ChapterPDF Available

Emotional Inhibition

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Emotional inhibition constitutes a dysfunctional verbal and nonverbal interaction between individuals. Emotional inhibition may be classified into genetic, repressive, suppressive, and deceptive inhibition, and the extreme form, emotional implosion. Overt emotional inhibition is characterized by reduced expressiveness, unemotional language, and shyness, all of which are related to dysfunctional bodily reactions and may be adaptive in a short-term social stress situation. In the long run, emotional inhibition is likely to have a harmful effect on the individual along any of three pathways: neurobiological, social-behavioral, and cognitive. There is implicit knowledge in most societies that emotional inhibition has negative health implications. Most psychotherapeutic techniques are directed at emotional behavior and experience and focus largely on changes in intra- and interindividual emotional regulation and the construction of meaning from emotional experience.
Content may be subject to copyright.
in emergency ambulance personnel: a review of the
literature. Emergency Medicine Journal 20, 75–78.
Ursano, R. J., Bell, C., Eth, S., et al. (2004). Practice
guideline for the treatment of patients with acute stress
disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder. American
Journal of Psychiatry 161(supplement), 3–31.
Wagner, S. L. (2005). Emergency response service person-
nel and the critical incident stress debriefing debate.
International Journal of Emergency Mental Health 7,
33–41.
Emotional Inhibition
H C Traue
The University of Ulm, Ulm, Germany
R M Deighton
The Cairnmillar Institute, Melbourne, Australia
ã2007 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
This article is a revision of the previous edition article
by H C Traue and R M Deighton, Volume 2, pp 32–38,
ã2000, Elsevier Inc.
Emotion and Inhibition
Domains of Emotional Inhibition
Pathways from Emotional Inhibition to Health Disorders
and Illness Behaviors
Rituals and Therapeutic Interventions
Glossary
Alexithymia A constellation of cognitive and affective
characteristics including difficulty iden-
tifying and communicating subjective
feelings, a restricted imaginative life,
and a concrete and reality-oriented style
of thinking.
Behavioral
inhibition
A concept encompassing several beha-
viors in about 15% of otherwise healthy
children in response to unfamiliar social
events, including reduced spontaneity,
subdued emotional expressiveness, shy-
ness, social avoidance and several pe-
ripheral physiological hyperactivities. It
is thought to be related to limbic-hypo-
thalamic arousal, in response to socially
stressful events.
Emotional
intelligence
The ability of an individual to adaptively
and effectively regulate his or her emo-
tional behavior in a social context. This
encompasses the ability to recognize sub-
jective feelings, to manage emotions, to
transform emotions into expressiveness
and action, to react empathetically, and
to shape relationships.
Myogenic pain Pain stemming from dysfunctional mus-
cular activity as part of motor behavior
in relation to stress, posture, movement,
and emotion (e.g., low back pain, ten-
sion type headache, repetitive strain
injury, and myofascial pain disorder).
Socialization The process by which an individual grad-
ually becomes integrated into the norm
and value system of a social group or a
society. It is based on the assumption
of interaction between the biological
organism and the social environment
during psychological development.
Torture One of the most severe stressors moti-
vated mainly by political ideologies and
perpetrated in about 60 states world-
wide. The adverse effects of torture
stem from the man made nature of these
stressors and induce specific and nonspe-
cific physical and mental disorders of
long duration because they shatter vic-
tims’ basic assumptions about human
benevolence.
Emotion and Inhibition
Emotions are essentially transactions between indivi-
duals and their social environment. They give personal
meaning to external and internal stimuli and com-
municate meaning from the individual to others.
Emotions are composed of interpretations of intero-
and exteroceptive stimuli, intentions, physiological
patterns of arousal, and motor behavior including
overt emotional expressiveness. The interaction of
these different components in the individual and
the social and physical environment are mediated
by the central nervous system. From a system regu-
lation point of view, emotional expressiveness has
two important functions: first, it serves a communi-
cative function in that it facilitates the regulation of
person–environment transactions and, second, the
feedback function of behavioral expressions controls
908 Emotional Inhibition
the intraindividual regulation of emotion. This means
that active responding toward the environmental trig-
ger may influence an experience indirectly through the
attenuation of a negative emotional stimulus or directly
through self-regulation. Thus, expressive behavior can
serve simultaneously as a component of emotional
processes and as a coping response. Three prominent
scientists of the turn of the century, all of whom were
active at a time of major discoveries in neurophysiolo-
gy, contributed to important developments in the
concept of inhibition. The neurophysiology of C. S.
Sherrington (born 1857), the theory of the higher ner-
vous system of I. P. Pavlov (born 1849), and the psy-
choanalysis of S. Freud (born 1856) transformed the
principle of inhibition into a key concept in neurophys-
iology (in the case of Sherrington) and higher mental
functioning (in the cases of Pavlov and Freud).
For many years, an inverse relationship between
expressive behavior and autonomic responsivity has
been documented, such that the inhibition of overt
emotional expressiveness can lead to an autonomic
overreaction. This has been considered to be a signif-
icant factor in the etiology and maintenance of psy-
chosomatic disorders. A number of early researchers
in the first two decades of the century reported mea-
surements of high physiological activity in subjects
suppressing emotional expression. These studies led
toward the concept of internalization and external-
ization, wherein two behavioral coping styles for
dealing with psychological tension were discerned:
behaviorally, outwardly directed, or physiologically,
within the individual. Following this concept, the
term internalizer has been used to describe a person
exhibiting a low level of overt expressiveness under
stress yet a high level of physiological excitation,
whereas an externalizer is characterized by high ex-
pressiveness and a low level of physiological expres-
siveness in social situations. Temoshok proposed
a model of internalizing and externalizing coping
styles integrating the severity of stressors which in-
tended to predict the occurrence of mental disorders
(dependent on degree of externalizing coping) and
somatic disorders (dependent on degree of internaliz-
ing coping). Another inhibition theory, put forward
by Pennebaker, summarizes the process by which fail-
ure to confront traumatic events results in poorer
health. The principal assumption of this theory is
that inhibiting ongoing behavior, thoughts, and feel-
ings requires physiological work. It has been sug-
gested that the increased autonomic responses of
internalizers may reflect the work of behavioral inhi-
bition. Over time, the work of inhibition acts as a
low-level cumulative stressor. As with all cumulative
stressors, sustained inhibition is linked to increases
in stress-related diseases and various other disorders
such as cardiovascular and skin disorders, asthma,
cancer, and also pain.
Domains of Emotional Inhibition
A model (see Figure 1) of how emotional stress, under
a given social situation, can trigger or modulate
health disorders is described later. Health disorders
and illness behavior are considered as different,
but related, processes, and distinct mechanisms may
contribute dependently and independently to differ-
ent aspects of a given disorder and its behavioral
consequences.
On a phenomenological level, emotional stress
can occur on a severity dimension ranging from
daily stressors, through more traumatic life events,
to more chronic or severe psychotrauma. A common
underlying factor among such situations is that each
needs to be coped with by the individual. Emotional
stress can be seen as being processed by way of an
inhibition-implosion dimension (to implode means
to collapse or cause to collapse inward in a violent
manner as a result of external pressure), modulated
by dispositional factors (innate, personality, and
socialization).
Innate and socialization factors are of particular
importance. Possible individual differences in limbic
mechanisms for opioidergic pathways corresponding
with increased vulnerability to stress induction have
been suggested. Emotional processing relating to
the inhibition–implosion idea has been discussed in
relation to several topics like control, suppression,
type C personality, repression, alexithymia, or ambiv-
alence. Each of these concepts covers different aspects
of overt emotional expressiveness from a personality
or coping perspective.
Inhibition is the most general term for the incomplete
processing of emotional stress when these stressors
induce bodily changes (physiological, endocrinologi-
cal, or immunological) and the cognitive, emotional,
and behavioral processing are dysfunctional such that
subjective experience and spontaneous expression
of emotions and action tendencies are separately
or simultaneously attenuated and intra- and interper-
sonal regulation is disturbed. This process may be a
product of innate and/or acquired behavior.
A classification of involved mechanisms differenti-
ates among the following: genetic inhibition, repres-
sive inhibition, suppressive inhibition, and deceptive
inhibition. All four classes of inhibition occur in every
state of psychophysiological arousal produced by
emotional stimulation.
Genetic inhibition reflects the genetically deter-
mined basis of behavioral inhibition. Studies working
with young children have classified those children
Emotional Inhibition 909
who were least able to initiate interaction in a social
situation with other children and adults as behavior-
ally inhibited. Most children’s degree of behavioral
inhibition has been shown to be stable over a period
of 5 years. In inhibited children, increased levels of
arousal, norepinephrine and salivary cortisol have
been found.
Repressive inhibition is defined as emotional pro-
cessing with attenuated subjective experience of
emotional arousal. Emotional expressive responses
in repressive inhibition can be based solely on cogni-
tive interpretation of the situation and are nonspon-
taneously organized. Because the individual is unable
to feel his or her own arousal, insufficient response
information is experienced, which in turn decreases
the need to express emotions or cope with an emotional
stressor. In addition, the cognitive interpretation of
the situation without the emotional component may
be wrong or misleading. Prolonged bodily arousal and
impaired coping could result. Repressive coping style
is the best known model for repressive inhibition.
Suppressive inhibition is best circumscribed by sup-
pression of emotional arousal. The emotional arousal
is recognized by the individual but spontaneous
expressive and cognitive behaviors are involuntarily
suppressed. Suppressive inhibition of emotions could
result from interactions between innate factors and
socialization. For exampleif individuals show increased
responses under stressful encounters, they are prone to
socialization conditions of punishment and negative
reinforcement, initiating a learning history with de-
creases in spontaneous expressiveness and increases in
bodily reactivity.
Finally, individuals under emotional stress, aware
of their bodily reactions and their urge for expressive-
ness, can voluntarily suppress this need or try to
Social context
historical, political,
cultural factors
Emotional stress
traumatic experience
critical event
social and daily stressors
Innate:
genetic
inhibition
Neurobiological pathways
hypothalamic-limbic overactivation
autonomic response specificity
interhemispheric communication
behavioral inhibition systems
Social-behavioral pathways
disturbed communication
impaired coping
reduced social support
operant conditioning
Cognitive pathways
thought control
alexithymic thinking
activation of fear networks
symptom misinterpretation
IIIness behaviors
symptom report
secondary gain
impairment/disability
seeking medical care
social isolation
Health disorder
onset of disease/episode
progression
chronification
severity/complexity
delayed healing
Learned:
repressive, suppressive,
and deceptive inhibition
Sudden onset:
emotional
implosion
Inhibition and emotional implosion
Disposition
innate factors
personality
socialization
Figure 1 Psychological pathway model of emotional inhibition with neurobiological, social-behavioral, and cognitive pathways between
stress/disposition, emotional processing, and subsequent health disorders or illness behaviors (adapted from Traue, 1998).
910 Emotional Inhibition
present a false response to a receiver, called deceptive
inhibition. Whether an individual is poker faced or
displays a false emotional response, such inhibition
consumes cognitive capacity, reducing the individual’s
coping capacity and providing additional stress.
In reviewing the psychophysiological and psy-
chosomatic data, one is led to the conclusion that
emotional inhibition is potentially harmful. However,
it should be noted that although the correlations
between bodily processes and the above four forms
of inhibition support such a notion, in certain cir-
cumstances inhibition can be beneficial for the indi-
vidual and his/her relationship with the social
environment. Inhibition becomes toxic when it is
related first to physiological, endocrinological hyper-
arousal or immunological dysfunction, second to long-
standing disregulation of emotions within the individ-
ual on a cognitive and behavioral basis, and third if
inhibition disturbs the individual’s social relations.
Inhibition constitutes a risk factor for health
under normal stressors. If the severity of stressors is
dramatically high, the mental and physical health
consequences are inevitable. In traumatic stress situa-
tions like rape, criminal bodily attacks, or torture, the
individual may well lose control over strong emo-
tional responses. The emotional responses of horror,
panic, and loss of control, could literally cause a
violent breakdown in the mental and bodily sys-
tems. Such an emotional implosion is visible in the
symptom pattern of posttraumatic stress disorder
(PTSD): cognitive and behavioral avoidance of trau-
ma stimuli, numbing of general and emotional respon-
siveness (emotional anasthesia), detachment from
other people, and persistent symptoms of arousal
such as disturbed sleep, exaggerated startle response,
and somatic complaints. In addition, persons with
PTSD suffer an increased risk of social phobia and
major depressive and somatizing disorders.
The psychological, physical, and social symptoms
in PTSD are a form of emotional processing that
describes an extreme form of inhibition. While
inhibition generally develops over a long time span
through interaction between innate and socialization
factors, implosion can occur in a very short time as a
result of a single event.
Pathways from Emotional Inhibition to
Health Disorders and Illness Behaviors
Emotional stress modulated through innate, person-
ality, and socialization factors can trigger, maintain,
or worsen health disorders and related illness behav-
ior through neurobiological, social-behavioral, and
cognitive pathways. With respect to illness behaviors,
the pathways include biases in symptom reporting,
secondary gain by presented symptoms, subjective
feeling of being impaired, pressure to seek medical
help, and social isolation.
Neurobiological Pathways
It can be assumed that emotional inhibition is strong-
ly neurobiologically based. The behavioral inhibition
system and the behavioral activation system have
been discussed as possible neurobiological structures.
Empirical evidence from between-subject studies
shows that inhibited, repressed, or suppressed emo-
tional expressiveness is linked to greater autonomic
arousal, both under conditions of emotion induction
and voluntary deception. There is rich empirical evi-
dence for neurobiological correlates of inhibition in
respiratory, cardiovascular, muscular, digestive, endo-
crine, and immune functions. Immune functioning is
of particular interest because it is the immune system
that may be relevant in all sorts of infectious, allergic,
and neoplastic illness processes. Inhibited style of
processing upsetting events can compromise immune
functions, resulting in higher serum antibody titers,
decreased monocyte counts, and poorer natural killer
cell activity. Other areas of research relevant to the
neurobiological pathways of inhibition include hypo-
thalamic-limbic overactivation, prolonged activation
of physiological response specificity, hemispheric
brain lateralization of emotion processing and faulty
interhemispheric communication, the neuroregula-
tion of action, and the behavioral activation and
behavioral inhibition systems.
Social-Behavioral Pathways
A variety of social-behavioral pathways connect
inhibition–implosion to health disorders and illness
behaviors. First, neurobiologically innate factors
(shyness, behavioral inhibition, hypersensitivity, in-
troversion) are superimposed by classical and operant
conditioning in the socialization of an individual.
Since individuals with these characteristics in early
childhood are more easily conditioned, the process
of socialization involves greater vulnerability to
them than it does for others. Under critical develop-
mental conditions, the gap between emotional ex-
pressiveness and physiological hyperactivity may
increase. A lack or deficit in emotional expressiveness
will hinder interpersonal communication. It is im-
plied that deficits in interpersonal communication
disturb the development of emotional competence
which is important for sharing experiences, maintain-
ing psychological and physical contact, and adapting
to the social environment. These are the deficiencies
in healthy coping competencies that Salovey and
Emotional Inhibition 911
Mayer termed emotional intelligence. Other conse-
quences of inhibited emotional expressiveness are
disturbed social relations resulting in social isolation
and a disrupted social support network.
Normally, persons respond to emotion-evoking
stimuli with emotional expression, and such reactive
expression is realized through facial muscle activity
and movements with reafferent neuronal signals in
the central nervous system, which contribute to the
individual’s emotional experience. However, subjec-
tive emotional experience does not depend mainly on
this nervous input as argued in the facial feedback
hypotheses, but feedback does contribute positively
to sensitivity toward the physiological aspects of
emotion. If this sensitivity is disrupted, an individual
will not perceive adequately increased muscle tension
or other autonomic nervous system reactions caused
by stress and consequently will not initiate healthy
relaxing behavior. Bischoff et al. demonstrated that
the hypothesis of deficient perception of muscle ten-
sion holds for myogenic pain. Patients with this kind
of pain were reliably less able to judge the extent of
their muscle tension than were controls.
It is conceivable that, under unfavorable circum-
stances, expressive behavior of mainly negative
emotions like anger and aggressiveness is punished
socially and thus justifiably avoided. The suppression
of expressive behavior can be realized by an addi-
tional increase in muscle activity. Such avoidance
behavior or inhibition is very adaptable in the short
term, and it helps to modify a socially stressful situa-
tion. This reduction of emotional expressiveness is
conditioned by the learning mechanism of negative
reinforcement (the avoidance of punishment).
Cognitive Pathways
Memories and thoughts of emotional stressors are
generally unpleasant. Increasing severity of the stress-
ful encounter makes imagining the event painful or
even unbearable in the case of traumatic experience.
Most individuals attempt to suppress or inhibit the
thoughts surrounding the events. As soon as the inhi-
bition work begins, the urge to distract oneself and
the mental energy put into thisprocess fuels the images.
Consequently, triggered intrusions and unwanted
thoughts make life more stressful than before. In addi-
tion, thought control interferes with natural ways of
coping (e.g., sharing the experience with important
others and thinking through the event). Therefore
inhibition may be dangerous because it hampers the
individual who has suffered a critical life event from
resolving the stressful experience cognitively and
behaviorally.
Other facets of problematic cognitive processing
include an alexithymic or low-level thinking style.
As part of the inhibition process, individuals may
tend to exclude the emotional content of the stressful
encounter from their language representation of the
event. Although this may help to avoid negative
emotionality in the short term, it impairs one’s own
complete processing and integration of the stressful
experience. The lack of integration into the self-
concept makes an individual prone to activation of
fear networks. Finally, impaired or unfinished cog-
nitive processing makes an individual prone to a
misinterpretation of bodily symptoms. Instead of
understanding bodily reactions as part of emotional
responses, the individual conceptualizes the bodily
reactions as symptom patterns and seeks medical
help for illnesses. Badly advised medical treatment
procedures result in iatrogenic diseases, trapping
the individual in a vicious cycle. Cognitive appraisal
of a situation depends partly on facial feedback as
a source of emotional information. When the expres-
sive components of emotional reactions are systemat-
ically repressed by inhibition, the individual unlearns
accurate assessment of stressful circumstances. This
learning mechanism occurs since the estimated load
of a stress situation is dependent not only on external
features of the situation, but also on the subjective
experience of stress-conditioned reactions. When,
however, an inhibited person takes bodily reactions
into account in the evaluation of a situation, his or her
judgment will be impaired when the original physio-
logical components of mainly negative emotions are
interpreted as symptoms.
In clinical studies, psychosomatic patients (e.g.,
suffering headache) have been found to report sig-
nificantly lower stress levels than control groups,
but showed nearly twice as much neck muscle tension
as the controls. Although the arousal and muscle
tension data indicated higher levels of stress, patients
were unable or not willing to report those stressors.
It appears that patients tend to interpret their stres-
sors in terms of bodily symptoms rather than as
underlying levels of stress.
Rituals and Therapeutic Interventions
There is at least some implicit knowledge in most
societies that emotional inhibition has negative health
implications. The conflicts resulting from the need
for emotional regulation on the one hand and the
need for disclosure, sharing, and catharsis on the other
lead to a variety of cultural phenomena to overcome
these adverse consequences. These include older
universal cultural rituals (such as rituals of grief or
lament) or religious acts such as confessions. The
Western (Wailing) Wall in Jerusalem, where Jews
have been going for centuries to deliver a written
912 Emotional Inhibition
prayer, is possibly an example of an ancient disclosure
phenomenon. Today people can deliver their prayer
to the Western Wall via the Internet, and similar
services are offered in connection to Christian con-
fession. Contemporary western societies have also
introduced psychotherapy for enhancing emotional
expressiveness. Here, talking or writing about emo-
tions is encouraged as well as acting out emotions, in
role plays. Assertiveness training aims at effective
expression of emotion, and catharsis-based techni-
ques like confrontation are modern remedies for anx-
iety, posttraumatic stress disorders, and the like. All
of these techniques seem to have in common that they
are directed at the construction of meaning from
emotional experience. Different cultures tend to
construe emotional experience (including stress) in
different ways, such that many non-Western cultures
tend to emphasize the somatic components of emo-
tional suffering, whereas Western cultures focus on
the psychological components. Such cultural differ-
ences could influence the pathways between emotion-
al inhibition and illness. Hence, clinicians should
take into account the relevant cultural conceptions
of emotional experience and behavior a client has
been exposed to when choosing interventions for
emotional inhibition. If a client emphasizes the somat-
ic components of his or her reaction to a very stressful
event, it may not be (dysfunctional) emotional inhibi-
tion, but rather a cultural construal of emotional
suffering. Only when culture-sensitive exploration
reveals lack of insight into relevant psychosocial fac-
tors should the possibility of dysfunctional emotion
processing be considered.
See Also the Following Articles
Anger; Emotions: Structure and Adaptive Functions;
Grieving; Social Support; Cardiovasular Disease, Stress
and; Torture.
Further Reading
American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and
statistical manual of mental disorders (4th edn.).
Washington. DC: American Psychiatric Association.
Bischoff, C., Traue, H. C. and Zenz, H. (eds.) (1989).
Clinical perspectives on headache and low back pain.
Hogrefe & Huber. Toronto/Lewiston/Gottingen/Bern.
Deighton, R. M. (2003). Culture, emotional inhibition &
somatization. Ann Harbour, MI: Proquest Digital
Dissertations (www.umi.com).
Deighton, R. and Traue, H. C. (2005) Emotional inhibition
and somatization across cultures, International Revue of
Social Psychology 18(1/2): 109–140.
Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S. and Snidman, N. (1988). Biological
bases of childhood shyness. Science 240(4849), 167–171.
Mauss, I. B. and Gross, J. (2004). Emotional suppression
and cardiovascular disease: is hiding your feelings bad
for your heart? In: Nyklic
ˇek, I., Temoshok, L. &
Vingerhoets, A. (eds.) Emotional Expression and Health.
Advances in theory, assessment, and clinical applications.
Hove/New York: Brunner-Routledge.
Nyklic
ˇek, I., Temoshok, L. and Vingerhoets, A. (2004).
Emotional Expression and Health. Advances in theory,
assessment, and clinical applications. Hove/New York:
Brunner-Routledge.
Pennebaker, J. W. (1995). Emotion, disclosure, and health:
an overview. In: Pennebaker, J. W. (ed.) Emotion, disclo-
sure, and health, pp. 3–10. Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Rime
´, B., Herbette, G. and Corsini, S. (2004). The social
sharing of emotion: Illusory and real benefits of talking
about emotional experiences. In: Nyklic
ˇek, I., Temoshok,
L. & Vingerhoets, A. (eds.) Emotional Expression and
Health. Advances in theory, assessment, and clinical
applications. Hove/New York: Brunner-Routledge.
Salovey, P. and Mayer, J. D. (1990). Emotional intelligence.
Imagination, Cognition, Personality 9, 185–211.
Schwartz, G. E. and Kline, J. P. (1995). Repression, emo-
tional disclosure and health. In: Pennebaker, J. W. (ed.)
Emotion, Disclosure, and Health. Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
Smith, C. E., Fernengel, K., Holcroft, C., Gerald, K. and
Marien, L. (1994). Meta-analysis of the association
between social support and health outcomes. Behavior
and Medicine 16, 352–362.
Temoshok, L. (1983). Emotion, adaption, and disease. In:
van Dyke, C. & Zegans, L. S. (eds.) Emotions in Health
and Illness. New York: Grune and Stratton.
Traue, H. C. (1998). Emotion und Gesundheit. Die psycho-
biologische Regulation durch Hemmungen. Heidelberg:
Spektrum.
Traue, H. C. (2001). Emotional Inhibition and Health. In:
Smelser, N. J. & Baltes, P. B. (eds.) The international
encyclopaedia of the social and behavioural sciences,
pp. 4449–4454.
Traue, H. C. and Pennebaker, J. W. (eds.) (1993). Emotion,
Inhibition and Health. Toronto/Lewiston/Gottingen/
Bern: Hogrefe & Huber.
Emotional Inhibition 913
... 23 In contrast, expressive suppression is a negative strategy that requires individuals to suppress the external expression of emotions, potentially leading to internal emotional suppression and tension, adversely affecting psychological and physiological health. 24 Individuals with D-type personalities typically exhibit social inhibition, making them more likely to adopt negative expressive suppression strategies to cope with stress and negative emotions. 9,11 In Chinese culture, characterized by collectivism, individuals emphasize emotional restraint to control emotions by suppressing internal emotional experiences and behaviors, aiming to maintain social harmony and respect for others. ...
... 40 Over time, individuals inclined to use the Expressive Suppression strategy may change the neural systems iScience Article associated with emotions and stress responses. 43 Research indicates that prolonged emotional suppression may lead to increased stress hormones such as cortisol, 44 enhanced sympathetic nervous system activity, 24 decreased frontal lobe activity, and increased amygdala activity. 45 These physiological changes may be related to alterations in the brain-gut axis, subsequently influencing gastrointestinal function and health. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study examines the relationship between gastrointestinal symptoms in patients with functional gastrointestinal disorders (FGIDs) and type D personality traits, as well as emotion regulation strategies. Analyzing a diverse group of FGID patients, we uncover significant effects of gender and age on gastrointestinal symptoms. Negative Affectivity emerges as a key predictor, positively associated with symptom severity, whereas Social Inhibition correlates negatively with Abdominal Pain. Additionally, our findings suggest that the expressive suppression strategy predicts heightened gastrointestinal symptoms, whereas cognitive reappraisal predicts lower levels of certain symptoms. These findings provide valuable insights for precise diagnosis and tailored treatments of FGIDs. Further research is warranted to explore underlying mechanisms and inform evidence-based interventions.
... Considering the negative consequences of emotional awareness deficits beyond the daily level, it can be inferred that emotional inhibition and repression play significant roles in exacerbating emotional exhaustion over time (Panagopoulou et al., 2002;Traue et al., 2016). When individuals cannot feel their emotional arousal, they do not sufficiently respond to emotional information, interpret the situation, or handle emotional stressors. ...
... When individuals cannot feel their emotional arousal, they do not sufficiently respond to emotional information, interpret the situation, or handle emotional stressors. Prolonged emotional awareness deficits may lead to difficulty coping with occupational stress and high emotional work demands (Traue et al., 2016). Thus, our findings confirmed that recognizing negative emotions is beneficial in the present and the long term. ...
Article
The present study aimed to examine the role of daily emotional processes in the relationship between countertransference (CT) management and emotional well‐being in novice school counselors. We collected data for 7 consecutive days from 305 novice school counselors in South Korea. Multilevel mediation analyses revealed that novice school counselors with low CT management ability were less likely to be aware of their emotions, increasing their daily emotional exhaustion. In contrast, novice school counselors with a high level of CT management ability were more likely to engage in daily decentering and, in turn, were less likely to experience daily emotional exhaustion. Although emotional inhibition (lack of emotional awareness) significantly exacerbated emotional exhaustion over time, decentering was not related to emotional exhaustion over time. The practical and theoretical implications of this study are discussed.
... Emotional inhibition is characterized by an individual's maladaptive interaction with his or her social environment and impacts upon physiological, cognitive, and social-behavioral processes (Traue et al., 2016). The first theoretical model that came to prominence in research attempting to account for the beneficial effects of WED had inhibition at its core (Sloan and Marx, 2004), and initial expressive writing treatment programs were guided by the Freudian concept of (dis)inhibition and its proposed influence on health (cf., Pennebaker and Beall, 1986;Pennebaker, 1989). ...
... As such, the repression of grief-related emotions induced by COVID-19 (e.g., cancellation of sport competition) may induce discrepancies between athletes' overtly expressed emotions and their concomitant physiological state; any such perpetual emotional dissonance can promote the development of exhaustion (Kenworthy et al., 2014;Romo, 2017). More broadly, the inhibition of emotions associated with stress has a negative impact on health (Traue et al., 2016). ...
Article
Full-text available
The widespread effects of the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic have negatively impacted upon many athletes’ mental health and increased reports of depression as well as symptoms of anxiety. Disruptions to training and competition schedules can induce athletes’ emotional distress, while concomitant government-imposed restrictions (e.g., social isolation, quarantines) reduce the availability of athletes’ social and emotional support. Written Emotional Disclosure (WED) has been used extensively in a variety of settings with diverse populations as a means to promote emotional processing. The expressive writing protocol has been used to a limited extent in the context of sport and predominantly in support of athletes’ emotional processing during injury rehabilitation. We propose that WED offers an evidence-based treatment that can promote athletes’ mental health and support their return to competition. Research exploring the efficacy of the expressive writing protocol highlights a number of theoretical models underpinning the positive effects of WED; we outline how each of these potential mechanisms can address the multidimensional complexity of the challenging circumstances arising from the COVID-19 pandemic (e.g., loss of earnings, returning to training and competition). Considerations and strategies for using WED to support athletes during the COVID-19 pandemic are presented.
... In the field of medical psychology, which views emotions as internal states expressed in response to external stimuli, behavioral inhibition refers to emotional avoidance behaviors that individuals exhibit in unfamiliar social situations. "Deceptive inhibition" is the deliberate suppression of an emotional response by an individual under emotional stress, resulting in a "false" emotional display while regulating emotional experiences (Traue et al., 2016). In contrast, our approach conceives emotions not as internal states but as culturally contextual practices that mobilize values and interests (Ahmed, 2008;Scheer, 2012). ...
Article
Full-text available
Ghosting has mostly been associated with app-dating. However, our research addresses "ghosting" as a broader field of face-to-face avoidance practices. The article highlights the role of avoidance in emotional practices to maintain ongoing relationships. In the case of contemporary Spain, the structural uncertainty that permeates intimate relationships-related to neoliberalism's emphasis on individualism in the shaping of subjectivities-is reinforced by the tension in the coexistence of romantic and confluent models of love, which translates into a lack of scripts when it comes to dealing with intimacy. Drawing on the results of an ethnographic research project based on interviews with adults in the city of Madrid, we examine the ways in which social actors adapt their behavior to the context through what we have called "ghosting emotions." This analytical tool accounts for those individual strategies, which, as a result of an exercise of emotional reflexivity, limit re-lationality by avoiding certain social practices in the shaping of intimacy. Thus, this article shows the concrete processes through which actors develop patterns shaping structural dimensions in contemporary intimacies when facing uncertainties. To safeguard individuality within relationships, these practices function as a means of enhancing a sense of control by leaving "an open door".
... However, and whether we like it or not, emotions play a critical role in decision-making, performance, and overall well-being. It is impossible, undesirable, and detrimental to stop people from experiencing them (Mauss & Gross, 2004;Traue et al., 2016). Considering the role of human emotions has now become vital in all aspects of society, going from the economic value of a worker to the well-being of a child. ...
Thesis
We live in a world where data is considered one of the most valuable of all resources. Data science is a domain of research that is focused on (a) how to collect useful and meaningful data, (b) how to extract key features and variables contained within the recorded data and (c) how to interpret results within a given theoretical framework to give meaning to the findings. My PhD work applied the logic of data science to the case study of emotional body movements in the cognitive field of affective sciences. Emotions are at the cornerstone of human societies; they bind Humans together and they exert a decisive influence on all aspects of adaptive behavior. Emotions have the ability to modulate heart rate or even voluntary motor actions by making us move faster or slower. Most importantly, emotions change the way we move offering non-verbal cues on our inner affective states. After an introductory part on data science (Chapter 1), I present an overview of the theoretical frameworks applicable to the concepts of emotion and affect (Chapter 2). Chapter 3 is a methodological section offering a guideline to the good methods in affective sciences. More specifically, I present a step-by-step tutorial on how to collect good data in the study of emotional body experiences in young healthy adults, sitting or moving (questionnaires, physiological measures, kinematic data). The final part of the PhD manuscript presents three show cases. These three show cases will demonstrate that emotional body experience can be studied from different methodological perspectives but within a common theoretical framework. The first show case (Chapter 4) is centered around the effects of odor molecules on physiological and affective reactions. The analysis techniques used in this show case are heart-rate, heart-rate variability analyses as well as questionnaires (GEOS, affect grid). The second show case (Chapter 5) is centered around a complete description of the effects of emotions on whole-body movements in actors. This show case uses kinematics (e.g., speed, jerk) and time-series analysis (cross-wavelet coherence, auto-correlations) to account for the underlying evolutionary meaning of emotional influence on kinematics. The third and final show case (Chapter 6) is centered around the prediction of the emotional state of an actor, solely based on its kinematics. The main analysis technique of this show case is a deep convolutional neural network. Finally, to conclude this PhD manuscript, the chapter 7 will provide a general discussion on the results and on the perspective offered by taking a data science perspective to help tackle new theoretical challenges, in the field of emotions.
... stopping oneself from pressing a button in a task). For this introduction section I will focus on the non-emotional inhibition task, as these are relevant for the thesis (for a review on emotional inhibition see Traue et al., 2016). As such, cold executive inhibition is often assessed on tasks that require a suppression of motor responses (prepotent inhibition where an automatic, initiated motor response has to be stopped. ...
Conference Paper
Attention is a fundamentally important cognitive process and is required to efficiently navigate the world. Whilst altered attentional processes have been frequently observed in autistic people the differences seen suggest that attentional processes are different, however not necessarily deficient. In fact, aspects of superior visual perceptual ability and enhanced perceptual capacity have frequently been reported. The goal of the present thesis was to extend our knowledge of enhanced perceptual capacity under the framework of the Load Theory and to extend the findings to more active components of attention. To address this aim, the first three empirical studies I conducted, assessed selective and executive attention in autism and in a fourth study I investigated the feasibility of a neurofeedback intervention. Specifically, in Chapter 2, I used behavioural markers of congruency effects to consider whether cognitive capacity would be increased for autistic people, analogous to the enhanced perceptual capacity previously reported. In Chapter 3, I investigated electrophysiological aspects of visual working memory capacity and filtering efficiency. The findings were further expanded upon in Chapter 4 by directly contrasting visual working memory capacity and perceptual capacity using electrophysiological markers. Finally, I sought to assess whether practical steps could be taken to address altered attention experienced by autistic adults. The feasibility of an online neurofeedback intervention was investigated to assess whether aspects of attention and mental health could be improved through the training programme (Chapter 5). The findings of the thesis were then summarised and further discussed, highlighting the contribution to the autism attention literature and offering practical recommendations to harness attentional strengths in autism.
... Facial expressions, also described as "honest signals" by Pentland, can also be used to measure the quality of an interaction between humans (Pentland, 2010). According to most emotion experts, an emotion leads to a facial expression, along with individual differences, e.g., inhibition of facial expressiveness (Traue et al., 2016) or display rules (Ekman et al., 2003). However, not every facial expression must be based on a context-specific emotional experience and the source prompting the expression may thus remain unknown, e.g., "Othello's error" (Kreisler, 2004), although this aspect will not be discussed in detail in the present paper. ...
Article
Full-text available
In this brief report, we present the results of our investigation into the impact of age on reactions in the form of facial expressions to positive and negative feedback during human-computer interaction. In total, 30 subjects were analyzed after a video-recorded mental task in the style of a Wizard of Oz scenario. All subjects and their facial reactions were coded using the Facial Expression Coding System (FACES). To summarize briefly, we can conclude from our facial expression analysis that compared with their younger counterparts, elderly people show significantly lower levels of negative expression in response to positive feedback from the technical system (" Your performance is improving! "). This result indicates that elderly people seem to benefit more from praise during interaction than younger people, which is significant for the design of future companion technologies.
Article
Emotion suppression may be linked to poor health outcomes through elevated stress-related physiology. The current meta-analyses investigate the magnitude of the association between suppression and physiological responses to active psychological stress tasks administered in the laboratory. Relevant articles were identified through Medline, PsychINFO, PubMed, and ProQuest. Studies were eligible if they (a) used a sample of healthy, human subjects; (b) assessed physiology during a resting baseline and active psychological stress task; and (c) measured self-report or experimentally manipulated suppression. Twenty-four studies were identified and grouped within two separate random effects meta-analyses based on study methodology, namely, manipulated suppression (k = 12) and/or self-report (k = 14). Experimentally manipulated suppression was associated with greater physiological stress reactivity compared to controls (Hg = 0.20, 95% CI [0.08, 0.33]), primarily driven by cardiac, hemodynamic, and neuroendocrine parameters. Self-report trait suppression was not associated with overall physiological stress reactivity but was associated with greater neuroendocrine reactivity (r = 0.08, 95% CI [0.01, 0.14]). Significant moderator variables were identified (i.e., type/duration of stress task, nature of control instructions, type of physiology, and gender). This review suggests that suppression may exacerbate stress-induced physiological arousal; however, this may differ based upon the chosen methodological assessment of suppression.
Article
Full-text available
Saat individu berusaha mengatasi dampak emosional menghadapi keluarga retak, individu mencoba untuk melakukan refleksi atas peristiwa tersebut meski kadang sulit karena individu larut pada emosi negatif (ruminasi). Namun, individu juga mampu melihat pengalamannya secara adaptif dari perspektif pengamat dengan merespon tekanan dengan berjarak, sehingga mampu memaknai atau mendapatkan solusi dari tekanan. Penelitian ini bertujuan untuk menggambarkan proses refleksi diri adaptif ketika merespon peristiwa retaknya keluarga dengan cara menulis narasi dengan kata ganti personal satu (perspektif pelaku) dan menulis narasi dengan kata ganti personal nama-diri (perspektif pengamat). Riset Kross dkk. (2014) menyatakan bahwa kata ganti personal nama-diri merupakan metode untuk refleksi diri yang dianggap lebih adaptif dibandingkan dengan menggunakan kata ganti personal satu (perspektif diri pelaku) yang menyebabkan efek larut pada emosi negatif. Penelitian ini menggunakan pendekatan kualitatif dengan metode analisis isi. Subjek penelitian berjumlah tujuh orang (5 wanita dan 2 pria). Data diperoleh dari teks tulisan subjek yang mengikuti eksperimen menulis naratif selama empat hari berturut-turut. Hasil penelitian menunjukkan bahwa instruksi menulis dengan bahasa yang berjarak menggunakan perspektif pengamat, jika dibandingkan dengan instruksi menulis dengan menggunakan perspektif pelaku, pada level tertentu dapat membantu proses refleksi diri adaptif dengan mekanisme yang berbeda.
Article
Full-text available
This study aims at investigating associations between culture variables and emotion knowledge (focusing on knowledge reflecting emotional inhibition and somatization) in a questionnaire administered to 5080 subjects from 29 countries. The nation-level analysis investigated the association between various aspects of emotion knowledge on the one hand (including expressiveness, physical sensations, emotional inhibition, and somatization) and culture scales on the other hand, such as Hofstede's (1980) individualism-collectivism. The analysis of the culture scales revealed an unexpected inverse association between Hofstede's individualism and the study's own measures of individualistic attitudes. This was interpreted as being indicative of an especially strong new wave of individualism among students in traditionally collectivistic countries due to erosion of traditional values and the economic and social effects of globalization, a phenomenon described as "neoteric estrangement" (operationalized as high Hofstedian collectivism but high individualistic attitudes as measured by the study). Neoteric estrangement was associated with emotion knowledge reports reflecting high emotional inhibition, low expressiveness, high somatization, and low reported stress (the last having been found to be associated with inhibited emotional processing in interindividual studies). These results are discussed in terms of being evidence for the presence of more socially prescribed inhibition in traditionally collectivistic countries, and/or reflecting the idea that having an ambivalent relationship to the culture one lives in can induce emotional inhibition.
Article
Full-text available
The methods used by this . . . research group range from neurochemical and psychophysiological assessment to rating scales for nonverbal behavior. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Chapter
Emotions enhance the communication between individuals and their social environment. They give personal meaning to external and internal stimuli and communicate meaning from the individual to others. They are composed of interpretations of intero- and exteroreceptive stimuli, cognitions, physiological patterns of arousal, and motor behavior including overt emotional expressiveness. Emotional inhibition constitutes part of a maladaptive interaction between an individual and his or her social environment. It may be classified into different domains including the extreme form emotional implosion. Overt emotional inhibition generally is characterized by reduced expressiveness, unemotional language, and shyness, all of which is related to dysfunctional bodily reactions and may be adaptive in a short-term socialstress situation. In the long run, emotional inhibition is likely to have a harmful effect on the individual, which may occur along any of three pathways: neurobiological, social-behavioral, and cognitive. A heuristic model featuring these pathways and linking emotional responses with health problems is proposed. Most psychotherapeutic techniques are directed at emotional behavior and experience and focus largely on changes in intra- and interindividual emotional regulation and the construction of meaning from emotional experience.
Article
A summary of an emotion-focused approach to therapy (EFT) and its research base is presented. In this view, emotion is seen as foundational in the construction of the self and is a key determinant of self-organization. People, as well as simply having emotion, also live in a constant process of making sense of their emotions. In EFT, distinctions between different types of emotion provide therapists with a map for differential intervention. Six major empirically supported principles of emotion processing guide therapist interventions and serve as the goals of treatment. A case example illustrates how the principles of EFT helped a patient overcome her core maladaptive shame and basic insecurity in a relatively brief treatment of depression.
Article
This article presents a framework for emotional intelligence, a set of skills hypothesized to contribute to the accurate appraisal and expression of emotion in oneself and in others, the effective regulation of emotion in self and others, and the use of feelings to motivate, plan, and achieve in one's life. We start by reviewing the debate about the adaptive versus maladaptive qualities of emotion. We then explore the literature on intelligence, and especially social intelligence, to examine the place of emotion in traditional intelligence conceptions. A framework for integrating the research on emotion-related skills is then described. Next, we review the components of emotional intelligence. To conclude the review, the role of emotional intelligence in mental health is discussed and avenues for further investigation are suggested.
Article
Examined the effect of different types of social support on physical, psychological, and stress-related health outcomes. Meta-analysis was used to aggregate research findings across 60 published and 7 unpublished studies. Results were grouped according to operationally defined categories of social support and types of health outcomes. Effect size results of the grouped studies of social support and types of health outcomes ranged from –.02 to .22. Effect size results were equivalent when comparing studies grouped by gender of Ss, longitudinal vs cross-sectional design, and high or low quality. These effect size results between social support and the health outcomes measured indicate small amounts of shared variance that may not be considered significant or generalizable. Findings counter assumptions that social support is associated with improved health outcomes. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
"Emotion, Disclosure and Health" addresses some of the basic issues of psychology and psychotherapy: how people respond to emotional upheavals, why they respond the way they do, and why translating emotional events into language increases physical and mental health. Drawing on work in clinical, social, personality, and health psychology, as well as medical anthropology, the authors address these issues, drawing some stimulating conclusions about how an understanding of disclosure and health may be applied in clinically useful ways. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)