ArticlePDF Available

Are puréed foods justified for infants of 6 months? What does the evidence tell us?

Authors:

Abstract

Current guidance recommends that infants be introduced to solid foods from 6 months, an age at which chewing skills are rapidly developing. However, there is a general assumption that the first solid foods should be offered as purees. This article considers the rationale for this practice and presents evidence that, far from being either necessary or beneficial, insistence on pureed foods may contribute to feeding and health problems.
Are pureed foods justified for infants of six months?
What does the evidence tell us?
Gill Rapley, PhD
This document is the Accepted Manuscript version of a Published Work that appeared in final form in the Journal
of Health Visiting in June 2016, copyright © MA Healthcare, after peer review and technical editing by the
publisher. To access the final edited and published work see [http://www.magonlinelibrary.com/toc/johv/current].
Key Points
No need for pureed foods for infants of six months
No evidence to support the use of pureed foods at six months
The use of pureed foods may predispose to feeding problems
The use of pureed foods may predispose to health problems
Key Words
Infant nutrition physiology
Complementary feeding
Child nutrition
Childhood eating and feeding disorders
Are pureed foods justified for infants of six months?
What does the evidence tell us?
Abstract
Nowadays it is recommended that infants be introduced to solid foods from six months, an
age at which chewing skills are rapidly developing. And yet there is a general assumption
that the first solid foods should offered as purees. This article considers the rationale for this
practice and presents evidence that, far from being either necessary or beneficial, insistence
on pureed foods may contribute to feeding and health problems.
Introduction
The current worldwide recommendation regarding infant feeding is that infants should be
exclusively breastfed for the first six months of life. They should then be introduced gradually
to a nutritious diet composed of a variety of complementary foods, with breastfeeding
continuing for at least the first two years (WHO, 2002; WHO 2005; WHO/UNICEF, 2003).
This extended transition, which is also known as weaning, is recognised as a crucial period
for the development of life-long healthy eating habits (Oddy et al, 2003; Rich-Edwards et al,
2004; Owen et al, 2005; Horta et al, 2007; Ip et al, 2007; Quigley et al, 2007; Duijts et al,
2010; Li et al, 2014).
In practice, the age at which weaning begins varies across time and cultures, from less than
one month old to as late as two years (Dettwyler, 1987; Bentley et al, 1991; Jarosz, 1993).
Yet, whatever the age of the infant, the expectation in industrialised societies is that the first
complementary foods will be offered as purees. Thus, in 2010, although 25% of UK infants
did not experience ‘solid’ food until five months or older, only 4% were offered this food as
something they could pick up with their fingers (McAndrew et al, 2012). But where is the
evidence for this practice?
The evidence for the use of purees
An extensive search of the academic literature revealed many papers dealing with the timing
of complementary feeding (e.g. (Butte et al, 2002; Kramer and Kakuma, 2012) and the need
to offer a variety of flavours from the outset (e.g. Gerrish and Mennella, 2001; Maier et al,
2008; Mennella et al, 2008). The majority of these papers make only a passing reference to
the fact that the first foods were pureed, and offer no justification for this choice even
where the infants concerned are nearer six months than four. A smaller number of studies
has been concerned with the age at which ‘lumpy’ foods should be introduced (e.g.
Northstone et al, 2001) but, again, the initial use of purees seems not to have been
questioned. Other than recent papers concerned with ‘baby-led weaning’ (BLW), none were
found that challenged this practice and none that provided support for it. On the contrary, a
wider search of the literature revealed compelling evidence against offering pureed foods.
This evidence is related to three key areas: the requirements for the acquisition of chewing
skills, the role of food format and presentation in the development of food preferences, and
the effects of pureeing on food.
The development of chewing
One rationale commonly offered for the use of purees is that the infant has to ‘learn to chew’.
Indeed, there is an implication that pureed foods are necessary for this learning to take
place. However, this belief is not borne out by the literature. The average full-term newborn
infant has innate reflexes that enable her/him to attach and suckle at the breast or on a
bottle teat, drawing in liquid food and swallowing it. By four months the position of her/his
larynx allows more space within the oral cavity (Delaney and Arvedson, 2008), s/he can
‘mouth’ foods and is beginning to lose the innate tongue protrusion (or ‘tongue thrust’) reflex,
which spontaneously expels non-liquids. However, s/he is still not able to bite or chew. If
s/he is to be given foods other than breastmilk or formula, it follows that they must be in
liquid or semi-liquid form, so that the infant can use an adapted sucking movement to deal
with them.
By six months the situation is very different. At this age the infant has developed a range of
oral motor skills that allow her/him to manage foods that require chewing (Wickenden, 2000;
Naylor, 2001). The ‘tongue thrust’ reflex has disappeared and the infant is able to ‘munch’
food with an up-and-down movement of the lower jaw. In addition, s/he is beginning to use
her/his tongue in a lateral manner, enabling her/him to move food to the sides of the mouth
for chewing (Naylor, 2001). Between six and nine months the infant becomes able to collect
together a bolus of food on the tongue and move it deliberately to the back of the mouth for
swallowing. At the same time, s/he develops the ability to swallow independently of the
sucking movement (Naylor, 2001). What is striking is that these rudimentary biting, chewing
and swallowing skills do not need to be learnt; they appear as part of the infant’s natural
development whether or not s/he has prior experience with semi-solid foods (Naylor, 2001).
It is worth noting that the ability to make chewing movements is independent, not only of
experience with purees but also of the presence of teeth. Although the first teeth commonly
appear at around six months the age when chewing movements are beginning these
are incisors, which are for biting, not chewing. Premolars and molars do not normally appear
until after the first birthday. In the absence of teeth, anecdotal evidence suggests that gums
are effective for chewing all but the toughest foods. In a study by Carruth and Skinner
(2002), the mean age for being able to chew firmer foods was ten and a half months, while
that for the eruption of the first molar was 15 months. By 20 months, all the children were
reportedly able to chew and swallow firmer foods, even though some still had no cheek
teeth.
Infants who are beginning to bite and chew require repeated opportunities to practise these
skills in order to become fully proficient in them (Illingworth and Lister, 1964; Archambault et
al, 1990; Northstone et al, 2001). Delaney (2010) found that experience accelerated the
development of chewing skills over and above what would be expected through maturation
alone, and showed that infants as young as eight months were able to manage a range of
textures more effectively than was previously thought to be the norm. She questioned the
accepted wisdom that experience with purees predisposes children to learn other oral skills,
suggesting that it may even delay the acquisition of those skills.
The development of food preferences
There is a wealth of research into the development of food preferences in infancy. We know,
for example, that infants who breastfeed tend to be more accepting of different flavours
during the weaning period than infants fed on standard infant formula (Forestell and
Mennella, 2007; Maier et al, 2008; Beauchamp and Mennella, 2009; Shim et al, 2011). In
addition, several studies (e.g., Maier et al, 2007; Mennella et al, 2008) have shown that,
when infants are repeatedly fed an individual flavour, they acquire an apparent liking for that
flavour. However, in spite of the known link between taste and texture in the perception of
flavour (Ganchrow and Mennella, 2003), the role of texture in food acceptance has rarely
been studied in children under one year, and never in relation to the first non-milk foods.
Northstone et al (2001), Coulthard et al (2009) and Northstone and Emmett (2013) examined
the impact of the timing of the move from purees to lumpier textures on the range of foods
children were willing to eat at 15 months, two years and seven years, but the possibility that
the need for this move could be eliminated, through the use of chewable foods from the
outset, has not been explored.
The potential importance of the visual attributes of the first ‘solid’ foods has, similarly, not
been examined. Much of the research into the development of food preferences focuses on
the number of times an infant or child is ‘exposed’ to a new food. However, the nature of the
‘exposure’ varies according to the focus of the study and the ages of the participants: In
utero, exposure occurs passively, via the smell and taste of the mother’s amniotic fluid
(Cooke and Fildes, 2011). For the young infant, ‘exposure’ means the active introduction of a
few drops of liquid, a flavoured drink, or a ‘bite’ of (semi-solid) food directly into her/his
mouth, via a bottle teat or spoon (e.g., Desor et al, 1973; Crook, 1978; Harris et al, 1990;
Mennella et al, 2008; Mennella et al, 2009), an experience that comprises taste, intra-oral
texture and that element of smell which is experienced inside the mouth. In research into the
responses of older children the term ‘exposure’ is used variously to mean opportunities to
see, smell and/or taste food before deciding whether to eat it (Sullivan and Birch, 1990;
Wardle et al, 2003; Williams et al, 2008; Hausner et al, 2012). There appear to be no
grounds for assuming that such dissimilar experiences are equally influential for the child
and yet they are discussed as if they are interchangeable (Cooke, 2007).
Food neophobia, or the reluctance to eat unfamiliar foods, is generally accepted as a normal
phenomenon during the pre-school years, emerging as the child becomes mobile, peaking
soon after the second birthday, and gradually dwindling thereafter (Cashdan, 1998; Cooke et
al, 2003; Nicklaus, 2009). To children in this age group, the appearance of a food (especially
its colour and shape) is key to whether they perceive it as new, whether or not they are in
fact already familiar with its taste (Carruth et al, 1998; Zeinstra et al, 2010). In studies of
children in the two-to-five-year age group the importance to the child of visual inspection is
tacitly acknowledged, in that the food is usually presented on a plate for her/him to eat or
not, as s/he chooses (Carruth et al, 1998; Dovey et al, 2008; Zeinstra et al, 2010). However,
with a few exceptions (e.g. Mennella et al, 2008), studies involving younger infants appear to
side-step the possible impact of the food’s appearance, both by presenting it on a spoon
directly to the mouth (Maier et al, 2008), thereby effectively preventing the infant from
examining it, and by offering it as a puree a format that offers few visual clues to its
composition.
Dovey et al (2008) explain the gradual decline in neophobia by the fact that the older a child
gets the fewer foods are new to them. If this reasoning is applied to the period during which
neophobia is increasing it follows that a change in the appearance of previously familiar
foods making them appear ‘new’ – has the potential to exacerbate the problem. Thus,
while one or more ‘blind’ intra-oral exposures to a pureed food may indeed make an infant
more likely to accept that food when it is presented in the same way on a subsequent
occasion (Maier et al, 2007; Mennella et al, 2008), there is no guarantee that s/he will be
equally willing to eat it if given the chance to inspect it nor that s/he will accept it in a
different format.
We do not know at what age the appearance of food may start to be important but the work
of Wright et al (2011) and Brown and Lee (2011) suggests that the establishment of links
between the taste, texture and appearance of food may be possible for infants as young as
six months. It follows that the beginning of weaning may be a crucial period not only for the
acceptance of new flavours but for building a store of visual references with which new foods
can be compared. It may be no coincidence that the period during which neophobia is
increasing commonly coincides with the move away from pureed foods towards food
presented in the same way as eaten by the rest of the family. Infants who are familiar with a
pureed food whether via flavour, appearance or both may need to begin the
familiarisation process afresh when ‘table’ foods are introduced.
‘Picky’ eaters commonly reject food on the basis of its appearance (Werthmann et al, 2015).
Not only may a puree itself deter the child visually, pureeing also reduces the diversity of
appearance that exists naturally between foods. This can lead to inaccurate deductions
about the nature of the food. Thus, an infant may reach excitedly for an orange-coloured
puree, only to discover that (being mango instead of carrot) it tastes very different from the
orange-coloured puree of yesterday. It is not difficult to see how children may decide to
reject, for example, all green foods, based on an unpleasant experience with one green
food. Some picky eaters are especially resistant to dishes in which several foods are mixed
together (Carruth et al, 2004a). This may be because mixing makes recognition of the
contents, whether by appearance, smell, taste or texture, even more difficult. Alternatively, it
may reflect a wariness induced by the common parental ‘trick’ of concealing disliked foods
within liked ones. As well as masking the nature of the constituents, combining foods into a
homogenous whole has the potential to hamper the operation of ‘sensory specific satiety
(Rolls et al, 1981), which allows the individual to become sated in relation to one food while
remaining willing to eat a different food something that is thought to be important for
ensuring a balanced intake of nutrients. The common practice of presenting infants with
several foods as a single puree may therefore have implications for health, as well as for
food acceptance.
Modelling, by parents or peers, has been shown to be effective in persuading children to eat
healthily (Brown and Ogden, 2004; Greenhalgh et al, 2009; Palfreyman et al, 2014), with
children as young as one year being positively influenced by others eating what appear to be
identical foods (Addessi et al, 2005; Shutts et al 2009). However this, too, relies on visual
clues that are denied to the infant who is presented with all her/his food in pureed form.
Werthmann et al (2015) found that children aged between 32 and 48 months were more
influenced by changes to the texture of a liked food than by alterations to either colour or
taste but how early such changes may begin to be important is not known. It is possible that
problems reportedly caused by the late introduction of lumpy foods (Northstone et al, 2001)
may be as much due to their unfamiliar appearance as to physical eating challenges
presented by their texture. All of this raises questions not only about the best time to
introduce table foods but about the wisdom of introducing foods as purees in the first place,
when complementary feeding begins at six months.
The effects of pureeing
Pureeing alters not only the appearance of food but also its cellular structure, thereby
affecting its textural properties and potentially increasing its viscosity (Waldron et al, 2003).
Viscous foods tend to adhere to the lining of the mouth, which may make them difficult to
manage intra-orally (Lucas et al, 2004). Delaney (2010) found evidence that purees are not
necessarily easily managed by infants and that their skills in this regard do not increase in
the way that those related to more solid textures do. She notes her surprise at these
findings, given the fact that “this texture is thought to be easy to chew” (Delaney, 2010: 71).
Pureeing affects the intensity and nature of the flavour of food, by allowing the dispersal, in
advance, of volatile elements that would normally be released gradually while chewing. It
therefore interferes with the way the individual experiences flavour (Wilson and Brown,
1997). A homogenous texture and the lack of a need to chew further detract from the
appreciation of flavour (Ganchrow and Mennella, 2003; Waldron et al, 2003) and preclude
the stimulation of other senses, thereby limiting the pleasure to be gained from eating
(Duizer, 2001; Spence and Shankar, 2010; Woods et al, 2010). Since enjoyment of eating
has been shown to be an important factor in children’s food choices (Skafida, 2013), the fact
that pureeing may reduce the potential for an enjoyable eating experience warrants
consideration.
Commercially prepared weaning foods may present an additional problem. As well as
lacking an equivalent nutrient value (Nazanin et al, 2011; Wright et al, 2011; Garcia et al,
2013), they rarely taste the same as home-cooked (pureed) versions of the same food
because of the effects of the processing to which they are subjected (Oey et al, 2008;
Kocadağli and Gökmen, 2014). This may result in them being difficult to identify by taste,
even for adults (Birch et al, 1998). Both Harris and Booth (2006) and Forestell and Mennella
(2007) have noted that it is possible that experience with commercial infant foods may hinder
infants’ acceptance of home-cooked foods, both pureed and ‘whole’.
Infants have been shown to be capable of regulating their own intake of food (Fox et al,
2006; Li et al, 2010; DiSantis et al, 2011; Brown and Lee, 2012). However, it is possible that
this ability may be disrupted if all the infant’s food is pureed. Chewing has been shown to
play an important role in appetite suppression in adults (French and Cecil, 2001; de Graaf,
2012), whereas runny foods are known to promote rapid swallowing (Gisel, 1991). In
particular, food that can be swallowed quickly makes it easier for a caregiver to override the
infant’s natural appetite control. Rapid transit of food through the mouth is the antithesis of
‘mindful eating’, which involves paying attention to the totality of the experience (Bays, 2009)
and allows the individual to become attuned to internal cues of hunger and satiety. Mindful
eating is recommended as a way for adults to avoid overeating (Wansink and Sobal, 2007;
Wansink, 2009; Beshara et al, 2013). It is possible that the encouragement of mindless
eating in infancy may be behind the apparently spontaneous reduction in intrinsic appetite
control seen in the preschool years (Rolls et al, 2000; Smith et al, 2013), through what Fox
et al have termed “interference with natural self-regulation” (Fox et al, 2006: S81).
As well as encouraging excessive intake, rapid oral processing of food may also impede
effective digestion, since it reduces the time available for the food to mix with saliva (Pocock
et al, 2013). It also allows less time for the appreciation of flavours (Zijlstra et al, 2009), albeit
that this may be considered an advantage if the food is disliked. Finally, over-reliance on soft
foods is thought to contribute to poor facial growth (Lieberman et al, 2004) and an increased
risk of dental, respiratory and aural problems (Corruccini, 1999; Kiliaridis, 2006;
Montgomery-Downs et al, 2007; Gibbons, 2012).
The argument for purees
In support of the use of purees, Reeves states that they are “ideal for being able to introduce
a known volume of food to the infant and provide essential nutrients” (Reeves, 2008: 108)
and that a diet containing both finger foods and purees “is the most likely to provide the
variety of foods and nutrients that a developing infant requires.” (Reeves, 2008: 109). This is
a view echoed by Caroli et al (2012). However, caregivers have yet to be shown to know
better than infants what their individual nutrient needs are at any one meal. Although Clara
Davis’s work from the 1920s (Davis, 1928) has been robustly challenged (Story and Brown,
1987; Strauss, 2006), no published studies exist that have disproved her claim that infants
and children are capable of choosing a balanced diet for themselves.
Reeves also asserts that “purees are an obvious transition food to bridge the gap between
liquid and solid foods” (Reeves, 2008: 108). Yet there appears to be no evidence that the
infant’s transition from a liquid diet to one that includes chewable foods requires her/him to
be offered foods that are themselves transitional in consistency. Stevenson and Allaire
(1991) have commented that the case for semi-solids is not proven but is based on child-
rearing beliefs and customs, while Reilly et al (2006) point out that many non-industrialised
cultures do not use ‘weaning foods’. Palmer (2011) notes that the belief that soft foods are
necessary may be perpetuated in part by the baby food industry, which has an interest in
encouraging parents to use commercially prepared products.
In a direct challenge to the belief that semi-solid foods are needed, the approach to the
introduction of solid foods known as ‘baby-led weaning’ (BLW) argues that the weaning
period need consist only of an overlap of liquid and chewable foods, during which the
chewable foods form a gradually increasing proportion of the diet (Brown and Lee, 2011;
Cameron et al, 2012), and that the changeover can be adequately negotiated by the infant
himself, as he becomes more proficient at chewing (Rapley, 2013). The available evidence
suggests that self-feeding with graspable foods is a viable alternative to the use of purees for
infants beginning weaning at six months (Wright et al, 2011; Cameron et al, 2012).
Discussion and conclusions
In recent years the majority of the research surrounding the period of complementary
feeding has focused on the timing of the introduction of new foods, based on perceived
nutritional requirements, the development of the gut and immune systems and, more
recently, the risk of allergy. The infant’s developing oral-motor abilities have largely been
ignored, with the result that the emphasis has been on the foods themselves and the
acceptance of new flavours, rather than on matching the consistency of the first foods to the
current eating skills of the infant. A search of the literature revealed no evidence to support
the routine use of pureed foods for normally developing healthy infants of six months.
Rather, the available research suggests that an insistence on purees at the beginning of
weaning may have negative consequences:
Purees may be more difficult to manage for infants who are beginning to be able to
chew than are chewable foods. They may also impede the development of more
advanced oral-motor skills.
Pureeing reduces the opportunity for infants to learn to recognise foods by their
appearance and to trust their ability to do so. This may lead to wariness, both with
previously experienced foods, with the same foods in other formats, and with new
foods, potentially contributing to picky eating and neophobia. It also reduces the
opportunities for parents to model the eating choices they wish their offspring to make.
Pureeing deprives infants of the opportunity to experience different textures and may
cause distortion of flavours. Either of these effects may impact on the enjoyment of
meals, either at the time, or when chewable foods are eventually introduced.
Overuse of purees may have implications for health, both in infancy and later. Food
that can be swallowed quickly may contribute to excessive weight gain and less
efficient digestion, while persistent lack of chewing movements may contribute to poor
facial growth, and thence to adverse dental, respiratory and aural health outcomes.
Perhaps more worrying is the possibility that the routine use of purees in infancy may have
distorted our learning about the feeding of infants. For example, although not generally
acknowledged, the focus of research into the early development of food preferences has
been on infants’ reactions to pureed foods, rather than on their responses to food per se.
Thus, Coulthard et al (2014), on finding that infants introduced to solid foods after five and a
half months ate less of a novel puree than did those who had started solids before this age,
concluded that infants are less accepting of new foods when the start of weaning occurs at
or after six months than when it occurs earlier. An alternative explanation may be that six-
month-olds are simply less accepting than younger infants of new foods presented as
purees. Similarly, the work of Delaney (2010) raises questions about the accuracy of our
understanding of the normal development of oral-motor skills, since the majority of what we
know has been accumulated by studying infants who are being, or have been given, pureed
foods.
In summary, if purees are not necessary, and may even be harmful, we need urgently to re-
assess their routine use for infants who do not need them.
References
Addessi E, Chiarotti F, Visalberghi E, Anzenberger G (2007) Response to novel food and the
role of social influences in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) and Goeldi's monkeys
(Callimico goeldii). American Journal of Primatology 69(11): 1210-1222
Archambault M, Millen K, and Gisel EG (1990) Effect of bite size on eating development in
normal children 6 months to 2 years of age. Physical & Occupational Therapy in Pediatrics
10: 29-47
Bays JC (2009) Mindful Eating: A Guide to Rediscovering a Healthy and Joyful Relationship
with Food. Shambhala Publications, Inc., Boston MA
Beauchamp GK, Mennella JA (2009) Early flavour learning and its impact on later feeding
behavior. Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition 48(1, Suppl): S25-S30
Bentley ME, Dickin KL, Mebrahtu S et al (1991) Development of a nutritionally adequate and
culturally appropriate weaning food in Kwara state, Nigeria: An interdisciplinary approach.
Social Science & Medicine 33(10): 1103-1111
Beshara M, Hutchinson AD, Wilson C (2013) Does mindfulness matter? Everyday
mindfulness, mindful eating and self-reported serving size of energy dense foods among a
sample of South Australian adults. Appetite 67: 25-29
Birch LL, Gunder L, Grimm-Thomas K, Laing DG (1998) Infants' consumption of a new food
enhances acceptance of similar foods. Appetite 30(3): 283-295
Brown A, Lee M (2011) A descriptive study investigating the use and nature of baby-led
weaning in a UK sample of mothers. Maternal & Child Nutrition 7(1): 34-47
Brown A, Lee M (2012) Breastfeeding during the first year promotes satiety responsiveness
in children aged 18-24 months. Pediatric Obesity 7: 382-390
Brown R, Ogden J (2004) Children's eating attitudes and behaviour: A study of the modelling
and control theories of parental influence. Health Education Research 19(3): 261-271
Butte NF, Lopez-Alarcon MG, Garza C (2002) Nutrient Adequacy of Exclusive Breastfeeding
for the Term Infant During the First Six Months of Life. WHO, Geneva
Cameron SL, Heath A-LM, Taylor RW (2012) How feasible is baby-led weaning as an
approach to infant feeding? A review of the evidence. Nutrients 4: 1575-1609
Caroli M, Mele RM, Tomaselli MA, Cammisa M, Longo F, Attolini E (2012) Complementary
feeding patterns in Europe with a special focus on Italy. Nutrition, Metabolism and
Cardiovascular Diseases 22(10): 813-818
Carruth, BR, Skinner JD (2002) Feeding behaviors and other motor development in healthy
children (2-24 months). Journal of the American College of Nutrition 21(2): 88-96
Carruth BR, Skinner J, Houck K, Moran J, Coletta F, Ott D (1998) The phenomenon of “picky
eater”: A behavioral marker in eating patterns of toddlers. Journal of the American College of
Nutrition 17(2): 180186
Carruth BR, Ziegler PJ, Gordon A, Barr SI (2004) Prevalence of picky eaters among infants
and toddlers and their caregivers’ decisions about offering a new food. Journal of the
American Dietetic Association 104(1): S57-S64
Cashdan E (1998) Adaptiveness of food learning and food aversions in children. Social
Science Information 37(4): 613-632
Cooke L (2007) The importance of exposure for healthy eating in childhood: A review.
Journal of Human Nutrition and Dietetics 20: 294-301
Cooke L, Fildes A (2011) The impact of flavour exposure in utero and during milk feeding on
food acceptance at weaning and beyond. Appetite 57(3): 808-811
Cooke L, Wardle J, Gibson EL (2003) Relationship between parental report of food
neophobia and everyday food consumption in 2-6-year-old children. Appetite 41: 205-6
Corruccini RS (1999) How Anthropology Informs the Orthodontic Diagnosis of
Malocclusions’ Causes. Edwin Mellen Press, Lewiston, NY
Coulthard H, Harris G, Emmett P (2009) Delayed introduction of lumpy foods to children
during the complementary feeding period affects child’s food acceptance and feeding at 7
years of age. Maternal & Child Nutrition 5: 75-85
Coulthard H, Harris G, Fogel A (2014) Exposure to vegetable variety in infants weaned at
different ages. Appetite, 78: 89-94
Crook CK (1978) Taste perception in the newborn infant. Infant Behavior & Development 1:
52-69
Davis CM (1928) Self-selection of diet by newly weaned infants: An experimental study.
American Journal of Diseases in Childhood 36(4): 651-679
de Graaf C (2012) Texture and satiation: The role of oro-sensory exposure time. Physiology
& Behavior 107(4): 496-501
Delaney AL (2010) Oral-motor Movement Patterns in Feeding Development. PhD
(Communicative Disorders). University of Wisconsin-Madison. Available online: http://www.
researchgate.net/publication/236849815_Oral-Motor_Movement_Patterns_in_
Feeding_Development
Delaney AL, Arvedson JC (2008) Development of swallowing and feeding: Prenatal through
first year of life. Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews 14(2): 105-117
Desor JA, Maller O, Turner R (1973) Taste in acceptance of sugars by human infants.
Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 84: 496-501
Dettwyler KA (1987) Breastfeeding and weaning in Mali: Cultural context and hard data.
Social Science & Medicine 24(8): 633-644
DiSantis KI, Collins BN, Fisher JO, Davey A (2011) Do infants fed directly from the breast
have improved appetite regulation and slower growth during early childhood compared with
infants fed from a bottle? International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity 8:
89. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-8-89
Dovey TM, Staples PA, Gibson EL, Halford JCG (2008) Food neophobia and ‘picky/fussy’
eating in children: A review. Appetite 50: 181-193
Duijts L, Jaddoe VWV, Hofman A, Moll HA (2010) Prolonged and exclusive breastfeeding
reduces the risk of infectious diseases in infancy. Pediatrics 126(1): e18-e25. doi:
10.1542/peds.2008-3256
Duizer L (2001) A review of acoustic research for studying the sensory perception of crisp,
crunchy and crackly textures. Trends in Food Science & Technology 12(1): 17-24
Forestell CA, Mennella JA (2007) Early determinants of fruit and vegetable acceptance.
Pediatrics 120: 1247-1254
Fox MK, Devaney B, Reidy K, Razafindrakoto C, Ziegler P (2006) Relationship between
portion size and energy intake among infants and toddlers: Evidence of self-regulation.
Journal of the American Dietetic Association 106(1, Suppl): S77-S83
French SJ, Cecil JE (2001) Oral, gastric and intestinal influences on human feeding.
Physiology & Behavior 74(4-5): 729-734
Ganchrow JR, Mennella JA (2003) The ontogeny of flavour perception. In: Doty RL, ed.
Handbook of Olfaction and Gustation (Neurological Disease and Therapy Series. 2nd edn.
Marcel Dekker, New York: 823-846
Garcia AL, Raza S, Parrett A, Wright CM (2013) Nutritional content of infant commercial
weaning foods in the UK. Archives of Disease in Childhood 98: 793-797
Gerrish CJ, Mennella JA (2001) Flavor variety enhances food acceptance in formula-fed
infants. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 73: 1080-1085
Gibbons A (2012) An evolutionary theory of dentistry. Science 336: 973-975
Gisel EG (1991) Effect of food texture on the development of chewing of children between
six months and two years of age. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 33: 69-79
Greenhalgh J, Dowey AJ, Horne PJ, Lowe CF, Griffiths JH, Whitaker CJ (2009) Positive and
negative peer modelling effects on young children's consumption of novel blue foods.
Appetite 52(3): 646-653
Harris G, Booth IW (2006) The nature and management of eating problems in pre-school
children. In: Cooper PJ, Stein A, eds. Childhood Feeding Problems and Adolescent Eating
Disorders. Routledge, Hove, UK: 92-122
Harris G, Thomas A, Booth DA (1990) Development of salt taste in infancy. Developmental
Psychology 26(4): 534-538
Hausner H, Olsen A, Moller P (2012) Mere exposure and flavour-flavour learning increase 2-
3 year-old children’s acceptance of a novel vegetable. Appetite 58: 1152-1159
Horta BL, Bahl R, Martines JC, Victora CG (2007) Evidence on the Long-term Effects of
Breastfeeding: Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses. WHO, Geneva
Illingworth RS, Lister J (1964) The critical or sensitive period, with special reference to
certain feeding problems in infants and children. Journal of Pediatrics 65(6): 839-848
Ip S, Chung M, Raman G et al (2007) Breastfeeding and maternal and infant health
outcomes in developed countries. Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality Evidence
Report/Technology Assessment no. 153: 1-186
Jarosz LA (1993) Liberian practices in feeding infants water, breastmilk and first food.
Ecology of Food and Nutrition 30(3-4): 221-240
Kiliaridis S (2006) The Importance of masticatory muscle function in dentofacial growth.
Seminars in Orthodontics 12(2): 110-119
Kocadağli T, Gökmen V (2014) Investigation of α-dicarbonyl compounds in baby foods by
high-performance liquid chromatography coupled with electrospray ionization mass
spectrometry. Journal of Agricultural and Food Chemistry 62(31): 7714-7720
Kramer MS, Kakuma R (2012) Optimal duration of exclusive breastfeeding (Review). The
Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 2012, Issue 8
Li R, Fein SB, Grummer-Strawn LM (2010) Do infants fed from bottles lack self-regulation of
milk intake compared with directly breastfed infants? Pediatrics 125: 1386-1393
Li R, Dee D, Li CM, Hoffman HJ, Grummer-Strawn LM (2014) Breastfeeding is associated
with reduced risk of infections at age 6 years. Pediatrics 134(Suppl 1): S13-S20
Lieberman DE, Krovitz GE, Yates FW, Devlin M, St Claire M (2004) Effects of food
processing on masticatory strain and craniofacial growth in a retrognathic face. Journal of
Human Evolution 46: 655-677
Lucas PW, Prinz JF, Agrawal KR, Bruce IC (2004) Food texture and its effect on ingestion,
mastication and swallowing. Journal of Texture Studies 35(2): 159-170
Maier A, Chabanet C, Schaal B, Issanchou S, Leathwood P (2007) Effects of repeated
exposure on acceptance of initially disliked vegetables in 7-month old infants. Food Quality
and Preference 18(8): 1023-1032
Maier A, Chabanet C, Schaal B, Leathwood P, Issanchou S (2008) Breastfeeding and
experience with variety early in weaning increase infants’ acceptance of new foods for up to
two months. Clinical Nutrition 27: 849-857
McAndrew F, Thompson J, Fellows L, Large A, Speed M, Renfrew MJ (2012) Infant Feeding
Survey 2010. Health and Social Care Information Centre, London
Mennella JA, Nicklaus S, Jagolino AL, Yourshaw LM (2008) Variety is the spice of life:
Strategies for promoting fruit and vegetable acceptance during infancy. Physiology &
Behavior 94(1): 29-38
Mennella JA, Forestell CA, Morgan LK, Beauchamp GK (2009) Early milk feeding influences
taste acceptance and liking during infancy. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 90(Suppl):
780S-188S
Montgomery-Downs HE, Crabtree VM, Capdevila OS, Gozal D (2007) Infant-feeding
methods and childhood sleep-disordered breathing. Pediatrics 120(5): 1030-1035
Naylor AJ (2001) Infant oral motor development in relation to the duration of exclusive
breastfeeding. In: Naylor AJ, Morrow A, eds. Developmental Readiness of Normal Full Term
Infants to Progress from Exclusive Breastfeeding to the Introduction of Complementary
Foods: Reviews of the Relevant Literature Concerning Infant Immunologic, Gastrointestinal,
Oral Motor and Maternal Reproductive and Lactational Development. Wellstart International
and the LINKAGES Project Academy for Educational Development, Washington DC: 21-26
Nazanin Z, Chowdry BZ, Zotor FB, Wray DS, Amuna P, Pullen FS (2011) Essential and
trace elements content of commercial infant foods in the UK. Food Chemistry 128(1): 123-
128
Nicklaus S (2009) Development of food variety in children. Appetite 52: 253-255
Northstone K, Emmett P (2013) The associations between feeding difficulties and
behaviours and dietary patterns at 2 years of age: The ALSPAC cohort. Maternal & Child
Nutrition 9(4): 533-542
Northstone K, Emmett P, Nethersole F, the ALSPAC Study Team (2001) The effect of age of
introduction to lumpy solids on foods eaten and reported feeding difficulties at 6 and 15
months. Journal of Human Nutrition and Dietetics 14(1): 43-54
Oddy WH, Sly PD, de Klerk NH et al (2003) Breast feeding and respiratory morbidity in
infancy: A birth cohort study. Archives of Disease in Childhood 88: 224-228
Oey I, Lille M, Van Loey A, Hendrickx M (2008) Effect of high-pressure processing on colour,
texture and flavour of fruit- and vegetable-based food products: A review. Trends in Food
Science & Technology 19(6): 320-328
Owen CG, Martin RM, Whincup PH, Smith GD, Cook DG (2005) Effect of infant feeding on
the risk of obesity across the life course: A quantitative review of published evidence.
Pediatrics 115(5): 1367-1377
Palfreyman Z, Haycraft E, Meyer C (2014) Development of the Parental Modelling of Eating
Behaviours Scale (PARM): Links with food intake among children and their mothers.
Maternal & Child Nutrition 10(4): 617-629
Palmer G (2011) Complementary Feeding: Nutrition, culture and politics. Pinter and Martin,
London
Pocock G, Richards CD, Richards DA (2013) Human Physiology. 4th edn. Oxford University
Press, Oxford
Quigley MA, Kelly YJ, Sacker A (2007) Breastfeeding and hospitalization for diarrheal and
respiratory infection in the United Kingdom Millennium Cohort Study. Pediatrics 119: e837-
e842
Rapley G (2013) Baby-led weaning: A developmental approach to the introduction of
complementary foods. In: Moran VM, ed. Maternal and Infant Nutrition and Nurture:
Controversies and Challenges. 2nd edn. Quay Books, London: 261-283
Reeves S (2008) Baby-led weaning. British Nutrition Foundation Nutrition Bulletin 33: 108-
110
Reilly S, Skuse D, Wolke D (2006) The nature and consequences of feeding problems in
infancy. In: Cooper PJ, Stein A, eds. Childhood Feeding Problems and Adolescent Eating
Disorders. Routledge, Hove, UK: 7-40
Rich-Edwards JW, Stampfer MJ, Manson JE et al (2004) Breastfeeding during infancy and
the risk of cardiovascular disease in adulthood. Epidemiology 15(5): 550-556
Rolls BJ, Rowe EA, Rolls ET, Kingston B, Megson A, Gunary R (1981) Variety in a meal
enhances food intake in man. Physiology & Behavior 26(2): 215-221
Rolls BJ, Engell D, Birch LL (2000) Serving portion size influences 5-year-old but not 3-year-
old children’s food intakes. Journal of the American Dietetic Association 100: 232-234
Shim JE, Kim J, Mathai RA, the STRONG Kids Research Team (2011) Associations of infant
feeding practices and picky eating behaviors of preschool children. Journal of the American
Dietetic Association 111(9): 1363-1368
Shutts K, Kinzler KD, McKee CB, Spelke ES (2009) Social information guides infants'
selection of foods. Journal of Cognition and Development 10(1-2): 1-17
Skafida V (2013) The family meal panacea: Exploring how different aspects of family meal
occurrence, meal habits and meal enjoyment relate to young children’s diets. Sociology of
Health & Illness 35(6): 906-923
Smith L, Conroy K, Wen H, Rui L, Humphries D (2013) Portion size variably affects food
intake of 6-year-old and 4-year-old children in Kunming, China. Appetite 69: 31-38
Spence C, Shankar MU (2010) The influence of auditory cues on the perception of, and
responses to, food and drink. Journal of Sensory Studies 25(3): 406-430
Stevenson RD, Allaire JH (1991) The development of normal feeding and swallowing.
Pediatric Clinics of North America 38(6): 1439-1453
Story M, Brown JE (1987) Do young children instinctively know what to eat? The studies of
Clara Davis revisited. The New England Journal of Medicine 316(2): 103-106
Strauss S (2006) Clara M. Davis and the wisdom of letting children choose their own diets.
Canadian Medical Association Journal 175(10): 1199-1202
Sullivan SA, Birch LL (1990) Pass the sugar, pass the salt: Experience dictates preference.
Developmental Psychology 26(4): 546-551
Waldron KW, Parker ML, Smith AC (2003) Plant cell walls and food quality. Comprehensive
Reviews in Food Science and Food Safety 2: 128-146
Wansink B. (2009) Mindless Eating, Hay House, London
Wansink B, Sobal J (2007) ‘Mindless eating: The 200 daily food decisions we overlook.
Environment and Behavior 39(1): 106-123
Wardle J, Herrera M-L, Cooke L, Gibson EL (2003) Modifying children’s food preferences:
The effects of exposure and reward on acceptance of an unfamiliar vegetable. European
Journal of Clinical Nutrition 57: 341-348
Werthmann J, Jansen A, Havermans R, Nederkoorn C, Kremers S, Roefs A (2015) Bits and
pieces. Food texture influences food acceptance in young children. Appetite 84: 181-187
WHO: World Health Organization (2002) Complementary Feeding: Report of the Global
Consultation convened jointly by the Department of Child and Adolescent Health and
Development and the Department of Nutrition for Health and Development. WHO, Geneva
WHO: World Health Organization (2005) Guiding principles for feeding non-breastfed
children 6-24 months of age. WHO, Geneva
WHO/UNICEF: World Health Organization/UNICEF (2003) Global Strategy for Infant and
Young Child Feeding. WHO, Geneva
Wickenden M (2000) The development and disruption of feeding skills: How speech and
language therapists can help. In: Southall A, Schwartz A, eds. Feeding Problems in
Children. Radcliffe Medical Press, Abingdon, Oxon: 3-24
Williams KE, Paul C, Pizzo B, Riegel K (2008) Practice does make perfect. A longitudinal
look at repeated taste exposure. Appetite 51: 739-742
Wilson CE, Brown WE (1997) Influence of food matrix structure and oral breakdown during
mastication on temporal perception of flavor. Journal of Sensory Studies 12(1): 69-86
Woods AT, Poliakoff E, Lloyd DM et al (2010) Effect of background noise on food
perception. Food Quality and Preference 22(1): 42-47
Wright CM, Cameron K, Tsiaka M, Parkinson KN (2011) Is baby-led weaning feasible?
When do babies first reach out for and eat finger foods? Maternal & Child Nutrition 7(1): 27-
33
Zeinstra CG, Koelen MA, Kok FJ, de Graaf C (2010) The influence of preparation method on
children’s liking for vegetables. Food Quality and Preference 21(8): 906-914
Zijlstra N, de Wijk RA, Mars M, Stafleu A, de Graaf C (2009) Effect of bite size and oral
processing time of a semi-solid on satiation. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 90: 269-
275
... The parents' knowledge and behaviors concerning their children's nutrition are strongly influenced by their educational level (Blissett & Haycraft, 2008;Rapley, 2016). Studies show that educational level can influence how mothers feed their children (Blissett & Haycraft, 2008), because they may be best informed about the introduction of foods. ...
... Blissett & Haycraft, 2008;Sausenthaler et al., 2007), as they may have more access to information about the introduction of foods. Educational level and family income can influence the attitudes and practices around the eating behaviours of children (Rapley, 2016). Indeed, a study carried out in Bangladesh showed that mothers with no formal education presented a higher risk of missing the correct timing for complementary food introduction (Kabir et al., 2012). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study analyses the eating routines and habits of Portuguese children aged 4 to 18 months, the methods of food introduction most frequently used, and the parents’ perceptions regarding the importance of food introduction for speech development. A sample of 297 parents filled out an online self-report questionnaire about their perceptions, knowledge, and routines regarding food introduction methods, and their relationship with speech development. This questionnaire showed high reliability (α=0.86). The majority of the Portuguese mothers that filled out the survey answered either that they breastfed their children or breastfed them in combination with bottle feeding. They also stated that they introduce new food to their children in the form of soups or purees and that they vary the flavors, textures, and consistencies frequently. Although the results showed a reasonable level of knowledge regarding the traditional and Baby Led Weaning (BLW) methods, there was a higher frequency of use of the traditional method in this sample when compared to BLW. There was not a significant correlation between the age of the parents and their perception of the importance of feeding for speech. In turn, parents with a higher level of education showed a greater awareness of the relevance of feeding in the development of speech. Parents with a higher number of children showed less awareness of the relationship between feeding and speech development.
... There is a general assumption that the giving of pureed food to infants by spoon is evidence-based but it is difficult to find justification for this view. Rather, it seems that it has simply become common practice in industrialised societies (Rapley, 2016a(Rapley, , 2016b. And yet neither the consistency of the food nor the means by which it finds its way to the infant's mouth has been considered as a confounding variable, even in the most recent studies relating to the introduction of solid foods. ...
Article
Full-text available
The weaning period, in which complementary foods are introduced into the infant’s diet, can be challenging, with dislike of novel foods known to be common. However, even though at six months (the current recommended age for the introduction of complementary foods) most infants are able to feed themselves with graspable pieces of food, the majority of the research to date assumes the use of spoon feeding and pureed food. The potential impact of the format of the food and the feeding method on the development of food preferences has not been explored. This paper describes the development and piloting of a coding scheme to examine infants’ responses to their first solid food offered in two formats – whole and pureed – and by two methods – self-feeding and spoon feeding, and its first use. The findings for the 10 infants in the study point to some important differences between the two feeding methods.
Article
Baby-led weaning (BLW), proposed as a new form of complementary feeding, has emerged as a real trend phenomenon in the media. Infants are seated at the family table from the age of 6 months, facing the foods they grab and bring to their mouth: they decide which foods they want to eat and what amount. The consumption of mashed foods and the use of a spoon are totally discouraged. BLW is increasingly used in nurseries and centers of young children. A bibliographic search carried out between 2000 and 2021 found 423 articles, of which 38 were selected. The clinical studies selected are 11 cross-sectional observational studies and two randomized controlled studies. BLW promotes breastfeeding, the early introduction of morsels, the respect of the child's appetite, the use of unprocessed foods, and the choice of “homemade” and friendliness. These benefits can nonetheless be reached with usual complementary feeding (SCF), according to current recommendations. Other benefits are claimed without scientific evidence such as easier achievement of dietary complementary feeding and an optimal growth with prevention of excess weight gain. BLW has some obvious downsides. The infant may not get enough energy, iron, zinc, vitamins, and other nutrients, or too much protein, saturated fat, salt, or sugar. The risk of choking, which must be distinguished from the physiological gagging reflex, has not been ruled out by scientific studies. Currently, the Nutrition Committee of the French Pediatric Society considers that the data published to date in terms of benefits and risks of BLW do not lend themselves to advice for this practice in preference over SCF carried out according to current recommendations.
Article
Full-text available
The current recommended age to start complementary feeding is 6 months. At this age, most infants are capable of picking up pieces of food and taking them to their mouth; and yet there is an expectation that spoon-feeding will feature—in the early weeks, at least—in the transition to solid foods. This article looks at the rationale for this practice and presents evidence that, far from being necessary, spoon-feeding may contribute to feeding and health problems.
Article
Objectives: To monitor infant's gross, fine and oral motor development patterns related to feeding. Design: An incomplete block design was used with 57 to 60 (sample = 98) mothers interviewed when their children were 2, 3, 4, 6, 8, 10, 12, 16 and 24 months (within +/- 5 days of birth date). Each mother had 5 to 6 interviews. Setting: Selected developmental feeding behaviors were monitored using in-home interviews conducted by trained interviewers (n = 2). At each interview, mothers reported the child's age when behaviors first occurred, and anthropometric measurements were performed. Subjects: Subjects were healthy white children who lived mostly in homes with educated two-parent families of upper socioeconomic status. Results: Mean behavioral ages were within normal ranges reported in the literature, whereas individuals exhibited a wide diversity in reported ages. Examples of gross motor skills (age in months, +/- SD) included sitting without help (5.50 +/- 2.08) and crawling (8.00 +/- 1.55). Mean ages for self-feeding fine motor skills showed children reaching for a spoon when hungry (5.47 +/- 1.44), using fingers to rake food toward self (8.87 +/- 2.58) and using fingers to self-feed soft foods ( 13.52 +/- 2.83). Oral behaviors included children opening their mouth when food approached (4.46 +/- 1.37), eating food with tiny lumps (8.70 +/- 2.03) and chewing and swallowing firmer foods without choking (12.17 +/- 2.29). Conclusions: Mean ages for feeding behaviors occurred within expected age ranges associated with normal development. However, mothers reported that individual children exhibited a wide age range for achieving these behaviors. Our results should be considered in counseling mothers about infant feeding practices.
Article
Changes in the technology of food preparation over the last few thousand years (especially cooking, softening, and grinding) are hypothesized to have contributed to smaller facial size in humans because of less growth in response to strains generated by chewing softer, more processed food. While there is considerable comparative evidence to support this idea, most experimental tests of this hypothesis have been on non-human primates or other very prognathic mammals (rodents, swine) raised on hard versus very soft (nearly liquid) diets. Here, we examine facial growth and in vivo strains generated in response to raw/dried foods versus cooked foods in a retrognathic mammal, the rock hyrax (Procavia capensis). The results indicate that the hyrax cranium resembles the non-human primate cranium in having a steep gradient of strains from the occlusal to orbital regions, but differs from most non-anthropoids in being primarily twisted; the hyrax mandible is bent both vertically and laterally. In general, higher strains, as much as two-fold at some sites, are generated by masticating raw versus cooked food. Hyraxes raised on cooked food had significantly less growth (approximately 10%) in the ventral (inferior) and posterior portions of the face, where strains are highest, resembling many of the differences evident between humans raised on highly processed versus less processed diets. The results support the hypothesis that food processing techniques have led to decreased facial growth in the mandibular and maxillary arches in recent human populations.
Article
Background: Picky or fussy eating is common in early childhood and associated with a decreased preference for a variety of foods. The aim of the current study was to test experimentally which sensory food feature influences food acceptance, which, in turn is an indication for fussy eating, in young children (32 - 48 months). Another aim was to evaluate if the behavioural measurement of food acceptance is related to parental reports of their child's fussy eating behaviour, parental feeding styles and children's BMI. Method: In a repeated-measures-design, three sensory features were manipulated separately (i.e., colour, texture and taste) while keeping the other two features constant. The baseline measurement consisted of a well-liked yoghurt, which was presented before each manipulation variant. The number of spoons that children (N = 32) consumed from each variant were registered as behavioural indication for food acceptance. Parental reports of children's eating behaviour and parental feeding styles; and children's BMI were also measured. Results: The manipulation of food texture caused a significant decrease in intake. Colour and taste manipulations of the yoghurt did not affect children's intake. Parental reports of children's fussy eating behaviour and parental feeding styles were not related to the behavioural observation of food acceptance. The behavioural measurement of food acceptance and parental accounts of fussy eating were not related to children's BMI. Conclusion: Food texture but not taste or colour alternations affected food acceptance, at least when consuming variations of a well-liked yoghurt. This knowledge is important for further research on picky-eating interventions. Parental reports of fussy eating did not concur with the behavioural observation of food acceptance. Further research is warranted to test whether these findings generalize to other food types.
Conference Paper
Background: Previous studies have shown that breastfeeding is associated with significant reductions in the risk of common infections among infants in developed countries; however, whether breastfeeding confers longer-term protection against infections is unknown. Methods: We linked data from the 2005-2006 Infant Feeding Practices Study-II and 2012 follow-up data collected when children were 6 years old. We used multivariate logistic regression, controlling for sociodemographics, to examine associations of any and exclusive breastfeeding duration and breastmilk intensity (categorized by tertiles of the percentage of milk feedings being breastmilk from age 0-6 months) with maternal reports of past-year infection (ear, sinus, throat, pneumonia/lung, urinary, cold/upper respiratory) among the 6-year-old children. Results: Among 1,292 respondents with complete data, the most common past-year infections at age 6 years were colds/upper respiratory (66%), ear (25%), and throat (24%) infections. No associations were found among breastfeeding and lung, urinary tract, or cold/respiratory infections. Prevalence of ear, sinus, and throat infections differed by breastfeeding duration, exclusivity, and breastmilk intensity (p<.02 for each). Children who were exclusively breastfed at least 6 months had lower odds of past-year sinus (adjusted odds ratio [AOR]=0.12, 95% confidence interval [95%CI]: 0.02-0.87), throat (AOR=0.20, 95%CI: 0.06-0.64), and ear (AOR=0.34, 95%CI: 0.13-0.89) infections versus those exclusively breastfed <4 months. Breastfeeding duration of ³6 months was associated with significantly reduced odds of the same infections. High breast milk intensity (>66.6% of child's milk feedings from 0-6 months were breastmilk) was associated with lower odds of sinus and throat infections. Conclusions: This is the first study of long-term associations of breastfeeding intensity, exclusivity, and duration, and infections in 6-year-old children in the U.S. The findings suggest that breastfeeding may protect against infection well beyond infancy. We will share some other important ways linked IFPS-II and follow-up study data can be used to examine longer-term, breastfeeding-related outcomes.
Article
Background: Previous studies have shown that breastfeeding is associated with reductions in the risk of common infections among infants; however, whether breastfeeding confers longer term protection is inconclusive. Methods: We linked data from the 2005-2007 IFPS II (Infant Feeding Practices Study II) and follow-up data collected when the children were 6 years old. Multivariable logistic regression was used, controlling for sociodemographic variables, to examine associations of initiation, duration, exclusivity of breastfeeding, timing of supplementing breastfeeding with formula, and breast milk intensity (proportion of milk feedings that were breast milk from age 0-6 months) with maternal reports of infection (cold/upper respiratory tract, ear, throat, sinus, pneumonia/lung, and urinary) and sick visits in the past year among 6-year-olds (N = 1281). Results: The most common past-year infections were colds/upper respiratory tract (66%), ear (25%), and throat (24%) infections. No associations were found between breastfeeding and colds/upper respiratory tract, lung, or urinary tract infections. Prevalence of ear, throat, and sinus infections and number of sick visits differed according to breastfeeding duration, exclusivity, and timing of supplementing breastfeeding with formula (P < .05). Among children ever breastfed, children breastfed for ≥9 months had lower odds of past-year ear (adjusted odds ratio [aOR]: 0.69 [95% confidence interval (95% CI): 0.48-0.98]), throat (aOR: 0.68 [95% CI: 0.47-0.98]), and sinus (aOR: 0.47 [95% CI: 0.30-0.72]) infections compared with those breastfed >0 to <3 months. High breast milk intensity (>66.6%) during the first 6 months was associated with lower odds of sinus infection compared with low breast milk intensity (<33.3%) (aOR: 0.53 [95% CI: 0.35-0.79]). Conclusions: This prospective longitudinal study suggests that breastfeeding may protect against ear, throat, and sinus infections well beyond infancy.
Article
Baby foods are exposed to elevated temperatures during processing treatments such as sterilization or spray drying. These treatments decompose sugars leading to the formation of α-dicarbonyl compounds that are of importance since they have been associated with several metabolic disorders. In this study, an analytical method based on high-performance liquid chromatography coupled with electrospray ionization mass spectrometry (HPLC-ESI-MS) was used to determine α-dicarbonyl compounds in baby foods. The method entailed aqueous extraction of α-dicarbonyl compounds from the samples and derivatization with o-phenylenediamine prior to chromatographic analysis. The results indicated that major degradation product was 3-deoxyglucosone in the samples including cereal-based infant formula, canned fruit and vegetable puree. Its concentration ranged between 3.9 and 827.1 mg/kg in infant formula and between 26.7 and 92.3 mg/kg in fruit puree samples. The concentrations of glucosone, 1-deoxyglucosone, 5-hydroxymethyl-2-furfural, furfural, glyoxal, methylglyoxal and dimethylglyoxal levels were rather low.