ArticlePDF Available

Implicit and Explicit Learning of a Sequential Postural Weight-Shifting Task in Young and Older Adults

Frontiers
Frontiers in Psychology
Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Sequence-specific postural motor learning in a target-directed weight-shifting task in 12 older and 12 young participants was assessed. In the implicit sequence learning condition participants performed a concurrent spatial cognitive task and in the two explicit conditions participants were required to discover the sequence order either with or without the concurrent cognitive task. Participants moved a cursor on the screen from the center location to one of the target locations projected in a semi-circle and back by shifting their center of pressure (CoP) on force plates. During the training the targets appeared in a simple fixed 5-target sequence. Plan-based control (i.e., direction of the CoP displacement in the first part of the target-directed movement) improved by anticipating the sequence order in the implicit condition but not in the explicit dual task condition. Only the young participants were able to use the explicit knowledge of the sequence structure to improve the directional error as indicated by a significant decrease in directional error over practice and an increase in directional error with sequence removal in the explicit single task condition. Time spent in the second part of the movement trajectory to stabilize the cursor on the target location improved over training in both the implicit and explicit sequence learning conditions, for both age groups. These results might indicate that an implicit motor learning method, which holds back explicit awareness of task relevant features, may be desirable for improving plan-based motor control in older adults.
This content is subject to copyright.
fpsyg-07-00733 May 21, 2016 Time: 13:40 # 1
ORIGINAL RESEARCH
published: 24 May 2016
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00733
Edited by:
Bernard J. Martin,
University of Michigan, USA
Reviewed by:
Luis Augusto Teixeira,
University of São Paulo, Brazil
Rachel O. Coats,
University of Leeds, UK
*Correspondence:
Simone R. Caljouw
s.r.caljouw@umcg.nl
Specialty section:
This article was submitted to
Movement Science and Sport
Psychology,
a section of the journal
Frontiers in Psychology
Received: 04 December 2015
Accepted: 02 May 2016
Published: 24 May 2016
Citation:
Caljouw SR, Veldkamp R
and Lamoth CJC (2016) Implicit
and Explicit Learning of a Sequential
Postural Weight-Shifting Task
in Young and Older Adults.
Front. Psychol. 7:733.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00733
Implicit and Explicit Learning of a
Sequential Postural Weight-Shifting
Task in Young and Older Adults
Simone R. Caljouw*, Renee Veldkamp and Claudine J. C. Lamoth
Center for Human Movement Sciences, University Medical Center Groningen, University of Groningen, Groningen,
Netherlands
Sequence-specific postural motor learning in a target-directed weight-shifting task in
12 older and 12 young participants was assessed. In the implicit sequence learning
condition participants performed a concurrent spatial cognitive task and in the two
explicit conditions participants were required to discover the sequence order either with
or without the concurrent cognitive task. Participants moved a cursor on the screen
from the center location to one of the target locations projected in a semi-circle and
back by shifting their center of pressure (CoP) on force plates. During the training the
targets appeared in a simple fixed 5-target sequence. Plan-based control (i.e., direction
of the CoP displacement in the first part of the target-directed movement) improved by
anticipating the sequence order in the implicit condition but not in the explicit dual task
condition. Only the young participants were able to use the explicit knowledge of the
sequence structure to improve the directional error as indicated by a significant decrease
in directional error over practice and an increase in directional error with sequence
removal in the explicit single task condition. Time spent in the second part of the
movement trajectory to stabilize the cursor on the target location improved over training
in both the implicit and explicit sequence learning conditions, for both age groups. These
results might indicate that an implicit motor learning method, which holds back explicit
awareness of task relevant features, may be desirable for improving plan-based motor
control in older adults.
Keywords: implicit motor learning, postural control, aging, older adults, sequence learning
INTRODUCTION
Repeating patterns or sequences occur often in our environments, and in many activities of daily
living a sequence of individual acts is performed in interaction with the environment. For example,
the sequence of asks needed to get a glass of water may include, leaning over to a cupboard,
opening the cupboard, grasping a glass, moving to the tap, and turning on the faucet. Prediction of,
adaptation to, and learning about, environmental regularities on the basis of preceding events may
assist in how the body needs to be moved in order to achieve the task goal, and requires adequate
postural control (Sturnieks et al., 2008;de Vries et al., 2014).
Aging yields an undeniable deterioration of postural control, as a consequence of a general age-
related deterioration of sensory and neuromuscular control mechanisms (Laughton et al., 2003;
Sturnieks et al., 2008). Consequently, maintaining postural stability during daily tasks becomes
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 1May 2016 | Volume 7 | Article 733
fpsyg-07-00733 May 21, 2016 Time: 13:40 # 2
Caljouw et al. Learning Sequential Postural Weight-Shifting
less automatic, and requires increased attention. Aging, however,
is not only associated with a decline in postural control, but also
with a deterioration of cognitive processes involving executive
functions and attention (Castel and Craik, 2003;Park et al.,
2003;Lovden et al., 2008;Verwey, 2010). Thus, older adults have
greater need for conscious attention to maintain good postural
control, due to impaired sensory and motor system functions. At
the same time, they suffer from reduced attentional and working
memory capacity. Since postural control becomes less automatic,
depending on the complexity of the motor task, the execution of
a concurrent cognitive task will lead to performance decrements
(Huxhold et al., 2006;Boisgontier et al., 2013).
The age-related decline of cognitive functions makes it
plausible that the ability to explicitly learn sequential motor skills
decreases with age, since explicit motor learning requires an
intention to learn and thus a contribution of strategic processes
such as attention, reasoning, and memory (Fitts and Posner,
1967). Young adults acquire more explicit knowledge about the
sequence structure than older adults (Howard and Howard, 2001;
Verneau et al., 2014). When older adults do acquire explicit
knowledge about a sequence, they would be less successful in
using this knowledge (Shea et al., 2006;Verneau et al., 2014).
Implicit learning, on the other hand, is considered to depend on
a phylogenetically older and more primitive system than explicit
learning (Reber, 1992). In implicit learning, learning occurs
without an intention to learn and without explicit knowledge
about the environmental regularities, it thus depends less on the
working memory capacity (Masters, 1992;Jimenez and Vazquez,
2005;Janacsek and Nemeth, 2012). Therefore, it is suggested that
implicit learning is more robust to the effects of age (Cherry and
Stadler, 1995;Song et al., 2009). Abundant research exist on the
influence of age on implicit motor learning used serial reaction
time tasks (Nissen and Bullemer, 1987;Willingham and Goedert-
Eschmann, 1999). In serial reaction time tasks participants have
to react as fast as possible on certain stimuli by pressing keys on
a keyboard, repeating sequences of stimuli are hidden between
random stimuli, in order to remain unknown to the participants
(for reviews see: Rieckmann and Backman, 2009;Howard and
Howard, 2013;King et al., 2013). With practice, participants
become faster due to general skill learning and sequence specific
learning. Sequence-specific learning is indicated by an abrupt
increase in response times when the sequential regularity is
removed. Older adults do show sequence learning in these tasks,
however, the rate and magnitude of learning declines when task
conditions become cognitively more demanding. This occurs
with increased complexity of the sequence due to alternating
random and ordered elements (Curran, 1997;Feeney et al., 2002;
Howard et al., 2004;Bennett et al., 2007;Simon et al., 2011) or due
to an additional working memory load in the form of a dual-task
(Frensch and Miner, 1994;Nejati et al., 2008;King et al., 2013;
Vandenbossche et al., 2014).
In contrast to serial reaction time tasks, taking advantage of
a repeating sequence of elements to improve postural responses
is proven to be difficult. Recent studies (Van Ooteghem et al.,
2008, 2010) question earlier positive findings of Shea et al. (2001)
who showed segment learning in a visuomotor tracking task in
which participants were asked to continuously track a target by
controlling their center of pressure (CoP; moving a platform
on a stabilometer on which they were standing). Chambaron
et al. (2006) suggested that performance improvements in
this previous study were not the result of segment-specific
visuomotor learning, but could be attributed to methodological
flaws, i.e., the selection of a repeating segment that was more
easy to perform than the random control segments. The studies
of Van Ooteghem et al. (2008, 2010) lend further support for
Chambaron et al. (2006) indicating no evidence for sequence
specific learning in a postural control task in which participants
had to maintain balance in response to a repeating pattern of
sequential platform manipulations. On the other hand, Orrell
et al. (2006) showed that application of an implicit motor learning
technique did improve balance performance on a stabilometer.
It is presently not known whether implicit motor learning
would occur for postural control tasks in which participants
produce postural adjustments to environmental regularities
of a sequential nature (instead of reactive to perturbations).
Therefore, in the present study, sequence-specific postural motor
learning in a target-directed weight-shifting task was assessed,
in both older and young participants. Instructing participants
to discover a sequence can attenuate the degree to which
participants gain awareness of the sequence structure, whereas
asking them to concurrently perform a task-irrelevant visuo-
spatial memory task can abolish awareness of the sequence
structure. Previous studies on motor sequence learning in upper
limb tasks indicate that explicit awareness of the sequence order
is a prerequisite for sequence-specific movement optimization
(Moisello et al., 2009, 2011;Oostwoud Wijdenes et al., 2016).
When explicit sequence knowledge is important for sequence-
specific motor learning in a postural task one would expect to find
interference from an added secondary task, especially in older
adults.
In the target-directed weight-shifting task, participants
control their CoP on a force platform by shifting their weight in
order to move a cursor on a screen in front of them (Jongman
et al., 2012;de Vries et al., 2014). Participants are asked to
move the cursor to a target that can appear in one of five
locations. A specific sequence of targets recurs throughout the
practice session. Motor skill in this task requires both adequate
planning and execution. One needs to predict the upcoming
target location to move efficiently to the right side and great
execution skill is required to control the cursor to stabilize
on the target location. The directional accuracy of the CoP
displacement in the first part of the target-directed movement
is a good proxy for the planning accuracy, a higher accuracy
of movement direction reflects better anticipation of the target
location, demonstrating a greater degree of plan-based motor
learning (Ghilardi et al., 2003, 2009). A previous study on
target-directed weight-shifting showed that the steadiness of the
movement increased (i.e., less velocity peaks and dwell time in
the vicinity of the target) when the upcoming target location
was predictable compared to when it was not (Jongman et al.,
2012). This finding suggests that knowledge of the sequence order
and anticipating the target location may improve not only the
initial planning of the target-directed movement (e.g., directional
accuracy in the first part of the movement), but also the visual
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 2May 2016 | Volume 7 | Article 733
fpsyg-07-00733 May 21, 2016 Time: 13:40 # 3
Caljouw et al. Learning Sequential Postural Weight-Shifting
guidance or control over the execution of the movement (e.g.,
homing-time necessary for corrective movements in the vicinity
of the target).
In the current study healthy older and young participants
repeatedly performed a sequence of voluntary displacements
of the CoP to assess whether (1) age affects the capacity for
sequence-specific postural motor learning, (2) explicit knowledge
about the sequence order leads to better movement optimization
than implicit learning, and (3) sequence-specific postural motor
learning degrades under dual task conditions. We hypothesized
that practicing the sequential target-directed weight shifting
task leads to sequence specific improvements in both age
groups. Since the attentional cost of postural control increases
in older adults, we expected also that beneficial effects of
sequence awareness on movement optimization are lower in
older individuals, especially when a cognitive task is performed
simultaneously.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Participants
Twelve young adults (23.9 ±4.3 years) and 12 older adults
(67.9 ±2.5 years) participated in the experiment. Participants had
no neurological or orthopedic disorders that might have an effect
on cognition or postural control and were able to walk and stand
unaided for at least 1 h. The local institution’s ethical committee
approved the study and the participants signed informed consent.
Apparatus and Task Environment
The experiment was conducted in the Computer Assisted
Rehabilitation Environment laboratory (CAREN; Motek
Medical). Participants stood on two force platforms (Bertec
FP4060-08). On a large screen, positioned 2.5 m in front of
the participant, a cursor provided online feedback of the CoP
displacements of the participant (de Vries et al., 2014). The
displacements of the CoP were displayed on the vertical screen
as cursor movements from left to right for the medio-lateral
component and from top to bottom for the anterior–posterior
component. At the start of the experiment participants stood
in a natural position with arms at the side and the software
positioned the cursor on the center target on the screen goal
targets were presented sequentially in one of five possible
locations on a hemicycle above the central target (north, east,
west, north-east, and north-west). When the cursor touched
the displayed target for 200 ms the target disappeared and
the next target appeared. The distance between the central
target and the radial targets was 72 cm on the screen and the
diameter of the target was 18 cm, this corresponded with a
CoP-displacement of 0.06 and 0.015 m, respectively. Participants
were instructed to move the cursor from the central target to
the appearing radial target and move the cursor back to the
central target when the radial target disappeared and the central
target appeared. They were instructed to make movements
as quickly and as accurately as possible. The participants
were not allowed to move their feet on the force plate. At the
start of the experiment a short training session of 30 radial
targets was performed to familiarize the participants with
the relationship between their body motions and the cursor
displacements.
Design of the Motor Sequence-Learning
Task
In the experimental conditions 20 consecutive blocks were
performed in which the targets were presented either in random
order (4 R-blocks), or in a fixed recurring sequence (16
S-blocks). See Figure 1 for an overview of the series of test
blocks used in each condition. The targets in the R-block were
never presented two times in a row and each target-to-target
movement (e.g., neutral to north, neutral to northwest) was
presented twice. For each condition the first two test blocks of
the experiment were R-blocks consisting of 21 targets. These
random blocks were considered baseline blocks; the first random
block was the random baseline-test (R-Base) and the second
one was used as random pre-test (R-Pre). Subsequently, 15
S-blocks were performed. In each S-block a simple sequence
of five different targets was presented three times. Thus, the
learning phase consisted of 45 sequence repetitions. The sequence
order differed between conditions and participants. The last
sequence block (S15) of this learning phase was used as a
sequence post-test (S-Post). In-between S-block 8 and S-block
9 a break was introduced to allow for a short-term recovery of
fatig. To analyse the effect of sequence removal after sequence
learning, S-Post was followed by a R-block of 21 targets (R-
Post). After R-Post the sequence was reintroduced in block S16
(S-rec), to reveal if sequence learning was retained after the
interfering introduction of a random phase. To diminish effects
of sequence expectation, the sequence was reintroduced without
any warning and immediately followed by another R-block. (R4).
This final R-block was also introduced to prevent participants
from obtaining sequence knowledge in retrospect by re-enacting
the last trials that were performed.
The Learning Conditions
Each participant performed this series of blocks three times
with different instructions. The order of the conditions was
counterbalanced across participants and a rest time of at
least 15 min was introduced between learning conditions. In
the explicit single-task condition participants were instructed
to discover the sequence in the task. In the explicit dual-
task condition participants were instructed to discover the
sequence in the learning phase and were required to perform a
concurrent visuo-spatial memory task. In the implicit condition
the participants were instructed that the targets appeared
in random order throughout this session and performed a
concurrent visuo-spatial memory task. An implicit single-
task condition was not included in the current experiment
since pilot studies showed that young participants became
easily aware of the sequence when the cognitive task was
absent.
The visuo-spatial memory task was an adaptation of the
Brooks Spatial Matrix task (Brooks, 1967). Participants listened to
a set of sentences composing a description of a spatial sequence
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 3May 2016 | Volume 7 | Article 733
fpsyg-07-00733 May 21, 2016 Time: 13:40 # 4
Caljouw et al. Learning Sequential Postural Weight-Shifting
FIGURE 1 | Description of the experimental setup. R, random block; S, Sequence block.
of locations, such as “In the starting square put a 1, in the next
square to the left put a 2, in the next square down put a 3.” For
the younger participants, recorded instructions were given for
number placement after every 10 target-directed movements, for
a total of 17 numbers. Older participants were given instructions
after every 13 target-directed movement and were required to
remember 13 numbers. This task was administered at the start
of the experiment and twice per learning condition (e.g., before
and after the break).
After each learning condition it was tested with a free recall
test whether the participants acquired awareness of the target
sequence. If participants were not able to correctly report the
sequence order a four-way forced-choice test was conducted,
to test sequence recognition. Prior to the experiment two
criteria were formulated regarding the sequence awareness in
the different conditions, (1) participants should become aware
of the sequence in the explicit single-task condition and (2)
participants should not become aware of the sequence in the
implicit condition. Participants were excluded from the data
analysis when they did not meet these criteria.
Data Analysis
Before determining the outcome measures from the CoP
coordinates the raw CoP position data were filtered using a low-
pass fourth order Butterworth filter, with a cut-off frequency of
5 Hz. Subsequently, plan-based and on line control processes
were isolated with a trajectory analysis of each target-directed
FIGURE 2 | Illustration of the center-out movement to a presented
radial target (white) and the outcome parameters directional error and
homing time. The figure is based on one trial of a young subject at the start
(A) and at the end (B) of a practice session. The angle (α) between the two
blue lines determines the directional error. The homing time is the time it takes
from leaving the dotted circle to stabilizing on the radial target (red trajectory).
At the start of the experiment the subject had a larger homing time and a
larger directional error than at the end of the practice session (left panel vs.
right panel).
movement (see Figure 2). Specifically, the directional error of
the first part of the CoP trajectory and the time it takes to
reach and stabilize on the radial target in the second part
(homing-time) of the CoP trajectory were selected to reflect
the plan-based and on-line control processes, respectively. The
directional error was determined at the point where the cursor
leaves the central target area (e.g., the exit point) and defined
as the angle between two lines; one line connecting the exit
point with the origin of the central target and the other
representing the ideal path connecting the origins of the central
target and the radial target. The homing time is the time it
takes from the exit point until the disappearance of the radial
target.
The transition from the sequence posttest (block S15; S-Post)
to the random posttest (R3; R-Post) resulted in large movement
errors on the first trial of the random block. This error was
possibly the result of learning the sequence, causing participants
to wrongly predict an upcoming target location in the random
target location condition. Therefore, the first trial of the 21
trials of the random posttest (block R3) has been removed from
analysis. We excluded from further analysis also the trials in
which the cursor traveled a distance less than 0.01 m in the
home area (6.14% due to technical difficulties) and trials in which
movement times were longer than 2.3 s (0.86%).
Statistical Analysis
All statistical analyses were applied to both outcome measures,
homing time and directional error, separately. Preliminary 2
(Age) ×3 (Condition) repeated-measures ANOVAs with Age
as the between-subjects factor and Condition as the within-
subjects factor were applied to the performance measures in
the random baseline-test (R-Base) to compare the initial task
performance of the young and older participants for the three
conditions. To assess the general training effects over the
sequence blocks, Age (young vs. old) ×Condition (implicit vs.
explicit single vs. explicit dual task) ×Test block (R-Pre vs.
S-post) ANOVAs were applied. To examine whether sequence-
specific learning occurred, sequence removal and sequence
reintroduction effects were tested with Test block (S-Post vs.
R-Post vs. S-Rec) ×Condition (explicit single task vs. explicit
dual-task vs. implicit) ×Age (young vs. old) ANOVAs. To
further explore significant effects, we performed post hoc tests
with Bonferroni corrected adjustments to protect the level of
significance.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 4May 2016 | Volume 7 | Article 733
fpsyg-07-00733 May 21, 2016 Time: 13:40 # 5
Caljouw et al. Learning Sequential Postural Weight-Shifting
RESULTS
Explicit Knowledge Assessment
In the explicit single-task condition participants were instructed
to discover the sequence while performing the target-directed
weight-shifting task. Upon completion, one older participant did
not show explicit knowledge of the sequence in both the free-
recall and the forced choice task, thus his results were excluded
from further analysis. All young participants acquired explicit
sequence knowledge in the explicit single-task condition and
successfully reported the sequence order in the free recall test. In
the implicit condition, where sequence knowledge should not be
available to the participants, two young and one older participant
revealed the right sequence in the explicit knowledge assessment
tests. The three participants that discovered the sequence order
in the implicit condition were excluded from further analysis.
After performing the explicit-dual task condition all the young
participants and none of the older participants were able to
recall the sequence. When the older adults without declarative
knowledge of the sequence were given the forced-choice test, four
of them were belatedly able to recognize the sequence order.
Interestingly, not all participants that discovered the sequence
order in the training phase also noticed the recurrence of
the sequence in block S16 (test block: S-rec), because it was
hidden between two random blocks (R-Post and R4). Only six
young and five older participants reported that they noticed the
recurrence of the sequence in the explicit single-task condition.
Three young and two older participants mentioned the sequence
recurrence in the explicit dual-task condition and none of the
participants noticed the sequence recurrence in the implicit
learning condition.
Cognitive Task Performance
During the experiment participants were asked to prioritize
the visuo-spatial memory task over sequence learning. Task
performance on the visuo-spatial memory test during sequence
learning was compared with task performance prior to the
experiment. As expected, no significant decrease in performance
on the cognitive task was observed for both the young and
older participants. When tested prior to the experiment the
young participants performed the visuo-spatial memory task
successfully, that is 17 out of 17 numbers were correctly placed.
Young participants correctly placed an average of 15.8 out of 17
numbers at the end of the dual task conditions (explicit dual task
and implicit condition). The older participants scored an average
of 6.9 out of 13 correct numbers before the intervention, while
this was 6.3 out of 13 at the end of the dual task conditions.
These results imply that participants followed the instructions
and were able to focus on the cognitive-task while performing the
target-directed weight-shifting task.
Baseline Motor Task Performance
Mean and standard deviations of homing-time and directional
error for old and young participants per condition on the first
block of trials with random target order, e.g., R-Base are depicted
in Figure 3. A significant main effect of Age was found for
homing-time [F(1,18) =10.22, p=0.005], with a longer homing-
time for the old than the young participants. No significant main
effect of Age was found for directional error. No significant main
effects of Condition nor significant interaction effects of Age by
Condition were revealed, indicating no significant performance
difference between the various conditions at the start of the
experiment.
Practice Phase (R-Pre vs. S-Post)
Mean and standard deviations of homing-time and directional
error for old and young participants per condition on the test
moments R-Pre and S-post are presented in Figure 3.
For homing-time, significant main effects of Age
[F(1,18) =17.55, p=0.001] and Test block [F(1,18) =34.25,
p<0.001] were found. Homing-time was shorter for the young
than for the old participants and performance improved with
practice in all learning conditions, indicated by a decrease in
homing-time. No significant main effect of Condition and no
interaction effects were found.
For directional error, a significant main effect of Test block
[F(1,18) =14.40, p=0.001] and a significant interaction
effect of Test block by Condition [F(2,36) =5.34, p=0.009]
was found. No significant main effect of Age was found. To
assess the difference between R-pre and S-post for each learning
condition separately, post hoc paired t-tests were performed with
an adjusted alpha of 0.017 to protect the level of significance.
Performance improvement over practice was revealed, indicated
by a significant decrease in directional error, in the explicit single
task condition [t(19) =4.13, p=0.001] and in the implicit
condition [t(19) =2.85, p=0.010], but not in the explicit dual
task condition.
Sequence Removal and Sequence
Recurrence Analysis (S-Post vs. R-Post
vs. S-Rec)
Means and standard deviations of homing-time and directional
error for old and young participants per condition on the test
moments S-Post, R-Post, and S-Rec are presented in Figure 3.
For homing-time, a significant main effect of Age
[F(1,18) =16.23, p=0.001] and a significant main effect of Test
block (S-Post, R-Post, S-Rec) was observed [F(2,36) =34.36,
p<0.001]. No significant main effect of Condition and no
significant interaction effects were found. Post hoc pairwise
comparisons with Bonferroni adjustments revealed that homing-
time significantly increased in R-Post compared to S-Post
(p<0.001) and then significantly decreased in S-Rec compared
to R-Post (p<0.001). The homing time was not significantly
different between the test blocks S-Post and S-Rec (p=0.072).
This implies that both young and older adults were able to
improve the time it takes to home in on the target by taking
advantage of the sequence structure, as indicated by a significant
increase in homing-time when the sequence is removed and a
decrease again when the sequence is reintroduced.
For directional error, significant main effects of Test block (S-
Post, R-Post, S-Rec); [F(2,36) =28.29, p<0.001) and Condition
[F(2,36) =3.51, p=0.040] were found. Furthermore, there
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 5May 2016 | Volume 7 | Article 733
fpsyg-07-00733 May 21, 2016 Time: 13:40 # 6
Caljouw et al. Learning Sequential Postural Weight-Shifting
FIGURE 3 | Mean value and standard-error bar for each age group on each test block showing; the absence of an aging effect at the beginning of the
experiment for directional error, a slower homing-time for the older participants, general practice effects on directional error and homing-time
(R-Pre vs. S-Post), and the effects of sequence removal and re-introduction (the phase between the two vertical dotted lines on the right side).
was a significant interaction between Test block and Condition
[F(4,72) =6.80, p<0.001] and also a significant interaction
between Age, Test block, and Condition [F(4,72) =3.09,
p=0.021]. To assess the difference between S-post and R-post
for each learning condition and age group separately, post hoc
pairwise comparisons were performed with a Bonferroni adjusted
alpha of 0.008 to protect the level of significance. For the
older participants a significantly higher directional error with
sequence removal was only observed in the implicit condition
and not in both explicit conditions. For the young participants a
significantly higher directional error with sequence removal was
observed in the implicit and explicit single task conditions but
not in the explicit dual task condition. To further explore the
robustness of the sequence learning effect, differences between
the two sequence blocks (S-post and S-rec) were assessed. In
the implicit condition the difference in directional error between
S-post and S-rec was not significant for both young and older
adults, indicating a robust sequence learning effect. Only for the
young adults in the explicit single task condition a significant
increase in directional error was observed in S-rec compared to
S-post (p<0.001), suggesting an interfering effect of the random
block (R-post), which was inserted between the two sequence
blocks (S-post and S-rec).
DISCUSSION
The aim of the current study was to examine the possible effect(s)
of implicit and explicit learning of a sequential postural task in
older adults compared to young adults. To this end, a target-
directed weight-shifting task was created in a virtual environment
in which targets were presented in a certain sequence or
randomly.
The results showed that participants were able to discover the
simple deterministic sequence order of five targets in the explicit
learning condition. In the implicit condition the concurrent
cognitive task prevented most participants from discovering the
simple sequence, as intended. In accordance with our hypothesis,
and in contrast to previous research using a postural perturbation
task (Van Ooteghem et al., 2008, 2010), we found evidence
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 6May 2016 | Volume 7 | Article 733
fpsyg-07-00733 May 21, 2016 Time: 13:40 # 7
Caljouw et al. Learning Sequential Postural Weight-Shifting
for sequence-specific postural motor learning. With practice
on the sequence blocks young and older participants showed
improved performance and an abrupt decrease in performance
when the sequential regularity was removed. However, depending
on age plan-based motor sequence learning, quantified by
improvements in directional error, was not optimal in all
learning conditions. Conversely sequence specific improvements
in homing time occurred in all participants regardless of learning
condition and age. The general observation that old and young
participants show sequence specific motor learning in a postural
control task is in contrast with the work of Van Ooteghem
et al. (2008, 2010). In a postural motor learning study in which
participants were exposed to a repeated sequence of platform
motions, sequence learning did not occur. Participants improved
with practice, but learning was not better for the repeating
sequence than for the random sequence (Van Ooteghem et al.,
2008, 2010). It should be noted that in our study the task
setup was different, instead of reacting to external perturbations,
participants generated active postural responses to aim for
visually presented targets in a sequential order and showed
sequence specific postural motor learning.
Adding explicit knowledge of the sequence order was not
a prerequisite for acquiring sequence-specific improvements
in target-directed weight-shifting. In both age groups in the
implicit condition, where explicit knowledge of the sequence
order was not acquired, homing time and directional error
improved during prolonged sequence practice. Subsequently,
after practice, homing time and directional error increased with
sequence removal (in test block R-post) and decreased again with
sequence reintroduction (in test block S-rec). This implies that
without knowledge of the sequence order and when distracted
by a cognitive task (in the implicit condition), both older and
young participants showed sequence-specific improvements in
the steadiness of the movement execution and in plan-based
control.
For improving plan-based control (directional error), explicit
information about the sequence order through self-discovery,
was in certain circumstances even detrimental. Participants who
intended to search for the sequence order and concurrently
performed the visuo-spatial cognitive task, did not improve
in directional error. Thus, our hypothesis that a concurrent
cognitive task disrupts postural sequence learning with less
interference on implicit than explicit motor learning was
supported for learning to control the direction of the initial part
of the target-directed movement. Studies using the serial reaction
time task found similar results; explicitly searching for a sequence
structure could disrupt motor sequence learning in conditions in
which there is not sufficient cognitive capacity available (Curran,
1997;Fletcher et al., 2005;Jimenez and Vazquez, 2005). The
multiple explicit requirements of the explicit dual task condition
is likely to have placed a high load on the processing limitations
of the working memory system, thereby retarding improvements
in the advance planning of the upcoming target-directed postural
movement in both young and older participants.
Sequence-specific improvements in the homing time occurred
in both older and young participants regardless of the learning
condition. Thus motor learning in the homing phase was
preserved when the cognitive capacity was overloaded by the
explicit attempts to acquire the sequence order in combination
with performing the visuo-spatial cognitive task. Therefore, it
can be assumed that changes in the homing phase are implicitly
achieved, unmolested by acquiring explicit sequence knowledge
and without much dependence on working memory (Willingham
and Goedert-Eschmann, 1999;Willingham et al., 2002).
Age-related difficulties with sequential motor learning were
observed for plan-based control (directional error) in the explicit
single task condition. Both young and older participants acquired
knowledge of the sequence order in this learning condition,
but only the young participants and not the older participants
showed sequence-specific improvements in directional error, as
revealed by the significant increase in directional error with
sequence removal (in test block R-post). However, for the young
participants, this sequence specific learning was less robust
compared to the implicit condition. This was evidenced by the
fact that reintroducing the sequence unbeknownst to participants
(in test block S-Rec) did not significantly decrease directional
error. Even though explicit knowledge was acquired, one did
not recover from the increase in directional error due to the
random block interference (in test block R-Post). Possibly one
needs to be fully aware of the recurrence of the sequence
before being able to express explicit sequence learning in plan-
based control of the movement (Willingham and Goedert-
Eschmann, 1999). In contrast, in the implicit condition plan-
based control improved (decrease in directional error) when the
sequence was reintroduced (in test block S-Rec). This indicates
a robust implicit memory of the sequence and the ability to
more accurately execute the initial part of the target-directed
movement without sequence awareness. The finding that, for
the older participants, sequence specific learning of directional
error occurred in the implicit condition and not in both explicit
conditions, supports the proposition that implicit motor learning
is relatively preserved with age in comparison with explicit
learning (Reber, 1992;Cherry and Stadler, 1995;Hedden and
Gabrieli, 2004;Shea et al., 2006;Brown et al., 2009;Gaillard et al.,
2009;Song et al., 2009).
A new finding of our work is that sequence practice not
only affects the initial plan-based part of the movement, but
also led to a progressive improvement of motor control late
in the movement trajectory in both young and older adults.
Successful achievement of the task goal depends not only on
correctly anticipating the target and planning a movement
with the right direction and extent, but also on the ability to
quickly adjust the ongoing movement based on visual feedback
about the effector and target (Glover, 2004;Caljouw et al.,
2006, 2011). When sequential arm-reaching movements are
practiced, control shifts from a reaction mode to an anticipatory
mode within a few practice trials, requiring far less online
visual control at the end of the practice session than at the
start of the practice session (Ghilardi et al., 2003, 2009). In
our target-directed weight-shifting task, the target area is not
easily reached given the inherent variability in postural control,
which is observed even during quiet standing in the start
position (Zatsiorsky and Duarte, 2000;Lamoth et al., 2009).
Even after practice, a directional error with a large standard
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 7May 2016 | Volume 7 | Article 733
fpsyg-07-00733 May 21, 2016 Time: 13:40 # 8
Caljouw et al. Learning Sequential Postural Weight-Shifting
deviation was still present. The presence of a directional error
highlights the need for corrections in the vicinity of the target
area, to achieve a rather stable final position within the target.
The homing time in older participants was substantially longer
than young participants, indicating less optimal control in the
vicinity of the target (Jongman et al., 2012;de Vries et al.,
2014). Despite this difference in performance, both older and
younger participants showed changes in homing time with
sequence removal and reintroduction after practice, indicating
age-invariant sequence-specific performance optimization late in
the movement trajectory.
Participants showed a progressive improvement in aim
direction and homing time for the sequence elements without
explicit awareness of the sequence in the implicit condition.
This is inconsistent with previous work on upper-limb motor
sequence learning suggesting that sequence awareness allows for
a progressive change in movement execution (Moisello et al.,
2009, 2011;Oostwoud Wijdenes et al., 2016). For example, in a
finger-opposition task procedural optimization of the movement
(reflected by a change in thumb-finger touch duration) was
reached only in conditions in which participants acquired explicit
sequence knowledge (Moisello et al., 2011). The observations
in our study do not support the suggestion that knowledge is
important for motor learning (Stanley and Krakauer, 2013;Wong
et al., 2015).
CONCLUSION
The results of the present study show that sequence learning in a
postural visuomotor control task is possible in both young and
older adults. The most robust learning effects for both groups
were observed for the implicit learning condition. Only the young
participants were able to decrease the directional error with
sequence practice in the explicit condition, implying that older
adults were hampered by the additional attentional cost of explicit
sequence monitoring. In contrast, focusing attention on task-
irrelevant aspects during sequence practice (i.e., the cognitive
task in the implicit condition) did not hamper improvement
in both aim direction and homing time in older adults. The
finding that introducing a secondary task that prevented the
accumulation of explicit knowledge resulted in a robust learning
effect provides further support for the notion that implicit motor
learning methods may be desirable for older adults.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
SC and CL made substantial contributions to conception and
design, acquisition of data, and analysis and interpretation
of data. RV made substantial contribution to analysis and
interpretation of data. SC, CL, and RV participated in drafting the
article and revising it critically for important intellectual content;
and give final approval of the version to be submitted and are
accountable for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions
related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are
appropriately investigated and resolved.
ACKNOWLEDGMENT
We thank Sander Woldhek for the data collection and Helco van
Keeken for programming the Computer Assisted Rehabilitation
Environment.
REFERENCES
Bennett, I. J., Howard, J. H. Jr., and Howard, D. V. (2007). Age-related differences
in implicit learning of subtle third-order sequential structure. J. Gerontol. Ser. B
Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 62, 98–103. doi: 10.1093/geronb/62.2.P98
Boisgontier, M. P., Beets, I. A., Duysens, J., Nieuwboer, A., Krampe, R. T.,
and Swinnen, S. P. (2013). Age-related differences in attentional cost
associated with postural dual tasks: increased recruitment of generic cognitive
resources in older adults. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 1824–1837. doi:
10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.07.014
Brooks, L. R. (1967). The suppression of visualization by reading. Q. J. Exp. Psychol.
19, 289–299. doi: 10.1080/14640746708400105
Brown, R. M., Robertson, E. M., and Press, D. Z. (2009). Sequence skill
acquisition and off-line learning in normal aging. PLoS ONE 4:e6683. doi:
10.1371/journal.pone.0006683
Caljouw, S. R., van der Kamp, J., Lijster, M., and Savelsbergh, G. J. (2011).
Differential effects of a visual illusion on online visual guidance in a stable
environment and online adjustments to perturbations. Conscious. Cogn. 20,
1135–1143. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.03.002
Caljouw, S. R., van der Kamp, J., and Savelsbergh, G. J. (2006). The impact of
task-constraints on the planning and control of interceptive hitting movements.
Neurosci. Lett. 392, 84–89. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.08.067
Castel, A. D., and Craik, F. I. (2003). The effects of aging and divided attention on
memory for item and associative information. Psychol. Aging 18, 873–885. doi:
10.1037/0882-7974.18.4.873
Chambaron, S., Ginhac, D., Ferrel-Chapus, C., and Perruchet, P. (2006). Implicit
learning of a repeated segment in continuous tracking: a reappraisal. Q. J. Exp.
Psychol. 59, 845–854. doi: 10.1080/17470210500198585
Cherry, K. E., and Stadler, M. A. (1995). Implicit learning of a nonverbal sequence
in younger and older adults. Psychol. Aging 10, 379–394. doi: 10.1037/0882-
7974.10.3.379
Curran, T. (1997). Effects of aging on implicit sequence learning: accounting
for sequence structure and explicit knowledge. Psychol. Res. 60, 24–41. doi:
10.1007/BF00419678
de Vries, E. A., Caljouw, S. R., Coppens, M. J., Postema, K., Verkerke, G. J.,
and Lamoth, C. J. (2014). Differences between young and older adults in the
control of weight shifting within the surface of support. PLoS ONE 9:e98494.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0098494
Feeney, J. J., Howard, J. H. Jr., and Howard, D. V. (2002). Implicit learning
of higher order sequences in middle age. Psychol. Aging 17, 351–355. doi:
10.1037/0882-7974.17.2.351
Fitts, P. M., and Posner, M. (1967). Human Performance. Belmont, CA:
Brooks/Cole Pub. Co.
Fletcher, P. C., Zafiris, O., Frith, C. D., Honey, R. A. E., Corlett, P. R., Zilles, K., et al.
(2005). On the benefits of not trying: Brain activity and connectivity reflecting
the interactions of explicit and implicit sequence learning. Cereb. Cortex 15,
1002–1015. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh201
Frensch, P. A., and Miner, C. S. (1994). Effects of presentation rate and individual
differences in short-term memory capacity on an indirect measure of serial
learning. Mem. Cogn. 22, 95–110. doi: 10.3758/BF03202765
Gaillard, V., Destrebecqz, A., Michiels, S., and Cleeremans, A. (2009). Effects of
age and practice in sequence learning: a graded account of ageing, learning, and
control. Eur. J. Cogn. Psychol. 21, 255–282. doi: 10.1080/09541440802257423
Ghilardi, M. F., Carbon, M., Silvestri, G., Dhawan, V., Tagliati, M., Bressman, S.,
et al. (2003). Impaired sequence learning in carriers of the DYT1 dystonia
mutation. Ann. Neurol. 54, 102–109. doi: 10.1002/ana.10610
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 8May 2016 | Volume 7 | Article 733
fpsyg-07-00733 May 21, 2016 Time: 13:40 # 9
Caljouw et al. Learning Sequential Postural Weight-Shifting
Ghilardi, M. F., Moisello, C., Silvestri, G., Ghez, C., and Krakauer, J. W.
(2009). Learning of a sequential motor skill comprises explicit and implicit
components that consolidate differently. J. Neurophysiol. 101, 2218–2229. doi:
10.1152/jn.01138.2007
Glover, S. (2004). Separate visual representations in the planning and control of
action. Behav. Brain Sci. 27, 3–24. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X04000020
Hedden, T., and Gabrieli, J. D. (2004). Insights into the ageing mind: a view from
cognitive neuroscience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 87–96. doi: 10.1038/nrn1323
Howard, D. V., and Howard, J. H. Jr. (2001). When it does hurt to try: adult age
differences in the effects of instructions on implicit pattern learning. Psychon.
Bull. Rev 8, 798–805. doi: 10.3758/BF03196220
Howard, D. V., Howard, J. H. Jr., Japikse, K., DiYanni, C., Thompson, A., and
Somberg, R. (2004). Implicit sequence learning: effects of level of structure,
adult age, and extended practice. Psychol. Aging 19, 79–92. doi: 10.1037/0882-
7974.19.1.79
Howard, J. H., and Howard, D. V. (2013). Aging mind and brain: is
implicit learning spared in healthy aging? Front. Psychol. 4:817. doi:
10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00817
Huxhold, O., Li, S. C., Schmiedek, F., and Lindenberger, U. (2006). Dual-
tasking postural control: aging and the effects of cognitive demand in
conjunction with focus of attention. Brain Res. Bull. 69, 294–305. doi:
10.1016/j.brainresbull.2006.01.002
Janacsek, K., and Nemeth, D. (2012). Predicting the future: from implicit
learning to consolidation. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 83, 213–221. doi:
10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.11.012
Jimenez, L., and Vazquez, G. A. (2005). Sequence learning under dual-task
conditions: alternatives to a resource-based account. Psychol. Res. 69, 352–368.
doi: 10.1007/s00426-004-0210-9
Jongman, V., Lamoth, C. J., van Keeken, H., and Caljouw, S. R. (2012). Postural
control of elderly: moving to predictable and unpredictable targets. Stud. Health
Technol. Inform. 181, 93–97.
King, B. R., Fogel, S. M., Albouy, G., and Doyon, J. (2013). Neural correlates of the
age-related changes in motor sequence learning and motor adaptation in older
adults. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:142. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00142
Lamoth, C. J., van Lummel, R. C., and Beek, P. J. (2009). Athletic
skill level is reflected in body sway: a test case for accelometry in
combination with stochastic dynamics. Gait Posture 29, 546–551. doi:
10.1016/j.gaitpost.2008.12.006
Laughton, C. A., Slavin, M., Katdare, K., Nolan, L., Bean, J. F., Kerrigan,
D. C., et al. (2003). Aging, muscle activity, and balance control: physiologic
changes associated with balance impairment. Gait Posture 18, 101–108. doi:
10.1016/S0966-6362(02)00200-X
Lovden, M., Schaefer, S., Pohlmeyer, A. E., and Lindenberger, U. (2008). Walking
variability and working-memory load in aging: a dual-process account relating
cognitive control to motor control performance. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc.
Sci. 63, 121–128. doi: 10.1093/geronb/63.3.P121
Masters, R. S. (1992). Knowledge knerves and know-how. The role of explicit versus
implicit knowledge in the breakdown of complex motor skill under pressure. Br.
J. Psychol. 83, 343–358.
Moisello, C., Avanzino, L., Tacchino, A., Ruggeri, P., Ghilardi, M. F., and
Bove, M. (2011). Motor sequence learning: acquisition of explicit knowledge
is concomitant to changes in motor strategy of finger opposition movements.
Brain Res. Bull. 85, 104–108. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2011.03.023
Moisello, C., Crupi, D., Tunik, E., Quartarone, A., Bove, M., Tononi, G., et al.
(2009). The serial reaction time task revisited: a study on motor sequence
learning with an arm-reaching task. Exp. Brain Res. 194, 143–155. doi:
10.1007/s00221-008-1681-5
Nejati, V., Garusi Farshi, M. T., Ashayeri, H., and Aghdasi, M. T. (2008). Dual
task interference in implicit sequence learning by young and old adults. Int.
J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 23, 801–804. doi: 10.1002/gps.1976
Nissen, M. J., and Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional requirements of learning:
evidence from performance measures. Cogn. Psychol. 19, 1. doi: 10.1016/0010-
0285(87)90002-8
Oostwoud Wijdenes, L., Brenner, E., and Smeets, J. (2016). Exposing sequence
learning in a double-step task. Exp. Brain Res. 234, 1701–1712. doi:
10.1007/s00221-016-4566-z
Orrell, A. J., Eves, F. F., and Masters, R. S. (2006). Implicit motor learning of a
balancing task. Gait Posture 23, 9–16. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2004.11.010
Park, H. L., O’Connell, J. E., and Thomson, R. G. (2003). A systematic review of
cognitive decline in the general elderly population. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 18,
1121–1134. doi: 10.1002/gps.1023
Reber, A. S. (1992). The cognitive unconscious: an evolutionary perspective.
Conscious. Cogn. 1, 93–133. doi: 10.1016/1053-8100(92)90051-B
Rieckmann, A., and Backman, L. (2009). Implicit learning in aging: extant patterns
and new directions. Neuropsychol. Rev. 19, 490–503. doi: 10.1007/s11065-009-
9117-y
Shea, C. H., Park, J. H., and Braden, H. W. (2006). Age-related effects in sequential
motor learning. Phys. Ther. 86, 478–488.
Shea, C. H., Wulf, G., Whitacre, C. A., and Park, J. H. (2001). Surfing the implicit
wave. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A 54, 841–862. doi: 10.1080/713755993
Simon, J. R., Howard, J. H. Jr., and Howard, D. V. (2011). Age differences in implicit
learning of probabilistic unstructured sequences. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci.
Soc. Sci. 66, 32–38. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbq066
Song, S., Marks, B., Howard, J. H. Jr., and Howard, D. V. (2009). Evidence for
parallel explicit and implicit sequence learning systems in older adults. Behav.
Brain Res. 196, 328–332. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.09.022
Stanley, J., and Krakauer, J. W. (2013). Motor skill depends on knowledge of facts.
Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:503. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00503
Sturnieks, D. L., St George, R., and Lord, S. R. (2008). Balance disorders
in the elderly. Neurophysiol. Clin. 38, 467–478. doi: 10.1016/j.neucli.2008.
09.001
Van Ooteghem, K., Frank, J. S., Allard, F., Buchanan, J. J., Oates, A. R., and
Horak, F. B. (2008). Compensatory postural adaptations during continuous,
variable amplitude perturbations reveal generalized rather than sequence-
specific learning. Exp. Brain Res. 187, 603–611. doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-
1329-5
Van Ooteghem, K., Frank, J. S., Allard, F., and Horak, F. B. (2010). Aging
does not affect generalized postural motor learning in response to variable
amplitude oscillations of the support surface. Exp. Brain Res. 204, 505–514. doi:
10.1007/s00221-010-2316-1
Vandenbossche, J., Coomans, D., Homblé, K., and Deroost, N. (2014). The effect of
cognitive aging on implicit sequence learning and dual tasking. Front. Psychol.
5:154. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00154
Verneau, M., van der Kamp, J., Savelsbergh, G. J., and de Looze, M. P. (2014). Age
and time effects on implicit and explicit learning. Exp. Aging Res. 40, 477–511.
doi: 10.1080/0361073X.2014.926778
Verwey, W. B. (2010). Diminished motor skill development in elderly:
indications for limited motor chunk use. Acta Psychol. 134, 206–214. doi:
10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.02.001
Willingham, D. B., and Goedert-Eschmann, K. (1999). The relation between
implicit and explicit learning: evidence for parallel development. Psychol. Sci
10, 531–534. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00201
Willingham, D. B., Salidis, J., and Gabrieli, J. D. (2002). Direct comparison of neural
systems mediating conscious and unconscious skill learning. J. Neurophysiol.
88, 1451–1460. doi: 10.1152/jn.00461.2001
Wong, A. L., Lindquist, M. A., Haith, A. M., and Krakauer, J. W. (2015).
Explicit knowledge enhances motor vigor and performance: motivation versus
practice in sequence tasks. J. Neurophysiol. 114, 219–232. doi: 10.1152/jn.00218.
2015
Zatsiorsky, V. M., and Duarte, M. (2000). Rambling and trembling in quiet
standing. Motor Control 4, 185–200.
Conflict of Interest Statement: The authors declare that the research was
conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could
be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Copyright © 2016 Caljouw, Veldkamp and Lamoth. This is an open-access article
distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY).
The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the
original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this
journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution
or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 9May 2016 | Volume 7 | Article 733
... Finally, we aimed to elucidate the training-related neural activation changes of VR-based weight-shift training using fNIRS. Based on earlier work, we expected that older adults would be able to improve weight-shifting, balance performance and limits of stability after a single training session 8 and that these changes would be maintained over 24 h 24 . We were uncertain about whether such improvements would hold when exposed to DT distraction 25 . ...
... These findings are in line with a systematic review on balance training in older adults 26 , and the effects sizes found after a 4-week 27 and 5-week 28 balance training program. Caljouw et al. 8 also found improvements in target-directed weight-shifting performance after a single-session of training. They revealed that an implicit training method, in which less focus was directed towards task details, led to better results than explicit training in older adults, possibly due to its relative independence of working memory 29 . ...
... Next, we could also demonstrate that weight-shift training effects were resilient to DT interference. Similar results were found in other studies investigating DT balance training 8,32 showing that DT performance remained intact at follow-up 25 . Even though participants in the current study received ST training, as no deliberate secondary task was added to the practice environment, the finding that DT performance improved may be explained by the fact that the VR wasp game combined several task components in the motor-cognitive domain. ...
Article
Full-text available
Mediolateral weight-shifting is an important aspect of postural control. As it is currently unknown whether a short training session of mediolateral weight-shifting in a virtual reality (VR) environment can improve weight-shifting, we investigated this question and also probed the impact of practice on brain activity. Forty healthy older adults were randomly allocated to a training (EXP, n = 20, age = 70.80 (65–77), 9 females) or a control group (CTR, n = 20, age = 71.65 (65–82), 10 females). The EXP performed a 25-min weight-shift training in a VR-game, whereas the CTR rested for the same period. Weight-shifting speed in both single- (ST) and dual-task (DT) conditions was determined before, directly after, and 24 h after intervention. Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy (fNIRS) assessed the oxygenated hemoglobin (HbO2) levels in five cortical regions of interest. Weight-shifting in both ST and DT conditions improved in EXP but not in CTR, and these gains were retained after 24 h. Effects transferred to wider limits of stability post-training in EXP versus CTR. HbO2 levels in the left supplementary motor area were significantly increased directly after training in EXP during ST (change < SEM), and in the left somatosensory cortex during DT (change > SEM). We interpret these changes in the motor coordination and sensorimotor integration areas of the cortex as possibly learning-related.
... The relation between implicit sequence learning [typically measured by reaction time (RT)] and WM capacity has been studied extensively (e.g., Bo, Jennett, & Seidler, 2012;Caljouw, Veldkamp, & Lamoth, 2016;Feldman, Kerr, & Streissguth, 1995;Guzmán, 2018;Kaufman et al., 2010;Unsworth & Engle, 2005;Weitz, O'Shea, Zook, & Needham, 2011;Yang & Li, 2012). However, previous studies reported mixed results regarding the relation between the two systems, potentially because researchers used different WM capacity tests (i.e., visuospatial, verbal, or numerical), but also different measures of learning in the SRT task (e.g., the difference in average RT between blocks with a training sequence and blocks with a random sequence, or the rate of RT improvement across blocks of the SRT task; for a review, see . ...
... For example, a number of previous studies found no relation between implicit sequence learning and WM capacity (Caljouw et al., 2016;Guzmán, 2018;Jimenez & Vazquez, 2005;Jongbloed-Pereboom, Nijhuis-van der Sanden, & Steenbergen, 2019;Masters, 1992;Meissner, Keitel, Südmeyer, & Pollok, 2016;Unsworth & Engle, 2005;Yang & Li, 2012). Unsworth and Engle (2005) reported that there were no differences in implicit learning in a manual version of the SRT between high and low WM capacity individuals. ...
... Based on a number of previous studies (e.g., Caljouw et al., 2016;Guzmán, 2018;Kaufman et al., 2010;Unsworth & Engle, 2005), a relation between WM capacity and implicit sequence learning would not be expected. However, if WM capacity and implicit learning at least partly rely on shared mechanisms, we should expect that the two systems are related to some extent . ...
Article
Full-text available
We investigated the relation between implicit sequence learning and individual differences in working memory (WM) capacity. Participants performed an oculomotor version of the serial reaction time (SRT) task and three computerized WM tasks. Implicit learning was measured using anticipation measures only, as they represent strong indicators of learning. Our results demonstrate that anticipatory behavior in the SRT task changes as a function of WM capacity, such that it increases with decreased WM capacity. On the other hand, WM capacity did not affect the overall number of correct anticipations in the task. In addition, we report a positive relation between WM capacity and the number of consecutive correct anticipations (or chunks), and a negative relation between WM capacity and the overall number of errors, indicating different learning strategies during implicit sequence learning. The results of the current study are theoretically important, because they demonstrate that individual differences in WM capacity could account for differences in learning processes, and ultimately change individuals’ anticipatory behavior, even when learning is implicit, without intention and awareness.
... Although the older adults appeared to be less likely to acquire a conscious, explicit representation of the sequence of stimuli or responses, a statistically significant difference was not found. This observation contrasts with age-related differences in explicit sequence monitoring reported by Caljouw et al. [53] in a postural visuomotor sequence learning task involving body weight shifts. Therefore, it may be that the influence of age on the acquisition of explicit sequence knowledge may be specific to the type of movement task or that our experiment was not sufficiently sensitive to uncover an age effect in the acquired explicit knowledge. ...
Article
Full-text available
Sequence learning in serial reaction time tasks (SRTT) is an established, lab-based experimental paradigm to study acquisition and transfer of skill based on the detection of predictable stimulus and motor response sequences. Sequence learning has been mainly studied in key presses using visual target stimuli and is demonstrated by better performance in predictable sequences than in random sequences. In this study, we investigated sequence learning in the context of more complex locomotor responses. To this end, we developed a novel goal-directed stepping SRTT with auditory target stimuli in order to subsequently assess the effect of aging on sequence learning in this task, expecting that age-related performance reductions in postural control might disturb the acquisition of the sequence. We used pressure-sensitive floor mats to characterise performance across ten blocks of trials. In Experiment 1, 22 young adults demonstrated successful acquisition of the sequence in terms of the time to step on the target mat and percent error and thus validated our new paradigm. In Experiment 2, in order to contrast performance improvements in the stepping SRTT between 27 young and 22 old adults, motion capture of the feet was combined with the floor mat system to delineate individual movement phases during stepping onto a target mat. The latencies of several postural events as well as other movement parameters of a step were assessed. We observed significant learning effects in the latency of step initiation, the time to step on the target mat, and motion parameters such as stepping amplitude and peak stepping velocity, as well as in percent error. The data showed general age-related slowing but no significant performance differences in procedural locomotor sequence learning between young and old adults. The older adults also had comparable conscious representations of the sequence of stimuli as the young adults. We conclude that sequence learning occurred in this locomotor learning task that is much more complex than typical finger-tapping sequence learning tasks, and that healthy older adults showed similar learning effects compared to young adults, suggesting intact locomotor sequence learning capabilities despite general slowing and normal age-related decline in sensorimotor function.
... Six participants in the not-informed group had gained a considerable amount of explicit knowledge, as they could verbalize the entire fixed sequence of length four at the end of the experiment (see Section 3.1 for detailed results). Since it is uncertain at which time point they gained consciously accessible knowledge of the sequence and we aimed to retain group characteristics as distinct as possible, they were excluded from analyses (Caljouw et al., 2016;Hirano et al., 2017). Based on the same reasoning, two participants in the informed group were excluded. ...
Article
Full-text available
Sequence learning in serial reaction time tasks (SRTTs) is usually inferred through the reaction time measured by a keyboard. However, this chronometric parameter offers no information beyond the time point of the button-press. We therefore examined whether sequence learning can be measured by muscle activations via electro-myography (EMG) in a dual-task paradigm. The primary task was a SRTT, in which the stimuli followed a fixed sequence in some blocks, whereas the sequence was random in the control condition. The secondary task stimulus was always random. One group was informed about the fixed sequence, and the other not. We assessed three dependent variables. The chronometric parameter premotor time represents the duration between stimulus onset and the onset of EMG activity, which indicates the start of the response. The other variables describe the response itself considering the EMG activity after response start. The EMG integral was analyzed, and additionally , tensor decomposition was implemented to assess sequence dependent changes in the contribution of the obtained subcomponents. The results show explicit sequence learning in this dual-task setting. Specifically, the informed group show shorter premotor times in fixed than random sequences as well as larger EMG integral and tensor contributions. Further, increased activity seems to represent response certainty, since a decrease is found for both groups in trials following erroneous responses. Interestingly, the sensitivity to sequence and post-error effects varies between the subcomponents. The results indicate that muscle activity can be a useful indicator of response behavior in addition to chronometric parameters.
... 有研究考察了情景记忆编码和提取阶段脑激活的关系,发现成功编码和成功提取所激活的 脑区有高度重合,因而认为提取是编码过程的再现,有着相同的神经活动模式 [71,72] 。但是,另 一些研究发现编码和提取存在一定的差异,表现出不对称的关系。例如,左侧前额叶更多地参 与编码加工,而右侧前额叶更多地参与提取加工;海马的前部在编码阶段有较多的激活,而提 取阶段选择性激活海马的后部区域 [73,74] ;编码和提取阶段海马激活的差异在海马头部相比海马 体和海马尾部更大 [75] [91,92] ,主要采用人工语法任务、序列反应时任务和统计学习任务等 加以研究。在个体出生后的头一年,内隐学习就已出现并成为婴幼儿时期个体主要的学习方式 [93][94][95][96] 。研究发现,8 个月大的婴儿通过单纯地接触人工语言就可以很快地学会不同音节之间的 转换,表现出对语言音节信息的敏感性 [95] 。9 个月大的婴儿在接触多元素视觉场景时,会对元 素之间共同发生的条件概率关系表现出敏感性 [97] 。6 个月和 12 个月大的婴儿已经学会了根据 先前的听觉刺激预测视觉刺激 [98,99] 。甚至有研究显示,在跨通道的学习中,婴儿表现出比成人 更大的优势 [100] 。总之,婴幼儿具有很强的内隐学习能力,并且这种能力可能随着他们的年龄 增长而更加完善。 有研究者 [101] 考察了 4 岁到 85 岁个体的内隐概率序列学习。结果发现,4-12 岁年龄组表现 出最强的学习效果。在 12 岁左右,内隐学习的成绩有显著的下降,在老年阶段内隐学习的成 绩更低。研究者认为,在 12 岁之前,个体的学习方式主要是内隐学习。之后,内隐学习开始 退居二线,随着年龄的进一步增长,个体的学习方式主要依靠外显学习,并且可以很好地协调 内隐学习和外显学习之间的关系 [102] 。进入老年,个体的内隐学习能力一般不会削弱 [103] ,但是 有些形式的内隐学习可能随着年龄的增长而有所下降 [104] 。另外,老年人内隐学习的能力会受 到任务难度、外显学习和工作记忆等其他认知能力的影响 [105] 。 内隐记忆通常是指个体在不需要有意识回忆过去经历过的信息的测验中,相比新信息,对 经历过的信息做出反应的速度更快,正确率更高,常以启动效应表示 [106][107][108] 。研究显示,在出 生后的头 9 个月里,年龄较大的婴儿在内隐记忆上的表现比年龄较小的婴儿更好;但在 9 个月 之后的生命发展过程中,内隐记忆表现相对稳定 [109][110][111][112][113] 。追踪研究发现,出生 9 个月后内隐记 忆成绩可以预测 3 岁左右的内隐记忆表现 [112] 。不过,也有研究得到了不一致的结果。例如, Vakil 等人 [114] 比较了儿童中期(6.5-8.5 岁)、青少年中期(13-14.5 岁)和成年早期(20.5-24 岁)三个年龄组被试的内隐记忆和外显记忆,发现儿童中期组个体的内隐记忆表现不如其它两 个年龄组,可能意味着内隐记忆在儿童期内仍在继续发展。 内隐记忆可能并非单一的成分 [115,116] ,对儿童期到成年期内隐记忆的发展研究显示,知觉 内隐记忆大多没有表现出明显的年龄差异 [60,[117][118][119] ,但儿童和成人的概念内隐记忆存在差异 [120,121] 。也就是说,儿童期内隐记忆的发展可能存在知觉和概念之间的分离。不过,也有研究 显示出知觉和概念内隐记忆有类似的发展轨迹 [114] 。另外,多数研究认为,成人外显记忆随年 龄增长有所下降 [122,123] ,65 岁后下降更加明显,但内隐记忆却在老化过程中保持完好 [124][125][126][127][128][129] 。 然而,最近有部分证据显示,发现内隐记忆不受老化影响的研究主要采用的是识别式启动,而 老年人相比年轻人在产生式启动上的成绩明显降低 [130][131][132][133] 。可见,内隐记忆的老化可能在识别 式和产生式启动之间存在分离。 ...
... Therefore, adequate visual feedback is keyed to postural skill transfer in older adults, who already have an increased reliance on the visual system for postural control. The behavioral results highlight the fact that visual cues can improve plan-based posture control in older adults when explicit awareness of posture-relevant features is desirable under environmental constraints (Caljouw et al., 2016). ...
Article
Full-text available
Skill transfer from trained balance exercises is critical to reduce the rate of falls in older adults, who rely more on vision to control postural responses due to age-dependent sensory reweighting. With an electroencephalography (EEG) minimum spanning tree (MST) structure, the purpose of this study was to compare the organization of supraspinal neural networks of transfer effect after postural training using full and intermittent visual feedbacks for older adults. Thirty-two older adults were randomly assigned to the stroboscopic vision (SV) ( n = 16; age = 64.7 ± 3.0 years) and control (16; 66.3 ± 2.7 years) groups for balance training on a stabilometer (target task) with on-line visual feedback. Center-of-pressure characteristics and an MST-based connectome of the weighted phase-lag index during the bilateral stance on a foam surface (transfer task) were compared before and after stabilometer training. The results showed that both the SV and control groups showed improvements in postural stability in the trained task ( p < 0.001). However, unlike the control group ( p = 0.030), the SV group who received intermittent visual feedback during the stabilometer training failed to reduce the size of postural sway in the anteroposterior direction of the postural transfer task (unstable stance on the foam surface) in the post-test ( p = 0.694). In addition, network integration for the transfer task in the post-test was absent in the SV group ( p > 0.05). For the control group in the post-test, it manifested with training-related increases in leaf fraction in beta band ( p = 0.015) and maximum betweenness in alpha band ( p = 0.018), but a smaller diameter in alpha ( p = 0.006)/beta ( p = 0.021) bands and average eccentricity in alpha band ( p = 0.028). In conclusion, stabilometer training with stroboscopic vision impairs generalization of postural skill to unstable stance for older adults. Adequate visual information is a key mediating factor of supraspinal neural networks to carry over balance skill in older adults.
Thesis
Full-text available
Predictability is increasingly recognized as an important principle in perception and motor learning. The pursuit of increased predictability seems to one of the main goals that the human system pursues. Therefore, providing predictability in one of the most challenging situations that humans face, namely multitasking, a promising line of research. In this thesis the impact of predictability was systematically investigated in five experiments. In the first four experiments predictability was achieved by implementing a repeating pattern in one task, or both tasks. Participants acquired knowledge of these patterns either explicitly or implicitly in several training sessions, under single-task or dual-task conditions. We tested whether this increased predictability helped dual-task performance after the training sessions. The results suggest that predictability is helpful for dual-task performance, although the benefits are confined to the predictable task itself. In a fifth experiment we focused on providing between task predictability, which led to a large performance improvement in both tasks, prompting the discussion about what constitutes a task, in the sense of when can two tasks be perceived as a single task comprising both, a theoretical problem we tried to tackle in one of the articles. Explanations for the findings, theoretical implications, methodological issues and suggestions for future research are given in the general discussion
Article
Full-text available
Motor sequence learning in persons with multiple sclerosis (pwMS) and healthy controls (HC) under implicit or explicit learning conditions has not yet been investigated in a stepping task. Given the prevalent cognitive and mobility impairments in pwMS, this is important in order to understand motor learning processes and optimize rehabilitation strategies. Nineteen pwMS (the Expanded Disability Status Scale = 3.4 ± 1.2) and 18 HC performed a modified serial reaction time task by stepping as fast as possible on a stepping tile when it lit up, either with (explicit) or without (implicit) knowledge of the presence of a sequence beforehand. Motor sequence learning was studied by examining response time changes and differences between sequence and random blocks during the learning session (acquisition), 24 h later (retention), and in three dual‐task (DT) conditions at baseline and retention (automaticity) using subtracting sevens, verbal fluency, and vigilance as concurrent cognitive DTs. Response times improved and were lower for the sequenced compared with the random blocks at the post‐ and retention tests (P's < 0.001). Response times during DT conditions improved after learning, but DT cost improved only for the subtracting sevens DT condition. No differences in learning were observed between learning conditions or groups. This study showed motor sequence learning, by acquisition and retention, in a stepping task in pwMS with motor impairments, to a similar degree as HC and regardless of learning conditions. Whether automaticity increased remains unclear.
Article
Full-text available
Normal adult aging is known to be associated with lower performance on tasks assessing the short-term storage of information. However, whether or not there are additional age-related deficits associated with concurrent storage and processing demands within working memory remains unclear. Methodological differences across studies are considered critical factors responsible for the variability in the magnitude of the reported age effects. Here we synthesized comparisons of younger and older adults' performance on tasks measuring storage alone against those combining storage with concurrent processing of information. We also considered the influence of task-related moderator variables. Meta-analysis of effect sizes revealed a small but disproportionate effect of processing on older adults' memory performance. Moderator analysis indicated that equating single task storage performance across age groups (titration) and the nature of the stimulus material were important determinants of memory accuracy. Titration of storage task difficulty was found to lead to smaller, and non-significant, age-differences in dual task costs. These results were corroborated by supplementary Brinley and state-trace analyses. We discuss these findings in relation to the extant literature and current working memory theory as well as possibilities for future research to address the residual heterogeneity in effect sizes. (194 words)
Article
Full-text available
Previous studies have demonstrated age-related deficits in implicit learning of higher order sequences in comparisons of college-age and elderly adults (e.g., J. H. Howard & D. V. Howard, 1997). This study examined whether these age deficits begin in middle age. Results showed a reliable age-related deficit in pattern sensitivity in “older” compared with “younger” middle-aged people, and age reliably predicted sensitivity to the sequence by using both speed and accuracy measures. The results are consistent with an age-related decline in a generic cognitive resource as reflected in T. A. Salthouse’s (1996) simultaneity mechanism of cognitive aging.
Article
Full-text available
Is it possible to learn to perform a motor sequence without awareness of the sequence? In two experiments, we presented participants with the most elementary sequence: an alternation between two options. We used a double-step pointing task in which the final position of the target alternated between two quite similar values. The task forced participants to start moving before the final target was visible, allowing us to determine participants' expectations about the final target position without explicitly asking them. We tracked participants' expectations (and thus motor sequence learning) by measuring the direction of the initial part of the movement, before any response to the final step. We found that participants learnt to anticipate the average size of the final step, but that they did not learn the sequence. In a second experiment, we extended the duration of the learning period and increased the difference in size between the target position changes. Some participants started anticipating the step size in accordance with the sequence at some time during the experiment. These participants reported having noticed the simple sequence. The participants who had not noticed the sequence did not move in anticipation of the sequence. This suggests that participants who did not learn this very simple sequence explicitly also did not learn it implicitly.
Article
Full-text available
Unlabelled: BACKGROUND/STUDY CONTEXT: It has been proposed that effects of aging are more pronounced for explicit than for implicit motor learning. The authors evaluated this claim by comparing the efficacy of explicit and implicit learning of a movement sequence in young and older adults, and by testing the resilience against fatigue and secondary tasking after learning. It was also examined whether explicit learning in older adults can be promoted by alleviating time constraints during learning. Methods: The alternating serial reaction time task (ASRTT) was used. Experiment 1 compared the benefits of receiving full instructions about the stimulus sequence relative to receiving no instructions in young (20-25 years) and older (50-65 years) adults during retention and during transfer to fatigue and secondary task conditions. Experiment 2 alleviated time constraints during the initial bouts of practice with full instructions. Results: Experiment 1 indicated that the older adults learned on the ASRTT and achieved similar performance as young adults when no instructions were given. In contrast to the young adults, learning was not superior in older adults who received full instructions compared with those who did not. Experiment 2 indicated that alleviating time constraints allowed some of the older adults to gain from instruction but only under relatively low time constraints, but there was no retention with rigorous time constraints. Conclusion: Explicit learning, but not implicit learning, declines in older adults. This is partly due to older adults difficulties to apply explicit knowledge. Less rigorous time constraints can help to ameliorate some of these difficulties and may induce levels of explicit learning in older adults that will result in superior performance compared with implicit learning. Implicit learning did occur under time constraints that prevented explicit learning.
Article
Full-text available
An important reason for falling in elderly is incorrect weight-shifting. In many daily life activities quick and accurate weight-shifting is needed to maintain balance and to prevent from falling. The present study aims to gain more insight in age-related differences in the control of weight-shifting. Nine healthy older adults (70.3±6.9 years) and twelve young adults (20.9±0.5 years) participated in the study. They performed a weight shifting task by moving the body's center of pressure, represented by a red dot on a screen, in different directions, towards targets of different sizes and at different distances projected on a screen. Movement time, fluency and accuracy of the movement were determined. Accuracy was quantified by the number of times the cursor hit the goal target before a target switch was realized (counts on goal) and by the time required to realize a target switch after the goal target was hit by the cursor for the first time (dwelling time). Fluency was expressed by the maximal deviation of the performed path with respect to the ideal path and the number of peaks, or inflections in the performed path. Significant main effects of target size, target distance and age on all outcome measures were found. With decreasing target size, increasing target distance and increasing age, movement time significantly increased and fluency and accuracy significantly decreased (i.e. increased number of peaks, maximal deviation, number of times on the goal target and longer dwelling time around the goal target). In addition, significant interaction effects of size*age and distance*age were found. Older adults needed more time to perform the weight-shifting task and their movements were less fluent and accurate compared to younger adults, especially with increasing task difficulty. This indicates that elderly might have difficulties with executing an adequate adaptation to a perturbation in daily life.
Article
Full-text available
We investigated the influence of attentional demands on sequence-specific learning by means of the serial reaction time task (Nissen and Bullemer, 1987) in young (age 18–25) and aged (age 55–75) adults. Participants had to respond as fast as possible to a stimulus presented in one of four horizontal locations by pressing a key corresponding to the spatial position of the stimulus. During the training phase sequential blocks were accompanied by (1) no secondary task (single), (2) a secondary tone counting task (dual tone), or (3) a secondary shape counting task (dual shape). Both secondary tasks were administered to investigate whether low and high interference tasks interact with implicit learning and age. The testing phase, under baseline single condition, was implemented to assess differences in sequence-specific learning between young and aged adults. Results indicate that (1) aged subjects show less sequence learning compared to young adults, (2) young participants show similar implicit learning effects under both single and dual task conditions when we account for explicit awareness, and (3) aged adults demonstrate reduced learning when the primary task is accompanied with a secondary task, even when explicit awareness is included as a covariate in the analysis. These findings point to implicit learning deficits under dual task conditions that can be related to cognitive aging, demonstrating the need for sufficient cognitive resources while performing a sequence learning task.
Article
Full-text available
It is often held that although explicit learning declines in the course of normal aging, implicit learning is relatively preserved. Here we summarize research from our group which leads us to argue that some forms of implicit learning do decline with adult age. In particular, we propose that there are age-related declines in implicit learning of probabilistic sequential relationships that occur across the adult lifespan, and that they reflect, at least in part, age-related striatal dysfunction. We first review behavioral evidence supporting this age-related decline and then evidence from patient groups, genetics, and neuroimaging supporting this striatal dysfunction hypothesis.
Article
Full-text available
Those in 20th century philosophy, psychology, and neuroscience who have discussed the nature of skilled action have, for the most part, accepted the view that being skilled at an activity is independent of knowing facts about that activity, i.e., that skill is independent of knowledge of facts. In this paper we question this view of motor skill. We begin by situating the notion of skill in historical and philosophical context. We use the discussion to explain and motivate the view that motor skill depends upon knowledge of facts. This conclusion seemingly contradicts well-known results in cognitive science. It is natural, on the face of it, to take the case of H.M., the seminal case in cognitive neuroscience that led to the discovery of different memory systems, as providing powerful evidence for the independence of knowledge and skill acquisition. After all, H.M. seems to show that motor learning is retained even when previous knowledge about the activity has been lost. Improvements in skill generally require increased precision of selected actions, which we call motor acuity. Motor acuity may indeed not require propositional knowledge and has direct parallels with perceptual acuity. We argue, however, that reflection on the specifics of H.M.'s case, as well as other research on the nature of skill, indicates that learning to become skilled at a motor task, for example tennis, depends also on knowledge-based selection of the right actions. Thus skilled activity requires both acuity and knowledge, with both increasing with practice. The moral of our discussion ranges beyond debates about motor skill; we argue that it undermines any attempt to draw a distinction between practical and theoretical activities. While we will reject the independence of skill and knowledge, our discussion leaves open several different possible relations between knowledge and skill. Deciding between them is a task to be resolved by future research.
Article
Full-text available
As the world's population ages, a deeper understanding of the relationship between aging and motor learning will become increasingly relevant in basic research and applied settings. In this context, this review aims to address the effects of age on motor sequence learning (MSL) and motor adaptation (MA) with respect to behavioral, neurological, and neuroimaging findings. Previous behavioral research investigating the influence of aging on motor learning has consistently reported the following results. First, the initial acquisition of motor sequences is not altered, except under conditions of increased task complexity. Second, older adults demonstrate deficits in motor sequence memory consolidation. And, third, although older adults demonstrate deficits during the exposure phase of MA paradigms, the aftereffects following removal of the sensorimotor perturbation are similar to young adults, suggesting that the adaptive ability of older adults is relatively intact. This paper will review the potential neural underpinnings of these behavioral results, with a particular emphasis on the influence of age-related dysfunctions in the cortico-striatal system on motor learning.
Article
Motor skill learning involves a practice-induced improvement in the speed and/or accuracy of a discrete movement. It is often thought that paradigms involving repetitive practice of discrete movements performed in a fixed sequence result in a further enhancement of skill beyond practice of the individual movements in a random order. However, sequence-specific performance improvements could arise without practice as a result of knowledge of the sequence order; knowledge could operate by either enabling advanced motor planning of the known sequence elements or by increasing overall motivation. Here we examined how knowledge and practice contribute to performance of a sequence of movements. We found that explicit knowledge provided through instruction produced practice-independent improvements in reaction time and execution quality. These performance improvements occurred even for random elements within a partially known sequence, indicative of a general motivational effect rather than a sequence-specific effect of advanced planning. This motivational effect suggests that knowledge influences performance in a manner analogous to reward. Additionally, practice led to similar improvements in execution quality for both known and random sequences. The lack of interaction between knowledge and practice suggests that any skill acquisition occurring during discrete sequence tasks arises solely from practice of the individual movement elements independent of their order. We conclude that performance improvements in discrete sequence tasks arise from the combination of knowledge-based motivation and sequence-independent practice; investigating this interplay between cognition and movement may facilitate a greater understanding of the acquisition of skilled behavior. Copyright © 2015, Journal of Neurophysiology.
Article
Dual-task designs have been used widely to study the degree of automatic and controlled processing involved in postural stability of young and older adults. However, several unexplained discrepancies in the results weaken this literature. To resolve this problem, a careful selection of dual-task studies that met certain methodological criteria are considered with respect to reported interactions of age (young vs. older adults)×task (single task vs. dual task) in stable and unstable postural conditions. Our review shows that older adults are able to perform a postural dual task as well as younger adults in stable conditions. However, when the complexity of the postural task is increased by dynamic conditions (surface and surround), performance in postural, concurrent, or both tasks is more affected in older relative to young adults. In light of neuroimaging studies and new conceptual frameworks, these results demonstrate an age-related increase of controlled processing of standing associated with greater intermittent adjustments.