ArticlePDF Available

Population recovery highlights spatial organization dynamics in adult leopards

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Polygynous species follow sex-specific spacing patterns to maximize reproductive success, and changes in population density under otherwise stable environmental conditions likely provoke sex-specific responses in spacing patterns. A classical dual reproductive strategy hypothesis posits that female home range size and overlap are set by habitat productivity and remain stable under increasing population density, whereas male home range size and overlap decrease with increased mate competition. An alternative dispersal-regulated strategy predicts that females relinquish part of their home range to philopatric daughters and form matrilineal clusters, while adult male spacing is stable with density-dependent subadult male emigration rates. We used 11 years of telemetry data to assess the response of adult leopard Panthera pardus spacing following the release of harvest pressure. Female annual home ranges and core areas were smaller than in males. Intersexual overlap was larger than intra-sexual overlap in males or in females. As leopard density increased, female home range size and inter-annual fidelity in home range use decreased, and females formed matrilineal kin clusters. In contrast, male leopards maintained large home ranges, and did not track female home range contraction. Spacing dynamics in adult leopards was consistent with dispersal-regulated strategies, and did not support a classical dual reproductive strategy. Our study suggests possible hidden lag effects of harvest disturbance on spacing dynamics that are not necessarily apparent when only assessing demographic recovery of harvested populations.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Population recovery highlights spatial organization
dynamics in adult leopards
J. Fattebert
1,2
, G. A. Balme
1,3
, H. S. Robinson
1,4
, T. Dickerson
1
, R. Slotow
2,5
& L. T. B. Hunter
1,2
1 Panthera, New York, NY, USA
2 School of Life Sciences, University of KwaZulu-Natal, Durban, South Africa
3 Department of Biological Sciences, University of Cape Town, Cape Town, South Africa
4 College of Forestry and Conservation, University of Montana, Missoula, MT, USA
5 Department of Genetics, Evolution and Environment, University College, London, UK
Keywords
density-dependence; dual reproductive strategy;
home range; Panthera pardus; South Africa.
Correspondence
Julien Fattebert, School of Life Sciences,
University of KwaZulu-Natal, Durban,
South Africa
Email: julien.fattebert@gmail.com
Editor: Matthew Hayward
Received 9 November 2015; revised 2 March
2016; accepted 2 March 2016
doi:10.1111/jzo.12344
Abstract
Polygynous species follow sex-specic spacing patterns to maximize reproductive
success, and changes in population density under otherwise stable environmental
conditions likely provoke sex-specic responses in spacing patterns. A classical
dual reproductive strategy hypothesis posits that female home range size and over-
lap are set by habitat productivity and remain stable under increasing population
density, whereas male home range size and overlap decrease with increased mate
competition. An alternative dispersal-regulated strategy predicts that females relin-
quish part of their home range to philopatric daughters and form matrilineal clus-
ters, while adult male spacing is stable with density-dependent subadult male
emigration rates. We used 11 years of telemetry data to assess the response of
adult leopard Panthera pardus spacing following the release of harvest pressure.
Female annual home ranges and core areas were smaller than in males. Intersexual
overlap was larger than intra-sexual overlap in males or in females. As leopard
density increased, female home range size and inter-annual delity in home range
use decreased, and females formed matrilineal kin clusters. In contrast, male leop-
ards maintained large home ranges, and did not track female home range contrac-
tion. Spacing dynamics in adult leopards was consistent with dispersal-regulated
strategies, and did not support a classical dual reproductive strategy. Our study
suggests possible hidden lag effects of harvest disturbance on spacing dynamics
that are not necessarily apparent when only assessing demographic recovery of har-
vested populations.
Introduction
Patterns of animal social and spatial organization (hereafter:
spacing) are assumed to reect individual attempts to maxi-
mize tness while accounting for the distribution of limiting
resources and competing conspecics (Mitchell & Powell,
2012). Broad-scale intra-specic variations in spacing often fol-
low gradients of habitat productivity and resource availability
across a speciesrange (McLoughlin, Ferguson & Messier,
2000; Marker & Dickman, 2005; Nilsen, Herndal & Linnell,
2005; Hayward et al., 2009). Population density can also inu-
ence spacing patterns (Dahle & Swenson, 2003), and high
levels of harvest have been shown to affect spacing patterns in
carnivores (Woodroffe et al., 2006; Davidson et al., 2011). In
polygynous species, each sex follows a different spacing strat-
egy to maximize reproductive success (dual reproductive strat-
egy: Clutton-Brock & Harvey, 1978; Sandell, 1989), and
changes in population density likely provokes sex-specic
responses on spacing (Maletzke et al., 2014).
As large carnivore population densities are driven largely by
prey availability (Carbone & Gittleman, 2002; Hayward,
OBrien & Kerley, 2007), it remains challenging to disentangle
the effects of resource availability and population density on
spacing (Benson, Chamberlain & Leopold, 2006). Experimental
manipulations of carnivore populations to enable rigorous
hypothesis testing are limited by signicant logistical and ethi-
cal constraints, but the study of harvested populations can give
insight into the effect of population density on a dual strategy
of spacing (Maletzke et al., 2014; Keehner et al., 2015). Clas-
sically, the dual reproductive strategy predicts that female
space-use patterns follow a foraging optimality rule of area
minimization, and that females achieve higher reproductive
success and maximize tness by using resources within famil-
iar, stable home ranges (Sandell, 1989). Therefore, when a
population is reduced below carrying capacity under otherwise
stable environmental conditions, female spacing should remain
stable when per capita resource availability is stable or
increased (Logan & Sweanor, 2001). In contrast, males
Journal of Zoology  (2016)  ª2016 The Zoological Society of London 1
Journal of Zoology. Print ISSN 0952-8369
maximize reproductive success by securing access to several
females, and mate competition constrains male space-use (San-
dell, 1989). When voids are created by the removal of male
conspecics through harvest, mate competition predicts an
increase in male home range size and intra-sexual overlap
(Logan & Sweanor, 2001).
Alternatively, spacing dynamics might be regulated by disper-
sal and philopatry. In polygynous mammals, natal dispersal is
generally male-biased and females tend to be philopatric (Green-
wood, 1980). Under a dispersal-regulated alternative to the classi-
cal dual strategy, females are predicted to relinquish part of their
home range to philopatric daughters at independence, and overlap
more with related females that can benet from local knowledge
of resources, and inclusive tness (Anderson, 1989; Lambin,
Aars & Piertney, 2001). In contrast, mate competition predicts
density-dependent subadult male emigration rates (Fattebert
et al., 2015a) with stable adult male spacing.
Understanding how animals adjust their spacing to changes in
population density that can impact their mating system has impli-
cations for how quickly populations may rebound from stochastic
events, or heavy harvest. Among solitary felids, sex-specic spac-
ing response to population decline following the classical dual
strategy has been documented in cougars Puma concolor ( Logan
& Sweanor, 2001; Maletzke et al., 2014). Conversely, in other
populations where harvest pressure has been reduced or an
expanding population is monitored, spacing dynamics have been
less predictable and more variable (bobcat Lynx rufus: Benson
et al., 2006; tiger Panthera tigris: Goodrich et al., 2010; Eura-
sian lynx Lynx lynx: Pesenti & Zimmermann, 2013). To address
this research gap, we assessed the two alternatives to the dual
strategy hypothesis in a recovering leopard Panthera pardus pop-
ulation with increasing density following release from harvest
(Balme, Slotow & Hunter, 2009).
Leopards are polygynous, solitary large carnivores with den-
sities and spacing related to prey densities at the inter-popula-
tion level (Marker & Dickman, 2005). Generally, subadult
female leopards are philopatric, and subadult males disperse
(Fattebert et al., 2013, 2015a). In this study, we measured
adult female and male leopards home range size, inter-indivi-
dual overlap, and delity of home range use over contrasting
population densities during 11 years. As the population density
increased, we predicted under a classical dual reproductive
strategy (1) female home range size and overlap to be set by
habitat productivity, and therefore to remain stable under stable
environmental conditions; (2) male home range size and over-
lap to decrease due to increased mate competition. Alterna-
tively, under a dispersal-regulated strategy, we predicted (3)
female home range size to decrease, and intra-sexual overlap
to increase while forming matrilineal kin clusters; and (4) male
spacing to remain stable (Table 1).
Materials and methods
Study area and study population
We studied leopard spatial organization in Phinda Private
Game Reserve (hereafter Phinda; 234 km
2
) in northern Kwa-
Zulu-Natal, South Africa (27°33-27°55
0S; 32°06032°260E)
Table 1 Predictions and corresponding expected observations for the two competing dual reproductive strategy hypotheses for home range size and overlap patterns in adult female male
and male leopards in Phinda Game Reserve, 20022012
Sex Hypothesis Predictions Expected observations
Female Classical dual
reproductive
strategy
Home range size and overlap are limited by resource availability.
Home range size and overlap are not density-dependent
No change in home range size and overlap
under stable prey availability and increasing
population density
Dispersal-regulated
strategy
Female philopatry leads to the formation of matrilineal kin clusters.
Adult female relinquish portion of their home range to philopatric
female progeny
Decrease in home range size and larger overlap
among related female conspecifics under
stable prey availability and increasing population density
Male Classical dual
reproductive
strategy
Home range size and overlap are limited by social interactions and
mate competition. Home range size and overlap are density
dependent under stable resource availability
Decrease in home range size and overlap under
increasing population density and stable prey availability
Dispersal-regulated
strategy
Mate competition drives male density-dependent male dispersal.
Home range size and overlap are not density-dependent
No change in home range size and overlap under
stable prey availability and increasing population density
2Journal of Zoology  (2016)  ª2016 The Zoological Society of London
Leopard spatial organization dynamics J. Fattebert et al.
from 2002 to 2012. Climate is subtropical with hot, humid
summers (OctoberMarch), and warm, dry winters (April
September). Mean monthly temperatures range from 33°C (Jan-
uary) to 19°C (July). Annual rainfall ranges from 550 to
1150 mm, and occurs mainly in summer (Fattebert et al.,
2013). Although strictly protected in Phinda, leopards move
freely across boundary fences and face greater mortality risk in
surrounding non-protected areas, from a combination of trophy
hunting, problem animal control, and illegal killing for skins
(Balme et al., 2009).
Based on the population demographic parameters documented
elsewhere (Balme et al., 2009; Balme, Slotow & Hunter, 2010),
we dened three separate temporal periods: (1) a Disturbance
period (20022004), when the population was in decline (annual
population growth rate k=0.978) due to high levels of mortality
[annual mortality rate (AMR) =0.401]; (2) a Recovery period
(20052008) following the implementation of sustainable harvest
protocols along with other conservation interventions in 2005,
when AMR declined to 0.134, annual population growth rate
increased (k=1.136), and leopard density increased from
7.2 1.1 to 11.2 2.1 individuals/100 km
2
; and (3) a Stabi-
lization period (20092012), when the population density pla-
teaued at putative carrying capacity (Figure S1). Populations of
the three main leopard prey species in this system (nyala Tragela-
phus angasii, impala Aepyceros melampus and warthog Phaco-
choerus africanus: Balme, Hunter & Slotow, 2007) were stable
over the study period (Figure S1). Overall competitor populations
(lions Panthera leo and spotted hyena Crocutta crocutta) were
also stable (Balme et al., 2010), and the leopard demographic
recovery between 20052008 was attributed to the decline of
human-caused mortality (Balme et al., 2009). During the course
of the study, all radio-tracked subadult females remained philopa-
tric, while emigration rate and natal dispersal distance in subadult
males increased with population density (Fattebert et al., 2015a).
Capture and tracking
We captured leopards in Phinda with a combination of cage-
trapping, free-darting, and soft-hold foot-snaring as described
by Balme et al. (2007). We estimated the age of leopards
using morphological cues such as teeth wear and yellowing,
head and body size, weight, facial scarring, tearing of the ears,
nose pigmentation, and development of a dewlap (Stander,
1997; Balme, Hunter & Braczkowski, 2012). We classied
leopards >3 years old as adults. Depending on the accessibility
for radiotracking within or outside the protected area, we tted
adult leopards with VHF (250 g, Sirtrack Ltd., New Havelock
North, Zealand, 0.5% of adult female body mass) or GPS col-
lars (420 g, Vectronic-Aerospace, Berlin, Germany, 1.2% of
adult female body mass).
We located VHF-collared individuals to approximately
100 m using ground homing or triangulation on average every
3 days. We programmed GPS collars to acquire 26xes
daily. Mean x acquisition success rate was 86.3 2.8%
(n=17 collars), and we further ltered GPS data for poten-
tially large locational errors removing 3D xes with Positional
Dilution of Precision (PDOP) >15 and 2D xes with
PDOP >5 (Lewis et al., 2007). Ezemvelo KwaZulu-Natal
Wildlife provincial authority granted permit for this research
(permit HO/4004/07), and the Animal Ethics Subcommittee of
the University of KwaZulu-Natal Ethics Committee approved
animal handling procedures (approval 051/12/Animal). All
transmitters were retrieved at the end of the study.
Home range size
We used telemetry relocations to compute seasonal (dry season:
AprilSeptember; wet season: OctoberMarch), and annual
(AprilMarch) xed kernel utilization distributions (UD; Ker-
nohan, Gitzen & Millspaugh, 2001). We restricted our analysis to
adult individuals with 50 relocations available in a given year
(Kernohan et al., 2001). We calculated smoothing factor band-
width h using the solve-the-equation plug-inmethod (Gitzen,
Millspaugh & Kernohan, 2006) using the Geospatial Modeling
Environment program (Beyer, 2012). We dened home ranges
and core areas using the 90 and 50% UD isopleths, respectively.
We used annual UDs for all subsequent analyses, as a paired t-test
showed no signicant difference in seasonal home range size
(Table S1). Vegetation greenness is an important driver of leop-
ard habitat use (Fattebert et al., 2015b), and we extracted the
mean annual Enhanced Vegetation Index (EVI; 537 m resolution
raster; WAMIS 2015) to each home range and core area as a
proxy for prey density at the individual range level. Vegetation
indices are commonly used to quantify vegetative productivity
(Duncan et al., 2015), and it is often assumed that a relationship
exists between productivity and prey density (Nilsen et al.,
2005). For comparison with the literature, we also present 95%
and 50% Minimum Convex Polygon (MCP; Hayne, 1949) annual
range size (Table S2).
Home range overlap
To assess the inter-individual overlap between concurrently
tracked individuals, we computed the three-dimensional utiliza-
tion distribution overlap index (UDOI; Fieberg & Kochanny,
2005) using the R package adehabitatHR (Calenge, 2006):
UDOI ¼Ai;jZ1
1 Z1
1 d
UDiðx;yÞd
UDjðx;yÞdxdy (1)
where d
UDiðx;yÞis the value of the UD of the animal iat the
point x, y. UDOI yields a single value per interaction, ranging
from 0 (no overlap) to 1 when two uniformly distributed UDs
completely overlap, and takes values >1 when the two UDs
overlap extensively but are not uniformly distributed (Fieberg
& Kochanny, 2005). To assess delity of home range use in
individual leopards tracked during consecutive years, we calcu-
lated intra-individual UDOI. For comparison with the literature,
we present the two-dimensional home range overlap indices
(Tables S3, S4).
Statistical analyses
To test for the effect of covariates on home range, and core
area size and overlap, we tted generalized linear mixed-effect
models (Bolker et al., 2009) with a Gaussian distribution using
Journal of Zoology  (2016)  ª2016 The Zoological Society of London 3
J. Fattebert et al. Leopard spatial organization dynamics
the R package lme4 (Bates, Maechler & Bolker, 2013).To
account for leopards tracked for more than 1 year, we tted
the identity of the individuals (range size) or the interacting
dyad (overlap) as a random intercept. To assess the general
spacing patterns of the leopard population, we rst tested for
sexual differences in home range and core area sizes, and for
differences in sex-specic overlap.
To test the classical and the dispersal-regulated dual strategy
hypotheses, we used an information theoretic approach. For
each sex, we explored the effect of study period (population
density) and EVI (habitat productivity) on home range size,
core area size, or overlap index, or their additive effect in a
full model:
Spatial measurement b0þb1study period þb2EVI þð1jIDÞ
(2)
Through direct observation of telemetered adult females with
cubs, we dened mother and daughters, or female siblings of
same or successive litters as related, and other females as of
unknown relatedness. To assess the existence of matrilineal kin
clusters, we included relatedness as a factor (relatedvs. un-
known) in the model set for femalefemale UDOI, adding one
parameter to the full model:
female-female UDOI b0þb1study period þb2EVI
þb3relatedness þð1jIDÞ(3)
For each spatial measurement, we used the Akaikes infor-
mation criterion corrected for small sample size (AICc) to
select for the most parsimonious model, including a null model
with no explanatory variable (Burnham & Anderson, 2002). If
top-ranking candidate models were within ΔAICc <2, we per-
formed model averaging to estimate unbiased coefcients of
the parameters using the R package MuMIn (Barton, 2013). To
identify informative parameters in nal models, coefcients
were deemed signicant when corresponding 90% condence
interval (CI) did not include 0 (Arnold, 2010). We ran all sta-
tistical analyses in R version 3.0.0 (R Core Team 2013). We
report mean standard error (SE) throughout unless stated
otherwise.
Results
We used telemetry data from 10 adult female leopards tracked
over 19 years, and 11 adult males tracked over 14 years in
Phinda between 2002 and 2012. We obtained 42 annual female
ranges (393 71 relocations over 9.3 0.5 months; Table S5)
and 25 annual male ranges (509 90 relocations over 6.8
0.7 months). Overall, female home ranges (29.5 1.6 km
2
)
and core areas (7.3 0.5 km
2
) were signicantly smaller than
male home ranges (74.0 7.5 km
2
;b
females
=42.6; 90% CI
60.1, 25.1) and core areas (20.2 2.5 km
2
;b
females
=
12.1; 90% CI 18.2, 6.1).
Inter-individual UDOI was larger in femalemale
(0.172 0.027, n=56) than in femalefemale (0.060
0.018, n=31; b
female-female
=0.113; 90% CI 0.181, 0.046),
or in malemale overlap (0.045 0.015, n=11; b
male-male
=
0.123; 90% CI 0.216, 0.031). Both sexes showed high
levels of annual home range use delity, and there was no sig-
nicant difference in intra-individual UDOI between females
(0.763 0.049, n=31) and males (0.686 0.074, n=13;
b
females
=0.131; 90% CI 0.048, 0.310).
Female spacing dynamics
In the Stabilization period, average female home range size
(23.3 2.1 km
2
,n=14) was smaller than during the Distur-
bance (33.7 3.7 km
2
,n=9; b
disturbance
=10.5; 90% CI 3.9,
17.0) and Recovery periods (32.1 2.2 km
2
,n=19;
b
recovery
=8.9; 90% CI 3.7, 14.1; Fig. 1) with no effect of
EVI (Table 2). Effect of study period on female core area size
was not informative (b
disturbance
=0.7; 90% CI 1.3, 2.7;
b
recovery
=0.6; 90% CI 1.1, 2.2; Fig. 1, Table 2). Inter-indi-
vidual femalefemale overlap was higher among related
females (0.133 0.089, n=5) than among females of
unknown relatedness (0.046 0.013, n=26; b
related
=0.088;
90% CI 0.010, 0.165), with no effect of study period or EVI
(Fig. 2, Table 3). Neither study period nor EVI affected inter-
individual femalemale overlap (Table 3). Intra-individual
female UDOI in the Disturbance period (0.982 0.035,
n=9) was higher than during the Recovery period
(0.645 0.086, n=14; b
recovery
=0.273; 90% CI 0.466,
0.084), and the Stabilization periods (0.722 0.060, n=8;
b
stabilization
=0.206; 90% CI 0.428, 0.017; Fig. 3), the latter
being not informative, with no effect of EVI (Table 4).
Male spacing dynamics
There was no effect of study period (population density) on
any of the spatial measurement in males (Figs 1-3, Tables 2-
4), although inter-individual male overlap seemed to decrease
over time (Fig. 2). There was no informative effect of EVI on
male home range size (b
EVI
=0.04; 90% CI 0.002, 0.008;
Table 2). Male core area increased with EVI (b
EVI
=0.01;
90% CI 0.0009, 0.02; Table 2). Inter-individual malemale
overlap decreased with EVI (b
EVI
=0.00008; 90% CI
0.0001, 0.00006; Table 3). There was no informative effect
of EVI on intra-individual male UDOI (b
EVI
=0.0006; 90%
CI 0.0010, 0.00005; Table 4).
Discussion
Adult female and male leopards in Phinda showed different
spacing dynamics in response to changes in population density.
As harvest declined and leopard density increased, females
contracted home range size and appeared to form matrilineal
kin clusters. This was reected by greater overlap among
related females compared to unrelated females, and decreasing
inter-annual delity in home range use as females relinquished
a portion of their home range to philopatric daughters. In con-
trast, male leopards did not track the contraction of female
home ranges. Males maintained large home ranges, and
overlapped less among each other. The observed dynamics in
spacing were consistent with dispersal-regulated sex-specic
4Journal of Zoology  (2016)  ª2016 The Zoological Society of London
Leopard spatial organization dynamics J. Fattebert et al.
strategies, and did not support a classical dual reproductive
strategy.
The overall spacing of adult leopards in Phinda was typical
of polygynous, solitary carnivores in which larger male home
ranges overlapped with several smaller female home ranges to
increase mating opportunities (Sandell, 1989). We also found
limited intra-sexual overlap among adult leopards of both sexes
under all levels of anthropogenic disturbance, indicative of
mutual avoidance and territoriality in both males and females
(Marker & Dickman, 2005). In both sexes, lack of seasonal
difference in home range size, and high delity in inter-annual
home range use reected the value of local knowledge and
defense of resources (resident tness hypothesis: Anderson,
1989).
Over time, the observed changes in leopard population den-
sity revealed subtle sex-specic dynamics in spacing of adult
individuals. As population density increased, female home
range size decreased independent of habitat productivity. When
given the opportunity, female leopards seem to use larger
home ranges than required for purely energetic needs (Grigione
et al., 2002; Jetz et al., 2004), to then apportion some of this
space to their female progeny (Fattebert et al., 2015a). On the
surface, this appears to contradict the rule of area minimization
(Grigione et al., 2002) and stable space-use patterns under a
stable environment (Maletzke et al., 2014). However, female
core area size did not change over the course of the study,
consistent with territoriality and exclusive use of resources at
the core area level (Benson et al., 2006).
We link such female spatial behavior to obligate female
philopatry documented in this leopard population over the
course of the study (Fattebert et al., 2015a). In a perturbed
system, lling the voids created by the removal of conspecics
is a potentially protable tactic for females to secure sufcient
space to later relinquish to philopatric daughters (Goodrich
et al., 2010). This led to the formation of matrilineal kin clusters,
Figure 1 Empirical mean (SE) annual home range (95% kernel) and core area (50% kernel) size of adult female and male leopards in Phinda
GR, 20022012. Temporal segments of the study were defined based on demographic parameters and estimated population density:
Disturbance (20022004), Recovery (20052008), and Stabilization (20092012).
Table 2 Selection of mixed-effect regression models exploring the
effect of study period and vegetative productivity (EVI) on home
range and core area size in adult female and male leopards in Phinda
Game Reserve, South Africa, 20022012. Temporal periods of the
study were defined based on demographic parameters and estimated
population density: Disturbance (20022004), Recovery (20052008),
and Stabilization (20092012)
Model Model parameters AICc ΔAICc w
Female home range Period 313.05 0 0.69
Period +EVI 315.67 2.62 0.19
null 317.00 3.94 0.10
EVI 319.07 6.01 0.03
Male home range null 250.15 0 0.53
EVI 250.76 0.61 0.39
Period 254.14 3.99 0.07
Period +EVI 257.04 6.90 0.02
Female core area null 217.41 0 0.52
Period 218.80 1.39 0.26
EVI 219.85 2.44 0.15
Period +EVI 221.50 4.09 0.07
Male core area EVI 186.18 0 0.49
null 186.32 0.14 0.46
Period 191.39 5.20 0.04
Period +EVI 192.79 6.61 0.02
AICc, Akaike Information Criteria adjusted for small sample sizes;
ΔAICc, (AICc) (AICc)
min
; w, Akaike weight.
Journal of Zoology  (2016)  ª2016 The Zoological Society of London 5
J. Fattebert et al. Leopard spatial organization dynamics
as evidenced by higher overlap among related adult females
compared to females with unknown relatedness. Philopatric
individuals should benet from local knowledge of resources,
in accordance with resident tness (Anderson, 1989). Females
are more likely to tolerate each other and benet from inclu-
sive tness, and should face the costs of dispersal only when
the costs of resource competition in the natal range are higher
(Lambin et al., 2001). Female dispersal might therefore only
occur under long-lasting stability of both elevated population
density, and socio-spatial organization (Fattebert et al., 2015a).
Spatial tactics of female leopards in Phinda were congruent
with the mounting evidence that matrilines spatially structure
the female segment of the population in other polygynous
mammals (brown bear Ursus arctos: Støen et al., 2005; tiger:
Goodrich et al., 2010; wild boar Sus scrofa: Podg
orski, Scan-
dura & Je
zdrzejewska, 2014).
Male leopards in Phinda did not follow the contraction of
female home ranges over time, as would be expected if male
home range size had been set by female availability (Grigione
et al., 2002). Under increased population density, males
appeared to maintain large home ranges and decrease inter-
individual overlap, although not signicantly, in order to maxi-
mize mating opportunities, while limiting access of other males
to these same females (Clutton-Brock & Harvey, 1978). It
would be easier for males to gain exclusive access to fewer
females in smaller home ranges, as large home ranges are
energetically costly to maintain (Jetz et al., 2004). However,
males did not retain exclusive access to all females, overlap-
ping only partially with some female home ranges (Fig. 2,
Table S3). Although complete overlap could ensure exclusive
access, it would yield mating opportunities with fewer female
individuals (Logan & Sweanor, 2001). Such partial inter-sexual
overlap is congruent with the promiscuous mating behavior of
female leopards to possibly confuse paternity as a strategy to
reduce the risk of infanticide (Balme & Hunter, 2013). Male
spacing stability also paralleled elevated subadult male emigra-
tion probability, and longer dispersal distances reported else-
where (Fattebert et al., 2015a).
Adult leopards followed a dual reproductive strategy, with
females and males showing different spatial dynamics under
increasing population density, and generally stable resource
availability. While the male segment of this leopard population
was the most impacted demographically by high human-
mediated mortality (Balme et al., 2009), female spacing
Table 3 Selection of mixed-effect regression models exploring the effect of study period and vegetative productivity (EVI) on adult leopard inter-
individual utilization distribution overlap index (UDOI) in Phinda Game Reserve, South Africa, 20022012. Temporal segments of the study were
defined based on demographic parameters and estimated population density: Disturbance (20022004), Recovery (20052008), and Stabilization
(20092012). For female intra-sexual overlap, the effect of individuals’ relatedness was also tested
Model Model parameters AICc ΔAICc w
FemaleFemale UDOI Relatedness
a
50.09 0 0.43
null 49.19 0.9 0.27
EVI +Relatedness 47.56 2.53 0.12
EVI 46.55 3.54 0.07
Period +Relatedness 45.89 4.2 0.05
Period +EVI +Relatedness 44.11 5.98 0.02
Period 43.94 6.15 0.02
Period +EVI 40.86 9.23 0.00
FemaleMale UDOI null 16.33 0 0.68
EVI 14.02 2.30 0.22
Period 12.07 4.25 0.08
Period +EVI 9.62 6.71 0.02
MaleMale UDOI EVI 37.49 0 0.93
null 32.28 5.31 0.07
Period 20.79 16.70 0.00
Period +EVI 19.48 18.01 0.00
a
Mothers and adult daughters or female siblings were defined as related, otherwise unrelated.
AICc, Akaike Information Criteria adjusted for small sample sizes; ΔAICc, (AICc) (AICc)
min
; w, Akaike weight.
Table 4 Selection of mixed-effect regression models exploring the
effect of study period and vegetative productivity (EVI) on intra-
individual utilization distribution overlap index (UDOI) in adult female
and male leopards in Phinda Game Reserve, South Africa, 2002
2012. Temporal segments of the study were defined based on
demographic parameters and estimated population density:
Disturbance (20022004), Recovery (20052008), and Stabilization
(20092012)
Model Model parameters AICc ΔAICc w
Female UDOI Period 4.73 0 0.55
null 6.82 2.10 0.19
EVI 7.43 2.70 0.14
Period +EVI 7.79 3.07 0.12
Male UDOI null 10.02 0 0.70
EVI 11.76 1.74 0.29
Period 18.96 8.94 0.01
Period +EVI 24.62 14.6 0.00
AICc, Akaike Information Criteria adjusted for small sample sizes;
ΔAICc, (AICc) (AICc)
min
; w, Akaike weight.
6Journal of Zoology  (2016)  ª2016 The Zoological Society of London
Leopard spatial organization dynamics J. Fattebert et al.
dynamically adjusted to population density following release
from harvest. These patterns do not support the classical pre-
dictions in response to changes in population density observed
in cougars where spacing patterns are stable in females, and
density-dependent in males (Logan & Sweanor, 2001; Malet-
zke et al., 2014). Spacing dynamics in this leopard population
Figure 2 Empirical mean (SE) inter-individual utilization distribution overlap index (UDOI) between adult female and male leopards in Phinda
GR, 20022012. Temporal segments of the study were defined based on demographic parameters and estimated population density:
Disturbance (20022004), Recovery (20052008), and Stabilization (20092012). FF, femalefemale; FM, femalemale; MM, malemale. Note
that during the Disturbance period only one overlapping male dyad was recorded.
Figure 3 Empirical mean (SE) intra-individual utilization distribution overlap index (UDOI) as a measure of fidelity of home range use in adult
female and male leopards in Phinda GR, 20022012. Temporal segments of the study were defined based on demographic parameters and
estimated population density: Disturbance (20022004), Recovery (20052008), and Stabilization (20092012).
Journal of Zoology  (2016)  ª2016 The Zoological Society of London 7
J. Fattebert et al. Leopard spatial organization dynamics
appeared to be regulated by dispersal patterns, that is resident
tness through philopatry in females, and mate competition
and emigration in males (Fattebert et al., 2015a).
Alternatively, an increase in population density led to a reduc-
tion in home range size in both males and females in bobcats
(Benson et al., 2006), and Eurasian lynx (Pesenti & Zimmer-
mann, 2013). The observed lack of change in male leopard home
range size might indicate that the male segment has not yet
reached a threshold of inverse density-dependence. This could
highlight possible lag in resilience of spatial patterns, as docu-
mented in a Eurasian lynx population, where social organization
pattern returned to the pre-disturbance after a delay of 3 years
(Breitenmoser-W
ursten et al., 2007). These various responses to
disturbance and changes in population density by solitary felids
suggests complexity of resilience mechanisms, and caution
should be exercised in extrapolating results across species, and
even across populations of the same species.
We demonstrated that release of anthropogenic harvest pres-
sure had different consequences on the socio-spatial organiza-
tion of female and male leopards. While male spacing
remained stable, female spacing continually decreased for
7 years following release from harvest. Despite the potential
for rapid numerical response to improved protection (Balme
et al., 2009), demographic perturbations in species like leop-
ards appears to have long-lasting effects on socio-spatial pat-
terns. Our study suggests possible hidden lag effects of harvest
disturbance that likely affect the broader population dynamics
beyond locally impacted populations (Fattebert et al., 2015a;
Pitman et al., 2015). Caution should therefore be exercised
when assessing impact of conservation measures as such lag
effects are not apparent when only assessing demographic
recovery.
Acknowledgements
The study was funded by Panthera, Albert and Didy Hartog,
the Stichting Timbo Foundation, the National Research Foun-
dation (FA 2004050400038 to GB), and the University of
KwaZulu-Natal Gay Langmuir Bursary (to JF). We thank
&Beyond, EKZNW and the neighboring land owners for
allowing us to conduct research on their reserves and are grate-
ful to everyone that assisted with eldwork, particularly V.
Mitchell, K. Pretorius, J. Mattheus and S. Naylor. Suggestions
from Lourens Swanepoel and Craig Tambling helped improve
the paper signicantly.
References
Anderson, P.K. (1989). Dispersal in rodents: a resident tness
hypothesis. Utah: American Society of Mammalogists Provo.
Arnold, T.W. (2010). Uninformative parameters and model
selection using Akaikes information criterion. J. Wildl. Mgmt.
74, 11751178.
Balme, G.A. & Hunter, L.T.B. (2013). Why leopards commit
infanticide. Anim. Behav. 86, 791799.
Balme, G., Hunter, L. & Slotow, R. (2007). Feeding habitat
selection by hunting leopards Panthera pardus in a woodland
savanna: prey catchability versus abundance. Anim. Behav. 74,
589598.
Balme, G., Slotow, R. & Hunter, L.T.B. (2009). Impact of
conservation interventions on the dynamics and persistence of
a persecuted leopard (Panthera pardus) population. Biol.
Conserv. 142, 26812690.
Balme, G.A., Slotow, R. & Hunter, L.T.B. (2010). Edge effects
and the impact of non-protected areas in carnivore
conservation: leopards in the Phinda-Mkhuze Complex, South
Africa. Anim. Conserv. 13, 315323.
Balme, G.A., Hunter, L. & Braczkowski, A.R. (2012).
Applicability of age-based hunting regulations for African
leopards. PLoS ONE 7, e35209.
Barton, K. (2013) MuMIn: multi-model inference. R package
version 1.9.5. Available at: https://cran.rproject.org/web/pack
ages/MuMIn.
Bates, D., Maechler, M. & Bolker, B. (2013) lme4: Linear
mixed-effects models using S4 classes. R package version
0.999999-2. Available at: https://cran.rproject.org/web/pack
ages/lme4.
Benson, J.F., Chamberlain, M.J. & Leopold, B.D. (2006).
Regulation of space use in a solitary felid: population density
or prey availability? Anim. Behav. 71, 685693.
Beyer, H.L. (2012) Geospatial modelling environment (Version
0.7.2.0). Available at: http://www.spatialecology.com/gme/.
Bolker, B.M., Brooks, M.E., Clark, C.J., Geange, S.W., Poulsen,
J.R., Stevens, M.H.H. & White, J.-S.S. (2009). Generalized
linear mixed models: a practical guide for ecology and
evolution. Trends Ecol. Evol. 24, 127135.
Breitenmoser-W
ursten, C., Zimmermann, F., Stahl, P., Vandel,
J.-M., Molinari-Jobin, A., Molinari, P., Capt, S. &
Breitenmoser, U. (2007). Spatial and social stability of a
Eurasian lynx Lynx lynx population: an assessment of 10 years
of observation in the Jura Mountains. Wildl. Biol. 13, 365
380.
Burnham, K.P. & Anderson, D.R. (2002). Model selection and
multi-model inference. A Practical Information-Theoretic
Approach. New York: Springer.
Calenge, C. (2006). The package adehabitat for the R software:
a tool for the analysis of space and habitat use by animals.
Ecol. Modell. 197, 516519.
Carbone, C. & Gittleman, J.L. (2002). A common rule for the
scaling of carnivore density. Science 295, 22732276.
Clutton-Brock, T.H. & Harvey, P.H. (1978). Mammals,
resources and reproductive strategies. Nature 273, 191195.
Dahle, B. & Swenson, J.E. (2003). Home ranges in adult
Scandinavian brown bears (Ursus arctos): effect of mass, sex,
reproductive category, population density and habitat type. J.
Zool. (Lond.) 260, 329335.
Davidson, Z., Valeix, M., Loveridge, A.J., Madzikanda, H. &
Macdonald, D.W. (2011). Socio-spatial behaviour of an
African lion population following perturbation by sport
hunting. Biol. Conserv. 144, 114121.
Duncan, C., Nilsen, E.B., Linnell, J.D.C. & Pettorelli, N. (2015).
Life-history attributes and resource dynamics determine
8Journal of Zoology  (2016)  ª2016 The Zoological Society of London
Leopard spatial organization dynamics J. Fattebert et al.
intraspecic home-range sizes in Carnivora. Remote Sens.
Ecol. Cons. 1,3950.
Fattebert, J., Dickerson, T., Balme, G., Slotow, R. & Hunter, L.
(2013). Long-distance natal dispersal in leopard reveals
potential for a three-country metapopulation. S. Afr. J. Wildl.
Res. 43,6167.
Fattebert, J., Balme, G., Dickerson, T., Slotow, R. & Hunter, L.
(2015a). Density-dependent natal dispersal patterns in a
leopard population recovering from over-harvest. PLoS ONE
10, e0122355.
Fattebert, J., Robinson, H.S., Balme, G., Slotow, R. & Hunter,
L. (2015b). Structural habitat predicts functional dispersal
habitat of a large carnivore: how leopards change spots. Ecol.
Appl. 25, 19111921.
Fieberg, J. & Kochanny, C.O. (2005). Quantiying home-range
overlap: the importance of the utilization distribution. J. Wildl.
Mgmt. 69, 13461359.
Gitzen, R.A., Millspaugh, J.J. & Kernohan, B.J. (2006).
Bandwidth selection for xed-kernel analysis of animal
utilization distributions. J. Wildl. Mgmt. 70, 13341344.
Goodrich, J.M., Miquelle, D.G., Smirnov, E.N., Kerley, L.L.,
Quigley, H.B. & Hornocker, M.G. (2010). Spatial structure of
Amur (Siberian) tigers (Panthera tigris altaica) on Sikhote-
Alin Biosphere Zapovednik. Russia. J. Mammal. 91, 737748.
Greenwood, P.J. (1980). Mating systems, philopatry and
dispersal in birds and mammals. Anim. Behav. 28, 11401162.
Grigione, M.M., Beier, P., Hopkins, R.A., Neal, D., Padley,
W.D., Schonewald, C.M. & Johnson, M.L. (2002). Ecological
and allometric determinants of home-range size for mountain
lions (Puma concolor). Anim. Conserv. 5, 317324.
Hayne, D.W. (1949). Calculation of size of home range. J.
Mammal. 30,118.
Hayward, M.W., OBrien, J. & Kerley, G.I.H. (2007). Carrying
capacity of large African predators: Predictions and tests. Biol.
Conserv. 139, 219229.
Hayward, M.W., Hayward, G.J., Druce, D.J. & Kerley, G.I.H.
(2009). Do fences constrain predator movements on an
evolutionary scale? Home range, food intake and movement
patterns of large predators reintroduced to Addo Elephant
National Park, South Africa. Biodivers. Conserv. 18, 887
904.
Jetz, W., Carbone, C., Fulford, J. & Brown, J.H. (2004). The
scaling of animal space use. Science 306, 266268.
Keehner, J.R., Wielgus, R.B., Maletzke, B.T. & Swanson, M.E.
(2015). Effects of male targeted harvest regime on sexual
segregation in mountain lion. Biol. Conserv. 192,4247.
Kernohan, B.J., Gitzen, R.A. & Millspaugh, J.J. (2001).
Analysis of animal space use and movements. In Radio
tracking and animal populations: 125166. Millspaugh, J.J. &
Marzluff, J.M. (Eds). San Diego: Academic Press.
\Lambin, X., Aars, J. & Piertney, S.B. (2001). Dispersal,
intraspecic competition, kin competition and kin facilitation:
a review of the empirical evidence. In Dispersal: Individual,
Population, and Community: 110122. Clobert, E.D.,
Dhondt, A.A. & Nichols, J.D. (Eds). Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Lewis, J.S., Rachlow, J.L., Garton, E.O. & Vierling, L.A.
(2007). Effects of habitat on GPS collar performance: using
data screening to reduce location error. J. Appl. Ecol. 44,
663671.
Logan, K.A. & Sweanor, L.L. (2001). Desert puma:
evolutionary ecology and conservation of an enduring
carnivore. Washington: Island Press.
Maletzke, B.T., Wielgus, R., Koehler, G.M., Swanson, M.,
Cooley, H. & Alldredge, J.R. (2014). Effects of hunting on
cougar social organization. Ecol. Evol. 4, 21782185.
Marker, L.L. & Dickman, A.J. (2005). Factors affecting leopard
(Panthera pardus) spatial ecology, with particular reference to
Namibian farmlands. S. Afr. J. Wildl. Res. 35, 105115.
McLoughlin, P., Ferguson, S. & Messier, F. (2000). Intraspecic
variation in home range overlap with habitat quality: a
comparison among brown bear populations. Evol. Ecol. 14,
3960.
Mitchell, M.S. & Powell, R.A. (2012). Foraging optimally for
home ranges. J. Mammal. 93, 917928.
Nilsen, E.B., Herndal, I. & Linnell, J.D.C. (2005). Can intra-
specic variation in carnivore home-range size be explained
using remote-sensing estimates of environmental productivity?
Ecoscience 12,6875.
Pesenti, E. & Zimmermann, F. (2013). Density estimations of
the Eurasian lynx (Lynx lynx) in the Swiss Alps. J. Mammal.
94,7381.
Pitman, R., Swanepoel, L., Hunter, L., Slotow, R. & Balme, G.
(2015). The importance of refugia, ecological traps, and scale for
large carnivore management. Biodivers. Conserv. 24,19751987.
Podg
orski, T., Scandura, M. & Je
zdrzejewska, B. (2014). Next of
kin next door philopatry and socio-genetic population
structure in wild boar. J. Zool. (Lond.) 294, 190197.
R Core Team. (2013) R: A language and environment for
statistical computing. Vienna: Foundation for Statistical
Computing. Available at: http://www.R-project.org/.
Sandell, M. (1989). The mating tactics and spacing patterns of
solitary carnivores. In Carnivore Behavior, Ecology, and
Evolution: 164182. Gittleman, J.L. (Ed). Ithaca: Cornell
University Press.
Stander, P.E. (1997). Field age determination of leopards by
tooth wear. Afr. J. Ecol. 35, 156161.
Støen, O.-G., Bellemain, E., Sæbø, S. & Swenson, J.E. (2005).
Kin-related spatial structure in brown bears Ursus arctos.
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 59, 191197.
WAMIS (2015) Wide Area Monitoring Information System.
Available at: http://wamis.meraka.org.za/products/long-term-
time-series (Accessed on 28 January 2015).
Woodroffe, R., Donnelly, C.A., Cox, D.R., Bourne, F.J.,
Cheeseman, C.L., Delahay, R.J., Gettinby, G., McInerney, J.P.
& Morrison, W.I. (2006). Effects of culling on badger Meles
meles spatial organization: implications for the control of
bovine tuberculosis. J. Appl. Ecol. 43,110.
Journal of Zoology  (2016)  ª2016 The Zoological Society of London 9
J. Fattebert et al. Leopard spatial organization dynamics
Supporting Information
Additional Supporting Information may be found in the online
version of this article:
Figure S1. Leopard density and prey biomass estimates,
Phinda Private Game Reserve, South Africa, 20022012.
Table S1. Summary statistics of consecutive seasonal home
range and core area size of adult females and adult male leop-
ards in Phinda Private Game Reserve, South Africa, 2002
2012.
Table S2. Summary statistics of annual home range size esti-
mated using the minimum convex polygon (MCP) method in
adult female and male leopards, Phinda Private Game Reserve,
South Africa, 20022012.
Table S3. Summary statistics of the inter-individual annual
home range percent area overlap in adult female and male
leopards, Phinda Private Game Reserve, South Africa, 2002
2012.
Table S4. Summary statistics of the intra-individual annual
home range percent area overlap in adult female and male
leopards, Phinda Private Game Reserve, South Africa, 2002
2012.
Table S5. Annual number of VHF and GPS telemetry reloca-
tions collected from adult leopards sampled in Phinda Game
Reserve, South Africa, 20022012.
10 Journal of Zoology  (2016)  ª2016 The Zoological Society of London
Leopard spatial organization dynamics J. Fattebert et al.
... Such prey requirements likely explain why both male and female Leopard home range sizes increase in arid areas where prey are scarcer (Simcharoen et al. 2008) and decrease in the more mesic prey-rich habitats (Bailey 1993;Stander et al. 1997;Marker and Dickman 2005;Odden and Wegge 2005;Simcharoen et al. 2008). Other factors that may influence home range sizes and movements therein include competition with conspecifics, intraguild competition, and whether leopards are persecuted (Marker and Dickman 2005;Fattebert et al. 2016;Comley et al. 2020;Le Roex et al. 2022). For example, previous studies have shown that Leopard trophy hunting, and the subsequent removal of individual leopards, can result in the expansion of the home ranges of any remaining leopards and, in some cases, increase overlap of territories (Marker and Dickman 2005;Fattebert et al. 2016). ...
... Other factors that may influence home range sizes and movements therein include competition with conspecifics, intraguild competition, and whether leopards are persecuted (Marker and Dickman 2005;Fattebert et al. 2016;Comley et al. 2020;Le Roex et al. 2022). For example, previous studies have shown that Leopard trophy hunting, and the subsequent removal of individual leopards, can result in the expansion of the home ranges of any remaining leopards and, in some cases, increase overlap of territories (Marker and Dickman 2005;Fattebert et al. 2016). ...
... Such rapid movement across and within territories is akin to the streaking behavior that has been observed in African elephants (Loxodonta africana) when they are forced to move through human-dominated corridor areas (Jachowski et al. 2013). In addition, Fattebert et al. (2016) showed that leopard home ranges tended to be larger, and not directly adjacent to one another, when conspecifics had been removed from the population through trophy hunting. Moreover, at some of our sites, particularly those that are more arid, the scarcity of prey may have forced leopards to expand their home ranges (Stander et al. 1997;). ...
Article
Full-text available
The size of the home range of a mammal is affected by numerous factors. However, in the normally solitary, but polygynous, Leopard (Panthera pardus), home range size and maintenance is complicated by their transitory social grouping behavior, which is dependent on life history stage and/or reproductive status. In addition, the necessity to avoid competition with conspecifics and other large predators (including humans) also impacts upon home range size. We used movement data from 31 sites across Africa, comprising 147 individuals (67 males and 80 females) to estimate the home range sizes of leopards. We found that leopards with larger home ranges, and in areas with more vegetation, spent longer being active and generally traveled faster, and in straighter lines, than leopards with smaller home ranges. We suggest that a combination of bottom-up (i.e., preferred prey availability), top-down (i.e., competition with conspecifics), and reproductive (i.e., access to mates) factors likely drive the variability in Leopard home range sizes across Africa. However, the maintenance of a large home range is energetically expensive for leopards, likely resulting in a complex evolutionary trade-off between the satisfaction of basic requirements and preventing potentially dangerous encounters with conspecifics, other predators, and people.
... Male leopards exhibit a strong defensive response to strangers, but appear to be more tolerant towards residential neighbours, which may be attributed to a 'dearenemy effect' (Christensen & Radford, 2018;Rafiq et al., 2020). Female leopards form matrilineal kin clusters, where related females display higher range overlap with each other than with unrelated females (Fattebert et al., 2016). According to the resident fitness hypothesis and as seen in other polygynous mammals, female leopards tolerate the costs of increased resource competition due to the benefits they gain from inclusive fitness through reduced kin competition (Fattebert et al., 2015(Fattebert et al., , 2016. ...
... Female leopards form matrilineal kin clusters, where related females display higher range overlap with each other than with unrelated females (Fattebert et al., 2016). According to the resident fitness hypothesis and as seen in other polygynous mammals, female leopards tolerate the costs of increased resource competition due to the benefits they gain from inclusive fitness through reduced kin competition (Fattebert et al., 2015(Fattebert et al., , 2016. ...
... Considerable research effort has focused on investigating leopard ranging patterns and scent-marking strategies (Bothma & Coertze, 2004;Fattebert et al., 2016;Marker & Dickman, 2005;Rafiq et al., 2020;Rodríguez-Recio et al., 2022;Rouse et al., 2021). Despite recent evidence suggesting that social factors structure leopard spatial organization (le Roex et al., 2022a; 2022b), we lack insights into how their sociospatial systems are organized and maintained. ...
Article
Intraspecific interactions shape animal social networks and regulate population dynamics. Species with solitary life histories rely on communication cues for population regulation, especially olfaction for many terrestrial mammals. Increasing evidence shows complex social structures among presumably solitary species and although social factors may play a key role in spatial organization, we lack insights into how species with solitary life histories structure and maintain sociospatial systems. Herein, we applied a social network approach to decode leopard, Panthera pardus, behaviour and interactions at marking sites that we monitored with camera traps. We found that leopard social units within our study area consisted of up to five individuals and that same-sex and opposite-sex interactions were equally likely to occur. Individuals behaved and responded differently depending on the type of interaction, serving both territorial and reproductive purposes. Temporal segregation allowed intersexual co-occurrence, while same-sex co-occurrence may be facilitated through familiarity with stable neighbours. Central individuals interacted within and outside their social unit and appeared fundamental to group stability. The removal of these individuals, such as through legal harvest or pre-emptively as an attempt to minimize depredation, may weaken social cohesion and ultimately affect population demography. Our findings on intraspecific co-occurrence in a solitary carnivore depict a complex social structure that can be important for population stability and might occur in other solitary species.
... Despite substantial variation in the home range sizes of two of Africa's largest territorial carnivores, lions Panthera leo and leopards Panthera pardus (Funston et al. 2003, Hayward et al. 2009, Loveridge et al. 2009, Balme et al. 2010, Davidson et al. 2011, Fattebert et al. 2016, it is unclear whether this variation is due to resource use or territory defence. ...
... Leopards are also territorial (Fattebert et al. 2016), but their territories are not exclusive (Stander et al. 1997), and some animals are transients (Bailey 1993). Furthermore, females may form matrilinear clusters and tolerate each other (Fattebert et al. 2016). ...
... Leopards are also territorial (Fattebert et al. 2016), but their territories are not exclusive (Stander et al. 1997), and some animals are transients (Bailey 1993). Furthermore, females may form matrilinear clusters and tolerate each other (Fattebert et al. 2016). Males and females react differently to changes in food and conspecific density but typically live singly in their own home ranges, with related females normally adjacent to one another (Fattebert et al. 2016). ...
... Along with abundant prey and optimum habitat, these conditions allow the population to be consistently maintained at or near capacity (Balme et al., 2019). Female leopards form matrilineal clusters under high-density conditions, with independent daughters remaining in the vicinity of their natal home range (Fattebert et al., 2015(Fattebert et al., , 2016Marker & Dickman, 2005). Intrasexual competition acts as the primary driver of individual home range size for both sexes in this population (le Roex et al., 2021), highlighting the importance of social factors at high density. ...
... Similar levels of space sharing were observed in leopard populations in Namibia (males: 0.24; females: 0.22; Marker & Dickman, 2005) and Botswana (females: 0.26; Steyn & Funston, 2009). Fattebert et al. (2016) found slightly lower overlap among same-sex conspecifics (males: 0.22; females: 0.16) and greater overlap for femaleemale pairs (0.66) under stable conditions in the Phinda-Mkhuze complex, South Africa. The spatial overlap observed in all these studies, however, far exceeds the 10% limit indicating exclusivity as proposed by Sandell (1989), suggesting that leopards may exhibit relaxed territoriality (Eide et al., 2004) across a range of conditions. ...
... Spatial proximity of related individuals and kin recognition are also both required for kin selection to influence space sharing (Hamilton, 1964;Hatchwell, 2010). The matrilineal kin clusters formed in leopard populations under high-density conditions (Fattebert et al., 2016) create a population structure in SSGR in which female kin selection can occur. Within this system, benefits to space sharing among kin appear to be restricted to mothers and daughters only. ...
Article
Territoriality (the defence of exclusive home ranges) is a strategy utilized within mammal populations to maximize individual fitness by monopolizing available resources. There is a trade-off, however, between acquiring the resources necessary for survival and reproduction and the cost of defending their exclusive use. Clarifying the sociospatial organization of wildlife populations is vital for understanding intraspecific competition and reproductive behaviour and, ultimately, conserving vulnerable or endangered species. We evaluated territorial behaviour in a solitary carnivore, the African leopard, Panthera pardus, under high-density conditions. We also assessed the influence of resource availability, sex-specific mating tactics and kinship on space sharing within the observed sociospatial structure. Both male and female leopards exhibited relaxed territoriality, with considerable intrasexual overlap occurring among both sexes, indicative of a risk aversion strategy. The risk of serious injury or death due to frequent territorial altercations in such a high-density system negated the benefits of strict spatial boundaries. Space sharing occurred more frequently in resource-rich areas: males overlapped more commonly in areas with high female density, and males and females overlapped more commonly in areas of high prey density. Males competed for access to females rather than monopoly of their home ranges; we hypothesize that the extralimital mating excursions undertaken by female leopards probably reduce the benefit of female monopoly (and consequently, of female defence) in this polygamous species. Space sharing among females was primarily driven by kinship; related females exhibited greater overlap than unrelated females, suggesting kinship benefits to space sharing among mother–daughter pairs. The contributions of resource availability, sex-specific mating tactics and kinship towards creating conditions permitting relaxed territoriality illustrate the complexity of ecological, demographic and behavioural factors involved in the sociospatial organization of solitary carnivores.
... 'frustrated dispersal') (Sweanor et al. 2000;Riley et al. 2006Riley et al. , 2014. Fattebert et al. (2015), Fattebert et al. (2016), andNaude et al. (2020) found that high hunting pressure on leopards increased the rate of female philopatry and caused a disruption of dispersal patterns in male leopards, which ultimately led to opportunistic male philopatry and localized inbreeding. In our study, male cougars had higher inbreeding coefficients than females across all sites, and although differences were not statistically significant with the exception of the Blue Mountains cougars, we observed an east-to-west gradient, which was particularly pronounced for males in the coastal regions, especially on the Olympic Peninsula. ...
Article
Full-text available
Conservation and management of wide-ranging carnivores like cougars (Puma concolor), which occur across human-altered landscapes can benefit from an in-depth understanding of their genetic status. Here, we apply the largest collection of multi-locus genotypes currently available for cougars (n = 1,903) to provide a comprehensive assessment of genetic diversity, gene flow, and source-sink dynamics for cougars occurring across Washington, United States and south-central British Columbia, Canada. We found that cougars in the Olympic, Cascade, Kettle, Selkirk, and Blue Mountains ecosystems are genetically differentiated into two clusters with varying degrees of admixture, indicating moderate levels of gene flow across the area with the exception of the Olympic Peninsula and the Blue Mountains which form more distinct genetic groups. We detected several first-generation migrants confirming long-distance movements within our study system, but also observed that migration rates between areas were asymmetrical, which is an indication of genetic source-sink dynamics. Genetic diversity and inbreeding followed a clinal east-to-west pattern with Olympic Peninsula cougars having the lowest genetic diversity and highest inbreeding coefficients among all sites. Spatial autocorrelation results for cougars did not follow sex-specific patterns suggesting that anthropogenic pressures such as habitat fragmentation and/or mortality sources may have an impact on their spatial dynamics. As cougar habitat in the northwestern United States continues to be affected by rising levels of urbanization and anthropogenic activities, long-term regional genetic monitoring represents a critical decision-support tool for formulating effective cougar conservation and management actions to prevent further genetic decline and promote long-term persistence of cougar populations.
... Although spatial capture-recapture analyses do not require all animals to have non-zero probability of being detected, we set camera stations at a spacing of ~ 2 km, a distance shorter than the smallest leopard home-range radius recorded in the literature (30 km 2 ; Bailey 1993; and 23 km 2 Fattebert et al. 2016), to ensure that individuals could be detected at multiple sites. This spacing of camera traps also allows for the sampling of spotted hyena populations, as typical clan home ranges are between 30 and 56 km 2 in similar (Braczkowski unpublished data), but more productive savannah environments of the Maasai Mara of Kenya and Serengeti, Tanzania East 1993a, 1993b;Boydston et al. 2003). ...
Article
Full-text available
African leopards (Panthera pardus pardus) and spotted hyenas (Crocuta crocuta) are data deficient across much of Africa, and there are only a handful of recent population estimates for these species from Uganda. This has conservation ramifications, as both species are important for wildlife tourism, and leopards are hunted for sport in several regions adjacent to national parks as part of a government-led revenue-sharing scheme to foster increased tolerance of wildlife. We ran a single-season camera-trap survey in each of the northern and southern sections of the Queen Elizabeth Conservation Area (2400 km 2), Uganda's second largest national park. We applied spatially explicit capture-recapture (SECR) models to estimate the population density and abundance of leopards and spotted hyenas in the northern Mweya and Kasenyi plains area, and the southern Ishasha sector. Leopard densities were estimated to be 5.03 (95% Highest Posterior Density, HPD = 2.80-7.63) and 4.31 (95% HPD = 1.95-6.88) individuals/100 km 2 for the north and south of the conservation area, respectively, while spotted hyena densities were 13.44 (95% HPD = 9.01-18.81) and 14.07 individuals/100 km 2 (95% HPD = 8.52-18.54) for the north and south, respectively. Leopard densities were in the middle range of those recorded in the literature, while sex ratios were what would be expected for this polygamous felid. Spotted hyena densities were on the higher end of those recorded for the species using spatially explicit capture-recapture (SECR) methods. Our work provides the first robust population estimate of leopards and spotted hyenas in the Queen Elizabeth Conservation Area of western Uganda.
... Therefore, the absence of baseline population estimates from protected areas could affect leopard conservation. Also, lack of population estimates hinders the proper understanding of the life history characteristics and ecological process of leopards, such as social organization, dispersal, spatial ecology, interspecific interactions, predation impact upon prey populations, and metapopulation dynamics for which density is among the primary drivers (Fattebert et al. 2016;Haswell et al. 2017;Hamer et al. 2021;Roex et al. 2021). Therefore, assessing the population density of leopards in the protected area helps in the effective conservation of leopards and enhances ecological understanding of their life history characteristic and critical ecological processes. ...
Article
Full-text available
Despite their high dietary and behavioural flexibility, leopards have lost > 70% of their historic range due to the causes like habitat loss, prey depletion and poaching. Precise abundance estimates are among the essential information required for leopard conservation. We provide estimates of leopard population density from Gir Protected Area, Gujarat, India, using camera traps deployed from May to June 2017. Our survey consists of 50 sampling sites covering an area of 200 km 2 in the western part of Gir. We used the likelihood-based spatially explicit capture-recapture (SECR) framework to assess leopard density in Gir. We identified 39 unique leopard individuals, among which 21 were males, 15 were females and three were of unknown sex. SECR analysis found the leopard density in Gir to be 19.90 ± 3.38 (S.E.) individuals/100 km 2 , where females had higher detection probabilities (0.14 ± 0.05 (S.E.)) than males (0.09 ± 0.02 (S.E.)). Males had a larger sigma value (σ) (in meters) (953.90 ± 92.99(S.E.)) than females (675.80 ± 84.78 (S.E.)). Possible reasons for the reported high leopard density are stringent protection measures which rule out chances of additive mortality in leopards, high preferred prey density, and limited effect of competitive interactions with Asiatic lions. A very high density of leopards could have implications for their own population regulation, prey population dynamics, and maintenance of the leopard population in the surrounding area through dispersal. The present study holds the potential to act as a baseline for future monitoring and inform management strategies for leopards in Gir.
... This has been reported in other areas (e.g. Odden & Wegge 2005, du Preez 2014, Fattebert et al. 2016) and is typical in species with a polygynous mating system, where females defend exclusive territories and male HRs overlap with several females, therefore accessing more mating opportunities (e.g. cougar, Puma concolor, Elbroch et al. 2016). ...
Article
Full-text available
Estimating large carnivore population size and understanding how individuals share space is crucial for their conservation, even more so now they are increasingly restricted to small, fenced game reserves where active management is often required. Combining data from GPS collars and camera traps, we estimated population size for leopards (Panthera pardus) on Ongava Game Reserve, northern Namibia, and investigated their spatio-temporal use of waterholes. Over three years of camera trapping, we identified a total of 29 individuals (including 12 adult or sub-adult females and 15 adult or sub-adult males). Based on the time interval over which they were observed, we defined 10 of these individuals as resident (four adult or sub-adult males and six adult or sub-adult females). The remaining 19 individuals (66%) were classified as transient. During the same period, we deployed two GPS collars, one on a resident adult male, the other on a resident adult female. Home range sizes from GPS data were estimated at 193 km 2 for the male and 122 km 2 for the female. Based on home range overlap found in the literature, we estimated Ongava's resident population to be composed of 2-4 males and 3-6 females. We found no evidence of exclusive use of waterholes by individuals, suggesting an absence of spatial avoidance. Our work highlights the importance of taking social status (resident vs transient) into account and of using multiple methods when estimating population size of leopards.
Article
Full-text available
Spatial patterns of and competition for resources by territorial carnivores are typically explained by two hypotheses: 1) the territorial defence hypothesis and 2) the searching efficiency hypothesis. According to the territorial defence hypothesis, when food resources are abundant, carnivore densities will be high and home ranges small. In addition, carnivores can maximise their necessary energy intake with minimal territorial defence. At medium resource levels, larger ranges will be needed, and it will become more economically beneficial to defend resources against a lower density of competitors. At low resource levels, carnivore densities will be low and home ranges large, but resources will be too scarce to make it beneficial to defend such large territories. Thus, home range overlap will be minimal at intermediate carnivore densities. According to the searching efficiency hypothesis, there is a cost to knowing a home range. Larger areas are harder to learn and easier to forget, so carnivores constantly need to keep their cognitive map updated by regularly revisiting parts of their home ranges. Consequently, when resources are scarce, carnivores require larger home ranges to acquire sufficient food. These larger home ranges lead to more overlap among individuals' ranges, so that overlap in home ranges is largest when food availability is the lowest. Since conspecific density is low when food availability is low, this hypothesis predicts that overlap is largest when densities are the lowest. We measured home range overlap and used a novel method to compare intraspecific home range overlaps for lions Panthera leo ( n = 149) and leopards Panthera pardus ( n = 111) in Africa. We estimated home range sizes from telemetry location data and gathered carnivore density data from the literature. Our results did not support the territorial defence hypothesis for either species. Lion prides increased their home range overlap at conspecific lower densities whereas leopards did not. Lion pride changes in overlap were primarily due to increases in group size at lower densities. By contrast, the unique dispersal strategies of leopards led to reduced overlap at lower densities. However, when human‐caused mortality was higher, leopards increased their home range overlap. Although lions and leopards are territorial, their territorial behaviour was less important than the acquisition of food in determining their space use. Such information is crucial for the future conservation of these two iconic African carnivores.
Article
Full-text available
Male targeted harvest regimes of carnivores are now widely accepted to result in increased sexually selected infanticide (SSI). Male targeted harvest regimes of males should therefore result in increased sexually segregated habitat use in infanticidal carnivores. We tested the effects of low and high levels of male hunting mortality and associated SSI on sexually segregated habitat use in mountain lions. The "no effect of hunting" hypothesis predicts that no sexual segregation would occur or that all female mountain lions would segregate from males because of sexual dimorphism. The "hunting effect" hypothesis predicts that females with kittens would segregate from younger immigrant males in the heavily hunted population during summer when kittens are vulnerable to SSI. We rejected the "no effect" hypothesis and accepted the "hunting effect" hypothesis for mountain lions. Females with kittens avoided immigrant males in the heavily hunted population during summer-others did not. This sexual segregation corresponded with females with kittens selecting for food-poor, high elevations in the heavily hunted population but not in the lightly hunted population. Avoidance of males and selection for high elevations resulted in prey switching by females with kittens from abundant primary prey in lower elevations to rare, sensitive and threatened secondary prey at higher elevations. It appears that remedial sport hunting of mountain lions to reduce predation on declining prey actually caused sexual segregation and increased predation on declining prey. We suggest that excess mortality of male carnivores could result in unanticipated cascade effects including sexual segregation and prey switching to declining prey.
Article
Full-text available
Home ranges capture a fundamental aspect of animal ecology, resulting from interactions between metabolic demands and resource availability. Yet, the understanding of their emergence is currently limited by lack of consideration of the covariation between intrinsic and extrinsic drivers. We analysed intraspecific home-range size (HRS) variation with respect to life histories and remotely sensed proxies of resource dynamics for 21 Carnivora species. Our best model explained over half of the observed variability in intraspecific HRS across populations of multiple species. At the species level, median HRS was smaller for omnivorous species and increased with increasing body mass (model R 2 = 0.66). Here, HRS scaled with body mass at 0.80, a value much closer to the expected allometric scaling of 0.75 than previously reported. At the intraspecific level, while much variation was driven by intrinsic factors (body mass, diet, social organization and sex; R 2 = 0.39), inclusion of spatiotemporal variation in extrinsic factors (average resource availability and seasonality) enabled explanation of a further 13% of observed variability in HRS. We found no evidence for interactions between intrinsic and extrinsic HRS drivers, suggesting a generally ubiquitous influence of resource availability on space-use. Our findings illustrate how spatial and temporal information on resource dynamics as derived by satellite data can significantly improve our understanding of HRS variation at the interspecific and intraspecific levels, and urge caution in interpreting HRS allometry in the face of large intraspecific variation. Moreover, our results highlight the importance of considering life-history constraints in modelling intraspecific space-use and HRS.
Article
Full-text available
Natal dispersal promotes inter-population linkage, and is key to spatial distribution of populations. Degradation of suitable landscape structures beyond the specific threshold of an individual’s ability to disperse can therefore lead to disruption of functional landscape connectivity and impact metapopulation function. Because it ignores behavioral responses of individuals, structural connectivity is easier to assess than functional connectivity and is often used as a surrogate for landscape connectivity modeling. However using structural resource selection models as surrogate for modeling functional connectivity through dispersal could be erroneous. We tested how well a second-order resource selection function (RSF) models (structural connectivity), based on GPS telemetry data from resident adult leopard (Panthera pardus L.), could predict subadult habitat use during dispersal (functional connectivity). We created eight non-exclusive subsets of the subadult data based on differing definitions of dispersal to assess the predictive ability of our adult-based RSF model extrapolated over a broader landscape. Dispersing leopards used habitats in accordance with adult selection patterns, regardless of the definition of dispersal considered. We demonstrate that, for a wide-ranging apex carnivore, functional connectivity through natal dispersal corresponds to structural connectivity as modeled by a second-order RSF. Mapping of the adult-based habitat classes provides direct visualization of the potential linkages between populations, without the need to model paths between a priori starting and destination points. The use of such landscape scale RSFs may provide insight into predicting suitable dispersal habitat peninsulas in human-dominated landscapes where mitigation of human–wildlife conflict should be focused. We recommend the use of second-order RSFs for landscape conservation planning and propose a similar approach to the conservation of other wide- ranging large carnivore species where landscape-scale resource selection data already exist.
Article
Full-text available
Natal dispersal enables population connectivity, gene flow and metapopulation dynamics. In polygynous mammals, dispersal is typically male-biased. Classically, the 'mate competi-tion', 'resource competition' and 'resident fitness' hypotheses predict density-dependent dispersal patterns, while the 'inbreeding avoidance' hypothesis posits density-independent dispersal. In a leopard (Panthera pardus) population recovering from over-harvest, we investigated the effect of sex, population density and prey biomass, on age of natal dispersal, distance dispersed, probability of emigration and dispersal success. Over an 11-year period , we tracked 35 subadult leopards using VHF and GPS telemetry. Subadult leopards initiated dispersal at 13.6 ± 0.4 months. Age at commencement of dispersal was positively density-dependent. Although males (11.0 ± 2.5 km) generally dispersed further than females (2.7 ± 0.4 km), some males exhibited opportunistic philopatry when the population was below capacity. All 13 females were philopatric, while 12 of 22 males emigrated. Male dispersal distance and emigration probability followed a quadratic relationship with population density, whereas female dispersal distance was inversely density-dependent. Eight of 12 known-fate females and 5 of 12 known-fate male leopards were successful in settling. Dispersal success did not vary with population density, prey biomass, and for males, neither between dispersal strategies (philopatry vs. emigration). Females formed matrilineal kin clusters, supporting the resident fitness hypothesis. Conversely, mate competition appeared the main driver for male leopard dispersal. We demonstrate that dispersal patterns changed over time, i.e. as the leopard population density increased. We conclude that conservation interventions that facilitated local demographic recovery in the study area also restored dispersal patterns disrupted by unsustainable harvesting, and that this indirectly improved connectivity among leopard populations over a larger landscape.
Code
Tools for performing model selection and model averaging. Automated model selection through subsetting the maximum model, with optional constraints for model inclusion. Model parameter and prediction averaging based on model weights derived from information criteria (AICc and alike) or custom model weighting schemes. [Please do not request the full text - it is an R package. The up-to-date manual is available from CRAN].
Article
As use of Akaike's Information Criterion (AIC) for model selection has become increasingly common, so has a mistake involving interpretation of models that are within 2 AIC units (ΔAIC ≤ 2) of the top-supported model. Such models are <2 ΔAIC units because the penalty for one additional parameter is 2 AIC units, but model deviance is not reduced by an amount sufficient to overcome the 2-unit penalty and, hence, the additional parameter provides no net reduction in AIC. Simply put, the uninformative parameter does not explain enough variation to justify its inclusion in the model and it should not be interpreted as having any ecological effect. Models with uninformative parameters are frequently presented as being competitive in the Journal of Wildlife Management, including 72 of all AIC-based papers in 2008, and authors and readers need to be more aware of this problem and take appropriate steps to eliminate misinterpretation. I reviewed 5 potential solutions to this problem: 1) report all models but ignore or dismiss those with uninformative parameters, 2) use model averaging to ameliorate the effect of uninformative parameters, 3) use 95 confidence intervals to identify uninformative parameters, 4) perform all-possible subsets regression and use weight-of-evidence approaches to discriminate useful from uninformative parameters, or 5) adopt a methodological approach that allows models containing uninformative parameters to be culled from reported model sets. The first approach is preferable for small sets of a priori models, whereas the last 2 approaches should be used for large model sets or exploratory modeling.
Article
Spacing patterns of large carnivores can affect demographic parameters of populations that, in turn, influence effective population size. As a result, better understanding of spatial structure can provide insight into effective conservation strategies. We examined home-range size, spacing characteristics, and changes in land tenure of radiocollared Amur tigers (Panthera tigris altaica) on the Sikhote-Alin Biosphere Zapovednik, Russia, from 1992 to 2006. We predicted that both sexes would maintain spatially exclusive home ranges and that subadult female tigers would tend toward philopatry and males would disperse. Home ranges (95% fixed kernel estimates; mean ± SD) of resident females (n = 20 home ranges of 14 females; 390 ± 136 km2) were significantly (P = 0•003) smaller than those of males (n 6 home ranges of 5 males; 1,385 ± 539 km2). Geometric mean overlap between adjacent females (0•11 ± 0•11 SD) did not differ from that between adjacent males (0•14 ± 0•12). All radiocollared male cubs dispersed (n = 7), but only 2 of 6 female cubs dispersed from their natal home ranges. When human-caused mortality was low, female tigers survived long enough to divide their home range with their daughters, resulting in smaller home ranges and a higher density of breeding females. All females reproduced in these smaller territories, suggesting that they maintained home ranges that were larger than needed to meet reproductive demands. However, when human-caused mortality was high, females often did not survive long enough to bequeath home ranges to daughters, and population density was apparently maintained well below carrying capacity. The impacts of poaching appear to extend beyond the direct loss of individuals, and therefore reserves must be well protected if they are to serve as source populations for adjacent, unprotected areas of tiger habitat.
Chapter
A majority of the carnivore species are primarily solitary, having very little contact with conspecifics (Gittleman, this volume). These solitary species have received less attention than the group-living species, which have attracted much interest (see reviews in Macdonald and Moehlman 1982; Macdonald 1983; Bekoff et al. 1984).