehensive overview of the field of expertise. With a discussion of research from psychology, neuroscience, sociology, philosophy, education, law and artificial intelligence, this is the definitive guide to the subject. It considers expertise on a number of levels, ranging from the neural to the psychological and the social. It critically evaluates current theories and approaches, and addresses issues of key importance for society.
Article
Full-text available
Chess is thought to be a game demanding high cognitive abilities to be played well. Although many studies proved the link between mastery in chess and high degree of intelligence, just few studies proved that chess practice can enhance cognitive abilities. Starting from these considerations, the main purpose of the present research was to investigate the potential benefits of in-presence chess lessons and on-line training on mathematical problem-solving ability in young pupils (8 to 11 years old). Five hundred sixty students were divided into two groups, experimental (which had chess course and on-line training) and control (which had normal school activities), and tested on their mathematical and chess abilities. Results show a strong correlation between chess and math scores, and a higher improvement in math in the experimental group compared with the control group. These results foster the hypothesis that even a short-time practice of chess in children can be a useful tool to enhance their mathematical abilities.
Book
This book helps educators and librarians prepare students to succeed in University Interscholastic League (UIL) Chess Puzzle. Children and Chess: A Guide for Educatorsis the first book to show the connection between accepted educational theories and chess. It features lesson plans teachers can use immediately, and from which they can learn the basics of the game. Since the plans meet academic goals through chess, teachers also learn that chess can be a part of reading, math, science, and social studies. An appendix showing how chess meets the requirements of curriculum standards is another plus. Children and Chess: A Guide for Educatorsis the first book to show the connection between accepted educational theories and chess. The relationship of chess to academic and humanistic educational goals is convincingly illustrated as curriculum and psychological theories from John D. McNeil, Mihaly Csikszentmihalyi, and Howard Gardner are outlined and applied to the question why chess?Children and Chessfeatures lesson plans teachers can use immediately, and from which they can learn the basics of the game. Since the plans meet academic goals through chess, teachers also learn that chess can be a part of reading, math, science, and social studies. An appendix showing how chess meets the requirements of curriculum standards is another plus. Grades 4-8.
Book
What does a chess master think when he prepares his next move? How are his thoughts organized? Which methods and strategies does he use by solving his problem of choice? To answer these questions, the author did a study, to which famous chess masters participated (Alekhine, Max Euwe, Reuben Fine, Tartakower and Flohr). This book is still useful for everybody who studies cognition and artificial intelligence. The studies involve participants of all chess backgrounds, from amateurs to masters. They investigate the cognitive requirements and the thought processes involved in moving a chess piece. The participants were usually required to solve a given chess problem correctly under the supervision of an experimenter and represent their thought-processes vocally so that they could be recorded. De Groot found that much of what is important in choosing a move occurs during the first few seconds of exposure to a new position. Four stages in the task of choosing the next move were noted. The first stage was the 'orientation phase', in which the subject assessed the situation and determined a general idea of what to do next. The second stage, the 'exploration phase' was manifested by looking at some branches of the game tree. The third stage, or 'investigation phase' resulted in the subject choosing a probable best move. Finally, in the fourth stage, the 'proof phase', saw the subject confirming with him/herself that the results of the investigation were valid. De Groot concurred with Alfred Binet that visual memory and visual perception are important and that problem-solving ability is of paramount importance. Memory is particularly important, according to de Groot (1965), in that there are no 'new' moves in chess and so those from personal experience or from the experience of others can be committed to memory. © 1965, Mouton Publishers, The Hague, The Netherlands. All right reserved.
Article
The aim of this study is to evaluate the benefit of chess in mathematics lessons for children with learning disabilities based on lower intelligence (IQ 70-85). School classes of four German schools for children with learning disabilities were randomly assigned to receive one hour of chess lesson instead of one hour of regular mathematics lessons per week for the duration of one school-year. Concentration and calculation abilities of children were measured before and after the year of study using standardised tests. The chess group was compared with the control group without chess lessons. Concentration abilities and calculation abilities for written tasks and gap tasks developed equally well in both groups. Calculation abilities for simple addition tasks and counting improved significantly more in the chess classes. We conclude that chess could be a valuable learning aid for children with learning disabilities. Transfer of chess lessons to improvement of basic mathematics skills has been observed.
Article
This causal-comparative study evaluated a 30-week chess instructional program implemented within special education math classes for students in the sixth, seventh, and eighth grades in a suburban middle school located in the southwestern United States. An analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was utilized to compare the adjusted means for the comparison and treatment groups on the students' math achievement as measured by end-of-year course grades and state assessment scores, the Texas Assessment of Knowledge and Skills (TAKS). Pretest scores and grade levels served as covariates. Results indicated a significant difference on four of the measures in favor of the treatment group: end-of-year course grades, overall TAKS math scale scores, and percentage scores on two specific TAKS math objectives: Numbers, Operations, and Quantitative Reasoning and Probability and Statistics. No significant differences were found between the groups on the other four TAKS math objectives: Patterns, Relationships, and Algebraic Reasoning, Geometry and Spatial Reasoning, Concepts and Uses of Measurement, and Underlying Processes and Mathematical Tools. Causation and generalizability are difficult due to the narrow scope of this study. However, these results are encouraging and suggest chess is a potentially effective instructional tool for students who receive special education services in math.