Conference PaperPDF Available

The Burden of Expertise

Authors:

Abstract

Evolutionary research indicates that underestimating one's capabilities maximizes individual fitness in extremely competitive conditions (Johnson & Fowler, 2011). Further, it has been reported that the top performers tend to underestimate their performance attributes in reference to their peers, which has been labeled as " the burden of expertise " (Dunning, 2005). With respect to athletes' performance, however, the general stand has been that positive self-perceptions about one's capabilities, even if they exceed one's actual capabilities, are adaptive (Bandura, 1997). This study compared youth elite players' and coaches' perceptions of players' skill level, and examined the relationship between this comparison and players' past and future record of playing international matches (N = 338, Mage = 17.8, SD = 1.1). A latent class analysis (Nylund, Asparouhov, & Muthén, 2007) was performed in order to identify subgroups within the population based on the players' and the coaches´ratingscoaches´ratings of the players' skills. The model with three classes was determent to be optimal (entropy = .76, likelihood ratio test p =.02). The classes consisted of 77 (class 1), 90 (class 2), and 100 (class 3) participants. Participants in both class 1 and class 2 indicated higher scores on all skills in comparison to the coaches' scores. The opposite pattern was obtained among the participants in the class 3, where the coaches' scored higher than the participants on all skills. Further, while controlling for age, a multinomial regression analysis (X² (6, N = 266) = 49.39, p ˂ .01) revealed that in comparison to the participants in class 3, both the participants in class 1 (OR = 0.66, p ˂ .05, 95% CI = 0.46, 0.95) and the participants in class 2 (OR = 0.82, p ˂ .05, 95% CI = 0.71, 0.91) had a reduced likelihood of playing international matches the next two years.
The Burden of Expertise
Erik Hofsetha, Tynke Toeringa, Geir Jordeta, Andreas Ivarssonb
a Department of Coaching and Psychology, Norwegian School of Sport Sciences bSchool of
Social and Health Sciences, Halmstad University
Evolutionary research indicates that underestimating one’s capabilities maximizes individual
fitness in extremely competitive conditions (Johnson & Fowler, 2011). Further, it has been
reported that the top performers tend to underestimate their performance attributes in
reference to their peers, which has been labeled as “the burden of expertise” (Dunning, 2005).
With respect to athletes’ performance, however, the general stand has been that positive self-
perceptions about one’s capabilities, even if they exceed one’s actual capabilities, are adaptive
(Bandura, 1997). This study compared youth elite players’ and coaches’ perceptions of
players’ skill level, and examined the relationship between this comparison and players’ past
and future record of playing international matches (N = 338, Mage = 17.8, SD = 1.1). A latent
class analysis (Nylund, Asparouhov, & Muthén, 2007) was performed in order to identify
subgroups within the population based on the players’ and the coaches´ ratings of the players’
skills. The model with three classes was determent to be optimal (entropy = .76, likelihood
ratio test p =.02). The classes consisted of 77 (class 1), 90 (class 2), and 100 (class 3)
participants. Participants in both class 1 and class 2 indicated higher scores on all skills in
comparison to the coaches’ scores. The opposite pattern was obtained among the participants
in the class 3, where the coaches’ scored higher than the participants on all skills. Further,
while controlling for age, a multinomial regression analysis ((6, N = 266) = 49.39, p ˂ .01)
revealed that in comparison to the participants in class 3, both the participants in class 1 (OR
= 0.66, p ˂ .05, 95% CI = 0.46, 0.95) and the participants in class 2 (OR = 0.82, p ˂ .05, 95%
CI = 0.71, 0.91) had a reduced likelihood of playing international matches the next two years.
Thus, the “burden” of expertise seems to be a phenomenon in youth elite soccer, predicting a
high future performance level. Unrealistically positive self-perceptions concerning skills
should therefore be discouraged. Consequently, coaches should provide players with feedback
and experiences that gives them insight into the limitations of their skills.
References
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York, NY: Freeman.
Dunning, D. (2005). Self-insight: Roadblocks and detours on the path to knowing thyself.
New York, NY: Psychology Press.
Johnson, D. D. P., & Fowler, J. H. (2011). The evolution of overconfidence. Nature, 477,
317320. doi:10.1038/nature10384
Nylund, K. L., Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. O. (2007). Latent class analysis and growth
mixture modeling: A monte carlo simulation study. Structural Equation Modeling, 14,
535-569.
Contact: hofsetherik@gmail.com
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
Mixture modeling is a widely applied data analysis technique used to identify unobserved heterogeneity in a population. Despite mixture models' usefulness in practice, one unresolved issue in the application of mixture models is that there is not one commonly accepted statistical indicator for deciding on the number of classes in a study population. This article presents the results of a simulation study that examines the performance of likelihood-based tests and the traditionally used Information Criterion (ICs) used for determining the number of classes in mixture modeling. We look at the performance of these tests and indexes for 3 types of mixture models: latent class analysis (LCA), a factor mixture model (FMA), and a growth mixture models (GMM). We evaluate the ability of the tests and indexes to correctly identify the number of classes at three different sample sizes (n D 200, 500, 1,000). Whereas the Bayesian Information Criterion performed the best of the ICs, the bootstrap likelihood ratio test proved to be a very consistent indicator of classes across all of the models considered.
Article
People base thousands of choices across a lifetime on the views they hold of their skill and moral character, yet a growing body of research in psychology shows that such self-views are often misguided or misinformed. Anyone who has dealt with others in the classroom, in the workplace, in the medical office, or on the therapist's couch has probably experienced people whose opinions of themselves depart from the objectively possible. This book outlines some of the common errors that people make when they evaluate themselves. It also describes the many psychological barriers - some that people build by their own hand - that prevent individuals from achieving self-insight about their ability and character. The first section of the book focuses on mistaken views of competence, and explores why people often remain blissfully unaware of their incompetence and personality flaws. The second section focuses on faulty views of character, and explores why people tend to perceive they are more unique and special than they really are, why people tend to possess inflated opinions of their moral fiber that are not matched by their deeds, and why people fail to anticipate the impact that emotions have on their choices and actions. The book will be of great interest to students and researchers in social, personality, and cognitive psychology, but, through the accessibility of its writing style, it will also appeal to those outside of academic psychology with an interest in the psychological processes that lead to our self-insight.
Article
Confidence is an essential ingredient of success in a wide range of domains ranging from job performance and mental health to sports, business and combat. Some authors have suggested that not just confidence but overconfidence--believing you are better than you are in reality--is advantageous because it serves to increase ambition, morale, resolve, persistence or the credibility of bluffing, generating a self-fulfilling prophecy in which exaggerated confidence actually increases the probability of success. However, overconfidence also leads to faulty assessments, unrealistic expectations and hazardous decisions, so it remains a puzzle how such a false belief could evolve or remain stable in a population of competing strategies that include accurate, unbiased beliefs. Here we present an evolutionary model showing that, counterintuitively, overconfidence maximizes individual fitness and populations tend to become overconfident, as long as benefits from contested resources are sufficiently large compared with the cost of competition. In contrast, unbiased strategies are only stable under limited conditions. The fact that overconfident populations are evolutionarily stable in a wide range of environments may help to explain why overconfidence remains prevalent today, even if it contributes to hubris, market bubbles, financial collapses, policy failures, disasters and costly wars.