ArticlePDF Available

Attachment and Risk Taking: Are They Interrelated?

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Attachment theory focuses on the bond that develops between child and caretaker and the consequences that this bond has on the child's future relationships. Adolescents attempt to define their identity by experiencing various risky behaviors. The first aim of the study was whether risk taking behavior differs according to attachment styles. The second was to examine risk taking behavior differences according to gender. The third aim of this study was to examine attachment X gender interaction effect for risk taking behavior. And final was to investigate attachment styles differences according to gender. Data were collected from 218 participants (114 female and 104 male) who are university students. The results of this study showed that attachment styles differentiated by risk taking behavior and males had higher risk taking score than females. It was also found out that there was significant attachment X gender interaction effect for risk taking behavior. And finally, the results showed that attachment styles differentiated according to gender.
Content may be subject to copyright.
AbstractAttachment theory focuses on the bond that
develops between child and caretaker and the consequences that
this bond has on the child’s future relationships. Adolescents
attempt to define their identity by experiencing various risky
behaviors. The first aim of the study was whether risk taking
behavior differs according to attachment styles. The second was
to examine risk taking behavior differences according to gender.
The third aim of this study was to examine attachment X gender
interaction effect for risk taking behavior. And final was to
investigate attachment styles differences according to gender.
Data were collected from 218 participants (114 female and 104
male) who are university students. The results of this study
showed that attachment styles differentiated by risk taking
behavior and males had higher risk taking score than females. It
was also found out that there was significant attachment X gender
interaction effect for risk taking behavior. And finally, the
results showed that attachment styles differentiated according to
gender.
Keywords—Attachment style, risk taking
I. INTRODUCTION
TTACHMENT theory was developed by Bowlby to
explain the process by which a bond develops
between a child and his/her caretaker and the functions
that this bond serves. Bowlby defined attachment as
strong emotional bonds that people develop against
important person for them. The tendency and requirement
of emotional bond establishment represent attachment
system which necessary to people continue their life.
Attachment theory focuses on the bond that develops
between child and caretaker and the consequences that
this bond has on the child’s future relationships [1].
Bowlby pointed out that experiences between caretaker
and child form child’s internal working model. Bowlby
identifies two key features of these working models of
attachment: (a) whether or not the attachment figure is
judged to be the sort of person who in general responds
to call for support and protection; and (b) whether or not
the self is judged to be the sort of person towards
whom anyone, and the attachment figure in particular, is
likely to respond in helpful way. The first concerns the
child’s image of other and the second concerns the child’s
image of the self [1, 2, 3]. Internal working models are
Ümit Morsünbül is with the Institute of Educational Sciences, Ankara
University, Cebeci, 06320, Ankara, Turkey, [E-mail:
morsunbulumit@gmail.com].
consolidated permanently from childhood to adolescence.
The end of adolescence is seen as period that internal
working models are permanent and more resistant to
change. According to Bowlby, adolescences and adults
use internal working model in relationships with people
who important for them [1].
Bartholomew defined four attachment styles by using
internal working model. These are secure, preoccupied,
dismissing and fearful attachment styles. According to this
model, the secure individuals have positive self and
positive others model, the preoccupied individuals have
negative self and positive others model, the dismissing
individuals have positive self and negative others model
and finally the fearful individuals have negative self and
others model. Secure individual is characterized by a
valuing of intimate friendships, the capacity to maintain
close relationships without losing personal autonomy, and
a coherence and thoughtfulness in discussing relationships
and related issues. Dismissing individual is characterized
by a downplaying of the importance of close
relationships, restricted emotionality an emphasis on
independence and self reliance, and lack of clarity or
credibility in discussing relationships. Preoccupied
individual is characterized by an over involvement in
close relationships, a dependence on other people’s
acceptance for a sense of personal well- being, a
tendency to idealize other people, and incoherence and
exaggerated emotionality in discussing relationships.
Fearful individual is characterized by an avoidance of
close relationships because of fear of rejection, a sense of
personal insecurity, and distrust others [2, 3].
According to Erikson the most important and basic
developmental task in adolescence is construction of
identity. In the process of identity construction,
adolescents make a lot of attempts related to life area
[4]. Some adolescents arrive at a clear and integrated
identity, others end up in a state of identity confusion. In
this process, adolescents show a lot of risk taking
behaviors.
The concept of risk taking was described different ways
by different authors. Psychologist have shown great
interest in question of risk and risk-taking. Underpinning
much of this approach lies a number of claims and
beliefs about how risk can be identified and understood
[5]. According to Jack risk taking is a part of normal
transitional behavior during adolescence [6]. In adolescence
risk-taking fulfils a basic developmental psychological
Attachment and Risk Taking: Are They
Interrelated?
Ümit Morsünbül
A
World Academy of Science, Engineering and Technology
Vol:3 2009-07-27
630International Scholarly and Scientific Research & Innovation
International Science Index Vol:3, No:7, 2009 waset.org/Publication/5243
need related to gaining autonomy. Taking risk is a means
of distancing themselves from parents and others and
forming their own identities. This arises from the basic
human need of having mastery and individuality. Pursuing
new risky activities and practices can therefore have
positive and negative outcomes [5]. Trimpop, argues that
risk taking is any consciously or non- consciously
controlled behavior with a perceived uncertainty about its
outcome, and/or about its possible benefits or costs for
the physical, economic or psycho-social well-being of
oneself or others [7]. Consequently, current literature
differentiate risk taking behavior as normative risk taking
behavior (healthy exploratory activity) and non-normative
risk taking behaviors (dangerous and high risk
exploratory). In this study risk taking behavior was taken
in hand as non-normative risk taking behavior.
Risk taking on the stage of adolescence is explained by
different viewpoints. Attachment view point has lead to
great strides in understanding the development of social
behavior, psychopathology and risk taking behavior in
adolescence. According to attachment viewpoint one’s
attachment style affects person’s coping styles and risk
taking behaviors in various situations [8]. Adolescence
period is seen as dramatic changes stage, and this period
is not same for all adolescents. For adolescent who has
secure attachment, these paths appear fairly straight,
smooth and easily traversed; for adolescent who has not
secure attachment these paths are twist, detours and
difficult [9]. The secure individuals think that their lives
are under their control. They are strong against stress
and when they have a problem they communicate with
their family and friends. Unsecure individuals have poor
coping strategies, when they have problem they either
regret it or show risk behavior [2, 5].
In the light of these knowledge, the purpose of this
study is to answer the following questions:
1. Does risk taking behavior differ according to
attachment styles?
2. Does risk taking behavior differ according to
gender?
3. Does risk taking behavior differ according to
attachmentXgender interaction effect?
4. Does attachment styles differ according to gender?
II. METHOD
A. Participants and Procedure
Data were collected from 218 participants who are
university students at Mersin University in Turkey. Of this
participants, 114 participants were female and 104 were
males. Their age range from 17 to 22, and mean age
was 20,15 (SD=1.41).
Data gathering took place during lesson time and
participants filled out the questionnaire nearly within 40
minutes. Participation was voluntary and confidentiality.
B. Measures
Attachment styles
To determine attachment styles RSQ (Relationship Scale
Questionnaire) were administered. It was developed by
Griffin and Bartholomew and adapted to Turkish by
Sümer and Güngör [10, 11]. RSQ consists of 18 items
which show 4 attachment styles. Secure and dismissing
attachment subscales contain 5 items and preoccupied and
fearful attachment subscales contain 4 items. Participants
rated each item on a 7- point scale ranging from “not at
all like me” to “very like me”. The Cronbach’s alpha for
each attachment styles ranged from .47 to .61. Internal
consistency for the attachment styles were relatively
low. It result from each attachment style category
combines two orthogonal dimensions: self model and
others model. Although RSQ has low internal consistency
its construct validity is high [10, 11, 12].
Risk taking Behavior
To determine risk taking behavior (ARTQ) The
Adolescent Risk-Taking Questionnaire were administered. It
was developed by Gullone, et al. and adapted Turkish by
Esen [13, 14]. ARTQ consists of 26 items. ARTQ include
3 subscales; risk taking related to social position (15
items), risk taking related to traffic (6 items) and risk
taking related to substance using 5 items). Participants rated
each item on a 5- point scale ranging from “not at all
like me” to “very much like me” The Cronbach’s alpha
for each subscale ranged from .62 to .84. ARTQ gives
only one score. If one’s score is high it shows high risk
taking behavior; if one’s score low it shows low risk
taking behavior [14].
III. RESULTS
In order to analysis the data Two-Way ANOVA and t
test were conducted. Means and standard deviations which
adolescents took from ARTQ according to attachment
styles and gender were shown in Table 1. Two-Way
ANOVA were conducted to determine whether the means
which adolescent took from ARTQ differentiate by
attachment styles and gender. These results were shown in
Table 2. The results of Two-Way ANOVA revealed that
adolescents’ risk taking means significantly differentiate by
attachment styles [F (3-210)= 81,86, p‹,01] and gender [F
(1-210)= 12,11, p‹,01]. According to attachment styles
fearful ( X=92.22) and preoccupied ( X=73.17) attachment
styles have higher risk taking scores than secure
(X=50.74) and dismissing ( X=54.39) attachment styles.
According to gender, females ( X=61.35) have lower risk
taking scores than males ( X=65.96).
World Academy of Science, Engineering and Technology
Vol:3 2009-07-27
631International Scholarly and Scientific Research & Innovation
International Science Index Vol:3, No:7, 2009 waset.org/Publication/5243
TABLE I
MEANS AND STANDART DEVIATIONS WHICH ADOLESCENTS
TOOK FROM ARTQ ACCORDING TO ATTACHMENT STYLES AND
GENDER
Attachment Gender N M Df
Female 20 44.29 15.58
Male 28 57.19 12.63
Secure
Total 48 50.74 15.86
Female 32 90.62 10.85
Male 12 93.83 16.94
Fearful
Total 44 92.22 12.67
Female 14 72.18 18.91
Male 22 74.14 16.68
Preoccupied
Total 36 73.17 17.34
Female 48 46.50 10.78
Male 42 62.28 16.54
Dismissing
Total 90 54.39 15.14
Female 114 61.35 24.25
Male 104 65.96 19.28
Total
Total 208 63.65 22.09
There is also significant attachmentXgender interaction
effect for risk taking behavior [F (3-S210)= 3.35, p‹,05]. In
order to determine source of the differences attachment x
gender interaction Scheffe test were conducted. Scheffe
test showed that there are significant differences between
secure females and fearful females, between secure
females and fearful males, between secure females and
preoccupied females, between secure females and
preoccupied males; significant differences between secure
males and fearful females, between secure males and fearful
males, between secure males and dismissing females;
significant differences between fearful females and
preoccupied males, between fearful females and dismissing
females, between fearful females and dismissing males;
significant differences between preoccupied females and
dismissing females, between preoccupied females and
dismissing males; significant differences between
preoccupied males and dismissing females, between
preoccupied males and dismissing males. And finally, there
are significant differences between fearful males and
preoccupied males, between fearful males and dismissing
females, between fearful males and dismissing males.
[Secure females ( X=44.29), secure males ( X=57.19),
fearful females
(X=90.62), fearful males ( X=93.83), preoccupied females
(X=72.18), preoccupied males ( X=74.14), dismissing
females ( X=46.50), dismissing males (X=62.28)].
T test was conducted to determine if there were any
significant differences between females and males by
attachment styles. Results were shown in Table 3.
There is significant difference between males
(X=4.16) and females ( X=3.85) in secure attachment
dimension (p‹,05). In fearful attachment dimension females
(X=4.39) got higher scores than males (X=3.81)
(p‹,01). In preoccupied attachment dimension males
(X=4.07) got higher scores than females ( X=3.69)
(p‹,01). And finally dismissing attachment dimension there
is not significant difference between males and females.
IV. DISCUSSION
The present study investigated whether risk taking
behavior differentiates by attachment styles. The result of
this study showed that attachment styles differentiated by
risk taking behavior. These results consistent with
previous studies [15, 16, 17]. According to the results of
this study individuals who have positive self model showed
lower risk taking behavior than individuals who have
negative self model. That is, individual who have fearful
or preoccupied attachment showed higher risk taking
behavior than individual who have dismissing or secure
attachment style. However, dismissing adolescents showed
higher risk taking behavior than secure adolescents and
fearful adolescents showed higher risk taking behavior
than preoccupied adolescents. The secure adolescents view
self and others positively, are comfortable with close
relationships, and feels in control of his or her life. The
dismissing adolescents view self positive but others
negative, are uncomfortable with closeness and intimacy.
They have poor coping strategies and show higher risk
TABLE II
RESULTS OF TWO-WAY ANOVA
Source SS df MS F P
Attachment 50196.28 3 16732.09 81.86 .000*
Gender 2476.10
1 2476.10 12.11 .001*
Attachment
* Gender
2188.765 3 729.588 3.35 .015**
Error 42921 210 204.384
Total 105953 217
* P‹,01 ** p‹,05
World Academy of Science, Engineering and Technology
Vol:3 2009-07-27
632International Scholarly and Scientific Research & Innovation
International Science Index Vol:3, No:7, 2009 waset.org/Publication/5243
taking behavior than secure adolescents. The preoccupied
adolescents have negative self and positive others model,
are dependent, lack self confidence, conform to other’s
wishes. The fearful adolescents have negative self and
others model, avoid relationships because of fear rejection,
are introverted and lacks self confidence. Since fearful
adolescents have both negative self and others model,
they show higher risk taking behavior than preoccupied
adolescents. Similar findings were found in others studies.
TABLE III
MEANS, STANDART DEVIATIONS, AND RESULTS OF
INDEPENDENT T-TEST
Gender N M SD T P
Female 114 3.85 .95 2.35 .019*
Secure
Male 104 4.16 1.003
Female 114 4.39 1.38 3.46 .001**
Fearful
Male 104 3.81 1.03
Female 114 3.69 .99 2.61 .009**
Preoccupied
Male 104 4.07 1.14
Female 114 4.71 1.12 .65 .51 Dismissing
Male 104 4.62 .95
* P‹,05 ** p‹,01
Cooper, Colins and Shaver investigated attachment style
differences in psychological symptomatology, self-concept,
and risky or problem behaviors in a community sample of
Black and White adolescents, 13 to 19 years old. Overall,
secure adolescents were the best-adjusted group, though not
necessarily the least likely to engage in risky behaviors.
Anxious (preoccupied) adolescents were the worst-adjusted
group, reporting the poorest self-concepts and the highest
levels of symptomatology and risk behaviors. In contrast,
avoidant (dismissing) adolescents reported generally high
levels of symptomatology and poor self-concepts but similar
levels of risk behaviors to those found among secures [15] .
Turner at. al. examined the relationships among
sociodemographic characteristics, family process, and the
initiation of health risk behaviors in early adolescence.
Results showed that students who received autonomy
support from parents were less likely to initiate sexual
intercourse. Students who were emotionally detached from
their parents were more likely to fight and use
substances. Those who were emotionally detached tended
to come from families with low levels of cohesion and
acceptance [16]. Consequently, it can be said that positive
self model decreases level of risk taking behavior but
negative self model increases level of risk taking
behavior.
The second aim of this study was investigation of risk
taking behavior differences according to gender. According
to the results of this study there are significant gender
differences in risk taking behavior. Results related to
gender and risk taking, revealed that males show higher
risk taking behavior than females. This result was
consistent with previous studies [18, 19]. When we look
studies related to risk taking and gender, similar results
can be seen. Studies of gender differences in harmful risk
taking and antisocial behavior suggest that male and
female adolescents respond differently to situational
stressors. This situation related to gender role. Girls may
have different ways of externalizing their response to
stress and anxiety in terms of antisocial and risk
behavior. When we look societies, especially collectivist
societies, girls prefer indoor activities but boys prefer
outdoor activities. Outdoor activities include more risk
than indoor activities. Therefore, generally, males may
show higher risk taking behavior than females[20]. The
socialization environment may determine gender
differences according to risk taking behavior. In cultures
characterized by broad socialization, individualism and
independence are promoted, and there is relatively less
restrictiveness on the various dimensions of socialization.
This leads to higher rates of risk taking. Cultures
characterized by narrow socialization individuals consider
obedience and conformity to the standards and
expectations of the community to be paramount and
punish physically or socially any deviation from the
norm. This leads to lower rates of risk taking [18]. In the
cultures characterized by narrow socialization, traditional
gender roles are prevalence. Thus girls attend more
indoor activities than outdoor activities. Turkish society
show more narrow socialization than broad socialization.
In Turkey, traditional gender roles are prevalence. Thus
girls make lower attempts related to life area as
compared with boys on adolescence. In traditional Turkish
culture, boys are supported to be more independent and
free, unlike girls are supported to be dependent[21].
Consequently, girls show lower risk taking behavior than
boys.
The third aim of this study was to examine attachment X
gender interaction effect for risk taking behavior. The
results showed that there was significant attachment X gender
interaction effect for risk taking behavior. According to
these results attachment and gender change adolescent’s
risk taking level together. According to result of
attachment X gender interaction which was very important
fearful females had higher risk taking behavior level than
secure, dismissing and preoccupied males.
The fourth aim of this study was investigation of
attachment styles differences according to gender. In this
World Academy of Science, Engineering and Technology
Vol:3 2009-07-27
633International Scholarly and Scientific Research & Innovation
International Science Index Vol:3, No:7, 2009 waset.org/Publication/5243
study gender differences were found in attachment styles
except from dismissing attachment style. Gender differences
were found previous studies [1, 22, 23]. Brennan, Shaver
and Tobey found differences in dismissing and fearful
attachment styles. In dismissing dimension more males
than females were dismissing and in fearful dimension
more females than males were fearful [23]. Morsünbül also
found similar results in fearful dimension [22].
Consequently, the study showed that, when adolescents
faced with risk taking behavior, their preferences may
depend on attachment style. When specialists try to
decrease rates of risk behavior among adolescent, they
should consider whether modification can be made in
attachment styles.
REFERENCES
[1] J. Bowlby, “Attachment and Loss: Vol. 2 Separation”, New York,
Basic Books, 1973.
[2] K. Bartholomew, “Avoidance of intimacy: An attachment
perspective”, Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 8, 1990
pp. 147-178.
[3] K. Bartholomew, & L. M. Horowitz, “Attachment styles among
young adolescents”, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
61, 1991, pp. .226-244.
[4] E. H. Erikson, “Identity: Youth and Crisis”, New York, Norton, 1968.
[5] A. France, “Towards a sociological understanding of youth and
their risk taking”, Journal of Youth Studies, 3, 2000, pp. 317-331.
[6] M. S. Jack, “Personal fable: a potential explanation for risk taking
behavior in adolescents”, Journal of Pediatric Nurse, 4, 1986, pp.
334-338.
[7] R. M. Trimpop, “The Psychology of Risk Taking Behavior”,
Amsterdam, North-Holland, 1994.
[8] M. S. Howard,.& F. J. Medway, “Adolescents’ attachment and coping
with stress”, Psychology in the Schools, 41, 2004, pp. 391-402.
[9] J. P. Allen, &D. Land, “Attachment in adolescence”, J. Cassidy, P. R.
Shaver (Ed), Handbook of Attachment Theory, Research and Clinical
Applications, New York, Gullford Press, 1999.
[10] D. Griffin, & K. Bartholomew, “Models of the self and other:
Fundamental dimensions underlying measures of adult attachment”,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 1994, pp. 430-
445.
[11] N. Sümer, & D. Güngör, “Yetiúkin ba÷lanma stilleri ölçeklerinin
Türk örneklemi üzerinde psikometrim de÷erlendirmesi ve
kültürlerarası bir karúılaútırma”, Turkish Journal of Psychology, 14
,1999, pp. 71-106.
[12] N. Sümer, “Yetiúkin ba÷lanma ölçeklerinin kategoriler ve boyutlar
düzeyinde karúılaútırılması”, Turkish Journal of Psychology, 21,
2006, pp. 1-22.
[13] E. Gullone, S. Moore, S. Moss, & C. Boyd, “The adolescent risk –
taking questionnaire: Development and psychometric evaluation”,
Journal of Adolescent Research, 15, 2000, pp. 231-251.
[14] B. K. Esen, “Akran düzeyleri ve cinsiyetlerine göre ö÷rencilerin
risk alma davranıúı ve okul baúarısının incelenmesi”, Turkish
Psychological Counseling and Guidance Journal, 2, 2003, pp. 17-26.
[15] M. L. Cooper, N. L. Collins & P. R. Shaver, “Attachment styles,
emotional regulation, and adjustment in adolescence”, Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1998, pp. 1380-1397.
[16] R. A. Turner, C. E. Irwin, J. Tschann & S. G. Millstein, “Autonomy,
relatedness, and the initiation of health risk behaviors in early
adolescence”, Health Psychology, 12, 1993, pp. 200-208.
[17] N. J. Bell, L. F. Forthun & S. Sun, “Attachment, adolescent
competencies, and substance use: Developmental Considerations in
the study risk behavior”. Substance Use & Misuse, 35, 2000, pp.
1177-1206.
[18] J. Arnett & L. B. Jensen, “Cultural bases of risk behavior: Danish
adolescents”, Child Development, 64, 1993, pp. 1842-1855.
[19] J. P. Byrness, D. C. Miller & W. D. Schafer, Gender differences in
risk taking: A meta analysis, Psychological Bulletin, 125, 1999, pp.
367-383.
[20] J. A. Chapman, C. Denholm & C. Wyld, “Gender differences in
adolescent risk taking: Are they diminishing ?”, Youth & Society, 40,
2008, pp. 131-154.
[21] O. Guneri, Z. Sümer & A. Yıldırım, Sources of self-identity among
Turkish adolescents, Adolescence, 34, 1999, pp. 535-546.
[22] Ü. Morsunbul, “ Ergenlikte b÷lanma stillerinin yaú ve cinsiyet
açısından incelenmesi”, XV. National Education Sciences Congress,
Mu÷la University, Mu÷la, 13-15 September, 2006
[23] K. Brennan, P. R. Shaver & A. E. Tobey, Attachment styles, Gender
and Parental problem drinking, Journal of Social and Personal
Relationships, 8, 1991, pp. 451-466.
Ümit Morsünbül is Ph.D student [Educational Psychology], at Ankara
University, Institute of Educational Sciences. He received B.S. in
Psychology, Ankara Universitiy in 2001. He completed his master
education between years 2002-2005. His Ph.D. began 2005 and continues
now, both at Ankara University Institute of Educational Sciences and his
master thesis were about identity status and attachment styles. His
academic interest areas are identity development, attachment, self
construal and risk taking.
World Academy of Science, Engineering and Technology
Vol:3 2009-07-27
634International Scholarly and Scientific Research & Innovation
International Science Index Vol:3, No:7, 2009 waset.org/Publication/5243

Supplementary resource (1)

Data
May 2016
... Similarly, Cooper et al. (1998) explain that adolescents with obsessive attachment styles show the highest risk taking behavior. In another study, Morsünbül (2009) examined the quadruple attachment style and risk-taking behaviors and suggested that attachment styles differ in terms of risk-taking behaviors. Accordingly, he explains that adolescents with a negative self-model (adolescents with fearful and obsessive attachment styles) show higher level of risk-taking behaviors than adolescents with a positive self-model (safe and avoidant attachment styles). ...
... (1998) saplantılı bağlanma biçimine sahip ergenlerin en yüksek düzeyde risk alma davranışı gösterdiklerini açıklamaktadır. Bir başka araştırmada ise Morsünbül (2009) dörtlü bağlanma stili ve risk alma davranışlarını incelemiş ve bağlanma stillerinin risk alma davranışları açısından farklılaştığını öne sürmüştür. Buna göre olumsuz benlik modeline sahip ergenlerin (korkulu ve saplantılı bağlanma stillerine sahip ergenler) olumlu benlik modeline sahip ergenlerden (güvenli ve kaçınan bağlanma stillerine sahip ergenler) daha yüksek düzeyde risk alma davranışları gösterdiğini açıklamaktadır. ...
Article
Bu araştırmanın amacı ergenlerde bağlanma stilleri ve akran baskısının risk alma davranışı üzerindeki etkisinin incelenmesidir. Araştırma betimsel araştırma yöntemlerinden tarama modeli kapsamında yürütülmüştür. Araştırmanın evreni İstanbul’da lisede okuyan öğrencilerdir. Evrendeki öğrencilerden seçkisiz örnekleme yönteminden basit seçkisiz örneklemeyle seçilen 340 öğrenci araştırmanın örneklemini oluşturmaktadır. Bu öğrencilerin 191’i kız ( % 56.2) 149’su erkektir (% 43.8). Araştırmada İlişki Ölçeği, Akran Baskısı Ölçeği ve Risk Alma Davranışı Ölçeği kullanılmıştır. Araştırmada bağlanma stilleri, akran baskısı ve risk alma davranışları puanları arasındaki ilişkiyi ölçmek için Pearson Momentler Çarpımı Korelasyon katsayısı kullanılmıştır. Daha sonra aralarında ilişki bulunan değişkenlere Hiyerarşik Regresyon analizi uygulanmıştır. Yapılan analiz sonucunda ilk adımda akran baskısı, risk alma davranışının tek başına %16’sını açıklarken ikinci adımda bağlanma stillerinden güvenli bağlanma stili %3’ünü ve üçüncü adımda bağlanma stillerinden saplantılı bağlanma stili ise %2’sini açıklamaktadır. Araştırma bulguları doğrultusunda risk alma davranışını akran baskısı değişkeninin diğer değişkenlere göre daha fazla açıkladığı görülmektedir.
... Un studiu similar realizat de către Jingyi Lu et al. [13] a concluzionat același lucru: persoanele cu atașamente nesigure își asumă mai multe riscuri în diverse domenii de viață, decât cele cu atașamente sigure. O altă cercetare realizată în rândul adolescenților, în care a fost evaluată asumarea riscului legat de poziția socială, trafic și folosirea substanțelor, arată că indivizii care au model de sine pozitiv (securizant și evitant) au demonstrat comportament de asumare a riscului mai mic decât persoanele care au model de sine negativ (anxios și dezorganizat) [14]. ...
Article
Attachment theory provides a powerful framework for understanding individual differences that affect interpersonal relationships, as well as a range of outcomes related to risky behaviors in various life domains. Existing research on the relationship between attachment styles and risk-taking is contradictory and confusing. On the one hand, there is research and the common belief that individuals with secure attachments view the world as a safe place, hence we expect these individuals to be more able to explore the world and take risks, and on the other hand, there is a body of research that highlights that individuals with insecure attachments are more likely to take on risky behaviours. The present article aims to bring more conceptual clarity in understanding and interpreting the conflicting findings about individuals’ ability to take risks based on attachment style.
... Risk-taking behaviour can be described as any deliberately or unconsciously managed behaviour with a perceived lack of control over its outcome, as well as its potential advantages or disadvantages for one's own or others' physical, financial or psychosocial well-being (Trimpop, 1994). Past studies have investigated the links between risk-taking behaviour and gender (Bergman & Scott, 2001;Michael & Ben-Zur, 2007;Morsünbül, 2009), personality (Alohali et al., 2018;McGhee et al., 2012;Nicholson et al., 2005;Skeel et al., 2007), age (Escobedo, 1997;Trimpop et al., 1998) and psychopathology (Hunt vd., 2005;Ruchkin et al., 2006;Wade et al., 2022). Nicholson et al. (2005) studied the effect of personality factors on risk-taking behaviour and reported that high extraversion (especially sensation-seeking) and openness supply the motivational force for risk-taking (Nicholson et al., 2005). ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective: In the related literature; the effects of emotion on behaviour have been commonly investigated. The main purpose of this research is to examine the influence of different emotions on risk-taking behaviour. Methods: Risk-taking behaviour have been investigated in three different conditions: happiness, fear and neutral emotion. The participants were manipulated by viewing photos of inducing happiness, fear or neutral emotion in the form of slides. The Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) was used to measure the participants' risk-taking tendencies. Results: In this study, a statistically significant difference was found in money earned from yellow and orange balloons depending on emotion. There was no statistically significant difference in pumps, explosions, and money earned from blue and total balloons among emotions. Conclusion: In line with the related literature, participants tend to collect more money in fear conditions. In addition, the fact that the money collected in happiness and fear conditions is more than neutral, reveals the effects of the emotions on risk-taking behavior.
... In the risk-taking dimension, it is stated in many studies that boys show more risk-taking behavior than girls (GÜLGEZ; KISAÇ, 2014;MORSÜNBÜL, 2013;ULUDAĞLI;SAYIL 2009;MORSÜNBÜL, 2009;GÜNDOĞDU et al., 2005;ROLISON;SCHERMAN, 2003;BYRENESS;SCHAFER, 1999;PARSONS et al., 1997;ARNETT;JENSEN, 1993). ...
Article
Full-text available
The aim of this research is to determine the attitudes of university students who receive sports education towards playing games containing physical activity. The group of the research consists of 405 (183 Male, 222 Female) students studying at the university. The results show that the motivation level of the research group to play games containing physical activity is high, and the mean score of women is higher than men, that the groups with higher education levels had a more positive approach to games in terms of educational status, and that there were different sub-dimensions in terms of the number of siblings. In terms of parental education level, the desire for playfulness in the children of parents who have undergraduate and graduate education was found to be higher. It has been concluded that people living in the village and big city have a higher level of enjoyment from the game compared to those living in the district.
... Bununla birlikte kendisini onunlayken daha güvende hissetmektedir (15). Bedensel, bilişsel ve toplumsal olarak ani değişimlerin yaşandığı ergenlik döneminde zihinsel becerilerin de yükselmesiyle birlikte kişi kendisi ve çevresinde bulunan kişilerle ilgili problemlerini araştırma dönemine geçmektedir ve kimliğinin oluşma hızı da artmaktadır (16). ...
... Based on the information obtained from children and adolescents with mental damage, Bowlby formed the foundations of his theory. His basic premise is the existence of a common need for a close emotional bond (attachment) in all humans (Çelik, 2004;Morsünbül, 2009). Bowlby and Ainsworth believed that baby, as a result of frequent in-teractions with mother, has gradually begun to predict his mother's behavior. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective: In recent decades, researchers have designed various tools for assessment and study of the interpersonal relationships from an attachment perspective. The purpose of this study was to evaluate the psychometric properties of the relationship scales questionnaire (RSQ), which is one of these tools. Methods: A total of 368 female undergraduate students of Ferdowsi University of Mashhad (academic year 2012-13) were selected by quota sampling method from all colleges of the university and were tested by RSQ. Internal consistency method was used to check the reliability of the questionnaire and exploratory factor analysis to assess its construct validity by SPSS 19. Results: The exploratory factorial analysis of RSQ showed 2 different patterns. The Collins and Read 3-factor model showed 40.16% of the total variance, and Simpson 2-factor model determined 35.36% of the total variance. The Cronbach α coefficients were 0.67 for 2-factor model, and 0.57 for 3-factor model. Conclusion: Both analyzed models showed a relatively average validity for the Iranian version of this questionnaire. Moreover, the 3-factor model had a higher acceptable validity.
... More specific to adolescents, connectedness in the form of bonding and emotional attachment to family or a partner were protective factors for adolescent sexual and reproductive health outcomes (Markham et al., 2010). Conversely, TA B L E 2 Adjusted associations among demographic, clinical and psychosocial factors and unintended pregnancy (n = 309) those without a secure emotional attachment or perception of belonging may take sexual risks that fill a perceived void as a means to compensate for social exclusion (Morsünbül, 2009). ...
Article
Almost half of all pregnancies (45%) in the United States (US) are unintended, with the highest concentration in women with low incomes. Targeted research is warranted to identify risk and protective factors that influence pregnancy intention to improve maternal/child health. Purpose To identify individual and interpersonal level associations to pregnancy intention to use as leverage points to build resilience. Method A cross‐sectional, secondary analysis of Medicaid eligible pregnant women in Kentucky (n = 309). Results Sixty‐two percent reported their current pregnancy was unintended. Older age, partnered, negative drug screen, and increased social support were associated with decreased odds of unintended pregnancy. For every 1 unit increase of belonging and tangible social support, women were 13% and 14% (respectively) less likely to have an unintended pregnancy (OR = 0.87, 95% CI = 0.78–0.97, p = .011, OR = 0.86, 95% CI = 0.77–0.95, p = .005). A positive drug screen was associated with an almost three‐fold increase in the odds of unintended pregnancy (OR = 2.88, 95% CI = 1.49–5.58, p = .002). Conclusion: Public health nurses can play a critical role in reducing unintended pregnancy rates by promoting social support, inclusion, and acceptance. There remains a critical need to identify barriers and facilitators to pregnancy planning for persons who use illicit drugs.
Article
Full-text available
Unintended pregnancy (UP) can negatively impact the health of mothers, children, and families. While Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACEs) are increasingly known to affect sexual health, the influence on pregnancy intention is not fully understood. This study examines the relationship between ACEs and UP and explores other related factors, using 5049 pregnant and postpartum women data from the Japan COVID-19 and Society Internet Survey (JACSIS). We measured participants’ pregnancy intentions, ACEs, family functioning, and social network size. Logistic regression analysis provided odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals (CI). The prevalence of UP was approximately 16.5% (n = 893). Cumulative ACEs were consistently associated with UP, even after adjusting for intermediate variables in adulthood. The odds ratio for UP with a single ACE was 1.00 (CI: 0.82–1.21) but rose significantly with multiple ACEs: 1.39 (CI: 1.10–1.76) with double, 1.38 (CI: 1.02–2.86) with triple, and 1.81 (CI: 1.37–2.39) with more. Additionally, bad family functioning and lack of social networks emerged as contributors to UP. In conclusion, this study showed that ACEs are potentially correlated with UP. A deeper understanding of the transition from childhood experiences to UP is important for health interventions, necessitating further investigation.
Article
Young adults are overrepresented in crash statistics compared to mature drivers. Research has addressed this concern by investigating factors related to risky driving; however, studies investigating altruistic driving are scarce. Altruistic driving, conceived as driving in a way that benefits others without expectation of benefit for oneself, may lead to less risky driving and reduction in crashes. The current study investigated psychological attachment, altruistic personality traits and Theory of Planned Behaviour (TPB) constructs as predictors of altruistic driving behaviours. The sample consisted of 17–25-year-old licensed drivers, living in Queensland, Australia, who had completed at least three quarters of mandated supervised driving practice with their mother or father as supervisor. Participants completed two linked online surveys. Survey 1 (N = 93) assessed sociodemographic factors, TPB variables, previous behavioural performance, attachment, altruism and intentions for altruistic driving. Survey 2, occurring two weeks later (n = 23), measured opportunity for and engagement in altruistic driving. The results of hierarchical multiple regression analyses supported the predictive value of the TPB constructs in explaining behavioural intention in general altruistic driving and two specific altruistic driving scenarios. However, altruism and attachment were not significant predictors of intention in any scenario. Bivariate correlations found that neither intentions or PBC (from Survey 1) were significantly associated with altruistic driving behaviours (from Survey 2) in any scenario. This research increases knowledge about motivators of altruistic driving by young drivers. This insight may be used to inform road safety interventions to motivate more altruistic driving, decrease aggression on the roads, and reduce young driver involvement in crashes.
Article
Full-text available
The authors conducted a meta-analysis of 150 studies in which the risk-taking tendencies of male and female participants were compared. Studies were coded with respect to type of task (e.g., self-reported behaviors vs. observed behaviors), task content (e.g., smoking vs. sex), and 5 age levels. Results showed that the average effects for 14 out of 16 types of risk taking were significantly larger than 0 (indicating greater risk taking in male participants) and that nearly half of the effects were greater than .20. However, certain topics (e.g., intellectual risk taking and physical skills) produced larger gender differences than others (e.g., smoking). In addition, the authors found that (a) there were significant shifts in the size of the gender gap between successive age levels, and (b) the gender gap seems to be growing smaller over time. The discussion focuses on the meaning of the results for theories of risk taking and the need for additional studies to clarify age trends.
Article
Full-text available
Bartholomew's (1990) four-category typology of adult attachment styles was compared with Hazan & Shaver's (1987) three-category typology in terms of three substantive issues. First, the same two dimensions were found to underlie both typologies, and the Bartholomew and Hazan & Shaver measures corresponded as predicted. Second, there were no gender differences on Hazan & Shaver's measure, in line with previous studies, but there were gender differences on Bartholomew's measure, especially in her two avoidant categories. More males than females were dismissing avoidants; more females than males were fearful avoidants. Third, a hypothesis advanced by Latty-Mann & Davis (1988) was confirmed. Adult children of alcoholics scored high on both avoidant and anxious-ambivalent scales of Hazan & Shaver's measure, and fell predominantly into Bartholomew's fearful-avoidant category, suggesting that at least some fearful adults are grown-up versions of the `disorganized, disoriented' children identified by Crittenden (1988) and by Main & Solomon (1990). These children are more common in families troubled by parental alcoholism, depression or abuse.
Article
Full-text available
A basic principle of attachment theory is that early attachment relationships with caregivers provide the prototype for later social relations. Working within an attachment framework, a new 4-group model of characteristic attachment styles in adulthood is proposed. In particular, two forms of adult avoidance of intimacy are differentiated: a fearful style that is characterized by a conscious desire for social contact which is inhibited by fears of its consequences, and a dismissing style that is characterized by a defensive denial of the need or desire for greater social contact. This distinction corresponds to two differing models of the self: people who fearfully avoid intimacy view themselves as undeserving of the love and support of others, and people who dismiss intimacy possess a positive model of the self that minimizes the subjective awareness of distress or social needs. The emotional and interpersonal ramifications of the two proposed styles of adult avoidance are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Compared to other life periods, adolescence is characterized by a heightened potential for risky behaviors. This study reports the systematic development and psychometric evaluation of a comprehensive Adolescent Risk-Taking Questionnaire (ARQ). It was developed using reports of 570 adolescents and was psychometrically evaluated with a sample of 925 adolescents between 11 and 18 years of age. Principal components analyses yielded a four-factor risk structure, and these factors were substantiated via a confirmatory factor analysis. One week test-retest and internal consistency indices were demonstrated to be sound. Age and gender differences were found to be consistent with reported trends in accident data. Older adolescents and boys reported lower risk perceptions and a higher frequency of risky behaviors than younger adolescents and girls, respectively, supporting the validity of the ARQ. Furthermore, consistent with past research, perceiving higher levels of risk typically related to lower levels of engaging in the respective behaviors.
Article
Three studies assessed the construct validity of the self- and other-model dimensions underlying the 4-category model of adult attachment (Bartholomew, 1990). Five methods were used to assess the hypothesized dimensions: self-reports, friend-reports, romantic partner reports, trained judges' ratings of peer attachment, and trained judges' ratings of family attachment. In each study, the convergent and discriminant validity of the dimensions were assessed by multitrait-multimethod matrices and by confirmatory factor analysis. Study 2 related the latent attachment dimensions to theoretically relevant outcome latent variables. As predicted, individuals' self models converged with direct measures of the positivity of their self-concepts, and individuals' other models converged with direct measures of the positivity of their interpersonal orientations. Study 3 related the latent attachment dimensions to 3 alternate self-report measures of adult attachment and showed that the 2 dimensions serve as an organizing framework for the different measurement approaches.