ArticlePDF Available

Behavioral Interventions for Infant Sleep Problems: A Randomized Controlled Trial

Authors:
  • WINK Sleep Pty Ltd

Abstract

Objectives: To evaluate the effects of behavioral interventions on the sleep/wakefulness of infants, parent and infant stress, and later child emotional/behavioral problems, and parent-child attachment. Methods: A total of 43 infants (6-16 months, 63% girls) were randomized to receive either graduated extinction (n = 14), bedtime fading (n = 15), or sleep education control (n = 14). Sleep measures included parent-reported sleep diaries and infant actigraphy. Infant stress was measured via morning and afternoon salivary cortisol sampling, and mothers' self-reported mood and stress. Twelve months after intervention, mothers completed assessments of children's emotional and behavioral problems, and mother-child dyads underwent the strange situation procedure to evaluate parent-child attachment. Results: Significant interactions were found for sleep latency (P < .05), number of awakenings (P < .0001), and wake after sleep onset (P = .01), with large decreases in sleep latency for graduated extinction and bedtime fading groups, and large decreases in number of awakenings and wake after sleep onset for the graduated extinction group. Salivary cortisol showed small-to-moderate declines in graduated extinction and bedtime fading groups compared with controls. Mothers' stress showed small-to-moderate decreases for the graduated extinction and bedtime fading conditions over the first month, yet no differences in mood were detected. At the 12-month follow-up, no significant differences were found in emotional and behavioral problems, and no significant differences in secure-insecure attachment styles between groups. Conclusions: Both graduated extinction and bedtime fading provide significant sleep benefits above control, yet convey no adverse stress responses or long-term effects on parent-child attachment or child emotions and behavior.
ARTICLE
PEDIATRICS Volume 137 , number 6 , June 2016 :e 20151486
Behavioral Interventions
for Infant Sleep Problems: A
Randomized Controlled Trial
Michael Gradisar, PhD,
a Kate Jackson, PhD, ClinPsyc,
a Nicola J. Spurrier, PhD,
b Joyce Gibson, MNutrDiet,
c
Justine W hitham, PhD,
a Anne Sved Williams, MBBS, FR ANZCP,
d, e Robyn Dolby, PhD,
b David J. Kennaway, PhDf
abstract
OBJECTIVES: To evaluate the effects of behavioral interventions on the sleep/wakefulness
of infants, parent and infant stress, and later child emotional/behavioral problems, and
parent-child attachment.
METHODS: A total of 43 infants (6–16 months, 63% girls) were randomized to receive either
graduated extinction (n = 14), bedtime fading (n = 15), or sleep education control (n =
14). Sleep measures included parent-reported sleep diaries and infant actigraphy. Infant
stress was measured via morning and afternoon salivary cortisol sampling, and mothers’
self-reported mood and stress. Twelve months after intervention, mothers completed
assessments of children’s emotional and behavioral problems, and mother-child dyads
underwent the strange situation procedure to evaluate parent-child attachment.
RESULTS: Significant interactions were found for sleep latency (P < .05), number of
awakenings (P < .0001), and wake after sleep onset (P = .01), with large decreases in sleep
latency for graduated extinction and bedtime fading groups, and large decreases in number
of awakenings and wake after sleep onset for the graduated extinction group. Salivary
cortisol showed small-to-moderate declines in graduated extinction and bedtime fading
groups compared with controls. Mothers’ stress showed small-to-moderate decreases
for the graduated extinction and bedtime fading conditions over the first month, yet no
differences in mood were detected. At the 12-month follow-up, no significant differences
were found in emotional and behavioral problems, and no significant differences in secure-
insecure attachment styles between groups.
CONCLUSIONS: Both graduated extinction and bedtime fading provide significant sleep benefits
above control, yet convey no adverse stress responses or long-term effects on parent-child
attachment or child emotions and behavior.
a School of Psychology, and cDepartment of Nutrition and Dietetics, Flinders University, Adelaide, South Australia,
Australia; bPublic Health and Health Promotion, SA Health, South Australia, Australia; dDepartment of Psychiatry,
and fSchool of Paediatrics and Reproductive Health, University of Adelaide, Adelaide, South Australia, Australia;
and eWomen’s and Children’s Health Network, Adelaide, South Australia, Australia
Dr Gradisar provided co-conception and design of the study, supervision of most of the analyses
and conducted the rest of analyses, signifi cant contribution to the interpretation of analyses,
and was main contributor to writing of manuscript; Dr Jackson provided co-conception and
design of the study, collected most of the data (pretreatment to 3 months), conducted most of the
analyses, provided signifi cant input to the literature reviewed, was a minor contributor to writing
of the manuscript, and provided critical evaluation of manuscript drafts; Dr Spurrier provided
co-conception and design of the study, critical evaluation of manuscript drafts, and interpretation
of fi ndings; Ms Gibson provided input into study design, collected most of the 12-month follow-up
data, and provided interpretation of fi ndings and critical evaluation of manuscript drafts;
Dr Whitham provided input into the study design, conducted data scoring and analyses of 12-month
To cite: Gradisar M, Jackson K, Spurrier NJ, et al. Behavioral Interventions
for Infant Sleep Problems: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Pediatrics.
2016;137(6):e20151486
WH AT’S KNO WN ON T HI S SUB JEC T: Recommended
guidelines exist for the treatment of nocturnal
wakefulness for infants, including graduated
extinc tion and bedtime fading. Such interven tions
have a solid evidence base for improving infant sleep,
yet lit tle is known about their contraindications.
WHAT THIS STUDY ADDS: Sustained improvements in
sleep latency were found for both trea tment groups,
but not controls, yet no signifi c ant differences
occurred in the infant salivary cortisol, parental
stress and mood, and attachment profi les between
all groups.
by guest on January 10, 2017Downloaded from
GRADISAR et al
Nocturnal wakefulness is normal
during early infant development
(ie, first 12 months).1–3 Night
wakings allow infants to signal
parents’ provision of sustenance
and comfort.1, 3, 4 At 2 months of age,
sleep homeostatic pressure develops,
potentially compressing nighttime
awakenings.2 Large declines in
nocturnal wakefulness occur (on
average) over the first 6 months
of age, and plateau thereafter,
2, 5
occurring after 24-hour circadian
rhythm stabilization at 3 to 6
months of age.6, 7 Infants’ nighttime
awakenings typically diminish by
the end of the first year of life3;
however, from 6 months, 16% to
21% of infants continue experiencing
nocturnal wakefulness such that
parents report their child has a
“sleep problem.”8, 9 Sleep problems
may be due to reduced sleep
homeostatic pressure2 and/or a
“coercive behavior trap, ” whereby
parents’ nocturnal responses are
more reinforcing than sleep.1 Aside
from an array of known daytime
impairments that follow from sleep
restriction in adults,
10 serious
consequences have been reported
for families who have an infant with
a sleep problem. Mothers of infants
with a sleep problem are more likely
to use physical punishment,
11, 12 and
are at an increased risk of developing
depression (eg, >2.0 odds ratio).9, 13–15
Also worth noting is that mothers
with emotional disturbances (ie,
stress, depression) are more likely to
report intrusive thoughts of harming
their infant,
16, 17 and some even
commit filicide.18–20 Thus, evidenced-
based treatments that rapidly
resolve infants’ sleeplessness while
minimizing family distress are vital.
Several interventions exist for
infants’ sleeplessness. Some with
the strongest evidence are based on
psychological learning theory (ie,
operant conditioning), where the
parents’ response to their infant’s
nocturnal cries are totally ignored
(extinction), or initially delayed
(eg, 2 minutes) and then gradually
extended up to 6 minutes on the
first night (graduated extinction).21
Despite a longstanding solid evidence
base for improving infant sleep and
maternal mood and stress both in the
short- and long-term,
21, 22 concerns
have nonetheless been raised. The
immediate stress experienced
by parents while undertaking
extinction-based methods can often
lead to ceasing such techniques.23, 24
However, arguably more important
(and central to the current article)
is that the stress associated with
extinction-based treatments might
elevate cortisol levels that could
have long-term consequences
of infant helplessness, and later
insecure parent-child attachments
and child emotional and behavioral
problems.3, 25 This antithesis to
using extinction-based methods
has strong support, to the extent
that more gentle approaches are
sought. Bedtime fading is one such
technique.21 Based on the physiologic
theory of sleep homeostasis,
26, 27
bedtime fading indirectly compresses
sleep by gradually limiting time in
bed, usually by delaying the infant’s
bedtime by 15 minutes each night.21
The current study’s aim is to compare
effects from graduated extinction
and bedtime fading against a sleep
education control on infants’ sleep,
stress, attachment, and emotional
and behavioral problems, as well
as maternal mood and stress, over
3 months of treatment and at a
12-month follow-up. The primary
outcome was infants’ sleep (sleep
latency, number of wakings, wake
after sleep onset), and secondary
outcomes were infants’ cortisol,
parents’ stress and mood, child-
parent attachment, and child
behavior. Descriptions and the timing
of these measures are presented in
Table 1.
METHODS
Participants
A total of 43 infants (mean age 10.8 ±
3.5 months, 6–16 months, 63% girls)
and their mothers (mean age 33.3 ±
4.8 years) and fathers (age 35.5 ±
6.4 years) were randomly assigned
to receive graduated extinction (n
= 14), bedtime fading (n = 15), or
sleep education control (n = 14).
Inclusion criteria were the following:
1 parent identifying their child
having a “sleep problem” (parents
responded with “yes” to the question
“Do you think your child has a sleep
problem?”)29; doctor/health nurse
check 1 month where the infant
was healthy and attained expected
weight gain; and typical infant
development (Infant Development
2
TABLE 1 Timing of Screening Measures, the Primary Outcome Variable, and Secondary Outcomes
Pretreatment 1 wk 1 mo 3 mo 12 mo
Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (screen for
postnatal depression)
Infant Development Inventory (screen typical
development)
Sleep diary (primary outcome [sleep])
Actigraphy (objective measure of sleep)
Salivary cortisol (secondary outcome: infant
stress)
•• •
DASS-21 (secondary outcome: parent stress/
mood)
••
Child Behavior Checklist 1.5–5.0 y (secondary
outcome: child behavior/emotion)
Strange situation procedure (secondary outcome:
child-parent attachment)
Child Behavior Checklist and DASS-21 were blind scored by JW; strange situation procedure was blind scored by RD;
actigraphy data were automatically scored using the ActiWare (v.5, Philips Respironics, Bend, OR) computerized algorithm
after manual correction of bedtimes from sleep diaries.28 Sleep diaries were scored by KJ.
by guest on January 10, 2017Downloaded from
PEDIATRICS Volume 137 , number 6 , June 2016
Inventory). Exclusion criteria were
the following: significant postnatal
depression (Edinburgh Postnatal
Depression Scale scores 15), and
indication of suicidality. Most parents
were in a marriagelike relationship
(92.7%), had higher education
qualifications (77.2%), and were
middle- to high-income earners
(>AUD$80 000; 48.7%). Mothers
were mainly responsible for their
infant’s sleep (69.3%; 26.9% shared
responsibility). Table 2 presents
the sample demographics. Parents
provided informed consent to
participate. The study was approved
by the Flinders University Social
and Behavioral Research Ethics
Committee.
Procedure
Parents contacted researchers
in response to advertisements at
pediatric outpatient clinics, child
care centers, health professional
private practices, newspapers, and
word-of-mouth. Parents completed
a questionnaires and a 90-minute
interview (conducted by K.J.)
assessing the infant’s medical and
sleep history. Eligible families
were randomly assigned via a
predetermined computerized block
randomization held by K.J. (random
allocation was not concealed from
K.J.). Due to ethics committee
requirements, parents were allowed
to swap conditions. This occurred
for 2 families (1 from graduated
extinction to sleep education control;
1 from sleep education control to
graduated extinction).
During pretreatment, families
completed sleep measures (7-day
sleep diary; ankle-worn activity
monitor [AW64 Minimitters;
Philips Respironics, Bend, OR]),
and maternal depressed mood
and stress (depression and stress
subscales of the Depression Anxiety
Stress Scale–Short Form [DASS-
21]). Parents collected infant saliva
samples in the morning (mean ±
SD time 8:59 AM ±1.27 hours) and
afternoon (mean ± SD time 3:38 PM
±1.24 hours) on 2 consecutive days
by using sterile cotton-tipped eye
spears.30 Blinded frozen samples
(–20°C) were thawed and centrifuged
before cortisol analysis in duplicate
by enzyme-linked immunosorbent
assay (Salimetrics, LCC, State College,
PA). The group origin of the samples
was blinded to the laboratory, and
samples were within acceptable
validity criteria (the intra-assay
coefficient was <10%, and the
interassay coefficients at 3.0 nM
and 27.5 nM were 13.5% and 9.3%,
respectively).31
Parents subsequently underwent
their individualized treatment
session. Graduated extinction
involved a set schedule of gradually
delaying parents’ response to their
infant’s cry (see Supplemental Table
4). Parents were instructed to put
their infant to bed awake and leave
the room within 1 minute. When
reentering the room, they comforted
their child, but avoided picking the
child up and turning the lights on.
Bedtime fading involved gradually
limiting the infant’s nocturnal
sleep opportunity (Supplemental
Table 4). For the sleep education
control, parents were provided
sleep information from a statewide
child health service (www. cyh. com;
Supplemental Table 4). Families
received a booklet describing their
intervention, and 24/7 cell phone
support (by K.J.). Of calls received,
these included clarification of
techniques, delaying treatment, and
clarifying measurements (saliva,
actigraphy), with no perceived
differences in the number of calls
between groups (unfortunately, no
systematic data were recorded for
these calls; thus, these data are based
on our retrospective recall).
Parents participated with their child
in the strange situation procedure
to assess child-parent attachment.32
This procedure used 8 standardized
interactions (including separations
and reunions) between the parent,
child, and a stranger (the stranger
was blind to children’s group
allocation), which were videotaped
and blind scored (by R.D.) according
3
TABLE 2 Demographic Characteristics of Participating Families, by Treatment Group
Graduated
Extinction
Bedtime Fading Education Control
Child
Age, mean ± SD, mo 10.8 ± 3.7 11.8 ± 3.4 9.61 ± 3.3
Boys, n (%) 5 (39) 5 (31) 5 (42)
Mother
Age, mean ± SD, y 31.38 ± 4.4 34.44 ± 5.1 33.82 ± 4.3
Education status, n (%)
Did not complete high school 1 (8.3)
Completed high school 2 (15.4) 3 (18.8) 1 (8.3)
Completed higher education 11 (84.6) 13 (81.3) 10 (83.3)
Father
Age, mean ± SD, y 33.38 ± 5.4 35.67 ± 7.0 37.73 ± 6.2
Education status, n (%)
Did not complete high school 1 (7.1) 3 (27.3)
Completed high school 2 (15.4) 5 (35.7)
Completed higher education 11 (84.6) 8 (57.1) 8 (72.2)
Family
Parental status, n (%)
Two parents 13 (100) 14 (87.5) 11 (91.7)
Single parent 2 (12.5) 1 (8.3)
Household income per annum, n
(%)*
<50 000 4 (26.7) 3 (25.0)
50 000–80 000 4 (33.3) 5 (33.3) 4 (33.3)
>80 000 8 (66.7) 6 (40.0) 5 (41.7)
Data missing for n = 1 for each of graduated extinction and bedtime fading groups. —, no data.
by guest on January 10, 2017Downloaded from
GRADISAR et al
to gold standard criteria. Recordings
of each child-parent dyad were coded
as either secure, insecure (avoidant),
insecure (resistant), and insecure
(disorganized).
Analysis
Linear mixed-model regressions
were used to test for significant
interactions on primary (sleep diary)
and secondary (infant cortisol;
maternal stress/mood) outcome
variables. Within-group Cohen’s
d effect sizes were calculated to
demonstrate changes within each
intervention. A time-of-day effect
was found between morning
and afternoon cortisol values
(pretreatment, P < .01), thus morning
and afternoon cortisol values are
analyzed separately. Fisher’s exact
test was used to assess differences in
attachment styles between groups.
A series of 1-way analyses of variance
assessed between-group differences
on the Child Behavior Checklist.
No effect sizes were available for
using bedtime fading on typically
developing infants, nor some of
the important secondary outcomes
tested in the current trial (eg, salivary
cortisol). Large effect sizes (Cohen’s
d > 0.80) for graduated extinction
(compared with controls) have been
reported for sleep.21 Based on a
power of 0.80, and a probability level
of .05, at least 21 participants per
group would be needed to detect a
large effect size.
RESULTS
Figure 1 presents the flow of
participants through each stage of
the study.
Infant Sleep
Figure 2 presents descriptive
statistics for infants’ sleep over the
3-month treatment and follow-up
period. Linear mixed model
regression analyses of sleep diaries
showed significant interactions for
sleep latency, P < .05, wake after
sleep onset, P < .0001, number of
awakenings, P = .01, and total night
sleep time, P < .01. From pretreatment
to the 3-month follow-up, sleep
latency showed large declines
for infants in both the graduated
extinction (–12.7 minutes; d = 0.87)
and bedtime fading (–10 minutes;
d = 1.04) groups, but no change for
the control group (+2.0 minutes; d =
–0.11). There was a very large decline
in the number of awakenings for
infants in the graduated extinction
group (d = 1.98), yet no changes for
infants in both the bedtime fading (d
= 0.10) and control (d = 0.13) groups.
Large improvements in wake after
sleep onset were found for infants in
the control (–31.7 minutes; d = 0.93)
and bedtime fading (–24.6 minutes;
d = 0.99) groups, with a very large
improvement found for the graduated
extinction group (–44.4 minutes;
d = 2.02). Finally, total sleep time
showed a moderate increase in the
graduated extinction condition (+0.32
hour), little change in the bedtime
fading condition (+0.09 hour), and
4
FIGURE 1
Participant fl ow through the randomized controlled trial.
by guest on January 10, 2017Downloaded from
PEDIATRICS Volume 137 , number 6 , June 2016
a moderate increase in the control
condition (+0.36 hour). Actigraphy
showed no significant interactions for
wake after sleep onset, P > .05, and
total sleep time, P > .05.
Infant and Parental Stress
Morning and afternoon infant cortisol
values for each group over time are
presented in Fig 3. No significant
interactions occurred for morning
cortisol, P > .05, yet there was for
afternoon cortisol values, P < .01.
From pretreatment to the 12-month
follow-up, morning cortisol showed
a small decline in the graduated
extinction group (d = 0.23), a
moderate drop in the bedtime fading
group (d = 0.62), yet no change
for the control group (d = 0.17).
Afternoon cortisol showed a large
decline in the graduated extinction
group (d = 0.89), a moderate decline
in the bedtime fading group (d =
0.61) and a small decline in the
control group (d = 0.39).
A significant interaction was found
for maternal stress, P < .01 (Fig 3).
There were moderate improvements
in all groups from pretreatment to
12-month follow-up (graduated
extinction: d = 0.51; bedtime fading:
d = 0.62; control: d = 0.64). However,
over the initial month of treatment,
mothers’ stress in the control group
remained somewhat unchanged (d =
0.16), whereas there was a moderate
stress reduction for mothers in the
graduated extinction group (d = 0.67)
and a large reduction in mothers’
stress in the bedtime fading group
(d = 0.86). Analysis of maternal
mood demonstrated no significant
interaction, P > .05. Mothers’ mood
improved from pretreatment to the
12-month follow-up in all conditions,
with small effects found for those in
the graduated extinction (d = 0.34)
and control (d = 0.39) conditions,
yet a large effect for mothers in the
bedtime fading group (d = 0.83).
Twelve-Month Follow-up: Child
Attachment and Emotional-
Behavioral Problems
Table 3 presents the percentage
of parent-child attachment
classifications in each group. No
significant differences were found
5
FIGURE 2
Means (95% confi dence interval [CI]) during and after treatment of infants in graduated extinction, bedtime fading, and sleep education control for (A)
sleep onset latency, (B) wake after sleep onset, (C) number of awakenings, and (D) total nocturnal sleep time.
by guest on January 10, 2017Downloaded from
GRADISAR et al
between secure and insecure
attachment styles between groups,
P > .05. There were no significant
differences between groups for any
emotional or behavioral problems on
the Child Behavior Checklist (all P >
.05; Table 3).
DISCUSSION
Compared with controls, graduated
extinction produced large decreases
in nocturnal wakefulness (time taken
to fall asleep, number of awakenings,
minutes awake after sleep onset),
yet a novel aspect of this trial was
evaluating bedtime fading, about
which there has been relatively little
research,
22 especially in typically
developing infants.33, 34 Bedtime
fading produced large decreases in
sleep latency compared with the
control group. The control group’s
sleep did show improvements in
nocturnal wakefulness and total
sleep, suggesting developmental
maturity and/or improvements from
sleep education.35 No significant
sleep changes were found by using
objective actigraphy, suggesting
sleep diaries and actigraphy measure
different phenomena (eg, infants’
absence of crying by parents vs
infants’ movements, respectively),
further suggesting infants may
still experience wakefulness but
do not signal to parents.36 We
do not interpret these data as
the infant “giving up, 25, 37 but
instead self-soothing.36 However,
a central research question in the
current study was: Do extinction-
based techniques produce
psychophysiological stress that leads
to later problematic emotions and
behavior, and thus insecure parent-
child attachment?
Child-Parent Stress, Emotions,
Behaviors, and Attachment
Relatively minor stressors (eg, brief
parental separation) elevate cor tisol
levels in newborn infants38; however,
the cortisol stress response diminishes
from 4 months of age.30, 38, 39
This may explain the lack of
significant cortisol elevation in the
graduated extinction condition in
the current study, especially as our
sample was older than 4 months of
6
FIGURE 3
Means (95% CI) during and after treatment of infants in graduated extinction, bedtime fading, and sleep education control for (A) infant salivary cortisol
morning samples, (B) infant salivary cortisol afternoon samples, (C) self-reported maternal stress, and (D) self-reported maternal mood.
by guest on January 10, 2017Downloaded from
PEDIATRICS Volume 137 , number 6 , June 2016
age (range 6–16 months; mean 10.8
months). We cannot conclude no
cortisol response occurred, as we did
not collect real-time cortisol data (ie,
plasma) during nocturnal treatment
implementation. Our diurnal cortisol
data indicate the active treatments
did not result in chronically elevated
levels over time (ie, values were
within normative limits),
40 which is
necessary for hypothalamic-pituitary-
adrenocortical dysregulation.38
This is a crucial point when
considering the chain of arguments
forming the hypothesis that
graduated extinction may lead to
problematic emotions and behaviors
in later child development.25 This
hypothesis requires a significant and
chronic cortisol elevation resulting
from graduated extinction, yet is
further disconfirmed by examining
our long-term emotional and
behavioral findings. No significant
difference in children’s emotions and
behaviors could be found between
groups 12 months after intervention,
with internalizing and externalizing
behaviors comparable across groups.
This lack of findings concur with a
recent 6-year follow-up assessment
of a large randomized controlled trial,
where no differences in problematic
behaviors and mental health were
found between children who received
behavioral sleep interventions and
those in the control group.41
The final argument against using
extinction-based methods for infant
sleep problems is the potential for
insecure child-parent attachment.25
No significant differences were
found in attachment styles between
groups, which suggests a lack of
evidence between infants’ sleep and
attachment.5 For parental stress,
mothers in both intervention groups
reported less stress than mothers
in the control group. The lack of
support for dysfunctional child-
parent relationships (ie, disinhibited
attachment, child-parent closeness
and conflict, global relationship) after
behavioral sleep interventions has also
been found in a recent 6-year follow-up
study.41 Altogether, our findings and
those of Price et al.41 are the only
studies that now form a preliminary
evidence base that suggests brief
behavioral sleep treatments may help
young children sleep, yet do not lead
to later emotional and behavioral
problems, or later parent-child
insecure attachment.25
Limitations and Future Research
Directions
Although the generalizability of
our findings to the population is
reduced with a small sample size,
they nevertheless support those of a
larger follow-up study of behavioral
sleep interventions for infants (n =
326).41 Although stress was measured
at different time points across
interventions, we did not measure
acute stress during interventions.
Changes in attachment across the
study would also have been interesting,
yet we note that 12 months of age is
the prime age for the strange situation
procedure.32 The current study
contrasted only 2 behavioral sleep
interventions. Future trials are needed
7
TABLE 3 Child Behavior Checklist Scores (Mean [95% CI] and Between-Group Effect Sizes [d]) at 12-Month Follow-up, by Treatment Group; Attachment
Classifi cations (n [%]) by Group
Graduated
Extinction, n = 13
Bedtime Fading,
n = 15
Education
Control, n = 12
Pd (Graduated
Extinction versus
Bedtime Fading)
d (Graduated
Extinction versus
Education
Control)
d (Bedtime Fading
versus Education
Control)
Problem scale scores
Internalizing
problems
Raw scores 5.00 (2.7–7.3) 6.06 (3.9–8.2) 5.08 (1.8–8.4) .77 –0.27 –0.02 0.21
T scorea 60, n
(%)
1 (6.3) 1 (8.3) .59
Externalizing
problems
Raw scores 10.6 (6.4–14.8) 10.6 (7.7–13.5) 10.2 (5.5–14.9) .98 0.01 0.06 0.06
T score 60, n (%) 1 (7.7) 1 (6.3) 2 (16.6) .67
Total problems
Raw scores 10.8 (6.8–14.7) 10.9 (8.2–13.6) 12.1 (7.6–16.6) .84 –0.02 –0.19 –0.20
T score 60, n (%) ——— —— —
Attachment
classifi cations
Secure 7 (54) 9 (60) 5 (62) —— —
Insecure (avoidant) 1 (8) 1 (7) 1 (13) —— —
Insecure (resistant) 2 (15) 3 (20) 1 (13) —— —
Insecure
(disorganized)
3 (23) 2 (13) 1 (13) —— —
—, not applicable.
a If T score in the borderline or clinical range; d > 0.50 = moderate effect, d > 0.20 = small effect; data missing for graduated extinction group (n = 1) and sleep education control group
(n = 1).
by guest on January 10, 2017Downloaded from
GRADISAR et al
to compare other interventions (eg,
extinction with parental presence,
cosleeping via room-sharing). Due
to ethical obligations and a coroner’s
inquiry in our home state,
42 bed-
sharing was not an option to evaluate
because of infant mortality risks,
and we have seen similar warnings
internationally.43, 44 Worth noting is
that, to our knowledge, we have been
unable to find a similar coronial finding
of an infant death due to graduated
extinction. Thus, we wish to highlight
to researchers the crucial need to build
the evidence base for establishing
safe and effective treatments for those
families who perceive their infant to
have a sleep problem.
Clinical Implications
Although graduated extinction is
based on learning theory,
1 sleep
homeostatic pressure would increase
over successive nights of less
sleep.26, 27 Likewise, bedtime fading
would increase sleep homeostatic
pressure, quickening sleep latency,
reducing the time between stimuli
(eg, cot) and the response (ie, sleep),
and thus infants “re-learn” to initiate
sleep. This theoretical overlap allows
these treatments to combine in
clinical practice (eg, bedtime fading
followed by graduated extinction).
Our data suggest introducing bedtime
fading will provide quick results
for improving sleep-onset latency.
Graduated extinction may then be
introduced to reduce nocturnal
wakefulness during the night (if
needed). Our data suggest sleep
education alone may not be enough
to help most families with an infant
who has a sleep problem.
CONCLUSIONS
This randomized controlled trial of
behavioral interventions for infant
sleep problems found meaningful
effects for both graduated extinction
and bedtime fading. Compared with
the control group, large reductions
in nocturnal wakefulness resulted
from each treatment. Despite
assertions that extinction-based
methods may result in elevated
cortisol, emotional and behavioral
problems, and insecure parent-infant
attachment, our data did not support
this hypothesis. Coupled with the
findings from Price and colleagues,
41
behavioral interventions appear to
improve sleep without detrimental
effects on the child or family. Further
replication studies by independent
groups are needed to confirm and
raise confidence in these findings.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The authors thank the families who
generously donated their time and
energy into this project, Dr Patricia
McKinsey Crittenden for her expert
advice regarding the conduct of
strange situation assessments, Ms
Laura Jarema for bravely being the
“stranger, ” and Mr Pawel Skuza and
Megan Gunnar for their conceptual
and statistical advice for cortisol
collection and analysis. We also
thank the Flinders University Social
and Behavioral Ethics Committee
for their impartial evaluation of
this project in response to an online
petition to cease this approved
research.
8
ABBREVIATION
DASS-21:  Depression Anxiety
Stress Scale–Short
Form
follow-up data and the minority of analyses under supervision, provided some input to the literature reviewed and was a minor contributor to writing of
manuscript, and provided critical evaluation of manuscript drafts; Dr Sved Williams provided assistance with design of the study, interpretation of fi ndings, and
critical evaluation of manuscript drafts; Dr Powell-Davies provided input into the study design, scored the infant-parent attachment, and provided interpretation
of fi ndings and critical evaluation of manuscript drafts; Dr Kennaway provided input into study design, expert input into salivary cortisol data collection, scoring
of salivary cortisol, interpretation of fi ndings, and critical evaluation of manuscript drafts; and all authors approved the fi nal manuscript as submitted.
This trial has been registered at the Australian New Zealand Clinical Trials Registry (identifi er ACTRN12612000813886)
DOI: 10.1542/peds.2015-1486
Accepted for publication Mar 21, 2016
Address correspondence to Michael Gradisar, PhD, c/o Flinders University, School of Psychology, GPO Box 2100, Adelaide, SA, 5001, Australia. E-mail: grad0011@
inders.edu.au
PEDIATRICS (ISSN Numbers: Print, 0031-4005; Online, 1098-4275).
Copyright © 2016 by the American Academy of Pediatrics
FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no fi nancial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.
FUNDING: Supported by Australian Rotary Health Fund, Channel 7 Children’s Research Fund, Faculty of Social and Behavioral Sciences.
POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: The authors have indicated they have no potential confl icts of interest to disclose.
by guest on January 10, 2017Downloaded from
PEDIATRICS Volume 137 , number 6 , June 2016
REFERENCES
1. Blampied NM, France KG. A b
ehavioral model of infant sleep
disturbance. J Appl Behav Anal.
1993;26(4):477–492
2. Jenni OG, Borbély AA, Achermann P.
Development of the nocturnal sleep
electroencephalogram in human
infants. Am J Physiol Regul Integr
Comp Physiol. 2004;286(3):R528–R538
3. Scher A. Attachment and sleep: a
study of night waking in 12-month-
old infants. Dev Psychobiol.
2001;38(4):274–285
4. Rottenberg J, Bylsma LM, Vingerhoets
AJJM. Is crying benefi cial? Curr Dir
Psychol Sci. 2008;17:400–404
5. Weinraub M, Bender RH, Friedman
SL, et al. Patterns of developmental
change in infants’ nighttime
sleep awakenings from 6 through
36 months of age. Dev Psychol.
2012;48(6):1511–1528
6. Jenni OG, Deboer T, Achermann P.
Development of the 24-h rest-activity
pattern in human infants. Infant Behav
Dev. 2006;29(2):143–152
7. Joseph D, Chong NW, Shanks ME,
et al. Getting rhythm: how do babies
do it? Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed.
2015;100(1):F50–F54
8. Martin J, Hiscock H, Hardy P,
Davey B, Wake M. Adverse associations
of infant and child sleep problems and
parent health: an Australian population
study. Pediatrics. 2007;119(5):947–955
9. Wake M, Morton-Allen E, Poulakis Z,
Hiscock H, Gallagher S, Oberklaid F.
Prevalence, stability, and outcomes
of cry-fuss and sleep problems in
the fi rst 2 years of life: prospective
community-based study. Pediatrics.
2006;117(3):836–842
10. Banks S, Dinges DF. Behavioral and
physiological consequences of
sleep restriction. J Clin Sleep Med.
2007;3(5):519–528
11. Johnson CM. Infant and toddler sleep:
a telephone survey of parents in one
community. J Dev Behav Pediatr.
1991;12(2):108–114
12. Korsch BM, Christian JB, Gozzi
EK, Carlson PV. Infant care and
punishment: a pilot study. Am
J Public Health Nations Health.
1965;55(12):1880–1888
13. Hiscock H, Wake M. Infant sleep
problems and postnatal depression:
a community-based study. Pediatrics.
2001;107(6):1317–1322
14. Dennis CL, Ross L. Relationships
among infant sleep patterns,
maternal fatigue, and development
of depressive symptomatology. Birth.
2005;32(3):187–193
15. Hatton DC, Harrison-Hohner J, Coste
S, Dorato V, Curet LB, McCarron DA.
Symptoms of postpartum depression
and breastfeeding. J Hum Lact.
2005;21(4):444–449, quiz 450–454
16. Fairbrother N, Woody SR. New mothers’
thoughts of harm related to the
newborn. Arch Women Ment Health.
2008;11(3):221–229
17. Jennings KD, Ross S, Popper
S, Elmore M. Thoughts of harming
infants in depressed and
nondepressed mothers. J Affect
Disord. 1999;54(1–2):21–28
18. Erikson L, Mazerolle P, Wortley R,
Johnson H. Maternal and paternal
licide: case studies from the
Australian Homicide Project. Child
Abuse Review. 2016;25(1):17–30
19. Trautmann-Villalba P, Hornstein C.
Children murdered by their mothers
in the postpartum period [in German].
Nervenarzt. 2007;78(11):1290–1295
20. Bartels L, Patricia E. Mothers who kill:
the forensic use and judicial reception
of evidence of postnatal depression
and other psychiatric disorders in
Australian fi licide cases. Melbourne
University Law Review 2013;15:37.
Available at: http:// www5. austlii. edu.
au/ au/ journals/ MelbULawRw/ 2013/ 15.
html. Accessed February 24, 2015
21. Mindell JA, Kuhn B, Lewin DS, Meltzer
LJ, Sadeh A; American Academy of
Sleep Medicine. Behavioral treatment
of bedtime problems and night
wakings in infants and young children.
Sleep. 2006;29(10):1263–1276
22. Morgenthaler TI, Owens J, Alessi C,
et al; American Academy of Sleep
Medicine. Practice parameters for
behavioral treatment of bedtime
problems and night wakings in
infants and young children. Sleep.
2006;29(10):1277–1281
23. Sadeh A, Tikotzky L, Scher A. Parenting
and infant sleep. Sleep Med Rev.
2010;14(2):89–96
24. Middlemiss W, Granger DA, Goldberg
WA, Nathans L. Asynchrony of mother-
infant hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal
axis activity following extinction of
infant crying responses induced
during the transition to sleep. Early
Hum Dev. 2012;88(4):227–232
25. Blunden SL, Thompson KR, Dawson
D. Behavioural sleep treatments
and night time crying in infants:
challenging the status quo. Sleep Med
Rev. 2011;15(5):327–334
26. Borbély AA. A two process model of
sleep regulation. Hum Neurobiol.
1982;1(3):195–204
27. Achermann P, Borbély AA.
Mathematical models of
sleep regulation. Front Biosci.
2003;8:s683–s693
28. So K, Adamson TM, Horne RSC.
The use of actigraphy for assessment
of the development of sleep/wake
patterns in infants during the fi rst
12 months of life. J Sleep Res.
2007;16(2):181–187
29. Ferber R. Solve Your Child’s Sleep
Problems. 2nd ed. New York, NY:
Fireside; 2006
30. Gunnar MR, White BP. Salivary
cortisol measures in infant and child
assessment. In: Singer LT, Zeskind PS,
eds. Biobehavioral Assessment of the
Infant. New York, NY: Guilford Press;
2001:167–189
31. Salimetrics. Inter- and intra-assay
coeffi cients of variability. Salimetrics,
USA. Available at: www. salimetrics.
com/ assets/ documents/ Spit_ Tips_-_
Inter__ Intra_ Assay_ Coeffi cients_ of_
Variability. pdf. Accessed September 4,
2015
32. Ainsworth MDS, Biehar MC, Waters
E, Wall S. Patterns of Attachment:
A Psychological Study of the Strange
Situation. New York, NY: Psychology
Press; 1978
33. Piazza CC, Fisher WW. Bedtime
fading in the treatment of pediatric
9
by guest on January 10, 2017Downloaded from
GRADISAR et al
insomnia. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry.
1991;22(1):53–56
34. Piazza CC, Fisher WW, Sherer M.
Treatment of multiple sleep problems
in children with developmental
disabilities: faded bedtime with
response cost versus bedtime
scheduling. Dev Med Child Neurol.
1997;39(6):414–418
35. Jones CHD, Owens JA, Pham
B. Can a brief educational
intervention improve parents’
knowledge of healthy children’s
sleep? A pilot test. Health Educ J.
2013;72:601–610
36. Sadeh A. Assessment of intervention
for infant night waking: parental
reports and activity-based home
monitoring. J Consult Clin Psychol.
1994;62(1):63–68
37. Sadeh A, Mindell JA, Owens J. Why
care about sleep of infants and
their parents? Sleep Med Rev.
2011;15(5):335–337
38. Larson MC, White BP, Cochran A,
Donzella B, Gunnar M. Dampening
of the cortisol response to
handling at 3 months in human
infants and its relation to sleep,
circadian cortisol activity, and
behavioral distress. Dev Psychobiol.
1998;33(4):327–337
39. Lewis M, Ramsay DS. Developmental
change in infants’ responses to stress.
Child Dev. 1995;66(3):657–670
40. Ivars K, Nelson N, Theodorsson A,
Theodorsson E, Ström JO, Mörelius
E. Development of salivary cortisol
circadian rhythm and reference
intervals in full-term infants. PLoS One.
2015;10(6):e0129502
41. Price AMH, Wake M, Ukoumunne OC,
Hiscock H. Five-year follow-up of harms
and benefi ts of behavioral infant
sleep intervention: randomized trial.
Pediatrics. 2012;130(4):643–651
42. South Australian Coroner’s Court.
Finding of Inquest (Inquest Number
25/2009). Adelaide, Australia: State
Coroner’s Court; 2010
43. Brandon M, Bailey S, Belderson P,
Larsson B. The role of neglect in child
fatality and serious injury. Child Abuse
Review. 2014;23(4):235–245
44. Kemp JS, Unger B, Wilkins D, et al.
Unsafe sleep practices and an analysis
of bedsharing among infants dying
suddenly and unexpectedly: results
of a four-year, population-based,
death-scene investigation study of
sudden infant death syndrome and
related deaths. Pediatrics. 2000;106(3).
Available at: www. pediatrics. org/ cgi/
content/ full/ 106/ 3/ E41
10 by guest on January 10, 2017Downloaded from
DOI: 10.1542/peds.2015-1486
; originally published online May 24, 2016; 2016;137;Pediatrics
Anne Sved Williams, Robyn Dolby and David J. Kennaway
Michael Gradisar, Kate Jackson, Nicola J. Spurrier, Joyce Gibson, Justine Whitham,
Trial
Behavioral Interventions for Infant Sleep Problems: A Randomized Controlled
Services
Updated Information & /content/137/6/e20151486.full.html
including high resolution figures, can be found at:
Supplementary Material
html
/content/suppl/2016/05/18/peds.2015-1486.DCSupplemental.
Supplementary material can be found at:
References
/content/137/6/e20151486.full.html#ref-list-1
at:
This article cites 38 articles, 9 of which can be accessed free
Subspecialty Collections
/cgi/collection/sleep_medicine_sub
Sleep Medicine /cgi/collection/psychiatry_psychology_sub
Psychiatry/Psychology
the following collection(s):
This article, along with others on similar topics, appears in
Permissions & Licensing
/site/misc/Permissions.xhtml
tables) or in its entirety can be found online at:
Information about reproducing this article in parts (figures,
Reprints /site/misc/reprints.xhtml
Information about ordering reprints can be found online:
rights reserved. Print ISSN: 0031-4005. Online ISSN: 1098-4275.
Grove Village, Illinois, 60007. Copyright © 2016 by the American Academy of Pediatrics. All
and trademarked by the American Academy of Pediatrics, 141 Northwest Point Boulevard, Elk
publication, it has been published continuously since 1948. PEDIATRICS is owned, published,
PEDIATRICS is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly
by guest on January 10, 2017Downloaded from
DOI: 10.1542/peds.2015-1486
; originally published online May 24, 2016; 2016;137;Pediatrics
Anne Sved Williams, Robyn Dolby and David J. Kennaway
Michael Gradisar, Kate Jackson, Nicola J. Spurrier, Joyce Gibson, Justine Whitham,
Trial
Behavioral Interventions for Infant Sleep Problems: A Randomized Controlled
/content/137/6/e20151486.full.html
located on the World Wide Web at:
The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, is
of Pediatrics. All rights reserved. Print ISSN: 0031-4005. Online ISSN: 1098-4275.
Boulevard, Elk Grove Village, Illinois, 60007. Copyright © 2016 by the American Academy
published, and trademarked by the American Academy of Pediatrics, 141 Northwest Point
publication, it has been published continuously since 1948. PEDIATRICS is owned,
PEDIATRICS is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly
by guest on January 10, 2017Downloaded from
... Importantly, camping out (Tables 3 and 4) was as effective as controlled crying but preferable for infants with separation anxiety, and less likely than controlled crying to lead to post-intervention deterioration of infant sleep (Matthey and Črnčec, 2012;Kahn et al., 2020). A comparison between controlled crying, bedtime fading and education (Tables 3 and 5) found that bedtime fading effectively reduced the time for infants to get to sleep and that all three approaches were effective (Gradisar et al., 2016). Notably, education was as effective as BSIs in the long term, and bedtime fading shared the strongest relationship with improvements in maternal mood and stress. ...
... Graduated extinction may then be introduced to reduce nighttime wakefulness, if necessary. Gradisar et al., 2016 Controlled crying with support from a clinical psychologist Israel Parallel group RCT examining the moderating role of infant separation anxiety on checking in and camping out. ...
... Sleep education was examined as an intervention, offered alongside BSIs and sometimes used as a control condition (Table 6). Education was associated with a broad range of benefits, including improvements in caregiver wellbeing and infant sleep, when used alone or in conjunction with moderate BSIs (Price et al., 2012b(Price et al., , 2012aSymon et al., 2012;Hall et al., 2015;Gradisar et al., 2016;Symon and Crichton, 2017). Importantly, use of an educational intervention led to a significant increase in maternal confidence and pleasure in being a mother, and decreased frustration, depression, anxiety and stress (Symon and Crichton, 2017). ...
Article
Full-text available
This PRISMA scoping review identifies and summarises 13 research papers, published between 2011 and 2021, specific to interventions aimed at improving the sleep of infants from birth to two years of age. The aim was to identify trends in study characteristics, overarching outcomes and recommendations, and to extract and make further suggestions for professional training and practice. Relevant literature often provides contradictory guidance, and caregivers increasingly seek individualised support via the growing sleep consultancy industry. Papers examining the efficacy, safety and parental perception of a variety of approaches to improving infant sleep-including cry it out, controlled crying, camping out, bedtime fading and educational interventions-were sourced. Findings suggested that authors most commonly recommended supporting caregivers with education about developmentally appropriate sleep, cues for tiredness, settling techniques, positive sleep norland.ac.uk/journal 2 routines, how to increase homeostatic sleep pressure and how to use moderate behavioural interventions if problems remain after the infant reaches six months of age. Outcomes also suggested this combination is safe, can improve infant sleep as well as main and secondary caregiver wellbeing, and helps caregivers improve their understanding of whether their infant has a sleep problem. Based on the scoping review outcomes, the authors recommend the routine inclusion of evidence-based information about common patterns of infant sleep and different approaches to supporting sleep in the professional training of early years and healthcare professionals. This has the potential to facilitate evidence-based individualised support for caregivers with their infant's sleep, promote safe selection and use of interventions, and improve infant, primary and secondary caregiver sleep quality and wellbeing.
... 24,25 Significant empirical support exists for behavioral and cognitive-behavioral interventions without evidence of negative effects on young children. 24,26 Circadian rhythm disturbances, most frequently delayed sleep-wake phase disorder in adolescents, are typically treated with a multimodal approach including sleep scheduling, bright light therapy, and/or melatonin used as a chronobiotic. Use of chronotherapy, in which the adolescent's sleep/wake times are advanced forward around the clock, may also be considered. ...
Article
Full-text available
Sleep concerns are common during pediatric cancer treatment and can last into sur-vivorship. The current systematic review sought to identify intervention studies that addressed sleep as a primary or secondary outcome during pediatric cancer treatment up to 5 years after completing treatment. Quality assessment was rated using the Grading of Recommendations Assessment, Development, and Evaluation (GRADE) system. The review identified 16 studies with a total of 943 participants that tested a wide range of interventions including psycho-educational, stress management techniques , medications, and physical activity. Most studies included tested interventions in small samples. None of the included studies had a high risk of bias for all domains, but all included studies had a high risk of bias for at least two risk domains. Several feasible pilot studies were identified that warrant further research to test efficacy. Implications for future research and clinical practice to manage sleep concerns are discussed.
... Some methods include brief entries into the child's room to settle them down with minimal interaction, avoiding actions such as picking up the child, cuddling, engaging in conversations, or offering food. 14 ...
Article
Sleep‐related disorders in children can significantly impact children's physical, emotional, and cognitive development and constitute a major source of parental concern. This comprehensive review aims to describe sleep‐related disorders commonly encountered in pediatric practice, their etiology, diagnosis, and management strategies. The review explores various disorders prevalent in different age groups, including insomnia, sleep apnea, parasomnias, and circadian rhythm disorders, highlighting the importance of early identification and intervention for optimal child health and well‐being.
Article
Full-text available
Context In adults, cortisol levels show a pronounced 24-hour rhythm with a peak in the early morning. It is unknown at what age this early-morning peak in cortisol emerges during infancy, hampering the establishment of optimal dosing regimens for hydrocortisone replacement therapy in infants with an inborn form of adrenal insufficiency. Objective We aimed to characterize daily variation in salivary cortisol concentration across the first year of life. Methods We conducted a systematic review followed by an individual participant data meta-analysis of studies reporting on spontaneous (ie, not stress-induced) salivary cortisol concentrations in healthy infants aged 0-1 year. A one-stage approach using linear mixed-effects modeling was used to determine the interaction between age and time of day on cortisol concentrations. Results Through the systematic review, 54 eligible publications were identified, reporting on 29 177 cortisol observations. Individual participant data were obtained from 15 study cohorts, combining 17 079 cortisol measurements from 1904 infants. The morning/evening cortisol ratio increased significantly from 1.7 (95% CI: 1.3-2.1) at birth to 3.7 (95% CI: 3.0-4.5) at 6 to 9 months (P < .0001). Cosinor analysis using all available data revealed the gradual emergence of a 24-hour rhythm during infancy. Conclusion The early-morning peak in cortisol secretion gradually emerges from birth onwards to form a stable morning/evening ratio from age 6 to 9 months. This might have implications for hydrocortisone replacement therapy in infants with an inborn form of adrenal insufficiency.
Article
Objectives Premature newborns are exposed to a great deal of over-stimulation, which can affect their cerebral development. For better sleep, certain practices should be recommended. The aim of this study was to evaluate the effect of a professional training program on the improvement of practices promoting respect for the newborn’s sleep-wake cycle. Materials and Methods This was an interventional study with a longitudinal, single-group, and before-and-after design. The experimental design followed a three-stage time series: Eight months before, three months after, then eight months after intervention. It targeted a comprehensive sample of 66 professionals. It took place between October 2020 and March 2022 at the Neonatal Intensive Care Unit of the Hassan II University Hospital in Fez, Morocco. It was based on an observation grid and a self-administered questionnaire, validated and tested with a Cronbach’s alpha reliability of 0.91. Results The light environment showed significant differences between the 1 st and 3 rd step (3.3% vs. 45.0%; P = 0.02; confidence interval [CI] = 13.644–10.456) for knowledge and (13.3% vs. 78.3%; P = 0.01; CI = 14.412–10.888) for practices; the noise environment showed a positive improvement between the 2 nd and 3 rd intervention (31.7% vs. 41.7%; P < 0.001; CI = 5.954–2.913) for knowledge and (65.0% vs. 73.3%; P < 0.001; CI = 3.597–1.236) for practices, with an average of (8.98 ± 0.30–28.15 ± 0.48; CI = 3.806–1.094) between the 1 st and 3 rd step. Practices surrounding sleep and wakefulness reported significant improvement between the three periods (14.35 ± 0.22 vs. 18.10 ± 0.35 vs. 19.90 ± 0.35; P P < 0.001; CI = 4.647–2.853) for sleep and (13.25 ± 0.48 vs. 22.27 ± 0.59; P < 0.001; CI = 10.563–7.471) for wakefulness with statistically significant correlations between knowledge and practices (0.426**) for sleep and (0.606**) for wakefulness. Conclusion The study demonstrated the positive impact of this sleep management and assessment program on the development of professional skills. Its implementation requires rigorous application of developmental support strategies for individualized care in neonatology.
Article
Full-text available
This paper reviews the evidence regarding the efficacy of behavioral treatments for bedtime problems and night wakings in young children. It is based on a review of 52 treatment studies by a task force appointed by the American Academy of Sleep Medicine to develop practice parameters on behavioral treatments for the clinical management of bedtime problems and night wakings in young children. The findings indicate that behavioral therapies produce reliable and durable changes. Across all studies, 94% report that behavioral interventions were efficacious, with over 80% of children treated demonstrating clinically significant improvement that was maintained for 3 to 6 months. In particular, empirical evidence from controlled group studies utilizing Sackett criteria for evidence-based treatment provides strong support for unmodifi ed extinction and preventive parent education. In addition, support is provided for graduated extinction, bedtime fading/positive routines, and scheduled awakenings. Additional research is needed to examine delivery methods of treatment, longer-term efficacy, and the role of pharmacological agents. Furthermore, pediatric sleep researchers are strongly encouraged to develop standardized diagnostic criteria and more objective measures, and to come to a consensus on critical outcome variables.
Article
Full-text available
Cortisol concentrations in plasma display a circadian rhythm in adults and children older than one year. Earlier studies report divergent results regarding when cortisol circadian rhythm is established. The present study aims to investigate at what age infants develop a circadian rhythm, as well as the possible influences of behavioral regularity and daily life trauma on when the rhythm is established. Furthermore, we determine age-related reference intervals for cortisol concentrations in saliva during the first year of life. 130 healthy full-term infants were included in a prospective, longitudinal study with saliva sampling on two consecutive days, in the morning (07:30-09:30), noon (10:00-12:00) and evening (19:30-21:30), each month from birth until the infant was twelve months old. Information about development of behavioral regularity and potential exposure to trauma was obtained from the parents through the Baby Behavior Questionnaire and the Life Incidence of Traumatic Events checklist. A significant group-level circadian rhythm of salivary cortisol secretion was established at one month, and remained throughout the first year of life, although there was considerable individual variability. No correlation was found between development of cortisol circadian rhythm and the results from either the Baby Behavior Questionnaire or the Life Incidence of Traumatic Events checklist. The study presents salivary cortisol reference intervals for infants during the first twelve months of life. Cortisol circadian rhythm in infants is already established by one month of age, earlier than previous studies have shown. The current study also provides first year age-related reference intervals for salivary cortisol levels in healthy, full-term infants.
Article
Full-text available
Although there is improved recognition of the pernicious long‐term harm that stems from living with neglect during childhood, neglect is rarely associated with child fatality. This article offers a re‐analysis of neglect in serious case reviews (cases of child death or serious injury related to maltreatment) in England (2003–11) from four consecutive government‐commissioned national two‐yearly studies. It draws on anonymised research information from 46 cases out of a total of over 800 cases. Each case was examined in depth using an ecological transactional approach, grounded in the child's experience, which promotes a dynamic understanding and assessment of the interactions between children and their families and the helping practitioners. The qualitative findings reported explore how circumstances came together when neglect had a catastrophic impact on the child and family presenting in six different ways (deprivational neglect, medical neglect, accidents with elements of forewarning, sudden unexpected deaths in infancy, physical abuse combined with neglect and young suicide). Each of the six categories raised particular issues over and above a common core of concerns around the relationship between the child and his or her parent or carer, and between parents/carers and professionals. © 2014 The Authors. Child Abuse Review published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd. ‘neglect is rarely associated with child fatality’ ‘Each of the six categories raised particular issues’ Key Practitioner Messages There are no easy answers to curbing potentially fatal neglect – practitioners should be supported to make careful well‐reasoned judgements. That neglect is not only harmful but can also be fatal should be part of any practitioner's mindset, as with other maltreatment. To guard against catastrophic neglect, children need to be physically and emotionally healthy and have a safe, healthy living environment. Practitioners need to be compassionate and sensitively attuned to the relationship between parents and children. ‘practitioners need to be compassionate and sensitively attuned to the relationship between parents and children.’
Article
Full-text available
This article examines Australian legal responses to filicide in circumstances where mothers have killed their young children. We consider the potential legal defences that may be raised where postnatal depression (‘PND’) and other psychiatric disorders are present in cases of filicide: insanity/mental impairment, diminished responsibility/ substantial impairment by abnormality of mind, and infanticide. We then examine 28 cases of filicide, including both cases where PND evidence was adduced, and cases where PND evidence was not adduced but other mental health issues were considered. We look at the forensic use of and judicial responses to PND and other evidence of mental illness: how do medical practitioners and judicial officers present impairment of the defendant’s mental capacity? We also speculate on differences in sentencing outcomes and consider the policy and research implications of our findings.
Article
Full-text available
This article examines Australian legal responses to filicide in circumstances where mothers have killed their young children. We consider the potential legal defences where postnatal depression (PND) and other psychiatric disorders could be raised in cases of filicide – insanity/mental impairment, diminished responsibility/substantial impairment by abnormality of mind, and infanticide. We then examine 28 cases of filicide, both in cases where there was PND, and cases in which there was no PND evidence adduced, but other mental health issues were considered. We look at the forensic use of and judicial responses to PND and other evidence of mental illness. How do medical practitioners and judicial officers present impairment of the defendant’s mental capacity? We also speculate on differences in sentencing outcomes and consider the policy and research implications of our findings.
Article
Full-text available
Randomized trials have demonstrated the short- to medium-term effectiveness of behavioral infant sleep interventions. However, concerns persist that they may harm children's emotional development and subsequent mental health. This study aimed to determine long-term harms and/or benefits of an infant behavioral sleep program at age 6 years on (1) child, (2) child-parent, and (3) maternal outcomes. Three hundred twenty-six children (173 intervention) with parent-reported sleep problems at age 7 months were selected from a population sample of 692 infants recruited from well-child centers. The study was a 5-year follow-up of a population-based cluster-randomized trial. Allocation was concealed and researchers (but not parents) were blinded to group allocation. Behavioral techniques were delivered over 1 to 3 individual nurse consultations at infant age 8 to 10 months, versus usual care. The main outcomes measured were (1) child mental health, sleep, psychosocial functioning, stress regulation; (2) child-parent relationship; and (3) maternal mental health and parenting styles. Two hundred twenty-five families (69%) participated. There was no evidence of differences between intervention and control families for any outcome, including (1) children's emotional (P = .8) and conduct behavior scores (P = .6), sleep problems (9% vs 7%, P = .2), sleep habits score (P = .4), parent- (P = .7) and child-reported (P = .8) psychosocial functioning, chronic stress (29% vs 22%, P = .4); (2) child-parent closeness (P = .1) and conflict (P = .4), global relationship (P = .9), disinhibited attachment (P = .3); and (3) parent depression, anxiety, and stress scores (P = .9) or authoritative parenting (63% vs 59%, P = .5). Behavioral sleep techniques have no marked long-lasting effects (positive or negative). Parents and health professionals can confidently use these techniques to reduce the short- to medium-term burden of infant sleep problems and maternal depression.
Article
Objective Insufficient and poor quality sleep is prevalent in children, and is a significant public health concern due to the negative consequences for health. Certain sleep-related behaviours are associated with improved sleep, and sleep behaviours are amenable to efforts targeted towards behaviour change. Parental educational interventions have been successful at improving parents’ knowledge and children’s health outcomes in some areas of health, but no population-based educational interventions have been designed to improve parents’ knowledge of healthy children’s sleep. This study aimed to design and test an educational tool to increase caregivers’ knowledge of healthy children’s sleep in a primary care setting. Setting A hospital-based pediatric primary care clinic where children attend for well-child and sick visits. Method Parents of 95 children aged three months to 12 years completed two surveys, one prior to and one after receiving the intervention. The surveys assessed children’s sleep habits (pre-intervention) and parental sleep beliefs and sleep knowledge (pre- and post-intervention). The intervention was a short, informational brochure. Results Following the intervention there was a significant increase in parents’ knowledge regarding children’s healthy sleep, and in the number of parents who planned to make positive behaviour changes regarding their child’s sleep practices. Conclusion Simple written educational interventions may be effective tools with which to increase parents’ knowledge and promote healthy sleep behaviours in children.
Article
Data drawn from the Australian Homicide Project were used to examine whether and how maternal and paternal filicide perpetrators differ in terms of motivations for filicide and childhood and adulthood adversities. In addition, key differences between filicide and non‐filicide perpetrators were examined. Data were collected across a number of states and territories in Australia between 2010 and 2013 through interviews with 231 men and women convicted of murder or manslaughter. Of these participants, 14 had perpetrated filicide. Detailed information on the developmental background of the perpetrators, as well as motives and situational contexts of the homicide incidents, was gathered through the interviews. Findings from the current study reveal some important gender differences among filicide perpetrators. For example, filicidal fathers are more likely to perpetrate accidental filicide, and to report unemployment, alcohol and drug problems and previous engagement in child abuse, while filicidal mothers are more likely to perpetrate altruistic or neglectful filicide, and to report mental health problems. In addition, male filicide perpetrators report a greater number of adversities compared to male non‐filicide perpetrators, while female filicide perpetrators display fewer adversities compared to their non‐filicide counterparts. ‘Findings from the current study reveal some important gender differences among filicide perpetrators’ Key Practitioner Messages Practitioners are uniquely placed to assist in filicide prevention. Opportunities for service providers to raise awareness of key risk factors for filicide are crucial. Health and social service providers are ideally placed to screen parents seeking support to uncover early warning risks. Mental health service support is crucial, particularly for mothers, as is screening parents for severe depression or suicidal ideations. Provision of parenting support skills to help parents cope with extreme stress appears salient. ‘Health and social service providers are ideally placed to screen parents seeking support to uncover early warning risks’
Article
Objectives To investigate the emergence of biological rhythms in the first months of life in human infants, by measuring age-related changes in core body temperature during night-time sleep, hormones (cortisol and 6-sulfatoxymelatonin) and the expression of a clock-controlled gene H3f3b in oral epithelial cells. Design Observational longitudinal study. Setting We measured overnight core body temperature, actigraphy, day–night urinary cortisol and 6-sulfatoxymelatonin, as well as circadian gene expression, in infants at home from March 2007 to July 2008 in Leicester. Participants We recruited 35 healthy Caucasian infants who were born at term. They were monitored from 6 to 18 weeks of age. Results At 8 weeks of age the day–night rhythm of cortisol secretion was the first to appear followed by 6-sulfatoxymelatonin 1 week later; at the same time that night-time sleep was established. At 10 weeks, the maximum fall in deep body temperature occurred with the onset of night-time sleep, followed at 11 weeks by the rhythmical expression of the H3f3b gene. Conclusions In human infants, there is a clear sequential pattern for the emergence of diurnal biological rhythms between 6 and 18 weeks of postnatal age, led by the secretion of cortisol and linked with the establishment of consolidated night-time sleep. It is likely that this represents part of a maturation and adaption process as infants gain equilibrium with their external environment after birth.