ArticlePDF Available

Affect Regulation Styles in Avoidant and Anxious Attachment

Authors:

Abstract

Previous research suggests that attachment style may affect how individuals cope with emotions. This study examines whether individuals with different attachment styles show distinct patterns in their ability to identify and regulate emotions. Participants (N = 96) completed measures assessing their attachment style, ability to cope with emotions, and emotional awareness. Results indicate a pattern in which individuals with avoidant attachment lack awareness into their emotional state and are less reactive to their emotions, while individuals with anxious attachment have increased emotional awareness, but struggle in both identifying their feelings and managing impulses.
Individual Differences Research
2014, Vol. 12, No. 3, pp. 123-130
©2014 Individual Differences Association, Inc.
www. idr-joumal.com
ISSN: 1541-745X
Affect Regulation Styles in Avoidant and
Anxious Attachment
Francis L. Stevens*
University o f Massachusetts, Boston
*Francis L. Stevens; Department of Psychology; 100 Morrissey Blvd., 2nd Floor Quinn
Administration Bldg., University o f Massachusetts- Boston, Boston, MA 02125;
fstevens@wheelock. edu.
ABSTRACT - Previous research suggests that attachment style may affect how individuals cope
with emotions. This study examines whether individuals with different attachment styles show
distinct patterns in their ability to identify and regulate emotions. Participants (N = 96) completed
measures assessing their attachment style, ability to cope with emotions, and emotional
awareness. Results indicate a pattern in which individuals with avoidant attachment lack
awareness into their emotional state and are less reactive to their emotions, while individuals with
anxious attachment have increased emotional awareness, but struggle in both identifying their
feelings and managing impulses.
The quality of child-parent attachment appears to be related to psychological
functioning later in life; insecure attachment has been shown to be related to poor mental
health outcomes (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007; Dozier, Stovall-McClough, & Albus,
2008). Several authors have suggested that secure attachment develops through validation
of the child’s emotions, which later in turn, becomes the basis for affect regulation as an
adult (Schore, 1994; Siegel, 2012). When secure attachment fails to take place, the child
is unable to effectively regulate their emotions and becomes vulnerable to developing
psychopathology. Much of psychopathology involves failure in the ability to self-regulate
emotion, and this deficit may be an organizing principle in psychopathology (Gross &
Munoz, 1995). Emotional dysregulation, in fact, may be synonymous with insecure
attachment in childhood (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2012).
Maternal sensitivity to infant distress is highly predictive o f secure attachment
(McElwain & Booth-LaForce, 2006) and has also been associated with less behavioral
problems, low levels of emotional reactivity, and more effective regulation of affect
(Leekers, Blankson, & O’Brien, 2009). A caregiver’s ability to effectively respond to a
child’s distress, thus, may be the basic building block for secure attachment as well as for
the child’s effective emotional self-regulation in future stressful situations. Children with
a secure attachment bond display less negative emotion over time (Kochanska, 2001),
suggesting that secure attachment may help the child to internally regulate their
distressful emotions. Braungart-Rieker, Garwood, Powers, and Wang (2001) found that
the ability to regulate affect partly mediated the relationship between maternal sensitivity
and attachment. This suggests that the child’s ability to regulate affect is largely
123
Stevens / Individual Differences Research, 2014, Vol. 12, No. 3, pp. 123-130
dependent upon the care giving environment. The development of secure attachment and
the ability to regulate affect may be a synchronous developmental process, congruent to
what has been referred to as an internal working model (Bretherton & Munholland, 2008;
Zimmermann, 1999) or reflective function (Fongay, 1997). The internal working model
or reflective function is an internalization of the caregiver that can act as a representative
model for self-soothing and regulation of affect.
Insecure attachment was first identified as three categories by Ainsworth, Blehar,
Waters, and Wall (1978) and, subsequently, was developed into a four-part matrix along
two dimensions by Bartholomew (1990). The first dimension reflects how the child views
the nature of the world, and the second dimension is how the child views the nature of the
self, which create the four attachment types (see Figure 1).
Figure 1
Model of Attachment
M od el of Se lf (an xi ous d ime ns ion )
Po sitive Neg ative
M od el o f Oth er Positive S ecu re A ttac hm en t A nx iou s A tta ch me nt
(avoidan t
dimensio n) N ega tive A vo ida nt At tac hm en t
(D ism issive )
(P reo cc up ied )
F earf ul At tac hm en t
Mikulincer, Shaver, and Pereg’s (2003) believe that insecure attachment rest along
these two dimensions o f avoidant and anxious attachment. Mikulincer, Shaver, and
Pereg’s (2003) model outlines two strategies for coping with neglect as a child: hyper
activating and deactivating strategies of emotion. A hyper-activating strategy is
associated with anxious attachment, acting out by being overly emotional in efforts to
receive parental attention. Tire second strategy involves deactivating emotions and is
associated with avoidant attachment. This strategy is used to dissociate from the feelings
of abandonment when a caregiver is unable to meet the child’s needs. How children view
themselves and the world have long-term consequences for the ways they cope with their
emotions. A child who sees the world as positive, will be able to rely on others to help
them regulate emotion; whereas, those who believe that they cannot rely on the world
will feel left to their own devices to cope with emotion. Those with avoidant attachment
become overwhelmed by intense emotion, and they cope through deactivating strategies
involving repression or dissociation away from emotion. Whereas, individuals with
anxious attachment display extreme emotions to gain attention from caregivers to have
their emotions soothed. Individuals with anxious attachment will attribute themselves as
the reason their caregiving needs are not being met and, consequently, develop a negative
model of self. If the Mikulincer, Shaver, and Pereg (2003) model is correct, those with an
avoidant attachment style should show deficits in recognizing and acknowledging
emotions, and those with anxious attachment should exhibit deficits in controlling their
response to emotions.
This study hypothesized that individuals with avoidant attachment will show deficits
in emotional awareness (Aware), as well as in emotional clarity (Clarity), on the
subscales of the Difficulties Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS). And, individuals high in
124
Stevens/Individual Differences Research, 2014, Vol. 12, No. 3, pp. 123-130
anxious attachment will show deficits in controlling their emotional responses, marked by
higher scores on the impulse control difficulties (Impulse) and difficulties in goal-
direction (Goals) subscales of the DERS. The other two subscales of the DERS, limited
access to emotion regulation strategies (Strategies) and non-acceptance of emotional
response (Non-accept) are not hypothesized to correlate with any particular attachment
style, since they are not specific to either controlling or having an awareness of emotion.
However, anxious and avoidant attachment may affect these emotional regulation
abilities as well. Further, it is also hypothesized that individuals with avoidant attachment
will show greater deficits in describing their feelings (DDF), as well as higher levels of
externally orientated thinking (EOT) on the Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20), than
individuals with anxious attachment. Lastly, it is predicted that both anxious-attached and
avoidant-attached individuals will show deficits in the ability to identify their feelings
(DIF) subscale of the TAS-20.
Method
Participants
Participants consisted of 96 undergraduates at a northeastern public university. The
sample consisted of 64 women and 32 men. The age range of the sample was 18 to 41
years old, with an average age of 21 years. The racial demographics of the participants
consisted of 46 percent Caucasian, 14 percent African-American, 19 percent Latino or
Hispanic, 17 percent Asian, and 4 percent other or Mixed Race
Materials and Procedure
Participants completed the Experiences in Close Relationships, Toronto Alexithymia
Scale, and the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scales. Participants took the study
online through the PsychData website, an online human subjects’ data pool software
program. Participants received course credit in undergraduate psychology classes for
their involvement.
Measures
The Experiences in Close Relationships (ECR) instrument (Brennan, Clark, &
Shaver, 1998) is a 36-item measure, designed to assess anxious and avoidant attachment.
The measure consists of two scales measuring anxious and avoidant attachment.
Cronbach’s alpha for the scale was shown to be a = .92 (Gillath, Sesko, Shaver, & Chun,
2010), and test-retest reliability appears robust, r = .71 for the avoidance subscale, and r
= .68 for the anxiety subscale, over a six month period (Lopez & Gormley, 2002).
The Difficulties in Emotional Regulation Scale (DERS) (Gratz & Roemer, 2004) is a
36-item measure, assessing how one copes with and handles emotions. It consists of six
subscales: non-acceptance of emotional responses, difficulties in goal-directed behavior,
impulse control difficulties, lack of emotional awareness, limited access to emotion
regulation strategies, and lack of emotional clarity. Higher scores are indicative of an
inability to regulate emotional responses. Gratz & Roemer (2004) reported a robust
Cronbach’s alpha (a = .93) for the overall scale, with the subscales ranging from a = .80
to .89. Gratz & Roemer (2004) further found the DERS is predictive o f self-harm
behavior, providing validity for the scale.
125
Stevens / Individual Differences Research, 2014, Vol. 12, No. 3, pp. 123-130
The Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20) (Bagby, Parker, & Taylor, 1994) is a 20-
item assessment, designed to measure alexithymia. The measure has three subscales:
difficulty in identifying feelings (DIF), difficulty in describing feelings (DDF), and
externally orientated thinking (EOT), which, measures an individual’s tendency to focus
attention on external sources. Cronbach’s alpha for the scale was ascertained at a = .81
and a test retest reliability correlation was established at r = .77 (Bagby, Parker, &
Taylor, 1994).
Results
Cronbach’s alphas, means, and standard deviations for all measures are listed in
Table 1. A correlational analysis was completed to identify deficits in emotional
regulation assessed by various subscales of the DERS by attachment type. The results of
the correlational analysis are listed below in Table 2.
Table 1
Summary o f all measures for
participants (N= 96)
aM SD
ECR .93 109.34 32.35
Anxious Attachment .93 63.09 22.35
Avoidant Attachment .91 46.25 17.34
DERS .95 82.09 12.11
Non-accept .93 13.52 6.57
Goals .90 13.53 5.72
Impulse .92 12.54 6.13
Aware .78 14.46 4.34
Strategies .92 17.22 7.67
Clarity .85 10.83 4.20
TAS-20 .89 48.11 12.61
Describing (DDF) .82 12.47 4.49
Identifying (DIF) .89 16.79 6.40
External Orientated (EOT) .62 18.84 4.25
Table 2
Difficulties in Emotional Regulation Scale
_____
(DERS) Subscale Correlations
_____
Non-accept Goals Impulse Aware Strategies Clarity
Avoidant
Attachment .347** .160 .284** .470** .350** .455**
Anxious
Attachment .522** .491** .569** .092 .647** .413**
Note. * p <.05, **p <.01
Attachment style was examined as it relates to alexithymia. A Pearson correlation
shows that both avoidant (r = .592, p < .01) and anxious attachment (r = .461, p < .01)
were both significantly related alexithymia, results are listed in Table 3. In order to
126
Stevens / Individual Differences Research, 2014, Vol. 12, No. 3, pp. 123-130
identify differences in alexithymia presentation between the two attachment types, two
linear regressions were completed, using avoidant and anxious attachment as two
separate dependent variables, and the TAS-20 subscales as the independent variables.
Both regressions were significant, but in the anxious attachment regression (F (3,92) =
15.38, p < .01), only the identifying emotions subscale (ft = .684, p <01) was a
significant predictor of anxious attachment; whereas, in the avoidant attachment
regression (F (3,92) = 16.73,/? < .01), only the externally orientated thinking subscale (ft
= .260, p < .05) was a significant predictor o f avoidant attachment.
Table 3
Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20) correlations
Describing Identifying External Orientated Scale Total
Avoidant
Attachment .544** .484** .451** .592**
Anxious
Attachment .369** .571** .118 .461**
Note. * p <.05, **p <.01
As hypothesized, anxious attachment type showed a significant positive correlation
with Goals (r = .491, p < .01) and Impulse (r = .569, p < .01) DERS subscales. Avoidant
attachment type failed to show a correlation with Goals, but did correlate with the
Impulse scale (r = .284, p < .01), albeit not nearly as strong; indicating that anxious
attachment has a much stronger effect on impulse control than avoidant attachment. The
hypothesis that avoidant attachment would show stronger correlations with the Aware
and Clarity subscales than anxious attachment also was partially supported. Both Aware
(r = .470, p < .01) and Clarity (r = .455, p < .01) were significantly correlated with
avoidant attachment, yet Clarity (r = .413, p < .01) was also correlated with anxious
attachment; indicating that both avoidant and anxious attachment types struggle in
clarifying their emotions. Both attachment types, anxious attachment (r = .647, p < .01)
and avoidant attachment (r = .35, p < .01), showed deficits in Strategies to regulate
emotions. The Non-accept subscale was significantly positively correlated with anxious
attachment (r = .522, p < .01) and avoidant attachment (r = .347, p < .01) to a lesser
degree.
Discussion
The results of this study are consistent with the findings of Pepping, Davis, and
O’Donovan (2013), showing positive correlations between avoidant and anxious
attachment and the DERS scale. The authors did not analyze the results by the subscales.
Further, the findings of the present study closely resemble those of Goodall, Trejnowska,
and Darling (2012) who found positive correlations between attachment type and the
specific DERS subscales included in this investigation.
Results from the linear regressions of the TAS-20 subscales on anxious and avoidant
attachment, partially supported the predicted hypotheses, and were consistent with the
above results between attachment type and difficulties in emotion regulation. Overall
results show that individuals higher in avoidant attachment are less focused on their own
emotions. Avoidant-attached individuals when compared to anxious-attached individuals
127
Stevens / Individual Differences Research, 2014, Vol. 12, No. 3, pp. 123-130
displayed a stronger focus on externally orientated thinking (EOT) and exhibited greater
deficits in awareness of feeling on the DERS. While anxious-attached participants
compared to avoidant-attached, were more aware o f their emotions, they still struggled in
clarifying those emotions. Anxious-attached individuals were also more likely to let their
emotions interfere with their goals and were more likely engage in impulsive behaviors.
Oskis, Clow, Hucklebridge, Bifulco, Jacobs, and Loveday (2013) found similar results
with TAS-20 in a sample of female adolescents; anxious-attached participants showed
greater deficits in identifying feelings (DIF), than avoidant-attached participants, who
showed greater deficits in describing (DDF) feelings than did the anxious-attached
participants.
This study’s findings support Mikulincer, Shaver, and Pereg’s (2003) model that
predicts that individuals cope differently with emotions based upon their attachment type.
Those with avoidant attachment prefer to avoid, not think about their feelings, making
them less aware of their emotions. Avoidant-attached individuals may benefit from this
strategy by not letting emotions interfere with their functioning or cause impulsive
behavior. Alternatively, anxious-attached individuals having an increased awareness of
their emotions, they may now struggle more in regulating these emotions. Anxious-
attached individuals struggle in correctly identifying their feelings and controlling
emotional impulses. These anxious-attached individuals may have more awareness into
their feelings; yet, without strong affect regulation skills, they are more likely to feel out
of control by their emotions and struggle in daily life tasks when emotionally
overwhelmed.
The results of these two distinct patterns of attachment suggest that it may be
important for clinicians to employ two different strategies when approaching emotions
based upon the client’s attachment type. Those with avoidant attachment may derive
greater therapeutic benefits from skills that focus on awareness of emotion so that they
may gain better insight into their emotions. Conversely, clients with anxious attachment
may experience greater therapeutic benefits from learning affect regulation skills in order
to better cope with difficult emotions. For these individuals, learning techniques to better
manage emotions and inhibit negative behaviors may prove to be more adaptive since
anxious-attached individuals are naturally more focused on their feelings, but struggle
with impulse control.
Although further research is needed, the results from this study support the predicted
relationship between attachment style and emotion regulation. Individuals with avoidant
attachment show robust deficits in recognizing emotion; while those with anxious
attachment exhibit greater difficulties in managing and controlling their response to
emotions. One notable limitation is that participants scoring high on both anxious and
avoidant attachment dimensions, indicative of a fearful attachment style were not
examined. These individuals may engage both hyper-activating and deactivating
strategies of emotion regulation, which render these participants’ deficient in
acknowledging, as well as in controlling their emotions. Individuals with fearful
attachment will likely need skill building in both raising awareness of emotion as well as
in affect regulation. Therefore, it is suggested that future research consider grouping
attachment style into the four purposed groups (Bartholomew, 1990) and examining
difficulties in emotion regulation by each attachment style.
128
Stevens / Individual Differences Research, 2014, Vol. 12, No. 3, pp. 123-130
References
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns o f
attachment: A psychological study o f the strange situation. Hillsdale, N J.: Erlbaum.
Bagby, R. M., Parker J. D. A., & Taylor, G.J. (1994). The Twenty-Item Toronto
Alexithymia Scale: I. Item selection and cross-validation o f the factor structure.
Journal o f Psychosomatic Research, 38, 23-32.
Bartholomew, K. (1990). Avoidance of intimacy: An attachment perspective. The
Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 7, 147-178.
Braungart-Rieker, J. M., Garwood, M. M., Powers, B. P., & Wang, X. (2001). Parental
sensitivity, infant affect, and affect regulation: Predictors of later attachment. Child
Development, 72(1), 252-270.
Bretherton, I., & Munholland, K. A. (2008). Internal working models in attachment
relationships: Elaborating a central construct in attachment theory. In: Cassidy J,
Shaver PR, editors. Handbook of attachment: Theory, research and clinical
applications. New York: Guilford Press
Brennan, K.. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report measurement of adult
attachment: An integrative overview. In J. A. Simpson & W. S. Rhodes (Eds.),
Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 46-76). New York, NY: Basic Books.
Dozier, M., Stovall-McClough, K. C., & Albus, K. E. (2008). Attachment and
psychopathology in adulthood. In J. Cassidy & P.R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of
attachment: Theory, research and clinical applications. New York: Guilford.
Fonagy, P., & Target, M. (1997). Attachment and reflective function: Their role in self
organization. Development and Psychopathology, 9(04), 679-700.
Gillath, O., Sesko, A.K., Shaver, P.R., & Chun, D.S. (2010). Attachment, authenticity,
and honesty: Dispositional and experimentally induced security can reduce self- and
other-deception. Journal o f Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 841-855.
Goodall, K., Trejnowska, A., Darling, S. (2012). The relationship between dispositional
mindfulness, attachment security, and emotional regulation. Personality and
Individual Differences, 52, 622-626.
Gross J. J., & Munoz, R. F. (1995). Emotion Regulation and Mental Health. Clinical
Psychology: Science and Practice, 2, 151 - 164.
Kochanska, G. (2001). Emotional development in children with different attachment
histories: The first three years. Child Development, 72(2), 474-490.
Leerkes, E. M„ Blankson, A. N& O ’Brien, M. (2009). Differential Effects of Maternal
Sensitivity to Infant Distress and Non-distress on Social-Emotional Functioning.
Child Development, 80(3), 762-775.
Lopez, F. G., & Gormley, B. (2002). Stability and change in adult attachment style over
the first-year college transition: Relations to self-confidence, coping, and distress
patterns. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 49,355-364.
McElwain, N. L., & Booth-LaForce, C. (2006). Maternal sensitivity to infant distress and
non-distress as predictors of infant-mother attachment security. Journal o f Family
Psychology, 20(2), 247.
Mikulincer, M. Shaver, P. R. & Pereg, D. (2003). Attachment theory and affect
regulation: The dynamics, development, and cognitive consequences of attachment-
related strategies. Motivation and Emotion, 27, 77-102.
129
Stevens / Individual Differences Research, 2014, Vol. 12, No. 3, pp. 123-130
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2012). An attachment perspective on psychopathology.
World Psychiatry, //(l), 11-15.
Oskis, A., Clow, A., Hucklebridge, F., Bifulco, A., Jacobs, C., & Loveday, C. (2013).
Understanding alexithymia in female adolescents: The role of attachment style.
Personality and Individual Differences, 54, 97-102.
Pepping, C. A., Davis, P. J., & O’Donovan, A. (2013). Individual differences in
attachment and dispositional mindfulness: The mediating role of emotional
regulation. Personality and Individual Differences, 54, 453-456.
Schore, A. N. (1994). Affect regulation and the origin of the self: The neurobiology of
emotional development. Psychology Press.
Siegel, D. J. (2012). The developing mind: How relationships and the brain interact to
shape who we are. Guilford Press.
Zimmermann, P. (1999). Structure and functions of internal working models of
attachment and their role for emotion regulation. Attachment and Human
Development, 1(5), 291-306.
130
CopyrightofIndividualDifferencesResearchisthepropertyofIndividualDifferences
Researchanditscontentmaynotbecopiedoremailedtomultiplesitesorpostedtoalistserv
withoutthecopyrightholder'sexpresswrittenpermission.However,usersmayprint,
download,oremailarticlesforindividualuse.
... On the other hand, anxious attachment is associated with hyperactivating ER strategies that develop as a way of catching the attention of the attachment figure to obtain soothing. Avoidantly attached individuals aim to dissociate from their emotions and are less focused on them which results in difficulties identifying emotions, while anxiously attached individuals are more aware of their emotions, but also have problems with clarifying them (Stevens, 2014). Moreover, anxious attachment was more often associated with engaging in impulsive behaviors and emotional interference with goals. ...
... Infants' attachment predicts attachment-relevant emotion regulation strategies (Girme et al., 2021) and underlying neural processes during the regulation of positive emotions (Moutsiana et al., 2014) in adulthood. The seemingly developmentally preceding nature of attachment, serving as a learning environment for the development of specific ER difficulties (Shaver & Mikulincer, 2007;Stevens, 2014), justified the inclusion of ER difficulties as a mediator in our models. ...
Article
Full-text available
Compulsive sexual behavior disorder (CSBD) was previously considered an attachment disorder, while emotion dysregulation was thought to potentially be a key characteristic of it. However, this theoretical model was not tested in previous empirical research. In our cross-sectional study, we tested whether emotional regulation (ER) difficulties can be adopted as an explanatory mechanism for the relationships between attachment avoidance and anxiety, as well as CSBD and its most prevalent behavioral presentation—problematic pornography use (PPU). Participants (n = 1002; Mage = 50.49 years, SD = 13.32; men: 50.2%) completed an online survey regarding the investigated variables. In mediation analyses, attachment avoidance and anxiety were treated as simultaneous predictors, ER difficulties as a mediating variable, with CSBD/PPU severity as dependent variables. Emotion regulation difficulties and attachment anxiety had a direct positive effect on both CSBD and PPU. The direct effect of attachment avoidance on PPU was non-significant, and significant for CSBD depending on the measure used. Moreover, all the relationships between both insecure attachment dimensions and CSBD/PPU symptom severity were at least partially mediated by ER difficulties. Our results corroborate the theoretical claim that ER difficulties may be a useful framework for explaining the impact of attachment insecurity on CSBD/PPU. Theoretical and practical implications of the findings are discussed.
... Other works have found that people characterized by avoidant attachment exhibit diminished awareness of their emotional state and demonstrate lower reactivity to their emotions. In contrast, individuals with anxious attachment display heightened emotional awareness but encounter difficulties in both recognizing their feelings and managing impulses [40]. ...
... Thus, attachment orientations that form early in life as a result of infants' interactions with their primary caregivers may lead to the later development of the traits and abilities of EI. The differences between the results for trait vs. ability EI may result from changes in the affective deficits presented in preschoolaged children who were included in some studies or avoidant adults' insights into their own emotional competencies [7,40,76]. However, these possibilities should be further examined in future work to elucidate the reasons for this finding. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
Attachment orientations reflect the inner representations of the self and others that are formed in early childhood. Emotional intelligence is seen as a set of skills involved in the expression of emotion, its effective regulation of emotion, and the use of feelings. Attachment orientations are thought to derive from infants’ experiences with their primary caregivers and are associated with a range of cognitions, emotions and behaviors, including the interpretation of emotional experiences, the use of coping mechanisms, and affect regulation strategies to deal with stressful events and as such are closely related to emotional experiences and the broader concept of emotional intelligence. This chapter presents the foundational studies and recent empirical findings on attachment theory and emotional intelligence and then discusses ways to integrate these two concepts from a dual psychodynamic-cognitive perspective.
... Dalam penelitian Smyth et al. (2015), individu anxious attachment menunjukkan reaktivitas kortisol akibat stres yang lebih besar. Mereka telah meningkatkan kesadaran emosional mereka, hanya saja mengalami kesulitan untuk mengidentifikasi dan mengelola perasaan itu (Stevens, 2014). Penelitian dari West et al. (1993) menemukan anxious attachment sebagai faktor yang memengaruhi psikopatologi dewasa. ...
Article
Full-text available
Emerging adulthood involves a tortuous period of transition. Individuals are often unprepared for the tasks of independence, and some are still dependent on others. This leads to attachment to certain anxiety figures. Attachment style becomes an individual's preference for independence which consists of secure attachment and insecure attachment. This research will focus on one of the two dimensions of insecure attachment, namely anxious attachment. Anxious attachment is an attachment style that is oriented towards a strong desire for attention, support, and close contact with a figure. A number of studies have found low life satisfaction in individuals who have an anxious attachment style, which will affect individual self-esteem. Life satisfaction is an individual's positive assessment of their achievements and quality of life according to predetermined standards. Meanwhile, self-esteem is a person's perception of themselves that involves a process of self-verification. The purpose of this study is to determine whether there is a role of self-esteem in the relationship between anxious attachment and life satisfaction in emerging adulthood. The participants involved in the study consisted of 452 emerging adulthood individuals aged 18-25 years. This study uses quantitative methods with measuring instruments Anxious Attachment Scale Items, The Satisfaction With Life Scale, and Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale. The results showed no relationship between anxious attachment and life satisfaction (p = 0.593 > 0.05), a negative relationship in anxious attachment with self-esteem (p = 0.027 < 0.05), and a positive relationship in life satisfaction and self-esteem (p = 0.000 < 0.05). Therefore, it can be concluded that there is no role of self-esteem as a mediator in the relationship between anxious attachment and life satisfaction in emerging adulthood so that the hypothesis in this study cannot be proven. ABSTRAK Emerging adulthood melibatkan masa peralihan yang berliku-liku. Individu sering kali tidak siap menghadapi tugas-tugas kemandirian sehingga ada di antara individu yang masih mengandalkan kebergantungan terhadap orang lain. Hal ini membawa individu kepada kelekatan terhadap figur-figur kecemasan tertentu. Gaya kelekatan menjadi preferensi individu terhadap kemandirian yang terdiri dari secure attachment dan insecure attachment. Penelitian ini akan berfokus kepada salah satu dari dua dimensi insecure attachment, yakni anxious attachment. Anxious attachment merupakan gaya kelekatan yang berorientasi pada keinginan kuat untuk mendapat perhatian, dukungan, serta berhubungan dekat dengan figur. Sejumlah penelitian menemukan rendahnya life satisfaction pada individu yang memiliki gaya kelekatan anxious attachment, yang nantinya akan memengaruhi self-esteem individu. Life satisfaction merupakan penilaian individu secara positif terhadap pencapaian serta kualitas hidup mereka sesuai dengan standar yang telah ditetapkan. Sementara itu, self-esteem merupakan persepsi seseorang terhadap dirinya sendiri yang melibatkan proses verifikasi diri. Tujuan dari penelitian ini adalah untuk mengetahui apakah terdapat peranan self-esteem dalam hubungan anxious attachment dengan life satisfaction pada emerging adulthood. Partisipan yang terlibat dalam penelitian terdiri dari 452 individu emerging adulthood berusia 18-25 tahun. Penelitian ini menggunakan metode kuantitatif dengan alat ukur Anxious Attachment Scale Items, The Satisfaction With Life Scale, dan Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale. Hasil penelitian menunjukkan tidak adanya hubungan antara anxious attachment dan life satisfaction (p = 0.593 > 0.05), adanya hubungan negatif pada anxious attachment dengan self-esteem (p = 0.027 < 0.05), dan adanya hubungan positif pada life satisfaction dan self-esteem (p = 0.000 < 0.05). Maka dari itu, dapat disimpulkan tidak ada peranan self-esteem sebagai mediator dalam hubungan anxious attachment dan life satisfaction pada emerging adulthood sehingga hipotesis dalam penelitian ini tidak dapat dibuktikan.
... 50s), offers a theoretical lens through which to understand the intricate interplay between early caregiver interactions and subsequent interpersonal relationships.Fraley and Shaver (2021) delineated between secure and insecure attachment styles, highlighting their profound impact on emotional regulation, coping strategies, and relational dynamics.Stevens (2014) further elucidated the intricate relationship between attachment patterns and emotional regulation, revealing the challenges faced by individuals with avoidant attachment styles in identifying and addressing their emotions, while those with anxious attachment styles grappled with regulating their responses, often leading to impulsivity ...
Article
Full-text available
The study aims to identify the differences in emotional intelligence and attachment styles between orphan and non-orphan adolescents. A qualitative method has been used to analyze the data. The sample comprised a total of 60 adolescents, of which 30 orphans and 30 nonorphan adolescents aged 13 to 19, were selected from different orphanages and schools in Rawalpindi and Islamabad. The unstructured interview was used to determine the differences in attachment styles and emotional intelligence of both groups of adolescents. The findings showed that there are differences in emotional intelligence and attachment styles among orphans and non-orphans, with orphans showing a low level of emotional intelligence and an insecure attachment style and non-orphans showing a high level of emotional intelligence and a secure attachment style when we compared both groups.
... First, internal working mode theory of attachment holds that emotion-regulation ability is the core link between attachment mode and emotional outcomes Mikulincer & Shaver, 2016). High emotion-regulation ability can activate secure attachment patterns and better perceive emotion changes in oneself or other people (Kim et al., 2014;Stevens, 2014); therefore developing effective emotion-regulation strategies is useful to manage negative emotions such as depression and anxiety (Pallini et al., 2016). Second, low emotion regulation is closely related to compulsive Internet activities (Casale et al., 2016). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study explored the effects of parental attachment, self-differentiation, and difficulties in emotion regulation on Chinese adolescents’ fear of missing out (FoMO) in 1,355 junior high school students. After demographic variables were controlled, parental attachment was significantly negatively associated with FoMO. Mediation analysis revealed that self-differentiation partially mediated the relationship between parental attachment and FoMO. Moderated mediation analysis further indicated that the direct path of parental attachment on FoMO and the mediating path of self-differentiation are regulated by difficulties in emotion regulation. The current findings highlight the protective role of emotion-regulation ability in reducing FoMO among adolescents. Prevention or intervention programs should consider improving the level of parent–child attachment and self-differentiation of adolescents to reduce FoMO.
... Indeed, attachment theory suggests that individuals' early experiences with their caregivers will shape their expectations about the trustworthiness of significant others in their life (Bowlby 1973). Attachment styles impact an individual's ability to identify, process, regulate, and cope with emotions (Stevens 2014). Avoidant attachment, anxious attachment, and disorganized attachment styles are associated with physical IPV perpetration and victimization (Spencer, Keilholtz, and Stith 2020), as IPV might be used in an attempt to prevent feared abandonment (Chesworth 2018). ...
Article
Although different studies have found that exposure to intimate partner violence (IPV) in childhood is linked to IPV perpetration in adulthood, others have revealed null effects. Previous research has also found that some factors might mediate the relationship between exposure to IPV in childhood and later IPV perpetration. As far as we know, no studies examined the role of aggression in this relationship. This study aimed to analyze the link between exposure to IPV in childhood and IPV perpetration in adulthood and verify whether this relationship was mediated by aggression and its components (verbal and physical aggression, anger, and hostility). Two hundred and forty five men convicted of domestic violence, from prison and community, were recruited. Correlation analysis revealed a positive link between exposure to IPV in childhood, aggression traits, and specifically physical aggression, and IPV frequency in adulthood. Overall aggression traits and physical aggression traits were statistically significant mediator variables explaining almost 15% of the overall effect of exposure to IPV in childhood on overall IPV frequency in adulthood. These findings unravel the role of aggression in the perpetration of physical and total IPV, claiming our attention to the need to include strategies to increase emotion regulation and problem-solving strategies in intervention programs.
... Indeed, in anxious individuals, hypervigilance toward signs of abandonment or rejection by their partner could lead them to perceive various situations as threatening to their relationship (e.g., partner's unavailability). Thus, anxious individuals, who may have difficulty regulating their emotions, may be at greater risk of using violent behaviors such as coercive control to regain a sense of security (e.g., watching their partner, forbidding them to see their friends; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2016;Stevens, 2014). This would be an inappropriate, yet reassuring, way to regain closeness with their partner and thus, satisfy their attachment needs (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2016). ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose Intimate partner violence (IPV) is a problematic and important social phenomenon (World Health Organization [WHO], 2021) that has been repeatedly linked to attachment insecurities (Velotti et al., 2020). However, the mechanisms linking these two variables remain understudied. This study examines the direct and indirect associations between attachment insecurities (anxiety and avoidance) and perpetration of IPV (psychological, physical, sexual, coercive control) through the use of dysfunctional communication patterns (i.e., demand/demand, I demand/my partner withdraws, my partner demands/I withdraw) and lower relationship satisfaction. Methods A sample of 613 men and non-binary people who consulted organizations providing help to individuals with relationship or IPV-related difficulties answered online questionnaires. A path analysis was conducted to test the direct and indirect associations between attachment insecurities and perpetrated IPV through communication patterns and relationship satisfaction. Results The results showed a direct and positive association between attachment anxiety and perpetration of coercive control, and an indirect and positive association between attachment insecurities and psychological violence through the demand/demand pattern. The results also revealed an indirect and positive association between attachment anxiety and psychological violence, sexual violence, and coercive control through the I demand/my partner withdraws pattern. Finally, an indirect and positive association was observed between attachment insecurities and all types of violence studied through lower relationship satisfaction. Conclusions This study provides key individual and relational correlates of IPV perpetration that can inform prevention and intervention among men and non-binary people.
Book
Full-text available
The image we have of ourselves is shaped during our childhood and is often influenced by various emotional wounds. Mindfulness and the Self describes four types of these wounds and shows how they can be healed and transformed through developing mindfulness and self-compassion. Grounded in the innovative Mindfulness-Informed Integrative Psychotherapy framework, this book presents a revolutionary phenomenological model of maladaptive schemas and redefines our understanding of mental disorders. It offers practical procedures to uncover hidden core beliefs and treat our most painful inner feelings—existential fear, shame, and loneliness. Beyond healing, this text will guide you in developing an Authentic and Transcendent Self while alerting you to common pitfalls on your spiritual journey. Drawing on contemporary therapeutic approaches and integrating insights from neuroscience, psychedelic research, and Buddhist psychology, the book includes 20 practical exercises and presents practices and techniques that may be used in psychotherapy and personal development. This book is an inspiring read for therapists, mindfulness practitioners, and anyone eager to integrate therapeutic insights into their journey toward a fulfilling life. It is indispensable for all seeking to overcome emotional barriers and enhance personal and transpersonal growth.
Article
The current study sought to examine attachment styles as mediators between intimate partner violence (IPV) and stress coping styles among Iranian women. Data were collected from September to December 2020. The study population was composed of 102 women who were referred to Social Emergency Centers in Isfahan and affected by domestic violence. They were given self-report questionnaires to measure IPV (the Haj-Yahia Violence Questionnaire), Stress Coping Styles (CISS), and Attachment Styles (AAQ). Structural Equation Modeling was used to test the hypothesized relationships. Sexual IPV victimization was related to both avoidant attachment (β = .229, p = .015) and anxious attachment (β = .245, p = .008). Anxious attachment style was related to emotion-oriented coping (β = .437, p = .000). There was a negative relationship between avoidant attachment and anxious attachment (β = −.237, p = .032) with avoidance-oriented coping. Sexual IPV victimization and economic IPV victimization were associated with avoidance-oriented coping (β = −.225, t = 0.816, p = .015; β = .188, t = 0.816, p = .044). Women who had experienced IPV and had an insecure attachment style were more likely to utilize avoidance and emotional coping strategies. For women who had a secure attachment style, there was no association was found between IPV victimization and coping styles. Attachment styles may influence the relationship between IPV and coping strategies in Iranian women who have experienced violence.
Article
Full-text available
Addressing shortcomings of the self-report Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS), two studies were conducted to reconstruct the item domain of the scale. The first study resulted in the development of a new twenty-item version of the scale—the TAS-20. The TAS-20 demonstrated good internal consistency and test-retest reliability, and a three-factor structure theoretically congruent with the alexithymia construct. The stability and replicability of this three-factor structure were demonstrated in the second study with both clinical and nonclinical populations by the use of confirmatory factor analysis.
Article
Full-text available
Recent interest in the relationship between individual differences in attachment and dispositional mindfulness has led to empirical investigations into this association. Although several studies have demonstrated an association between attachment and mindfulness, little is known as to why these two seemingly distinct constructs are related. In a sample of 572 individuals we explored whether the relationship between attachment and mindfulness was mediated by emotion regulation difficulties. Difficulties in emotion regulation fully mediated the association between attachment anxiety and mindfulness, and between attachment avoidance and mindfulness. Thus, the mechanism by which attachment and mindfulness are related is through emotion regulation difficulties. Theoretical and clinical implications are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
A basic principle of attachment theory is that early attachment relationships with caregivers provide the prototype for later social relations. Working within an attachment framework, a new 4-group model of characteristic attachment styles in adulthood is proposed. In particular, two forms of adult avoidance of intimacy are differentiated: a fearful style that is characterized by a conscious desire for social contact which is inhibited by fears of its consequences, and a dismissing style that is characterized by a defensive denial of the need or desire for greater social contact. This distinction corresponds to two differing models of the self: people who fearfully avoid intimacy view themselves as undeserving of the love and support of others, and people who dismiss intimacy possess a positive model of the self that minimizes the subjective awareness of distress or social needs. The emotional and interpersonal ramifications of the two proposed styles of adult avoidance are discussed.
Chapter
Full-text available
Bowlby's proposals about the formation, development, function, and intergenerational transmission of internal working models of self and attachment figures are scattered across the three volumes of his seminal trilogy Attachment and Loss (1969/1982, 1973, 1980) and his book A Secure Base (1988). To make these ideas more accessible as a whole, we summarize them in the first section of this chapter. In the second section, we discuss possible elaborations and extensions of the working-model construct by drawing on the literature from neuroscience and memory development. In the third section, we turn to studies conducted by attachment researchers. Specifically, we focus on the extent to which findings obtained with representational measures of attachment suitable for adults and children support, extend, and raise new questions about Bowlby's conceptualization of working models. We conclude with suggestions for future research. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
This study explored whether patterns of stability or change in adult attachment styles were associated with corresponding changes in self- confidence, problem coping styles, and distress. Two hundred seven students completed measures of the key variables near the start and toward the end of their freshman year. Participants were classified into change groups on the basis of their successive scores on a measure of adult attachment style. Results indicated that (a) change group had significant main effects on self-confidence ratings and problem coping styles, and (b) time and change group had significant main and interaction effects on the set of distress measures. Implications of the findings for an attachment theory-informed perspective on the transitional adjustment of entering freshmen are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
If we were to limit our discussion in this chapter to evidence linking attachment behavioral strategies in infancy with adult psychopathology, this would be a relatively brief chapter. The evidence specifically linking infants' attachment behavioral strategies to psychopathology in adulthood is limited to a few longitudinal studies. In this chapter, therefore, we cast our net more broadly, looking at associations between attachment-relevant events in childhood and later psychopathology. In addition, we examine the association between concurrently assessed attachment states of mind and psychopathology. We limit our consideration of attachment states of mind to Main and colleagues' (Main & Goldwyn, 1984; Main et al, 2003) formulation and operationalization. Although the constructs of "attachment style" (Hazan & Shaver, 1987, 1994) and "attachment states of mind" share a conceptual framework, there are key differences that lead to different operationalizations. Main and colleagues' system assesses state of mind with respect to attachment as a function of discourse coherence and defensive strategy. By contrast, attachment style assesses the individual's self-reported style of forming adolescent and adult attachments. As expected, given the different operationalizations, these variables are not strongly related to each other. Because we are interested in differences in processing attachment-related thoughts and feelings, we deal only with findings linking attachment states of mind with psychopathology in this chapter. We progress through the major psychopathological disorders, considering first the Axis I disorders, or clinical syndromes. We start with mood disorders, followed by anxiety disorders. Both of these groups of disorders are heterogeneous with regard to heritability and symptomatology; therefore, it would be surprising if clear findings emerged with regard to attachment-related issues without further specification of parameters. We move from there to a discussion of dissociative disorders. Although dissociative phenomena have been discussed throughout the 20th century, the recognition of dissociative disorders as a bona fide diagnostic category is relatively recent. Nonetheless, the evidence linking attachment in infancy and attachment-related traumas to later dissociative symptoms, and the evidence linking concurrent states of mind with dissociative symptoms, converge to form a compelling picture. We consider eating disorders next; these disorders are often comorbid with personality disorders and mood disorders. We end the discussion of Axis I disorders with schizophrenia, a disorder that is highly heritable. From there, we move to a consideration of two of the most prevalent Axis II, or personality, disorders: borderline personality disorder and antisocial personality disorder. We include these two disorders because they are prevalent and largely distinct from Axis I disorders. Genetic involvement in personality disorders is variable, with relatively high heritability for antisocial personality disorder and low heritability for borderline personality disorder. The concept of borderline personality disorder emerged from the perspective of problematic early relationships with caregivers. For each disorder or group of disorders considered, we begin with a general description of the disorder(s) and with evidence regarding genetic involvement. We then discuss attachment theory's contributions to an understanding of the disorder(s). From there we move to a consideration of the empirical evidence linking attachment phenomena to the disorder(s). (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Adults with alexithymia retrospectively report emotional difficulties with caregivers during childhood. However, the association between attachment style and alexithymic traits may be evident at an earlier stage than adulthood, i.e. during adolescence. Sixty school-based healthy females aged 9–18 years (mean 14.08, SD 2.71 years) participated in an Attachment Style Interview (ASI) and completed the Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20). Greater levels of alexithymia were exhibited by both anxious and avoidant insecurely attached groups compared to securely attached participants. Fear of separation (characteristic of anxious attachment style) predicted both overall alexithymia scores and the specific alexithymic trait of ‘difficulty identifying feelings’ (DIF). Constraints on closeness (an avoidant attachment attitude) predicted ‘difficulty describing feelings’ (DDF). Low felt attachment to primary caregiver was a predictor of ‘externally oriented thinking’ (EOT). These findings indicate that features of anxious and avoidant insecure attachment styles are differentially related to the separate facets of alexithymia in female adolescents. Specifically, the findings concerning fear of separation may reflect the adolescent struggle for autonomy and the resulting effects on the affect regulation system. Our results also suggest that the normative differentiation of the emotional and cognitive aspects of alexithymia may occur on a developmental trajectory.
Article
Ethological attachment theory is a landmark of 20th century social and behavioral sciences theory and research. This new paradigm for understanding primary relationships across the lifespan evolved from John Bowlby's critique of psychoanalytic drive theory and his own clinical observations, supplemented by his knowledge of fields as diverse as primate ethology, control systems theory, and cognitive psychology. By the time he had written the first volume of his classic Attachment and Loss trilogy, Mary D. Salter Ainsworth's naturalistic observations in Uganda and Baltimore, and her theoretical and descriptive insights about maternal care and the secure base phenomenon had become integral to attachment theory. Patterns of Attachment reports the methods and key results of Ainsworth's landmark Baltimore Longitudinal Study. Following upon her naturalistic home observations in Uganda, the Baltimore project yielded a wealth of enduring, benchmark results on the nature of the child's tie to its primary caregiver and the importance of early experience. It also addressed a wide range of conceptual and methodological issues common to many developmental and longitudinal projects, especially issues of age appropriate assessment, quantifying behavior, and comprehending individual differences. In addition, Ainsworth and her students broke new ground, clarifying and defining new concepts, demonstrating the value of the ethological methods and insights about behavior. Today, as we enter the fourth generation of attachment study, we have a rich and growing catalogue of behavioral and narrative approaches to measuring attachment from infancy to adulthood. Each of them has roots in the Strange Situation and the secure base concept presented in Patterns of Attachment. It inclusion in the Psychology Press Classic Editions series reflects Patterns of Attachment's continuing significance and insures its availability to new generations of students, researchers, and clinicians.