ArticlePDF Available

Have LEGO Products Become More Violent?

PLOS
PLOS ONE
Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Although television, computer games and the Internet play an important role in the lives of children they still also play with physical toys, such as dolls, cars and LEGO bricks. The LEGO company has become the world’s largest toy manufacturer. Our study investigates if the LEGO company’s products have become more violent over time. First, we analyzed the frequency of weapon bricks in LEGO sets. Their use has significantly increased. Second, we empirically investigated the perceived violence in the LEGO product catalogs from the years 1978–2014. Our results show that the violence of the depicted products has increased significantly over time. The LEGO Company’s products are not as innocent as they used to be.
Content may be subject to copyright.
RESEARCH ARTICLE
Have LEGO Products Become More Violent?
Christoph Bartneck
1
*, Qi Min Ser
1
, Elena Moltchanova
2
, James Smithies
3¤
,
Erin Harrington
3
1HIT Lab NZ, University of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand, 2Mathematics and Statistics, University
of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand, 3School of Humanities and Creative Arts, University of
Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand
¤Current address: Kings College London, 22 Kingsway, London WC2B 6LE, United Kingdom
*christoph.bartneck@canterbury.ac.nz
Abstract
Although television, computer games and the Internet play an important role in the lives of
children they still also play with physical toys, such as dolls, cars and LEGO bricks. The
LEGO company has become the worlds largest toy manufacturer. Our study investigates if
the LEGO companys products have become more violent over time. First, we analyzed the
frequency of weapon bricks in LEGO sets. Their use has significantly increased. Second,
we empirically investigated the perceived violence in the LEGO product catalogs from
the years 19782014. Our results show that the violence of the depicted products has
increased significantly over time. The LEGO Companys products are not as innocent as
they used to be.
Introduction
Violence has become an everyday occurrence in television, games and toys, and this is of par-
ticular concern when it comes to media aimed at children. Such violence has increased over
time; for instance, [1] demonstrates that television programmes aimed at children contain
more violence that other forms of programming, and [2] found a statistically significant
increase in the duration of violence in G-rated English language childrensfilms released
between 1937 and 1999.
Effects of violence
Many psychologists have investigated the influence of media on the development of children.
Previous studies have found that violent media and games are linked to negative behaviours
[37], cognition, and affect [5,8]. For instance, it has been demonstrated that frequent expo-
sure to violent content results in desensitization (the elimination of emotional, cognitive, and
behavioural responses to violence) [9], and is also linked to a decrease in empathy [6]. While
this correlation between violence and negative social outcomes could be due to said desensitiza-
tion, it could also be argued that people who are prone to violent behaviours are attracted to
violent media [10]. Conversely, prosocial video games, in which a player must help other char-
acters, have been demonstrated to have positive social outcomes [7].
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401 May 20, 2016 1/27
a11111
OPEN ACCESS
Citation: Bartneck C, Min Ser Q, Moltchanova E,
Smithies J, Harrington E (2016) Have LEGO
Products Become More Violent? PLoS ONE 11(5):
e0155401. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401
Editor: Chris T. Bauch, University of Waterloo,
CANADA
Received: October 26, 2015
Accepted: April 28, 2016
Published: May 20, 2016
Copyright: © 2016 Bartneck et al. This is an open
access article distributed under the terms of the
Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits
unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any
medium, provided the original author and source are
credited.
Data Availability Statement: The data from this
study is available at http://www.bartneck.de/
publications/2016/legoViolence/data.zip and at
Figshare with the DOI: 10.6084/m9.figshare.3207703.
Funding: The authors have no support or funding to
report.
Competing Interests: The authors have declared
that no competing interests exist.
That said, some studies contradict these findings; for instance, a recent study by [11] sug-
gests that there is little to no correlation between violent media and aggressive behaviours.
These contradictary study results could be due to ambiguous definitions or poorly designed
research [11,12]. So, while this is a much-studied area, these discrepancies suggest that we
do not yet have a clear understanding of the effects of violent media content upon childrens
development.
Definition and measurement of violence
Although violence is widespread in contemporary society, researchers struggle to accurately
define or measure it. Many broad definitions exist, such as that of the American Psychological
Association (2015), which defines violence in terms of extreme aggression, citing examples
such as assault, rape or murder [13]. Despite this, they do not provide a detailed coding scheme
on how to quantify the intensity of violent acts.
Several coding schemes are available to assess the violent behaviour of children [14]. Such
measurements are often used to investigate the relationship between exposure to violent media
and aggressive behaviour [15]. Violence ratings for video games and movies, such those sup-
plied by the US-based Entertainment Software Rating Board (ESRB), are usually decided by a
panel of parents, who discuss the content of the material submitted before rating it [16,17]; the
same is the case for the ratings assigned to films in the United States by the Motion Picture
Association of America (MPAA). Although this method avoids the need for a robust violence
coding definition, it is less than useful as a protective mechanism due to increasing levels of
violence in the wider media and entertainment industries, and the potential resultant desensiti-
zation of the raters themselves. For example, the iconic first person shooter Doom, which
was released in 1993 and is considered by many to be the archetype of modern 3D shooting
games, was put on the index by the Federal Department for Media Harmful to Young Persons
in Germany, which meant it could not be advertised, and only sold to adults. However, in 2011
the game and its sequel were removed from the index based on a request from its makers. Cul-
tural values had clearly shifted to the point where the game was no longer considered harmful,
indicating an adjustment in the assessment of video game violence.
Many studies that conduct media and game violence correlational studies use a quantitative
approach by simply counting the number of violent acts performed [18], although some
entirely omit the method used to classify violence, possibly distinguishing violent material
using their gut feelings or by using extreme cases of violence against non-violence [3,4,19].
Content analysis is often used to analyze the violence in television shows. In this widely-
used method, the research defines a list of violent acts that the coders then count during the
television show [20]. Content analysis thereby focuses on the quantity of violent acts, but is
unable to make any statements about the intensity of the violence. While this makes the study
much easier to conduct, it also results in a trade-off in validity. A study conducted by [21]
found that the nature, degree, and amplitude of a particular violent act are just as important
as the number of times it is committed, thus invalidating, or at least rendereding less helpful,
most previous quantitative violence studies.
Toy violence and child development
In an increasingly technological world, many violence studies are conducted in the field of
media and video games. However, even children in the digital age are usually exposed not only
to TV and video games, but to a mixture of digital and non-digital toys such as trains, Barbie
dolls, and LEGO bricks.
Have LEGO Products Become More Violent?
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401 May 20, 2016 2/27
Studies relating to the use of toy guns by children indicate how complicated the issues sur-
rounding violent toys and childhood development are. In a study conducted by [22], it was
found that toy guns correlated with a higher rate of antisocial behaviour in children as com-
pared to nonviolent toys. This result backs up previous research findings by Carlsson-Paige
and Levin, Frazier et al., and Silva (as cited in [8]) that violent toys are detrimental to childrens
healthy development. Pediatricians also advise against buying toys that promote violence for
children as the toys provided by parents represent important values [23]. On the other hand,
toy guns are used in play therapy by professional therapists [24], as it was found that playful
aggression is highly beneficial for child development [25]. One possible reason for the stark
contrast in findings could be the distinct differences between serious aggressive behaviour and
playful aggressive behaviour [25]. The intention to harm is found to be the major factor distin-
guishing serious aggression from playful aggression [25]. Supervision is a crucial component
when allowing playful aggressive behaviour, as reinforcement should be given to ensure devel-
opmentally appropriate play [25].
Although the aforementioned studies assess the effects of toy violence in child development,
there is little research studying how violent the toys themselves have become; that is, how the
toysdesign might connote, encourage or depict violence, aggression or anti-social behaviour.
This study is based on a supposition that media violence might influence the development of
non-digital toys as well as digital toys like video games, resulting in greater amounts of violence
in toys as a whole. By ignoring the complex relationships between digital and non-digital toys,
researchers miss a crucial component in the imaginative lives of children.
This study aims to help fill this gap in the literature of toy violence by analysing LEGO prod-
ucts. The LEGO company has become the worlds largest toy company [26], and it produced
more than 60 billion bricks in 2014 alone. LEGO products are sold in more than 140 countries,
making it one of the most widely available toys in the world. LEGO bricks have even become a
transmedia phenomenon [27] that has, at times, a cult-like status amongst its fans [28]. LEGO
bricks, first patented in 1958, have also enjoyed a continuous production run, making them
one of the few products that have been sold continuously for more than half a century. LEGO
products therefore allow us to analyze the toy market from a historical perspective, comple-
menting other studies including one that suggested the faces of LEGO Minifigures have
become increasingly diverse and aggressive over time [29]. This study addresses gaps left by
previous research in related areas. For instance, Oppel conducted a product analysis of toy
guns and their use from a design perspective [30]. He discovered that an increasing number of
LEGO and Playmobile sets have guns (see page 60) but it is not clear how the weapons were
counted. The study only considered the sets from 19902013, thus limiting its scope.
This study explores a similar set of questions in relation to LEGO sets. The design of the
core LEGO brick itself has remained largely unchanged, but the models released by LEGO,
including their specialist pre-formed bricks and playsets, have changed and developed over
time. Certain sets certainly seem to have increasingly aggressive themes, such as set 44001
Pyrox(see Fig 1), from LEGOs Hero Factory range, which is described as a battle-ready
fire minotaur. Products and play sets like these seem to have become more frequent in recent
years, particularly in comparison to LEGOs products from the 1970s.
Research questions
In light of this anecdotal evidence, we were interested in exactly how violent LEGO products
have become. To answer this question we conducted two studies. First, we investigated the
weapon bricks produced over time. More specifically, we analyzed changes to the proportion
of LEGO bricks that are weapons and the proportion of LEGO sets that contain weapons,
Have LEGO Products Become More Violent?
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401 May 20, 2016 3/27
along with the number of new weapon bricks introduced each year. Second, we investigated
how the perceived violence of LEGO products has changed over time.
We would like to point out that our study does not aim to investigate the effects the depic-
tion (direct, implied, or potential) of violence in LEGO products has on children. Although
such a study would be desirable it presupposes that we have a clear understanding of how vio-
lent LEGO products are in the first place. Our study focuses on this first step: understanding
how violent LEGO products are, and if this has changed over time.
LEGO Weapon Bricks
It is reasonable to assume that the inclusion of weapon bricks, such as swords, guns, and can-
nons in LEGO sets indicates a degree of violence (direct, implied, or potential). That said, there
are two limitations to basing a method on this assumption. First, not every weapon brick in a
set may be used in the model for this purpose. Second, there are bricks that are not designed to
be specifically used as weapons, but that may be used as such in the model. Moreover, multiple
non-weapon bricks can be assembled to represent a weapon. The Star Wars Death Star, for
example, is certainly a weapon, but it consists of many non-weapon bricks. By our definition,
the Death Star itself would not be considered a weapon brick.
We used the LEGO set inventory lists from BrickLink.com as the basis for this analysis.
BrickLink.com is the worlds largest online marketplace for after-market LEGO trading. Their
database of LEGO parts and sets, though imperfect, is the most complete and up-to-date data
source that is publicly available. We used BrickLink.coms part category of Minifig, Weapon
as the definition of a weapon brick. We checked all 155 parts in this category and all of them
were weapon or weapon related. Although imperfect, this seems to be the best approximation
for a definition for a weapon brick. Our definition of weapon brick is likely to underestimate
the available weapons since it does not include all weapons, such as those available in the highly
weaponized Bionicle theme.
Fig 1. LEGO set 44001 Pyrox (Removed from this Manuscript due to licensing restrictions of PLOS
One. Image available at: http://www.bartneck.de/publications/2016/legoViolence/44001.jpg.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401.g001
Have LEGO Products Become More Violent?
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401 May 20, 2016 4/27
Data Processing
The data from BrickLink.com contains the exact inventory of every brick in every set the
LEGO company released since 1949. The data for the year 2015 is still incomplete since not all
sets scheduled for this year have yet been released. It is also important to note that the Brick-
Link.com database records only the release of a set and not the period over which the set was
sold. The Death Star (set 10188) was released in 2008 and is recorded as such in the BrickLink.
com database. However, it has been sold continuously since then and is possibly the longest
selling set so far, given that most sets are not typically sold longer than two years. Another
important consideration is that our analysis does distinguish between shapes and colors of
individual weapon bricks. Although the BrickLink.com database only gives a single identifica-
tion number to a part, we consider a brick in different colors to be a different weapon brick. A
brown tube possibly represents a stick while a grey tube of the exact same design could repre-
sent a metal rod. While an attack with both would be painful, a metal rod could certainly be
interpreted as more dangerous.
Measurements
The number of sets released per year and the number of bricks in every set varies considerably
across time. The number of sets released each year, for example, has increased dramatically. In
the year 1970, 35 different sets were released. This number grew to 419 sets in 2010. It is there-
fore necessary to count not only the number of sets that contain weapon bricks, but to contex-
tualise this number with regards to the total number of sets released in that year.
The same holds true for the raw number of weapon bricks per year, which have to be con-
sidered in proportion to the total number of bricks of that year. The latter is defined as all the
bricks in all the sets of a given year, including duplicates. A certain set might contain three
times the same brick. This will add a count of three to the total number of bricks of that year.
We define the measurements as:
The count of weapons is the count of new weapon bricks designs released per year. This mea-
sure does not take colour variations into account.
The proportion of weaponed sets is the count of all sets that contain at least one weapon brick
in a given year divided by the total number of sets in that year.
The proportion of weapon bricks is the count of all weapon bricks in all sets of a given year
divided by the total count of all bricks in all sets of that year.
Results
A generalized linear model (GLM) from a Poisson family was fitted to analyze the possible
temporal trend in the count of weapons (M1), and a linear model was fitted to analyse the pos-
sible temporal trend in the proportion of weapon bricks (M2) and the proportion of weapon
sets (M3) released in each respective year. Due to the nature of the variable and the need to sat-
isfy the linear model assumptions, the logit transformation was used on the response, and the
interpretation of the trends for the models M2 and M3 are thus in terms of odds rather than
proportions. Year of release was the only covariate in the model. The models were fitted within
a Bayesian paradigm [31] using WinBUGS software [32] and R software [33], including the
package R2WinBUGS [34]. Non-informative Gaussian priors with mean 0 and variance 100
2
were used for the regression coefficients, 10000 iterations were run after a 5000 iterations
Have LEGO Products Become More Violent?
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401 May 20, 2016 5/27
burn-in and the convergence was assessed by visual inspection. The model details and the
trend estimated and credible intervals (CI) are shown in Table 1.
For the weapons count, a strong positive annual trend of an average 10.8% was found (95%
CI: 8.513.5%).
Next we investigated the temporal dynamics of the proportion of weapon bricks and
weapon sets. The two were strongly correlated (r
2
= 0.78) as shown in Fig 2. The odds of a
Table 1. Estimated trends in the count of weapons, proportion of weapon bricks and sets in LEGO by year of release.
Response Variable YModel Likelihood Posterior mean estimate for (exp(b)1)*100% 95% CI
M1 weapon count Y*Poisson(exp(a + bt)) 10.8 (8.5;13.5)
M2 proportion of w. bricks log Y
1YNðaþbt;tÞ
5.7 (3.18.4)
M3 proportion of w.sets log Y
1YNðaþbt;tÞ
7.6 (5.610.1)
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401.t001
Fig 2. Weaponed sets across time.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401.g002
Have LEGO Products Become More Violent?
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401 May 20, 2016 6/27
brick or a set having weapons increased annually by an average of 5.7% (95% CI: 3.18.4%)
and 7.6%(95% CI: 5.610.1%) respectively. However, the dynamic is not monotonic: the pro-
portion of weaponed sets increased steadily for the first twenty years and dropped below 5% in
2001 before climbing back up to nearly 30% in 2014 (Fig 2).
Discussion
The first weapon bricksa sword, a halberd, and a lancewere released in the year 1978. We
will therefore focus our analysis on the years 19782014. Fig 3 shows the count of weapons
across time.
Weapon bricks are largely associated with themed LEGO sets. The first weapon bricks,
described above, were part of the Castle theme. In 1989 the Pirates theme, which included
handguns and cannons, was introduced. This also resulted in a spike in the proportion of
weapon bricks. 1995 showed another spike. This is associated with the introduction of the
Fig 3. Number of weapons over time.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401.g003
Have LEGO Products Become More Violent?
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401 May 20, 2016 7/27
Aquazone themes, which contain harpoons and knives. In the years 2005 and 2006, many new
weapon bricks associated to the Bionicle theme were released.
While the release of the Star Wars theme in 1999 only introduced one new weapon brick,
the light saber, more weapons followed in 2007 in the shape of rifles and blasters. The Star
Wars theme started a trend of licensed LEGO franchise products [35], but this did not appear
to cause many changes in the trends of proportion of weapon bricks or proportion of weap-
oned sets. The increase in violence due to the Star Wars theme might have initially been offset
by the discontinuation of several highly-weaponed themes such as Insectoids, UFO, and Rock
Raiders [35]. The launch of the themes based on the Lord of the Rings films sparked a new
series of weapon bricks in 2012 and the following years.
There has been a significant increase in the proportion of weaponed sets and the proportion
of weapon bricks across time. Today nearly 30% of LEGO sets contain at least one weapon
brick, and this number does not even include weapons that consist of an assembly of non-
weapon bricks.
The year 2001 seems to be a turning point in several respects. There were no new weapon
bricks introduced and the proportion of weaponed sets fell below five percent. This dip could
be related to the financial problems the LEGO Group encountered, which resulted in the
retrenchment of an estimated 1000 employees [36]. These dramatic changes might have led to
a stagnation in the number of new bricks and sets being created. It may have also relatde to
the revision in the companys mission to be more children-oriented [37], which might have
resulted in the release of more children-friendly.
Perceived violence of LEGO products
The LEGO company has released more than 12,000 sets since 1949. It seems unpractical to
attempt to analyze all these sets individually. Instead we decided to use the LEGO product cat-
alog that the company produced once or twice a year since 1950. These catalogs do not only
show most of the LEGO sets, but they also put them into the context of a play scenario (see
Fig 4). The Minifigures and models act out their intended behaviors in these scenes. The
LEGO catalogs are therefore an ideal data set through which to analyze the violence in LEGO
products.
Measurements
We were unable to identify a suitable measurement for assessing the violence in still images.
We therefore adapted the violence coding scheme from the study conducted by [21]. As the
coding scheme was originally designed to rate television violence, we simplified it to fit our
study by removing aspects of the coding scheme that were irrelevant to the rating of still
images. For example, the mode of verbal aggression and the duration of the act were impossible
to code for still images. The section for coding attractiveness of violence was also removed as it
was irrelevant to our study.
The simplified violence coding scheme contained eight questions:
1. mode(s) of physical violence
2. mode(s) of nonverbal psychological aggression
3. atmosphere of violent act(s)
4. realization of violence
5. consequences of violence
Have LEGO Products Become More Violent?
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401 May 20, 2016 8/27
6. temporal distance
7. clarity and vividness of violent act(s)
8. intensity of violence (a summative rating)
The answers for the first three questions were nominal and in the form of check boxes. Par-
ticipants could choose multiple answers. The responses for questions four to eight were ordinal
and in the form of radio buttons where only one answer could be selected. Questions three to
eight only became available if the participants selected at least one type of physical or psycho-
logical violence. The questionnaire also included demographic questions such as gender, age,
and ethnicity.
Setup
We used the Crowdflower platform to recruit and execute our study. Results acquired from
crowdsourcing platforms such as Crowdflower and Amazon Mechanical Turk are similar in
quality to results that are obtained from traditional methods [38].
We conducted a pilot study in which we asked twenty images to be rated by 20 participants.
We then performed a power analysis to estimate how many participants would be required to
detect an increase in violence perception with the probability of at least 80%. We have esti-
mated that at least 15 images per decade with at least three raters per image would be suffi-
cient, and thus we set up the experiment so that each image was rated by at least three
participants.
Participants could rate as many images as they desired but they had to spend at least 20 sec-
onds rating each image. Participants that answered more quickly than this were banned from
further providing any answers. This procedure ensured the quality of the responses obtained.
Fig 4. A page from the 2014 LEGO catalog (Removed from this Manuscript due to licensing
restrictions of PLOS One. Image available at: http://www.bartneck.de/publications/2016/legoViolence/
legoCatalogPage.png.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401.g004
Have LEGO Products Become More Violent?
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401 May 20, 2016 9/27
Stimuli
We were only able to acquire the LEGO catalogs from the year 1973 forward in a sufficient
quality. The catalogs were processed into one image per page. Several pages were excluded
since they contained content that did not show any actual LEGO products, such as advertise-
ments for the LEGOLAND theme parks. In addition, we excluded pages that did not use the
standard LEGO scale, such as those featuring DUPLO or Junior products. The remaining 1576
images were resized proportionally to a maximum width of 768 pixels.
According to Browser Display Statistics from W3Schools (2015) [39], 88.7% of visitors to
their site had a screen resolution of 1280x1024 pixels or higher. Therefore the study was
designed to show participants images with a maximum width of 573 pixels, to ensure that the
majority of participants could see the image in its entirety whilst answering the questions side-
by-side, as seen in Fig 5 below. Participants could view the full-sized image with a width of 768
pixels in a separate window when they clicked on the image. The order of the catalog images
was randomized.
Participants
The participants were recruited using CrowdFlower on 19 May 2015. Of the 161 participants
who took part in the study, 70.2% were males, 25.5% were females, and the rest had missing
gender data. The mean age of the participants was 31.2 with a standard deviation of 9.01 in a
range of 18 to 54 years old. The workers were recruited to participate willingly in the current
study using a variety of channels on CrowdFlower, and were paid a rate of $6 per hour. The
participants in the experiment had the opportunity to enter a contributor satisfaction rating for
our experiment within the Crowdflower system. The average rating was 4.4 our of 5, which
indicates that the contributors felt the amount of workload and pay was reasonable as com-
pared to other jobs available in CrowdFlower.
Fig 5. Screenshot of example task (Removed from this Manuscript due to licensing restrictions of
PLOS One. Image available at: http://www.bartneck.de/publications/2016/legoViolence/task.png.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401.g005
Have LEGO Products Become More Violent?
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401 May 20, 2016 10 / 27
Results
A total of 4728 responses from 161 workers were collected, resulting in the average of 29.3
responses per person. Participants spent an average of 37 seconds on each page. Of those 152
have entered valid age and gender, shown in Table 2, and 148 have entered valid ethnicity data
as follows: 33 Caucasian, 30 Latino/Hispanic, 6 Middle Eastern, 45 South Asian, 18 East Asian
and 16 Other.
Out of 4728 total responses, 359 were deleted due to the overly low trust score (<0.70), as
were a further 11 due to technical problems during the survey. The trust score is a parameter
provided by Crowdflower and is based on the responses participants make to quality control
questions in all the jobs they accept. We excluded participants who have a trust score lower
than 70%.
Responses to each of the survey questions were checked for consistency. If a participant
responded to question two Mode of nonverbal physical aggressionwith both noneand
threatening, intimidation, then this response was deemed inconsistent and omitted from fur-
ther analysis. Note, that the questions 4 to 8 were only answered if the image was perceived as
violent as defined by the participants answer to question 1 or 2.
Since the answer options for within questions 13 were not mutually exclusive, each answer
option was analyzed separately using logistic regression. Questions 48, on the other hand,
had mutually exclusive answer options that increased in intensity and were therefore analyzed
using ordinal cumulative logistic regression with proportional odds [40]. All the models were
adjusted for sex, age group and ethnicity, although none of those factors was found to have had
a significant effect on the findings. Perhaps somewhat surprisingly, people in the youngest age-
group, 1824 were found to perceive violence more often than 2544 year olds, but less often
than 4554 year olds.
Repeated measures were taken into account via random effect terms for individual partici-
pants and individual catalogue pages. The models were fitted within a Bayesian paradigm [31]
using WinBUGS software [32] and R software [33], including the package R2WinBUGS [34].
All the random and fixed effects were given non-informative priors. The technical details of
the ordinal multinomial logistic regression model can be found in the WinBUGS code supplied
in the Appendix B. model A total of 5000 iterations were monitored after a 5000 burn-in. Con-
vergence was confirmed by visual inspection. The technical specifications of the model and the
WinBUGS code use in estimation can be found in Appendices B and C respectively.
The odds of presence of any type of physical violence (question 1) were perceived to increase
significantly at an average rate of 19% per year (95% CI: 1623%); see Fig 6. By 20102015,
about 40% of the catalog images were perceived as containing some kind of violence. The fast-
est growth has occurred in cases of shooting (estimated average 17% per year with 95% CI: 14
21%) and the slowest in cases of hitting with weapons / tools / knockouts (estimated average
7% per year with 95% CI: 69%). No cases of perceived sexual violence were reported.
The odds of the presence of nonverbal psychological aggression (question 2)classified as
forcing, subjection, pressuring (1), threatening, intimidation (2), violating ones human rights
Table 2. Demographics of the LEGO violence perception Survey: Sex and Age.
Age: Male Female
1824 28 9
2534 54 17
3544 18 7
4554 11 8
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401.t002
Have LEGO Products Become More Violent?
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401 May 20, 2016 11 / 27
(3), and irony, scorning gestures(4)were estimated to grow significantly at 11.7%, 16.1%,
14.2% and 8.4% respectively with the 95% CI: 9.014.8%, 12.919.9%, 7.226.9%, 3.515.1%
respectively (see Fig 7).
The odds of presence of violent atmospheres (question 3) perceived as humorous or comic
(1), neutral or unclear (2), and quarrelsome (3) all increased significantly at approximately the
same rates of 7.8% (95% CI: 6.810.8%). The increase in the odds of exciting and frightening
situations was estimated at 11.8% and 13.1% respectively with 95% CI: 11.014.4% and 11.6
17.5% (see Fig 8).
The level of seriousness of the realization of violence (question 4) was perceived to increase
slightly: while in 1980 an estimated 63% of all violence were perceived as playful, as opposed to
Fig 6. Rate of increase in the mode of perceived physical violence (Question 1 of the Survey). The options were as follows:
0 = none, 1 = shooting, 2 = threatening or forcing with guns, 3 = fist-fighting, pushing, striking, 4 = hitting with weapons/tools/knockout,
5 = strangling, 6 = poisoning, 7 = slashing, 8 = sexual violence, 9 = kidnapping/ tying up/ arresting, 10 = damaging property, 11 = other.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401.g006
Have LEGO Products Become More Violent?
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401 May 20, 2016 12 / 27
threatening and trying to kill, in 2015 this proportion came down to 49% (see Fig 9). The
trend, however, was not statistically different from zero.
The seriousness of the portrayed consequences of violence (question 5), on the other hand,
has significantly increased: the odds of harmful consequences as opposed to none at all were
estimated to increase by an average 2.7% per year (95% CI: 1.34.4%) (see Fig 10).
The realism of the violence in terms of its temporal distance (question 6) changed signifi-
cantly over time: the odds of the violent scene being placed further into the future increased by
an average 17% per year (95% CI: 1523%). In 1980 around 10% of the catalog pages were esti-
mated to be set in the future, while by 2015 that proportion climbed to an estimated 90% (see
Fig 11).
Fig 7. Mode of nonverbal psychological aggression (Question 2 of the Survey). The options were as follows: 1 = forcing, subjection,
pressuring 2 = threatening, intimidation 3 = violating ones human rights 4 = irony, scorning gestures 5 = other.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401.g007
Have LEGO Products Become More Violent?
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401 May 20, 2016 13 / 27
The clarity and vividness of the dramatization of violent acts has also increased significantly,
with the odds of falling into a more violent category increasing by an average of 3.0% a year,
95% CI: 0.95.1%), (see Fig 12).
The brutality of the portrayed violence was perceived to have increased significantly: while
in 1980 only an estimated 20% were perceived to be moderately or brutally violent, in 2015 the
corresponding proportion was estimated at 58%, (see Fig 13). Finally, there was also a clear
move towards futuristic scenes (see Fig 11).
Discussion
The perceived violence in LEGO products has increased significantly over the years. The chances
of observing violence in a LEGO catalog pages has increased steadily by 19% per year. Currently,
Fig 8. Dramatization: atmosphere of violent act(s) (Question 3 of the Survey). The options were as follows: 1 = humorous, comic;
2 = neutral or unclear; 3 = quarrelsome; 4 = exciting, adventurous; 5 = frightening, threatening, horrific.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401.g008
Have LEGO Products Become More Violent?
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401 May 20, 2016 14 / 27
around 40% of all pages were coded by participants as indicating some type of violence. In par-
ticular, scenarios involving shooting and threatening behaviour have increased over the years.
The perception of nonverbal psychological aggression increased at a similar rate. The atmo-
sphere of the violent acts is predominately perceived as exciting. The LEGO company often
claimed that their violence normally happens within a humorous context, yet the results show
that humorousis the least likely atmosphere. Material harm is the most frequent consequence
of the violent acts followed by mild harm or injuries.
Conclusions
The results from both studies, weapons count and perceived violence, showed significant expo-
nential increases of violence over time. LEGO products have become significantly more violent.
Fig 9. Seriousness: realization of violence (Question 4 of the Survey). The percentage on the Y-axis refers to the estimated
probability of falling into each category.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401.g009
Have LEGO Products Become More Violent?
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401 May 20, 2016 15 / 27
This increase is not in line with their policy that LEGO products aim to discourage pretend
violence as a primary play incentive. The designs are meant to enrich play with engaging con-
flict scenarios where aggression might be used for the purpose of overcoming imaginary evil
[41]. The violence in LEGO products seems to have gone beyond just enriching game play.
It is unlikely that the LEGO company is the only toy manufacturer whose products have
become increasingly violent; for instance, Oppel has already provided initial evidence that
Playmobile has followed a similar trajectory. Within the spectrum of available products today,
LEGO sets might still be comparatively or relatively harmless. The question remains, though,
why violence has increased so much in general.
According to Smith and Zuiker (as cited in [42]), creators and producers of games and mov-
ies strive to push the limits of what violent media is allowed to be released to prevent their audi-
ence from getting bored of similar content. This creates content that is increasingly creative
Fig 10. Seriousness: the consequences of violence (Question 5 of the Survey). The percentage on the Y-axis refers to the
estimated probability of falling into each category.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401.g010
Have LEGO Products Become More Violent?
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401 May 20, 2016 16 / 27
and violent. To catch the attention of their customers, toy manufacturers are similarly locked
in a metaphorical arms race for exciting new products. In this race they do not only compete
with other toy manufacturers but also with television and video games, which have also become
more violent over the years.
It is unclear how this battle for attention can be moderated. Most countries do have policies
and government agencies in place to protect children from excessively violent toys and media.
Only Afghanistan, of all countries, has recently banned all toy guns in an attempt to curb a
culture of violence [43]. It remains to be seen if they will be able to implement and enforce
this law.
To provide effective protection from the effects of violence, government agencies need
tools to assess the violence of toys and media. As indicateed earlier, most tools available for
evaluating television programs are based on a count of violent acts without paying much
Fig 11. Realism: temporal distance (Question 6 of the Survey). The percentage on the Y-axis refers to the estimated probability of
falling into each category.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401.g011
Have LEGO Products Become More Violent?
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401 May 20, 2016 17 / 27
attention to the nature and intensity of the violent acts. The situation for video games is even
more ambiguous. While both movies and video games are subject to the monitoring of inde-
pendent agencies, the exact method for their assessment remains informal. For this study we
adapted a questionnaire initially used for television for still images. To our knowledge, there is
no other tool available for this purpose. We hope that our questionnaire might be of use to
other researchers when assessing the nature and intensity of violence in still images.
Limitations and future studies
The definition of weapon brickdoes not necessarily include all possible weapons as it does
not cover neutral bricks that could be built into weapons. As LEGO toys are bricks, and the
nature of the toy is for smaller bricks to be built into something larger, studies on LEGO
Fig 12. Dramatization: clarity and vividness of violent act(s) (Question 7 of the Survey). The percentage on the Y-axis refers to the
estimated probability of falling into each category.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401.g012
Have LEGO Products Become More Violent?
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401 May 20, 2016 18 / 27
violence becomes more complex. In most cases of LEGO play, especially in larger weapons like
cannons, slingshots, and Death Stars, many neutral bricks must be assembled into a weapon
before it can be played with. Books such as Forbidden LEGOteach players how to build vio-
lent models using LEGO bricks [44]. With the increase in availability and popularity of such
books, weapons built from neutral bricks would perhaps increase as well. On the other hand,
bricks that were designed to be weapons but were not used as weapons during play were also
classified as a weapon in this study. As there are many ways to view and use a brick, some play-
ers might not see a weapon brick as a weapon, but instead use it as a neutral brick or decora-
tion. For example, though a sword is designed by LEGO to be a weapon, it may be used by
some players as a decoration for a LEGO house. It is impossible to account for the specific
use of bricks, by either adult collectors or children, although it is reasonable to assert that the
images provided with themed sets offer suggested play scenarios.
Fig 13. Intensity of violence (Question 8 of the Survey. The percentage on the Y-axis refers to the estimated probability of falling into
each category.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401.g013
Have LEGO Products Become More Violent?
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401 May 20, 2016 19 / 27
Future studies could also conduct a comparison between different toys. The LEGO products
are certainly not the only toys that have changed over time. Two comparisons could be of par-
ticular interest. First, a comparison to Playmobile, another long standing product line, would
allow for a historical review of the changes in their respective products. Second, a comparison
to other construction brick systems could reveal how the LEGO products developed within this
specific toy market segment. The Megablocks toys, which are completely compatible to the
LEGO brick system, would be a good starting point. Megablocks is currently offering sets that
are linked to violent computer games and film franchises such as Terminator, Call of Duty,
Halo and Assassins Creed. In particular the Call of Duty product line is clearly based on mod-
ern military (see Fig 14), whereas the LEGO company has not yet released a battle tank. Com-
pared to other toys, the LEGO products could still be considered to be relatively mild. Mads
Nipper, the LEGO Groups Senior Vice President in Global Innovation in Marketing even
declared that We will never produce realistic toys for playing war.[41]
There is also a definite difference between perceived violence in images and perceived vio-
lence in the real world [45]. The images from LEGO catalogs may create an impression of com-
edy, which may have resulted in lowered violence ratings. Moreover, intentions are easier to be
measured in reality as compared to images. Future studies could measure the intentions and
violence levels in children playing with LEGO through behaviour observation.
Towards the end of our study we noticed a small inconsistency in the questionnaire. In
question 6 Temporal Distancethere is no option for the 19th century (18011900). Option
five covers the range of 19001950 and option six is earlier than 1801. This inconsistency is
probably based on the misinterpretation of the definition of the 19th century. This inconsis-
tency occurs in the original questionnaire. Only very few sets fall into this category and hence
we believe that it has no major impact on the results. Future studies should describe option six
as prior to 1900to prevent any confusion.
Fig 14. Call of Duty set by Megablocks (Removed from this Manuscript due to licensing restrictions of
PLOS One. Image available at: http://www.bartneck.de/publications/2016/legoViolence/callOfDuty.png.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401.g014
Have LEGO Products Become More Violent?
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401 May 20, 2016 20 / 27
Appendix A: Simplified Coding Scheme for Lego Catalogue
Violence
1. Mode of physical violence (Choose ALL mode(s) of physical violence that can be found):
none
shooting
threatening or forcing with guns
fist-fighting, pushing, striking
hitting with weapons/tools/knockout
strangling
poisoning
slashing
sexual violence
kidnapping/ tying up/ arresting
damaging property
other
2. Mode of nonverbal psychological aggression (Choose ALL mode(s) of psychological vio-
lence that can be found.)
none
forcing, subjection, pressuring
threatening, intimidation
violating ones human rights
irony, scorning gestures
other
3. Dramatization: atmosphere of violent act(s) (Choose ALL atmosphere(s) of violence that
can be found.)
humorous, comic
neutral or unclear
quarrelsome
exciting, adventurous
frightening, threatening, horrific
4. Seriousness: realization of violence (Choose the HIGHEST option that can be found in the
image)
cannot code
playful aggression
Have LEGO Products Become More Violent?
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401 May 20, 2016 21 / 27
threatening/hostile gesturing
trying to insult
insulting
trying to kill
killing/suicide
5. Seriousness: the consequences of violence (Choose the HIGHEST option that can be found
in the image.)
portrayed not at all
no harm
only material harm
mild harm or injuries
moderate injuries (medical care needed)
severe injuries (hospital care needed)
death
6. Realism: temporal distance (Choose the HIGHEST option that can be found in the image.)
cannot code
future
modern (after 90s)
near past (from 50sto90s)
near history (19001950)
earlier than 19th century
7. Dramatization: clarity and vividness of violent act(s) (Choose the HIGHEST option that
can be found in the image).
cannot code
very scant/unclear depiction
quite scant/unclear depiction
moderate depiction
quite detailed and graphic depiction
very detailed, graphic depiction
8. Intensity of violence (Choose the HIGHEST option that can be found in the image).
mild
moderate
brutal
Have LEGO Products Become More Violent?
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401 May 20, 2016 22 / 27
Appendix B: Bayesian ordinal multinomial logistic regression with
random effects
Let Y
i
denote an ordinal categorical response of a person ito a question about set jon a scale
from 1 to K. For example, in Question 8 of our LEGO violence questionnaire, the intensity of
violence could be graded as mild, moderate, or brutal. We then assume that the cumulative
probability of Y
i
taking on a particular value is structured as following
logitðPrðYij kÞÞ¼akþbyearjþXigþZiþzj
for k=1,...,K1 where α
1
<α
2
<...<α
k1
;βis the effect of the year of the set release
year
j
;γis the vector of effects of respondent-specic covariates, such as age, sex and ethnicity,
encoded in matrix X
i
; and η
i
and z
j
are person- and set-specic random effects respectively.
the coefficient βwas assigned a Gaussian prior with mean 0 and variance 10
3
whereas the
elements of the vector gamma were assigned Gaussian priors with means 0 and variances 10
2
.
The priors for the random effects were:
ZiNð0;tZÞ
and
zjNð0;tzÞ
with the precision (inverse variance) parameters τ
η
and τ
z
being assigned Gamma priors
Gamma(0.01, 0.01).
Finally, for the regression intercepts, the following priors were used:
a1Uð0;20Þ
and
ajþ1ajUð0;20Þ
for j=1,...,k2.
Sensitivity analysis confirmed that the priors were non-informative.
Binary logistic regression can be considered within the same framework as a special case of
the above model when K= 2. More about modeling of ordinal categorical data in general and
ordinal multinomial logistic regression in particular may be found in [40].
Appendix C: WinBUGS code for ordinal multinomial logistic
regression with random effects
# ORDINAL LOGISTIC REGRESSION
model;
{
# likelihood using categorical distribution
#Y[i] takes discrete values between 1 and K corresponding to ordi-
nal multinomial categories
for(i in 1:N){
Y[i] *dcat(p [i,1:K])
}
# categorical probabilities obtained from cumulative
probabilities
for(i in 1:N){
Have LEGO Products Become More Violent?
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401 May 20, 2016 23 / 27
p[i,1] <- p.cum [i,1]
p[i,K] <- 1-p.cum [i,K-1]
for(k in 2:(K-1)){ # categories
p[i,k] <- p.cum [i,k]-p.cum [i,k-1]
}}
# cumulative probabilities
for(i in 1:N){
for(k in 1:(K-1)){ # categories
p.cum [i,k] <- 1/(1+exp(-phi [i,k]))
}}
# linear regression
for(i in 1:N){
for(k in 1:(K-1)){
phi [i,k] <-
# intercepts a and annual trend b (primary parameter of interest)
a[k]+bYR [i]+
# adjustment for demography
beta.a.g [A.G [ID [i]]]+beta.sex [SEX [ID [i]]] +beta.ethnic [ETHNIC
[ID [i]]]+
# person- and set-specific random effects
eta [ID [i]]+zeta [SET [i]]
}}
# intercepts a [] should be ordered
for (i in 1:(K-1)) {
da [i] *dunif(0, 20)
}
a[1] <- da [1]
for (i in 2:(K-1)) {
a[i] <- a [i-1] + da [i]
}
# priors
b*dnorm(0,1.0E-3)
beta.a.g [1] <- 0
for(ag in 2:4){
beta.a.g [ag] *dnorm(0,1.0E-2)
}
beta.sex [1] <- 0
beta.sex [2] *dnorm(0,1.0E-2)
beta.ethnic [1] <- 0
for(et in 2:6){
beta.ethnic [et] *dnorm(0,1.0E-2)
}
for(j in 1:J){
eta [j] *dnorm(0,tau.eta)
}
for(js in 1:S){
zeta [js] *dnorm(0,tau.set)
}
tau.eta *dgamma(.01,.01)
Have LEGO Products Become More Violent?
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401 May 20, 2016 24 / 27
tau.set *dgamma(.01,.01)
# priors for missing ETHNIC
for(j in 1:J){
ETHNIC [j] *dcat(p.ethnic [])
A.G [j] *dcat(p.a.g [])
SEX [j] *dcat(p.sex [])
}
p.ethnic [1:6] *ddirch(alpha [1:6])
p.a.g [1:4] *ddirch(alpha1 [1:4])
p.sex [1:2] *ddirch(alpha2 [1:2])
}
Acknowledgments
Disclaimer: LEGO
1
is a trademark of the LEGO Group of companies which does not sponsor,
authorize or endorse this study. This study has been approved by the IRB of the HIT Lab NZ.
Author Contributions
Conceived and designed the experiments: CB QMS JS. Performed the experiments: QMS. Ana-
lyzed the data: CB QMS EM. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: QMS EM. Wrote
the paper: CB QMS EM JS EH.
References
1. Wilson BJ, Smith SL, Potter WJ, Kunkel D, Lintz D, Colvin CM, et al. Violence in Childrens Television
Programming: Assessing the Ricks. Journal of Cummunication. 2002; 52(1):535.
2. Yokota F, Thompson KM. Violence in G-rated animated films. The Journal of the American Medical
Association. 2000 May; 283(20):27162720. doi: 10.1001/jama.283.20.2716 PMID: 10819958
3. Molitor F, Hirsch K. Childrens toleration of real-life aggression after exposure to media violence: A repli-
cation of the Drabman and Thomas studies. Child Study Journal. 1994; 24(3):191207.
4. Drabman RS, Thomas MH. Does media violence increase childrens toleration of real-life aggression?
Developmental Psychology. 1974; 10(3):418421. doi: 10.1037/h0036439
5. Anderson CA, Bushman BJ. Effects of Violent Video Games on Aggressive Behavior, Aggressive Cog-
nition, Aggressive Affect, Physiological Arousal, and Prosocial Behavior: A Meta-Analytic Review of the
Scientific Literature. Psychological Science. 2001; 12(5):353359. Available from: http://pss.sagepub.
com/content/12/5/353. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00366 PMID: 11554666
6. Anderson CA, Shibuya A, Ihori N, Swing EL, Bushman BJ, Sakamoto A, et al. Violent Video Game
Effects on Aggression, Empathy and Prosocial Behaviour in Eastern and Western Countries: A Meta-
Analytic Review. Psychological Bulletin. 2010; 136(2):151173. doi: 10.1037/a0018251 PMID:
20192553
7. Greitemeyer T, Mügge DO. Video Games Do Affect Social Outcomes: A Meta-Analytic Review of the
Effects of Violent and Prosocial Video Game Play. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2014;
40(5):578589. doi: 10.1177/0146167213520459 PMID: 24458215
8. Chang N. Reasoning with Children About Violent Television Shows and Related Toys. Early Childhood
Education Journal. 2000; 28(2):8589. Available from: http://link.springer.com/article/10.1023/A%
3A1009595219927. doi: 10.1023/A:1009595219927
9. Krahe B, Moeller I, Rowell L, Kirwil L, Felber J, Berger A. Desensitization to media violence: Links with
habitual media violence exposure, aggressive cognitions, and aggressive behavior. Journal of Person-
ality and Social Psychology. 2011; 100(4):630646. doi: 10.1037/a0021711 PMID: 21186935
10. Funk JB, Baldacci HB, Pasold T, Baumgardner J. Violence exposure in real-life, video games, televi-
sion, movies, and the internet: is there desensitization? Journal of Adolescence. 2004; 27(1):2339.
Available from: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0140197103000939. doi: 10.1016/j.
adolescence.2003.10.005 PMID: 15013258
11. Ferguson CJ. Does Media Violence Predict Societal Violence? It Depends on What You Look at and
When. Journal of Communication. 2015; 65(1):E1E22. Available from: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/
doi/10.1111/jcom.12129/abstract. doi: 10.1111/jcom.12129
Have LEGO Products Become More Violent?
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401 May 20, 2016 25 / 27
12. Goldstein J. Violent Video Games. Boston: The MIT Press; 2011.
13. American Psychological Association. Violence; 2016. Available from: http://www.apa.org/topics/
violence/.
14. Dahlberg LL, Toal SB, Swahn M, Behrens CB. Measuring Violence-Related Attitudes, Behaviors, and
Influences Among Youths: A Compendium of Assessment Tools. 2nd ed. Atlanta, GA: Centers for
Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Injury Prevention and Control; 2005.
15. Bensley L, Van Eenwyk J. Video games and real-life aggression: review of the literature. Journal of
Adolescent Health. 2001; 29(4):244257. Available from: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/
pii/S1054139X01002397. doi: 10.1016/S1054-139X(01)00239-7 PMID: 11587908
16. Wikipedia. Motion picture rating system; 2015. Available from: https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?
title=Motion_picture_rating_system&oldid=666744431.
17. Wikipedia. Entertainment Software Rating Board; 2015. Page Version ID: 666852163. Available from:
https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Entertainment_Software_Rating_Board&oldid=666852163.
18. Cumberbatch G, Jones I, Lee M. Measuring violence on television. Current Psychology. 1988; 7(1):10
25. Available from: http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/BF02686661. doi: 10.1007/BF02686661
19. Weaver AJ, Jensen JD, Martins N, Hurley RJ, Wilson BJ. Liking Violence and Action: An Examination
of Gender Differences in Childrens Processing of Animated Content. Media Psychology. 2011; 14
(1):4970. doi: 10.1080/15213269.2010.547829
20. Gunter B, Harrison J. Violence on television: an analysis of amount, nature, location, and origin of vio-
lence in British programmes. London: Routledge; 1998.
21. Mustonen A, Pulkkinen L. Television violence: A development of a coding scheme. Journal of Broad-
casting & Electronic Media. 1997; 41(2):168189. Available from: http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/
10.1080/08838159709364399. doi: 10.1080/08838159709364399
22. Turner CW, Goldsmith D. Effects of toy guns and airplanes on childrens antisocial free play behavior.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1976; 21(2):303315. Available from: http://www.
sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/0022096576900448. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(76)90044-8 PMID:
1271008
23. Glassy D, Romano J. Selecting Appropriate Toys for Young Children: The Pediatricians Role. Pediat-
rics. 2003; 111(4):911913. Available from: http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/111/4/911.
PMID: 12671134
24. Laue, CE. Toy Guns in Play Therapy: Controversy and Current Practice [PhD Thesis]. The Chicago
School of Professional Psychology; 2015. Available from: http://gradworks.umi.com/36/88/3688574.
html.
25. Hart JL, Tannock MT. Young Childrens Play Fighting and Use of War Toys. Encyclopedia on Early
Childhood Development. 2013;p. 16. Available from: http://www.child-encyclopedia.com/Pages/PDF/
Hart-TannockANGxp1.pdf.
26. Davidson J. Lego Is Now The Largest Toy Company In The World. Time Inc.; 2014. Available from:
http://time.com/money/3268065/lego-largest-toy-company-mattel/.
27. Wolf M. LEGO Studies: Examining the Building Blocks of a Transmedial Phenomenon. Routledge;
2014.
28. Baichtal J, Meno J. The Cult of LEGO. No Starch Press; 2011.
29. Bartneck, C, Obaid, M, Zawieska, K. Agents with facesWhat can we learn from LEGO Minfigures. In:
1st International Conference on Human-Agent Interaction; 2013. p. III21. Available from: http://hai-
conference.net/ihai2013/proceedings/pdf/III-2-1.pdf.
30. Oppel W. Raeuber Und GendarmWaffen Im Kinderzimmer [Masters]. Bauhaus-Universitaet Wei-
mar; 2013. Available from: http://walter.oppel.net/docs/oppel-masterarbeit-v2-small.pdf.
31. Gelman A, Carlin JB, Stern HS, Dunson DB, Vehtari A, Rubin DB. Bayesian Data Analysis. CRC
Press; 2013.
32. Lunn DJ, Thomas A, Best N, Spiegelhalter D. WinBUGSa Bayesian modelling framework: concepts,
structure, and extensibility. Statistics and Computing. 2000; 10:325337. doi: 10.1023/
A:1008929526011
33. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria; 2014. Avail-
able from: http://www.R-project.org/.
34. Sturtz S, Ligges U, Gelman A. R2WinBUGS: A Package for Running WinBUGS from R. Journal of Sta-
tistical Software. 2005; 12(3):116. Available from: http://www.jstatsoft.org. doi: 10.18637/jss.v012.i03
35. Brickipedia;. Available from: http://lego.wikia.com/wiki/LEGO_Wiki.
36. Herman S, Herman S. A million little bricks: the unofficial illustrated history of the LEGO phenomenon.
New York: Skyhorse Pub.; 2012.
Have LEGO Products Become More Violent?
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401 May 20, 2016 26 / 27
37. Robertson DC, Breen B. Brick by brick: how LEGO rewrote the rules of innovation and conquered the
global toy industry. New York: Crown Business; 2013.
38. Bartneck C, Duenser A, Moltchanova E, Zawieska K. Comparing the similarity of responses received
from experiments conducted in Amazons Mechanical Turk to experiments conducted with traditional
methods. PLOS One. 2015; 10(4):e0121595. Available from: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/article?
id=10.1371/journal.pone.0121595. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0121595 PMID: 25876027
39. W3. Browser Display Statistics;. Available from: http://www.w3schools.com/browsers/browsers_
display.asp.
40. Agresti A. Analysis of Ordinal Categorical Data. Hoboken, New Jersey: Wiley; 2010.
41. The LEGO Group. Fighting Machines: Good vs. Evil in Conflict Play; 2015. Available from: http://
parents.lego.com/en-us/childdevelopment/conflict-play.
42. Weaver AJ. A Meta-Analytical Review of Selective Exposure to and the Enjoyment of Media Violence.
Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media. 2011; 55(2):232250. doi: 10.1080/08838151.2011.
570826
43. The Guardian. Afghanistan bans toy guns to curb culture of violence; 2015. Available from: http://www.
theguardian.com/world/2015/jul/22/afghanistan-bans-toy-guns-after-eid-al-fitr-injuries-to-curb-culture-
of-violence.
44. Pilegaard U, Dooley M. Forbidden LEGO: build the models your parents warned you against! San
Francisco, CA: No Starch Press; 2007. Available from: http://www.loc.gov/catdir/toc/ecip0619/
2006026363.html.
45. Mendelson AL, Papacharissi Z. Reality vs. Fiction How Defined Realness Affects Cognitive & Emo-
tional Responses to Photographs. Visual Communication Quarterly. 2007; 14(4):231243.
Available from: http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/15551390701730224. doi: 10.1080/
15551390701730224
Have LEGO Products Become More Violent?
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0155401 May 20, 2016 27 / 27
... Students also discuss the LEGO Group's open innovation ventures, including LEGO Mindstorms, LEGO Architecture, and LEGO Ideas, all which in various ways involve the company's efforts to solicit concepts for product development from its adult users [11]. In addition, the class analyzes aspects of LEGO fan culture [12,15]; race, identity, and the LEGO minifigure [16,17]; the LEGO Group's evolving stance toward depicting war and violence in its products [18]; and the company's recent initiatives in corporate social responsibility, including commitments to environmental and social sustainability in its partnerships, processes, packaging, and products [19,20]. ...
... Empires, politics and armies were represented within specific societies via such toys (Brown, 1990;Ray, 2006), markets began to generate demand for these toys, and eventually manufacturers provided goods that would appeal to customers regardless of the politics of a specific time (Brown, 1993;Benson, 1998). In this light, how children related to toys from both a cultural and historical standpoint has attracted attention (Jacobson, 1999;Patico, 2011), while one aspect of the literature on toys has focused since the 1960s on violent play among children (Andreas, 1969;Bartneck et al., 2016;Hellendoorn & Harinck, 1997). More recently, the literature has asked whether some toys are appropriate for children or whether they should be banned. ...
Article
Full-text available
This article focuses on toys that are associated with war, weapons and violence (‘conflict toys’), which hold particular salience in parts of the world that have undergone conflicts, such as the Korean peninsula. Using sources ranging from economic reports to defector testimonies and field research in both South and North Korea, the article shows that despite largely different economic and political paths, the two Koreas have developed toy markets that address shared histories. The article suggests that conflict toys can perform an important function in inter-Korean relations: they allow the two Koreas to experience and potentially transcend the status quo. The article further argues that both Koreas can polarise their populations quickly through a controlled propaganda machine in the North, and an omnipresent globalised culture in the South: in both cases, a political message of either conflict or reconciliation could reach many children quickly via toys, and thus be a driver for change for anyone who was willing to use such levers.
Chapter
Full-text available
The study contains the argumentation of the importance of the cooperation between librarian-informational activity and scientific researches. The author defines the informational basis and resources of the digital science and focuses on the importance of the involving libraries in the forming of the scientific data. The synergetic approaches to the cooperation between libraries and digital science are cЕeaЗed. The libЕaЕiaБ ЖЗИdieЖǯ fИБdaАeБЗalЖ Вf Зhe cВgБiЗiЙe cВБceГtion of the scientific communication are developed to contribute to the exchanging of the scientiЖЗЖǯ kБВКledge iБ Зhe elecЗЕВБic eБЙiЕВБАeБЗ. The author analyses the librarian development, which is oriented on the joining the various researches. In addition, the author points out the necessity to train professionals of the new generation for the digital science.
Article
У статті здійснено аналіз проблем використання lego-конструювання та робототехніки на заняттях з дітьми дошкіль- ного віку. Автори доводять, що розвиток здібностей до конструювання активізує психічні процеси дитини, спонукає інтерес до творчого вирішення поставлених завдань, винахідливість та самостійність, ініціативність, самореалізацію, бажання втілити нові й оригінальні ідеї, а отже, сприяє розвитку обдарованості здобувача дошкільної освіти. Метою цього дослідження є висвітлення нових аспектів Lego-освіти дошкільників у різних видах діяльності. У статті здійснено аналіз етапів, методів, прийомів Lego-освіти дітей дошкільного віку в контексті пропедевтич- ної роботи з робототехніки. Заняття з Lego спрямовані на розвиток усіх освітніх сфер дитини (інтелектуальна, мовленнєва, творча тощо). Мовленнєвий розвиток дошкільника на lego-заняттях формує здатність аналізувати завдання та обговорювати результати практичної діяльності (опис дизайну виробу, матеріалів, розповідь про хід дії та побудову плану діяль- ності, побудова логічних послідовних висловлювань у міркуваннях, обґрунтуванні, формулюванні висновків). Вони також спрямовані на усне спілкування із застосуванням спеціальної термінології; використання інтерв’ю для отримання інформації та складання схеми розповіді; застосування мультимедійних технологій для генеруван- ня та презентації ідей. Зміст психолого-педагогічної роботи сприяє розвитку особистості дитини на основі цінностей соціальної культу- ри, що забезпечують оволодіння способами поведінки, творчим та активним відтворенням комунікативного досвіду. Організаційні форми соціального та особистісного розвитку дітей дошкільного віку в умовах організації спіль- ної діяльності з дорослими та іншими дітьми та самостійної вільної діяльності обрано відповідно до проблеми дослідження: сюжетно-рольова гра; творчі ігри; педагогічні ситуації; бесіда; розповідання історій; обговорення ситуації; обговорення дій; спільна діяльність; колективна робота; вправи. Методи, прийоми роботи з Lego також сприяють формуванню фізичної культури дітей дошкільного віку, куль- тури здоров’я, первинних цінностей здоров’я та здорового способу життя відповідно до цілісного підходу до здоров’я людини як єдності її фізичного, психологічного та соціального благополуччя.
Book
Full-text available
With a foreword by award-winning author and space critic Ceridwen Dovey, this edited collection of work by students in Flinders University's Archaeology of Modern Society class looks at how space objects are connected to everyday life.
Chapter
This chapter draws upon theories of gender performativity and the history of dolls, dollhouses, and toy theaters to explore dramatic play—the performative bricolage of playing out narratives with LEGO toys—from early thematic playsets to the ‘for girls’ LEGO Friends product line. Beyond gendering individual play themes, the ideology that emerges in this chapter also genders the toy medium itself, masculinizing construction play and feminizing dramatic play. This ideology thereby bifurcates both the LEGO medium and its play.
Article
It was made Toy of the Century in 2000 and its popularity never wanes, but in this article we examine the Lego and Duplo brick systems and their potential educational, therapeutic, creative and social benefits.
Article
Full-text available
Computer and internet based questionnaires have become a standard tool in Human-Computer Interaction research and other related fields, such as psychology and sociology. Amazon's Mechanical Turk (AMT) service is a new method of recruiting participants and conducting certain types of experiments. This study compares whether participants recruited through AMT give different responses than participants recruited through an online forum or recruited directly on a university campus. Moreover, we compare whether a study conducted within AMT results in different responses compared to a study for which participants are recruited through AMT but which is conducted using an external online questionnaire service. The results of this study show that there is a statistical difference between results obtained from participants recruited through AMT compared to the results from the participant recruited on campus or through online forums. We do, however, argue that this difference is so small that it has no practical consequence. There was no significant difference between running the study within AMT compared to running it with an online questionnaire service. There was no significant difference between results obtained directly from within AMT compared to results obtained in the campus and online forum condition. This may suggest that AMT is a viable and economical option for recruiting participants and for conducting studies as setting up and running a study with AMT generally requires less effort and time compared to other frequently used methods. We discuss our findings as well as limitations of using AMT for empirical studies.
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Emotional facial expressions are essential for agents. The LEGO company developed hundreds of facial ex-pressions for their Minifigures, which are often the centerpiece of LEGO construction. We investigate and present a summary of the development of the facial expression for all LEGO Minifigures that were released between 1975 and 2010. Our findings are based on several statistical tests that are performed on data gathered from an online questionnaire. The results show that the LEGO company started in 1989 to dramatically increase the variety of facial expressions. The two most frequent expressions are happiness and anger and the proportion of happy faces is decreasing over time. Through a k-cluster analysis we identified six types of facial expression: disdain, confidence, concern, fear, happiness, and anger. Our cluster analysis shows that toy design has become a more complex design space in which the imaginary world of play does not only consist of a simple division of good versus evil, but a world in which heroes are scared and villains can have superior smile. In addition we tested if the perception of the face changes when the face is presented in the context of a complete Minifigure. The impression of anger, disgust, sadness and surprise were significantly influenced by the presence of context information. The distinctiveness of the faces was, however, not significantly improved. The variation in skin color did also not change the perception of the Minifigure's emotional expression.
Article
Television is often accused of showing too much violence. However, it is rare that anyone stops to ask what this statement means. Violence on Television provides an objective analysis of the violence on television, how much there is and what form it takes.
Article
This article presents 2 studies of the association of media violence rates with societal violence rates. In the first study, movie violence and homicide rates are examined across the 20th century and into the 21st (1920-2005). Throughout the mid-20th century small-to-moderate correlational relationships can be observed between movie violence and homicide rates in the United States. This trend reversed in the early and latter 20th century, with movie violence rates inversely related to homicide rates. In the second study, videogame violence consumption is examined against youth violence rates in the previous 2 decades. Videogame consumption is associated with a decline in youth violence rates. Results suggest that societal consumption of media violence is not predictive of increased societal violence rates.
Article
This article presents 2 studies of the association of media violence rates with societal violence rates. In the first study, movie violence and homicide rates are examined across the 20th century and into the 21st (1920–2005). Throughout the mid-20th century small-to-moderate correlational relationships can be observed between movie violence and homicide rates in the United States. This trend reversed in the early and latter 20th century, with movie violence rates inversely related to homicide rates. In the second study, videogame violence consumption is examined against youth violence rates in the previous 2 decades. Videogame consumption is associated with a decline in youth violence rates. Results suggest that societal consumption of media violence is not predictive of increased societal violence rates.
Article
Whether video game play affects social behavior is a topic of debate. Many argue that aggression and helping are affected by video game play, whereas this stance is disputed by others. The present research provides a meta-analytical test of the idea that depending on their content, video games do affect social outcomes. Data from 98 independent studies with 36,965 participants revealed that for both violent video games and prosocial video games, there was a significant association with social outcomes. Whereas violent video games increase aggression and aggression-related variables and decrease prosocial outcomes, prosocial video games have the opposite effects. These effects were reliable across experimental, correlational, and longitudinal studies, indicating that video game exposure causally affects social outcomes and that there are both short- and long-term effects.