ChapterPDF Available

You Are What You Eat: Foraging Specializations and Their Influence on the Social Organization and Behavior of Killer Whales

Authors:
  • Fisheries and Oceans Canada, Pacific Biological Station

Abstract and Figures

The feeding ecology of predators can have a profound effect on their life history and behaviour. The killer whale—the apex marine predator—has a cosmopolitan distribution throughout the world’s oceans. Globally, it is a generalist predator with a diverse diet, but regionally, different socially and genetically isolated killer whale populations can have highly specialized foraging strategies involving only a few types of prey. In the eastern North Pacific, the three sympatric killer whale lineages have distinct dietary specializations: one feeds primarily on marine mammals, another on salmon, and the third appears to specialize on sharks. These ecological specializations are associated with distinct patterns of seasonal distribution, group size, social organization, foraging behavior, and acoustic activity. Divergent foraging strategies may have played a major role in the social isolation and genetic divergence of killer whale populations.
Content may be subject to copyright.
75
J. Yamagiwa and L. Karczmarski (eds.), Primates and Cetaceans: Field Research
and Conservation of Complex Mammalian Societies, Primatology Monographs,
DOI 10.1007/978-4-431-54523-1_4, © Springer Japan 2014
Chapter 4
You Are What You Eat: Foraging
Specializations and Their Infl uence
on the Social Organization and Behavior
of Killer Whales
John K.B. Ford and Graeme M. Ellis
J. K.B. Ford (*) • G. M. Ellis
Fisheries and Oceans Canada, Pacifi c Biological Station , Nanaimo , BC , Canada V9T 6N7
e-mail: John.K.Ford@dfo-mpo.gc.ca
76
Abstract The feeding ecology of predators can have a profound effect on their life
history and behaviour. The killer whale—the apex marine predator—has a cosmo-
politan distribution throughout the world’s oceans. Globally, it is a generalist preda-
tor with a diverse diet, but regionally, different socially and genetically isolated
killer whale populations can have highly specialized foraging strategies involving
only a few types of prey. In the eastern North Pacifi c, the three sympatric killer
whale lineages have distinct dietary specializations: one feeds primarily on marine
mammals, another on salmon, and the third appears to specialize on sharks. These
ecological specializations are associated with distinct patterns of seasonal distribu-
tion, group size, social organization, foraging behavior, and acoustic activity.
Divergent foraging strategies may have played a major role in the social isolation
and genetic divergence of killer whale populations.
Keywords Apex predator Feeding ecology Orcinus orca
4.1 Introduction
Ecological specialization is an important factor promoting the evolution of biologi-
cal diversity and speciation (Futuyma and Moreno 1988 ; Robinson et al. 1996 ;
Dieckmann and Doebeli 1999 ; Schluter 2001 ; V i a 2001 ). Optimal foraging theory
predicts that selection will generally favor dietary specialization, as specialists
have a competitive advantage over generalists in foraging effi ciency (as in the
adage, “the jack-of-all-trades is the master of none”) (Stephens and Krebs 1986 ;
Futuyma and Moreno 1988 ; Robinson et al. 1996 ). Such selection may drive the
divergent evolution of a wide variety of adaptive traits involving morphology,
physiology, and behavior of populations or subpopulations with different foraging
strategies or in contrasting environments. Divergent selection between sympatric
populations may lead to assortative mating, reproductive isolation, and, ultimately,
speciation (Dieckmann and Doebeli 1999 ; Schluter 2001 ; V i a 2001 ; McKinnon
et al. 2004 ) .
Killer whales, the largest of the dolphins (family Delphinidae), provide an excep-
tional opportunity to gain insight into the processes and outcomes of ecological
specialization and divergence in a highly social and versatile mammalian predator.
This species (only a single species, Orcinus orca , is currently recognized) is one of
the most widely distributed mammals on the planet. It has a cosmopolitan distribu-
tion in all the world’s oceans, from the pack ice edges in both the Northern and
Southern Hemispheres through the equatorial tropics (Ford 2002 ). Although rare in
many regions, it is relatively common in cool, productive, high-latitude waters, par-
ticularly in nearshore areas. Despite their wide distribution, killer whales are not
abundant, with a minimum estimated global population of 50,000, but probably not
greatly more (Forney and Wade 2006 ). Killer whales occupy the top trophic posi-
tion in the oceans and have no predators. As a species, killer whales could be con-
sidered generalist predators, with an extremely diverse array of more than 140
J.K.B. Ford and G.M. Ellis
77
species of vertebrates and invertebrates—from small schooling sh to the largest of
the cetaceans—recorded as prey (Ford 2009 ). However, fi eld studies in several
global regions have revealed that local populations can have remarkably specialized
diets and may forage selectively for only a very small subset of the prey species that
the predator is capable of consuming. In this chapter, we provide a description of
three distinct killer whale lineages that co-occur in coastal waters of the northeast-
ern Pacifi c, focusing in particular on the infl uence that ecological specialization
appears to have had on their divergent lifestyles, including habitat use patterns,
social structure, behavior, and use of underwater sound. We also provide a brief
overview of how these lineages came to be identifi ed and known in these waters and
of recent work in other regions that suggests that ecological specialization is char-
acteristic of this apex social predator.
4.2 Discovery of Killer Whale Lineages
in the Eastern North Pacifi c
Before the 1970s, scientifi c understanding of the killer whale was poor and was
based almost entirely on anecdotal or opportunistic observations rather than on
dedicated scientifi c studies (Martinez and Klinghammer 1970 ). However, a live-
capture fi shery for killer whales that developed during the late 1960s in nearshore
waters of southern British Columbia, Canada, and northern Washington State, USA,
highlighted the need for basic abundance and life history data for management. As
a result, in 1972 our late colleague, Michael Bigg, initiated eld studies of killer
whales in this area based primarily on the identifi cation of individuals from photo-
graphs of natural markings on the whales’ dorsal fi n and grey “saddle patch” at the
base of the n. This technique was considered quite novel and unproven at the time,
but Bigg quickly showed that it was an effective means of collecting reliable popu-
lation abundance and life history data on these diffi cult-to-study animals (Bigg et al.
1976 ). We joined this fi eld effort at different points in the 1970s and, working
together with Bigg and our colleague Ken Balcomb in Washington State, broadened
the study’s scope to include social organization, foraging ecology, behavior, and
vocalizations (Bigg et al. 1987 ).
By the late 1970s, it was apparent that two different types of killer whales coex-
isted in the region. One type, named “residents,” lived in stable groups of 10 to 25
and were found reliably in predictable “core areas” throughout at least summer and
fall. A second type was found in the same waters but only rarely and sporadically.
These whales were observed alone or in small groups of 2 to 6, tended to swim close
along shorelines, often erratically, and were never seen to mix with the larger “resi-
dent” groups. As it was thought that these whales were merely passing through the
home ranges of the residents, they were named “transients” (Bigg 1982 ). Resident
and transient killer whales were occasionally observed within a few hundred meters
of each other but showed no obvious reaction to the presence of the other whales
and did not intermingle. However, resident groups frequently mixed with other
4 You Are What You Eat: Foraging Specializations and Their Infl uence
78
residents and transients with other transients. Although residents and transients
were clearly socially isolated, it was not certain what these two types represented.
Initially it was thought that transients were individuals that had dispersed from resi-
dent groups, possibly in other regions, and were adopting a “low profi le” behavior
while transiting core areas of residents. However, subtle differences in dorsal n
shape and pigmentation suggested an underlying genetic distinction between them.
As the number of observations of feeding grew in the early 1980s, evidence mounted
that residents and transients were distinct ecotypes with fundamentally different
diets—residents prey on fi sh and transients on marine mammals (Bigg et al. 1985 ,
1987 ). That these two types of whales specialize on such different kinds of prey
helped explain the growing number of differences we observed in the movement
patterns, social structure, vocalizations, and behavior of residents and transients.
To our surprise, in the early 1990s we discovered a third type of killer whale,
named “offshores,” in British Columbian waters (Ford et al. 1992 ; Ford et al. 2000 ).
These whales have slightly different n shapes than residents and transients and
appear to be somewhat smaller in body size. Offshore killer whales generally prefer
the outer continental shelf, and it was only when we expanded our study area to
include these waters that we found these whales. Residents and transients also use
these outer waters, and offshores have recently made more frequent appearances in
nearshore areas (Dahlheim et al. 2008 ). Despite their mostly sympatric distribution,
all three killer whale types maintain social isolation from each other (Ford et al.
2000 ). From the few available observations of predation by offshore killer whales
and their patterns of behavior and vocal activity, it appears that they are primarily or
entirely sh feeders with a probable specialization on sharks (Ford et al. 2000 ,
2011 ; Jones 2006 ; Dahlheim et al. 2008 ).
In addition to our own long-term studies in British Columbia and Washington
State, numerous other researchers have undertaken eldwork on various aspects of
the life history, ecology, and behavior of killer whales, both in our study area and in
adjacent coastal waters. Over the years, these efforts have together provided a much
improved understanding of the divergent ecological specializations of residents and
transients and the role these have played in defi ning the lifestyles of these lineages.
4.3 Population Delineation of Lineages
Resident, transient, and offshore killer whale lineages are sympatric in coastal
waters of the eastern North Pacifi c from California to the Aleutian Islands in
Alaska. Molecular studies have confi rmed what earlier observations suggested
that the three lineages are genetically distinct and gene fl ow between them is mini-
mal or absent (Stevens et al. 1989 ; Hoelzel et al. 1998 ; Barrett-Lennard 2000 ;
Morin et al. 2010 ). At least two of these lineages—residents and transients—are
represented by multiple discrete populations of typically a few hundred individuals.
Four populations have been described for residents (Matkin et al.
1999 ; Ford et al.
2000 ; Matkin et al. 2007a ). Each population ranges over roughly 1,300- to
J.K.B. Ford and G.M. Ellis
79
1,800-km sections of coastline that overlap substantially. Despite overlapping dis-
tribution, each population generally occupies rather discrete areas, especially dur-
ing summer and fall. Groups of resident whales from adjacent populations have
been observed in close proximity on a few occasions, but no intermingling has
taken place. However, groups belonging to the same population frequently join and
travel together, occasionally forming large multigroup aggregations that may per-
sist for several days. DNA fi ngerprinting indicates that mating takes place between
groups within each resident population, and intermating between populations is
extremely rare (Barrett- Lennard 2000 ) .
Transient killer whales are subdivided into at least ve regional populations,
each typically composed of 100–300 individuals (Bigg et al. 1987 ; Black et al.
1997 ; Ford and Ellis 1999 ; Matkin et al. 1999 , 2007a ). Groups of transients within
each population regularly intermingle and, in contrast to residents, they will also
associate with members of adjacent transient populations during the infrequent
occasions when they roam into the range of another population (Ford and Ellis
1999 ; Ford et al. 2007 ). Offshore killer whales appear to consist of a single popula-
tion of at least 250 animals that ranges widely over the continental shelf, from
southern California to the eastern Aleutian Islands, Alaska (Ford et al. 2000 ; Matkin
et al. 2007a ; Dahlheim et al. 2008 ). The extent of potential movements beyond the
continental shelf for any of these whales is unknown because of the lack of eld
effort in offshore waters.
4.4 Dietary Specialization
Gaining insight into the feeding habits of free-ranging cetaceans is diffi cult because
predation usually takes place underwater and out of sight. We have studied the diets
of resident, transient, and offshore killer whales using three different methods:
(1) direct observation of predation when it takes place at the surface, (2) collection
of prey fragments left in the water column following a kill, and (3) recovery of prey
remains from the stomachs of beach-cast carcasses. Others have also used chemical
analyses of skin and blubber biopsy samples collected from killer whales to infer
diet from stable isotope ratios, fatty acids, and levels of various types of contami-
nants (e.g., Krahn et al. 2007 ).
Surface observations and identifi cation of prey fragments from kills indicate that
the diet of resident killer whales in British Columbia (Fig. 4.1 ) consists primarily of
teleost shes, in particular the Pacifi c salmonids ( Oncorhynchus spp.) (Fig. 4.2 :
Ford et al. 1998 ; Saulitis et al. 2000 ; Ford and Ellis 2006 ). Non-salmonid shes
such as lingcod ( Ophiodon elongatus ), Dover sole ( Microstomus pacifi cus ), and
Pacifi c halibut ( Hippoglossus stenolepis ) have also been identifi ed from predation
events, but these represent less than 3 % of observed kills. A surprising result of our
prey fragment sampling has been the pronounced preference that residents have
for Chinook salmon ( Oncorhynchus tshawytscha ). In total, more than 70 % of iden-
tifi ed salmonid kills have been Chinook, despite this species being one of the least
4 You Are What You Eat: Foraging Specializations and Their Infl uence
80
common of the fi ve salmonid species available in the whales’ habitat (Ford and Ellis
2006 ). Chinook predominated in our samples even when other salmonids, such as
sockeye ( O. nerka ) and pink ( O. gorbuscha ) salmon, were far more abundant in
foraging areas during summer spawning migrations, outnumbering Chinook by as
many as 500 sh to 1 (Ford et al. 1998 ; Ford and Ellis 2006 ). Chum salmon ( O. keta )
are signifi cant prey during a short period in the fall, but Chinook still appear to be
taken preferentially. Prey remains recovered from beach-cast carcasses of residents
are generally consistent with our observations of predation. Chinook salmon has
been identifi ed in most stomach contents to date, and various non-salmonids and
squid have also been represented occasionally (Ford et al. 1998 ).
I t i s m o s t p r o b a b l e t h a t t h e w h a l e s p r e f e r e n c e f o r d i f f e r e n t s a l m o n i d s a n d o t h e r
prey species for that matter—is proportional to their relative profi tability. Chinook
are by far the largest of the Pacifi c salmon, commonly reaching sizes of more than
20 kg, and they tend to have the highest lipid content of the salmonids, enhancing
their net energy density. Chum salmon are the second largest salmonid and can reach
10 kg or more. The much smaller sockeye and pink salmon seem to be of little inter-
est to the whales, despite their brief but often great abundance during summer.
In striking contrast to resident killer whales, transient killer whales (Fig. 4.3 )
have only been observed to hunt and consume endothermic prey, primarily marine
mammals and occasionally seabirds. In British Columbia, Washington State, and
Southeast Alaska, the most frequent prey species by far (about 50 % of kills) is the
harbour seal ( Phoca vitulina ), a small (average, 60–80 kg) pinniped that is common
throughout nearshore waters of the region (Fig. 4.2 ) (Ford et al. 1998 ; Matkin et al.
Fig. 4.1 A male resident killer whale surfaces following capture of a Chinook salmon, the primary
prey species of this ecotype. (Photograph by M. Malleson)
J.K.B. Ford and G.M. Ellis
81
2007a , b ) . H a r b o u r p o r p o i s e ( Phocoena phocoena ) a n d D a l l s p o r p o i s e ( Phocoenoides
dalli ) t o g e t h e r m a k e u p a b o u t o n e - q u a r t e r o f o b s e r v e d k i l l s , w i t h t h e r e m a i n d e r c o m -
posed of Steller sea lions ( Eumetopias jubatus ) , C a l i f o r n i a s e a l i o n s ( Zalophus cali-
fornianus ) , P a c i c white-sided dolphins ( Lagenorhynchus obliquidens ) , m i n k e
whales ( Balaenoptera acutorostrata ) , n o r t h e r n e l e p h a n t s e a l s ( Mirounga angustiros-
tris ) , a n d v a r i o u s s e a b i r d s p e c i e s ( F o r d e t a l . 1998 , 2005 ) . S w i m m i n g d e e r
( Odocoileus hemionus ) a n d m o o s e ( Alces alces ) h a v e o n r a r e o c c a s i o n s b e e n r e p o r t e d
to be killed by killer whales in the region, almost certainly transients (Pike and
MacAskie
1969 ; M a t k i n e t a l . 1999 ) . S e a b i r d s d o n o t s e e m t o b e a n i m p o r t a n t p r e y
item of transient killer whales. Only a minority of seabirds that are harassed and
killed by transients are ultimately consumed: most are abandoned. Interaction with
seabirds usually involves juvenile whales and may represent play behavior that ulti-
mately functions to develop prey handling skills (Ford et al. 1998 ; S a u l i t i s e t a l . 2000 ) .
Transients have not been observed to take any fi sh species, nor have any sh remains
been identifi ed in stomach contents of beach-cast carcasses of transients (Ford et al.
1998 ; Saulitis et al. 2000 ; Heise et al. 2003 ).
Fig. 4.2 Frequency distribution of prey species observed to be consumed by resident ( top , n = 439
kills) and transient ( bottom , n = 251 kills) killer whales in coastal waters of British Columbia,
Washington State, and southeastern Alaska. [Data from Ford et al. (
1998 ), Ford and Ellis ( 2006 ),
and Ford and Ellis (unpublished data)]
4 You Are What You Eat: Foraging Specializations and Their Infl uence
82
There is little evidence that transient individuals or matrilines specialize on par-
ticular types or species of marine mammals, despite the very different tactics needed
to capture and kill them (harbour seals versus Dall’s porpoise, for example; Ford
et al. 1998 ). Our long-term monitoring of transient predation has shown that the
variety of prey species taken by particular individuals or groups is strongly corre-
lated with the cumulative number of predation events documented for those animals
(Ford et al. 1998 ). Predation of minke whales by transients in our study area is
uncommon, but a particular matriline (the T18 group) has been involved in more
cases than one would expect by chance (Ford et al. 2005 ; J.K.B.F. and G.M.E.,
unpublished data). This matriline also hunts more typical prey, such as harbour seals
and porpoises. There are no records of transients in our study area having success-
fully killed large whales such as adult gray ( Eschrichtius robustus ), humpback
( Megaptera novaeangliae ), fi n ( Balaenoptera physalus ), or blue ( Balaenoptera
musculus ) whales. Indeed, foraging transients rarely show any reaction to these
potential prey species despite their frequent presence in their vicinity (Jefferson
et al. 1991 ). This indifference is likely related to the diffi culty in catching the fast-
swimming fi n and blue whales and the risk of injury posed by defensive responses
from gray and, especially, humpback whales (Ford and Reeves 2008 ). Gray whale
calves and juveniles, however, are frequently targeted by foraging transients in cen-
tral California (Ternullo and Black 2002 ) and around the eastern Aleutian Islands,
Alaska (Barrett-Lennard et al. 2005 ; Matkin et al. 2007 ).
Offshore killer whales are the least known of the three lineages in the region.
They have been observed consuming a probable Pacifi c halibut (Jones 2006 ) and
possibly blue sharks ( Prionace glauca ) and Chinook salmon (Dahlheim et al. 2008 ).
Fig. 4.3 A female transient killer whale hunting for the preferred prey of this ecotype, harbour
seals. (Photograph by J. Towers)
J.K.B. Ford and G.M. Ellis
83
Stomach contents of a killer whale identifi ed as an offshore by mtDNA analysis
included two carcharinid sharks and two opah ( Lampris regius , a large pelagic tele-
ost fi sh; Morin et al. 2006 ). Recently, we observed offshore killer whales feeding on
multiple Pacifi c sleeper sharks ( Somniosus pacifi cus ) (Ford et al. 2011 ). A diet con-
sisting largely of sharks, with their abrasive skin, might explain the extreme tooth
wear that appears to be common in offshore killer whales (Ford et al. 2011 ). Stable
isotope ratios and fatty acid profi les determined from skin and blubber biopsy sam-
ples also suggest that the diet of offshore killer whales is distinct from that of either
resident or transient lineages (Herman et al. 2005 ; Krahn et al. 2007 ).
4.5 Social Organization
Similar to most delphinids, killer whales are highly social, group-living animals.
However, the social structure of resident, transient, and offshore killer whales dif-
fers considerably, and these differences appear to be related to and are likely deter-
mined by their respective ecological specializations. Resident killer whales live in
matrilines that are exceptionally stable in composition. A typical matriline is com-
posed of an older female, her sons and daughters, and the offspring of her daughters.
Because longevity of females can reach 80 years and females have their fi rst viable
calf at about 14 years (Olesiuk et al. 2005 ), a matriline may contain as many as four
generations of maternally related individuals. More than 30 years of demographic
data have demonstrated that dispersal from the matriline is virtually absent in resi-
dent killer whales—both males and females remain in their natal group for life
(Bigg et al. 1990 ; Ford et al. 2000 ; Ellis et al. 2007 ). In no case has an individual
whale been observed to leave its matriline and join another on a long-term basis,
other than in a few rare cases involving orphans.
Members of resident matrilines travel together and they seldom separate by more
than a few kilometers or for more than a few hours. Contact is maintained among
matriline members by the exchange of discrete, stereotyped underwater calls that
are unique to the group (Ford 1989 , 1991 ; Miller et al. 2004 ). Matrilines frequently
travel in the company of certain other matrilines that are closely related, based on
high degrees of call similarity, and likely shared a common maternal ancestor in the
recent past. Matrilines that spend the majority of their time together are designated
as pods (Bigg et al. 1990 ). Pods are less stable than matrilines, and member matri-
lines may spend days or weeks apart. However, matrilines still spend more time
with others from their pod than with those from other pods. In British Columbia,
resident pods are on average composed of three matrilines (range = 1–11; Ford et al.
2000 ), with a mean total size of 18 whales (range = 2–49; Ford et al. 2000 ). Residents
often form large temporary aggregations involving multiple matrilines and pods,
especially at times when prey densities are high.
A level of social structure above the resident pod is the clan, which is defi ned by
patterns of call similarity. Clans are composed of pods that share a portion of their
repertoire of stereotyped calls. Different clans have no calls in common. Pods
4 You Are What You Eat: Foraging Specializations and Their Infl uence
84
belonging to a clan are likely descendants of an ancestral pod, and their acoustic
similarities refl ect this common heritage. Call repertoires are traditions passed on
across generations by vocal learning, and calls actively or passively change in struc-
ture or use over time. Calls are retained within the lineage because of the lack of
dispersal from matrilines. Clans are sympatric, and the two to nine pods that make
up each clan frequently travel together as well as with pods from different clans
(Ford 1991 ; Yurk et al. 2002 ).
Transient killer whale society lacks the closed, strictly matrilineal structure seen
in residents. Transients usually travel in groups of two to six individuals, much
smaller than the typical size of resident matrilines and pods. In contrast to residents,
offspring often disperse from the natal matriline for extended periods or perma-
nently (Bigg et al. 1987 ; Ford and Ellis 1999 ; Baird and Whitehead 2000 ). Female
offspring usually leave their natal group around the time of sexual maturity and
travel with other transient groups. These young females usually give birth to their
rst calf shortly after dispersing. Once dispersed, these females may rejoin their
natal matriline occasionally, but generally only for brief periods after they have
calves of their own. Male dispersal does take place, but the pattern is less clear
because of uncertainty in the status of many individuals in the population. The
range of transients appears to extend beyond our study area, possibly into offshore
waters, and gaps of many years can occur between sightings of individuals (Ford
and Ellis 1999 ; Ford et al. 2007 ). There are numerous cases of mothers and a single
adult son staying together for decades, but few where a mother and more than one
adult son have persisted. Male siblings may disperse from these groups at puberty,
but if so they must leave our study area as none has been resighted after disappearing
from the natal group, either as a member of another group or as a lone individual.
All lone adult males found in the study area appear to have lost their mothers
through mortality. These individuals often travel alone or associate with a variety of
different transient matrilines, but rarely with other lone males. The associations of
transient matrilines are very dynamic, and they do not form consistent groupings
equivalent to resident pods. Also, in contrast to residents, transient populations do
not seem to be acoustically subdivided into clans. Instead, all transients in a popula-
tion share a distinctive set of calls, although some additional calls or variants of
shared calls may be specifi c to a subregion or portion of the population (Ford 1984 ;
Deecke et al. 2005 ) .
The typically small size of transient groups is likely a result of the foraging strat-
egy of this lineage. Transients generally hunt other marine mammals with stealth:
they swim quietly to prevent detection by their acoustically sensitive prey, and
attack using the element of surprise (Ford 1984 ; Barrett-Lennard et al. 1996 ). This
strategy no doubt constrains group size, as larger groups such as those of residents
would increase the probability of the predators being detected by their prey. Small
groups may also be most energetically effi cient for transients when hunting smaller
marine mammals such as harbour seals (Baird and Dill 1996 ).
A s w i t h m o s t d e t a i l s o f t h e i r l i f e h i s t o r y a n d b e h a v i o r , t h e s o c i a l o r g a n i z a t i o n o f
offshore killer whales is poorly understood. Their group sizes tend to be relatively
large, certainly much larger than those of transients, and possibly larger on average
J.K.B. Ford and G.M. Ellis
85
than residents. Groups of 2 to 100 or more individuals have been documented in
encounters with offshores off the coast of British Columbia, with about half involv-
ing 20 or more individuals. These larger groups probably represent temporary gath-
erings of smaller social units, possibly related to prey density as in residents. We
have documented persistent bonds lasting more than a decade between females and
adult males, which likely represent mothers and their adult sons. However, we have
not observed long-term associations between reproductive females, as seen in the
multi-generation matrilines of residents. This nding suggests a dynamic society
with dispersal from the natal matriline as in transients, but frequent formation of
larger aggregations as in residents.
4.6 Seasonality and Habitat Use
All three lineages of killer whales are found in coastal waters of the northeastern
Pacifi c throughout the year, but there are signifi cant differences in their seasonality
and patterns of habitat use. The seasonal movements of resident killer whales are
closely tied to those of their primary prey. Several studies have demonstrated cor-
relations between resident whale occurrence in nearshore waters and the aggregate
abundance of multiple salmon species migrating through nearshore waters to coastal
spawning rivers in British Columbia and Washington State (Heimlich-Boran 1986 ;
Guinet 1990 ; Nichol and Shackleton 1996 ). However, these analyses were under-
taken before it was known that these whales forage selectively for Chinook salmon
and shun the smaller but much more abundant pink and sockeye salmon (Ford et al.
1998 ; Ford and Ellis 2006 ). Correlations of whale occurrence with these abundant
salmonids are thus incidental, and the whales are instead attracted by migrating
Chinook salmon, which pass through these migratory corridors in lower numbers
but concurrently with the smaller species. Movements of resident killer whales in
this area during October and November are clearly associated with fall migrating
chum salmon, which the whales do consume (Nichol and Shackleton 1996 ; Ford
and Ellis 2006 ). Interestingly, a different population of resident killer whales in
south-central Alaska moves into Prince William Sound during midsummer, where
they forage extensively for coho salmon ( Oncorhynchus kisutch ) (Saulitis et al.
2000 ). Neither Chinook nor chum salmon are common in this area at this time of
year, although these same whales feed on Chinook and chum salmon in other areas
and times of year (C. Matkin, personal communication).
During winter and spring, resident whales mostly vacate their summer habitat in
nearshore waters and appear to range widely along the outer exposed coast. It is
likely that the whales maintain their focus on Chinook salmon prey during this time
of year. Most other salmonid species are pelagic and unavailable to the whales
during this time of year, but nonmigratory or early spawning runs of Chinook are
found in these outer coast waters (Ford and Ellis 2006 ). Residents may also increase
their consumption of non-salmonid species such as Pacifi c halibut during winter
and spring.
4 You Are What You Eat: Foraging Specializations and Their Infl uence
86
Compared to residents, transient killer whales have a relatively uniform pattern
of occurrence in nearshore waters throughout the year, likely because their primary
prey species—harbour seal, harbour porpoise, Dall’s porpoise, and Steller sea
lion—are nonmigratory and available in all months of the year. However, there is an
interesting seasonal peak in local occurrence along the west coast of North America
that appears to coincide with the pupping season of harbour seals. In Glacier Bay,
the northern limit of the range of the so-called “West Coast” transient population
(~58°30N latitude), transient whale occurrence peaks in June and July ( Matkin
et al. 2007 ). Near the southern extent of their range, around Vancouver Island (~48°
to 51°N latitude), there is an obvious peak in occurrence during August and
September (Baird and Dill 1995 ). Both these periods coincide with local peaks of
pupping and weaning of harbour seals, which exhibits a latitudinal cline in timing
along the West Coast (Temte et al. 1991 ). Pups are likely easy and abundant prey for
transients, and the whales appear to move in accordance with their seasonal
availability.
Offshore killer whales appear to exhibit a diffuse seasonal shift in distribution
along the West Coast of North America. The majority of sightings in the southern
portion of their known range, off central and south California, have been recorded
during fall and winter (September to March; Dahlheim et al. 2008 ). Sightings in
Alaska, the northern portion of the range of offshore killer whales, have taken place
only during April to September, but there is minimal observer effort during winter
in this area (Dahlheim et al. 2008 ; C. Matkin, personal communication). Off British
Columbia, roughly the latitudinal midpoint of their range, sightings of offshores
have been recorded in all months. Without a better understanding of the primary
prey species of offshore killer whales, it is not possible to interpret the signifi cance
of this apparent seasonal distribution shift.
Differences are also apparent in ner-scale patterns of habitat use by the three
killer whale lineages. Residents congregate during summer and fall in core feeding
areas in locations where geography and tidal currents act to concentrate migratory
salmon (Heimlich-Boran 1988 ; Nichol and Shackleton 1996 ; Saulitis et al. 2000 ;
Ford 2006 ). During the peak of salmon abundance, the majority of matrilines in a
resident population may gather in these core areas, and individual matrilines or pods
may spend weeks in a relatively restricted area that the whales could transit in a day
or two. There are distinctions among the movement patterns of different resident
pods within a population’s overall range. Although most resident groups may be
observed in most parts of the range, particular pods and matrilines have preferred
areas that they frequent more often than other groups (Osborne 1999 ; Ford 1991 ,
2006 ; Ford et al. 2000 ; Hauser et al. 2007 ), likely because of the benefi t of foraging
in familiar areas where individuals have experience in locating local concentrations
of prey.
While in their core summer feeding areas, resident killer whales spend
50–65 % of their time foraging (Heimlich-Boran 1988 ; F o r d 1989 ; Morton 1990 ).
Between foraging bouts, the whales group together and socialize or rest, which
together represent about 30–40 % of their time. In at least two resident populations,
the whales may also spend considerable time rubbing their bodies on certain
J.K.B. Ford and G.M. Ellis
87
shelving, pebble beaches that have been used traditionally for many years (Ford 1989 ;
Matkin et al. 1999 ).
In contrast to resident whales, transient killer whales typically do not remain for
long in any particular location. They are almost constantly on the move, swimming
from one prey hotspot to the next. Because of their apparent reliance on stealth for
capturing marine mammals, it is no doubt more productive for transients to hunt
elsewhere once potential prey is alert to their presence. By covering 75–150 km of
coastline per day, transients tend to undergo more frequent extensive travel through-
out their range than do residents. Nonetheless, as with residents, at least some tran-
sient groups have preferred areas within the overall population range, where local
knowledge of the location of pinniped haulouts or predictable concentrations of
small cetaceans may serve to improve hunting effi ciency (Ford and Ellis 1999 ).
Compared to residents, transients dedicate considerably more time to foraging and
traveling (>75 % of their activity budget: Morton 1990 ; Baird and Dill 1995 ).
Socializing and resting activities, which comprise about one-third of the activity
budget for residents, are seldom exhibited by transients (<10 % of activities; Morton
1990 ; Baird and Dill 1995 ; Barrett-Lennard et al. 1996 ; Deecke et al. 2005 ). Beach
rubbing has not been reported for transients.
Details of habitat use by offshore killer whales are not yet clear because of the
comparatively infrequent encounters with this population. Long-distance move-
ments appear to be undertaken frequently by offshore whales. Several identifi ed
individuals have been observed at the extremities of the population’s known range,
which extends more than 4,000 km from the Aleutian Islands to Southern California
(Dahlheim et al. 2008 ). Any potential habitats that may be used preferentially by a
subset of the offshore population, and what prey species may drive their move-
ments, have yet to be described.
4.7 Foraging Behavior
The distinct diets of killer whale lineages are associated with corresponding con-
trasts in their foraging behavior. When foraging, members of a resident killer whale
matriline or pod spread out, often over areas of several square kilometers, with
individuals or small subgroups diving and surfacing independently while swimming
generally in the same direction. They maintain contact and likely coordinate move-
ments through the frequent exchange of loud underwater calls, which are effective
to ranges of 10–25 km (Ford 1989 ; Miller et al. 2004 ; Miller 2006 ). When foraging
in coastal inlets, channels, and straits, individuals and small maternal groups usually
forage along the shoreline, while other whales, particularly mature males, forage
alone farther from shore and in deeper water. Foraging resident whales dive for
2–3 min (Ford 1989 ; Morton 1990 ) to depths typically less than 30 m, but occasion-
ally to more than 150 m (Baird et al. 2005a ). These depths are similar to those used
by their primary prey species, Chinook salmon (Candy and Quinn 1999 ).
4 You Are What You Eat: Foraging Specializations and Their Infl uence
88
Foraging resident killer whales fi nd prey using echolocation, which may be
effective for detecting Chinook salmon at ranges of 100 m or more (Au et al. 2004 ).
By foraging in loosely dispersed groups, the detection rate of scattered salmon is
likely enhanced. However, residents whales do not appear to cooperatively herd or
capture prey. Rather, prey capture is undertaken primarily by individuals with occa-
sional cooperation from offspring, siblings, or other close matrilineal kin. The
majority of salmonid prey items captured by adult females and subadults are brought
to the surface, where they are broken up for sharing within the matriline or for pro-
visioning young offspring (Ford and Ellis 2006 ). Adult males usually capture and
consume salmonid prey alone.
In contrast to residents, transient killer whales forage in near silence in an appar-
ent attempt to minimize detection by their acoustically sensitive marine mammal
prey (Ford 1984 ; Morton 1990 ; Barrett-Lennard et al. 1996 ; Deecke et al. 2005 ).
Transients rarely exchange underwater calls while hunting for prey (Deecke et al.
2005 ), and echolocation click production is also greatly suppressed (Barrett-
Lennard et al. 1996 ). Both pinniped and cetacean prey have excellent hearing abili-
ties at the frequencies used by killer whales for calling and echolocation and could
detect and potentially evade approaching transients if they were to vocalize (Barrett-
Lennard et al. 1996 ; Deecke et al. 2005 ). As vocalizing would likely incur high
costs in terms of reduced rates of prey capture, transients appear to depend on pas-
sive listening to detect and approach prey from a distance, likely cueing on the
animals’ vocalizations or swimming noises (Barrett-Lennard and Heise 2006 ).
There is little cost associated with the production of underwater sounds for resident
whales because salmonids and most other fi sh have relatively low hearing sensitiv-
ity to such frequencies and are unlikely to detect approaching whales at a distance
(Barrett-Lennard et al. 1996 ; Deecke et al. 2005 ).
T r a n s i e n t k i l l e r w h a l e s e m p l o y t w o f a i r l y d i s t i n c t m o d e s o f f o r a g i n g : n e a r s h o r e
and open water. When foraging nearshore, the whales swim in relatively tight groups
and follow the contour of the shoreline, round headlands, and enter bays without
hesitation (Morton 1990 ; B a r r e t t - L e n n a r d e t a l . 1996 ) . T h e y o f t e n c i r c l e s m a l l i s l e t s
and reefs, particularly those that serve as pinniped haulouts. Resident whales, in
contrast, forage along more direct routes, usually swimming from headland to head-
land. Dive durations of foraging transient whales are typically twice the duration of
the 2- to 3-min dives of residents, and may exceed 10 min (Morton 1990 ) . N e a r s h o r e
foraging is generally associated with capture of pinniped prey, particularly harbour
seals (Baird and Dill 1995 ; B a r r e t t - L e n n a r d e t a l . 1996 ; S a u l i t i s e t a l . 2000 ) . W h e n
foraging in open water, transient groups spread out over a larger area, with individu-
als swimming several hundred meters apart, often roughly abreast. Most prey cap-
tured during open water foraging are porpoises or dolphins, but seals or sea lions
may also be taken (Barrett-Lennard et al. 1996 ; S a u l i t i s e t a l . 2000 ) .
Transients share the majority of their prey (Baird and Dill 1995 ), likely to an
even greater extent than do residents because of the larger body masses of most
marine mammal prey items. Transient group members frequently use cooperative
hunting tactics to catch and subdue their prey (Baird and Dill 1995 ; Ford et al.
1998 ). Predation on Steller sea lions, for example, can be extended events that
J.K.B. Ford and G.M. Ellis
89
may entail risk of injury to the attacking whales. These prey can be large (up to
1,000 kg in males) with sizeable canine teeth that can infl ict signifi cant wounds
during defensive or retaliatory actions. Groups of transient killer whales attack
single sea lions in open water by circling the animal so as to prevent it from reach-
ing shore, while individuals take turns rushing toward the prey and ramming it or
striking it with their tail fl ukes. This action may continue for 1–2 h until the animal
is suffi ciently debilitated so that it can be safely grasped, drowned, and shared
among group members. Transients may also hunt fast-swimming Dall’s porpoise
using a cooperative “tag team” tactic where individuals take turns chasing the prey
animal to exhaustion. Transients have been also been observed to herd groups of
50+ Pacifi c white- sided dolphins into confi ned or shallow bays where individuals
can be readily captured. Transients hunt these diffi cult-to-capture species in sig-
nifi cantly larger groups than when foraging for the smaller harbour seals (Ford
et al. 1998 ). These groups often represent temporary associations of smaller, stable
social units.
4.8 Acoustic Communication
As do most delphinids, killer whales have a well-developed acoustic communica-
tion system. However, as noted earlier, the types and extent of vocalization show
major differences among lineages. Resident killer whales frequently exchange stri-
dent calls from stable repertoires of a dozen or more call types. These learned call
types or their variants are specifi c to clans, pods, and matrilines, and thus encode the
matrilineal genealogy of individuals (Ford 1991 ). This specifi city likely enhances
the effectiveness of these calls as intragroup contact signals, especially when whales
are dispersed and traveling in association with other matrilines or pods. These
group-specifi c dialects may also play a role as a behavioral mechanism to prevent
inbreeding. As there is no dispersal from the natal matriline, resident killer whales
would be at considerable risk of inbreeding without a reliable means of distinguish-
ing between kin and non-kin mating partners. Group-specifi c call repertoires appear
to serve such a function (Ford 1991 ), and genetic studies have shown that resident
whales mate with individuals that are outside the pod or clan and are acoustically
dissimilar (Barrett-Lennard 2000 ).
Although transient killer whales spend much of their time foraging for marine
mammals in silence, they become highly vocal while attacking and consuming their
prey (Ford 1984 ; Deecke et al. 2005 ). Calling at such times likely carries little cost
as stealth is no longer needed, and it may help coordinate cooperative attack tactics
within the group or serve other social functions after the kill is made. Similar to resi-
dent killer whales, transients have repertoires of distinctive stereotyped call types.
Unlike residents, however, these repertoires generally do not differ among groups.
As there is dispersal from the natal matriline in this ecotype, group-specifi c calls
would not be expected. Also, dispersal reduces the risk of inbreeding, so the require-
ment for an acoustic outbreeding mechanism may be reduced in transients.
4 You Are What You Eat: Foraging Specializations and Their Infl uence
90
The sh-eating offshore killer whales are as vocal as resident killer whales.
Preliminary analyses indicate that offshores produce stereotyped calls that are dis-
tinct from any of those of residents or transients, but it is not yet known whether any
calls are specifi c to particular groups. As our understanding of the social dynamics
of this poorly known lineage improves, patterns of call use should become clearer.
4.9 Specializations in Other Regions
Field studies in other global regions have provided additional evidence that ecologi-
cal specializations are typical of most killer whale populations. Although these
populations are not as well known as resident and transients in the eastern North
Pacifi c, it is apparent that at least in some cases their specializations have had simi-
lar infl uences on patterns of social structure, behavior, and vocal activity. Off the
northern coast of Norway, a population of killer whales moves seasonally in relation
to their primary prey, the Atlantic herring ( Clupea harengus ) (Similä 1997 ). In
coastal fjords where herring congregate in high densities during fall and winter, the
whales employ a cooperative foraging tactic known as “carousel feeding” to capture
these small schooling fi shes: this involves a group of whales encircling and herding
a school of herring into a tight ball close to the surface. Once the school is concen-
trated, individuals dive under the school and strike it with their tail ukes. Fish
stunned directly by the physical blow from the fl ukes or the associated loud cavita-
tion sound are then eaten individually (Similä and Ugarte 1993 ; Simon et al. 2005 ).
These herring-eating killer whales appear to live in matrilineally organized pods
similar in size to those of sh-feeding resident killer whales, but it is not known
whether they share the same extreme stability (Similä 1997 ). They are highly vocal
and have pod-specifi c call repertoires as observed in resident killer whales (Strager
1995 ), which would suggest a stable pod structure.
In the Strait of Gibraltar, a small population of killer whales appears to specialize
on predation of bluefi n tuna ( Thunnus thynnus ) as the fi shes enter and exit the
Mediterranean Sea during their breeding migration (Reeves and Notarbartolo di
Sciara 2006 ). To catch these swift tuna, the whales employ an endurance-exhaustion
technique involving protracted chases at swimming speeds of 12–14 km/h for peri-
ods of 30–40 min (Guinet et al. 2007 ). Killer whales can sustain suffi cient swim-
ming speeds necessary to catch small to medium (0.8–1.5 m) tuna using this
technique but appear unable to match the swimming ability of larger fi sh.
On the coast of Patagonia, Argentina, a small population of killer whales uses a
novel, but risky, hunting technique that involves intentional stranding in the shal-
lows to capture young southern sea lions ( Otaria fl avescens ) and southern elephant
seals ( Mirounga leonina ) at the water’s edge (Lopez and Lopez 1985 ). Whales hunt
cooperatively and share their prey with others in the group (Hoelzel 1991 ). A simi-
lar beaching tactic is used by killer whales in the sub-Antarctic Crozet Islands when
hunting southern elephant seal pups (Guinet 1992 ). As do mammal-hunting tran-
sients in the northeastern Pacifi c, whales in both these Southern Hemisphere
J.K.B. Ford and G.M. Ellis
91
locations have small group sizes, hunt mostly in silence, and appear to locate prey
by passive listening (Guinet 1992 ; J.K.B.F., unpublished data).
Three distinct forms of killer whales—known as types A, B, and C—have been
described in circumpolar waters of the Antarctic (Pitman and Ensor 2003 ). These
sympatric forms differ in pigmentation patterns, genetic structure (mtDNA
sequences), patterns of habitat use, and diet (Pitman and Ensor 2003 ; Krahn et al.
2008 ; LeDuc et al. 2008 ). Type A killer whales are found mostly in ice-free waters
where they apparently feed mainly on cetaceans, particularly Antarctic minke
whales ( Balaenoptera bonaerensis ). Type B whales forage primarily in loose pack
ice and appear to specialize on seals. These whales exhibit a novel hunting tactic in
which group members coordinate their swimming movements to create a large wave
that washes seals off ice fl oes (Visser et al. 2008 ). Type C whales inhabit dense pack
ice and appear to be fi sh feeders, having been observed preying on Antarctic tooth-
sh ( Dissostichus mawsoni ) (Pitman and Ensor 2003 ; Krahn et al. 2008 ). They are
substantially smaller than other Antarctic killer whales, with adults approximately
1–3 m shorter in length than type A individuals (Pitman et al. 2007 ). Type C whales
tend to have larger group sizes than mammal-hunting types A and B, which is con-
sistent with the pattern of group sizes versus prey type in the northeastern Pacifi c.
Unfortunately, too little is known about these Antarctic ecotypes to determine
whether ecological specialization has infl uenced their social structure, behavior,
and acoustics in ways similar to those of lineages in other regions.
4.10 Conclusions
The killer whale is a highly versatile social predator that has evolved to successfully
occupy a variety of specialized ecological niches in the world’s oceans. In so doing,
this species has assumed a variety of distinct lifestyles that have been shaped by
these ecological specializations. In the eastern North Pacifi c, the three killer whale
lineages have distinct patterns of seasonal distribution, group size, social organiza-
tion, foraging behavior, and acoustic activity, which can be related to their preferred
type of prey and the strategies the animals use to acquire it. Some similar patterns
are apparent among killer whales in other regions, although a lack of fi eld data pre-
vents a more complete assessment of the parallels between ecotype and life history
or behavior for these populations.
Although different killer whale lineages may be genetically distinct, there is no
evidence that dietary preferences result from any genetic predisposition. Globally,
there is no congruence between killer whale ecotype and genotype (Hoelzel et al.
2002 ; LeDuc et al. 2008 ). Instead, ecological specializations appear to represent
behavioral traditions that likely evolved independently in different regions. It is
plausible that ecological divergence could arise in sympatry with, for example, the
innovation of a novel foraging tactic in a particular matriline that allowed predation
on a new type of prey. If this matriline and its descendants became further special-
ized on this prey type, rates of association with other groups that do not adopt this
4 You Are What You Eat: Foraging Specializations and Their Infl uence
92
new diet may diminish over time, leading to social segregation and reproductive
isolation. Such a process could lead ultimately to speciation. Resident and transient
killer whale lineages in the northeastern Pacifi c have been suggested to represent
incipient species (Baird et al. 1992 ) and Antarctic type A, B, and C killer whales to
represent distinct species (LeDuc et al. 2008 ).
There are still many questions concerning ecological specialization in killer
whales that remain to be answered. For example, to what extent might specializa-
tions constrain a lineage’s ability to switch to alternative prey species in a changing
environment? The preferences for fi sh and marine mammal prey exhibited by resi-
dent and transient killer whales, respectively, are extremely strong, and there is no
evidence that one ecotype ever switches to the prey type of the other or has the
behavioral fl exibility to do so. Marine mammals in coastal waters of the northeast-
ern Pacifi c can discriminate between lineages and will fl ee from transients but show
indifference to residents (Ford and Ellis 1999 ; Deecke et al. 2002 ), suggesting that
if residents ever hunt marine mammals, it must occur extremely rarely. The suite of
specialized behaviors that make resident killer whales adept at locating and catching
Chinook salmon likely would be ineffective for hunting marine mammals. Transients
would similarly be ill equipped to adopt a fi sh-feeding lifestyle.
The extent of dietary fl exibility of killer whales has implications for their poten-
tial role in driving marine ecosystem dynamics. It has been proposed that a shift to
sea otter predation by mammal-hunting killer whales in the Aleutian Islands resulted
in a precipitous decline in sea otter abundance that started in the mid-1980s (Estes
et al. 1998 ). This shift is thought to be a response to reduced availability of the
whale’s presumed primary prey in the region, harbour seals and Steller sea lions.
In an extension of this hypothesis, Springer et al. ( 2003 ) postulated that the decline
of sea otters was the last in a series of population collapses of prey species of
mammal- hunting killer whales in the northern Gulf of Alaska that was triggered by
the decimation of the great whales by industrial whaling in the nineteenth and twen-
tieth centuries. This hypothesis has been challenged on various grounds (Trites et al.
2007 ; Wade et al. 2007 ), particularly because there is no evidence that the great
whales (especially adults) have ever played an important role in the diet of killer
whales (Mizroch and Rice 2006 ; Ford and Reeves 2008). Although it may be
possible that predation by killer whales could result in depletion of targeted prey
species, dietary specializations could have signifi cant constraints on the directions
that subsequent prey shifts may take.
To date, most ecologically specialized killer whale populations, including sym-
patric fi sh-eating and mammal-eating ecotypes, have been described in highly pro-
ductive cold temperate or polar waters, likely the result of the diversity of abundant
prey types available in these high latitudes, which has provided the opportunity for
niche partitioning. It may well be that killer whales in less productive tropical or
subtropical waters are generalist predators that include a greater variety of prey in
their diets (Baird et al. 2005b ). For example, a high incidence of killer whale teeth
scars on humpback whales using breeding grounds off the west coast of Mexico
suggests that predation in this area, especially on calves, may be extensive (Steiger
et al.
2008 ). This prey resource is seasonal, however, as humpbacks only occupy
J.K.B. Ford and G.M. Ellis
93
these breeding grounds for 3 to 5 months in winter. Because there is no evidence
that killer whales follow migrating humpback whale mothers and calves to their
high-latitude feeding grounds, it is likely that the predators shift to alternative prey
species for the remainder of the year.
Globally, killer whales form a mosaic of distinct populations, some overlapping
and others geographically discrete, that are ecologically specialized to greater or
lesser degrees. Each population is likely to have foraging tactics, activity patterns,
social organization, and acoustic behavior that have been shaped by its dietary spe-
cialty. Highly specialized populations can be expected to have lifestyles that are
closely adapted to their foraging strategy, whereas more generalist populations may
be relatively less constrained by any particular prey type. In certain regions, such as
the northeastern Pacifi c, some parts of this mosaic are becoming fairly clear. In
other regions, such as the Antarctic, a fascinating picture is emerging but signifi cant
knowledge gaps remain to be lled. In regions where killer whales are little studied,
such as in sparsely inhabited tropical waters, there is much yet to be discovered.
Only when all the components of this global mosaic of killer whale populations
have been described will we have a complete appreciation of the range of ecological
specializations and lifestyles of this multifaceted and resourceful predator.
Acknowledgments We thank R. Baird, C. Chapman, V. Iriarte, C. Matkin, and J. Watson for
helpful comments on earlier drafts of this chapter, and M. Malleson and J. Towers for kindly
allowing us to use their photographs.
References
Au WWL, Ford JKB, Horne JK, Newman Allman KA (2004) Echolocation signals of free-ranging
killer whales ( Orcinus orca ) and modeling of foraging for Chinook salmon ( Oncorhynchus
tshawytscha ). Acoust Soc Am 115(2):901–909
Baird RW, Abrams PA, Dill LM (1992) Possible indirect interactions between transient and resi-
dent killer whales: implications for the evolution of foraging specializations in the genus
Orcinus. Oecologia, 89:125–132
Baird RW, Dill LM (1995) Occurrence and behavior of transient killer whales: seasonal and pod-
specifi c variability, foraging, and prey handling. Can J Zool 73:1300–1311
Baird RW, Dill LM (1996) Ecological and social determinants of group size in transient killer
whales. Behav Ecol 7:408–416
Baird RW, Hanson MB, Dill LM (2005a) Factors infl uencing the diving behaviour of sh-eating
killer whales: sex differences and diel and interannual variation in diving rates. Can J Zool
83:257–267
Baird RW, McSweeney DJ, Bane C, Barlow J, Salden DR, Antoine LK, LeDuc RG, Webster DL
(2005b) Killer whales in Hawaiian waters: information on population identity and feeding
habits. Pac Sci 60:523–530
B a i r d R W , W h i t e h e a d H ( 2 0 0 0 ) S o c i a l o r g a n i s a t i o n o f m a m m a l e a t i n g k i l l e r w h a l e s : g r o u p s t a b i l -
ity and dispersal patterns. Can J Zool 78:2096–2105
Barrett-Lennard LG (2000) Population structure and mating systems of northeastern pacifi c killer
whales. Ph.D. Thesis, University of British Columbia, Vancouver
Barrett-Lennard LG, Ford JKB, Heise KA (1996) The mixed blessing of echolocation: differences
in sonar use by fi sh-eating and mammal-eating killer whales. Anim Behav 51:553–565
4 You Are What You Eat: Foraging Specializations and Their Infl uence
94
Barrett-Lennard LG, Heise K (2006) The natural history and ecology of killer whales. In: Estes JA,
DeMaster DP, Doak DF, Williams TM, Brownell RL (eds) Whales, whaling and ocean ecosys-
tems. University of California Press, Berkeley, pp 163–173
Barrett-Lennard LG, Matkin CO, Ellifrit DK, Durban J, Mazzuca L (2005) Black and white versus
gray: estimating kill rates, consumption rates, and population-level impacts of transient killer
whales feeding on gray whales. Abstract, 16th Biennial Conference on the Biology of Marine
Mammals, San Diego, 12–16 December 2005, p 26
Bigg MA (1982) An assessment of killer whale ( Orcinus orca ) stocks off Vancouver Island, British
Columbia. Rep Int Whal Comm 32:655–666
Bigg MA, Ellis GM, Ford JKB, Balcomb KC (1987) Killer whales: a study of their identifi cation,
genealogy and natural history in British Columbia and Washington State. Phantom Press,
Nanaimo
Bigg MA, Ford JKB, Ellis GM (1985) Two sympatric forms of killer whales off British Columbia
and Washington. In: Sixth Biennial Conference on the Biology of Marine Mammals, Vancouver,
Canada, November 1985
Bigg MA, MacAskie IB, Ellis G (1976) Abundance and movements of killer whales off eastern
and southern Vancouver Island with comments on management. Unpublished report, Arctic
Biological Station, Department of Fisheries and Environment, Ste Anne-de-Bellevue
B i g g M A , O l e s i u k P F , E l l i s G M , F o r d J K B , B a l c o m b K C I I I ( 1 9 9 0 ) S o c i a l o r g a n i z a t i o n a n d g e n e -
alogy of resident killer whales ( Orcinus orca ) in the coastal waters of British Columbia and
Washington State. Rep Int Whal Comm Spec Issue 12:383–405
Black NA, Schulman-Janiger A, Ternullo RL, Guerrero-Ruiz M (1997) Killer whales of California
and western Mexico: a catalog of photo-identifi ed individuals. NOAA Technical Memorandum
NOAA-TM-NMFS-SWFSC-247
Candy JR, Quinn TP (1999) Behaviour of adult Chinook salmon ( Oncorhynchus tshawytscha ) in
British Columbia coastal waters determined from ultrasonic telemetry. Can J Zool 77:
1161–1169
Dahlheim ME, Schulman-Janiger A, Black N, Ternullo R, Ellifrit D, Balcomb KC (2008) Eastern
temperate North Pacifi c offshore killer whales ( Orcinus orca ): occurrence, movements, and
insights into feeding ecology. Mar Mamm Sci 24:719–729
Deecke VB, Ford JKB, Slater P (2005) The vocal behaviour of mammal-eating killer whales: com-
municating with costly calls. Anim Behav 69:395–405
Deecke VB, Slater P, Ford JKB (2002) Selective habituation shapes acoustic predator recognition
in harbour seals. Nature (Lond) 420:171–173
Dieckmann U, Doebeli M (1999) On the origin of species by sympatric speciation. Nature (Lond)
400:354–357
Ellis GM, Ford JKB, Towers JR (2007) Northern resident killer whales in British Columbia: photo-
identifi cation catalogue 2007. Pacifi c Biological Station, Fisheries and Oceans Canada,
Nanaimo.
http://www.pac.dfo-mpo.gc.ca/sci/sa/cetacean/default_e.htm
Estes JA, Tinker MT, Williams TM, Doak DF (1998) Killer whale predation on sea otters linking
oceanic and nearshore ecosystems. Science 282:473–476
Ford JKB (1984) Call traditions and dialects of killer whales ( Orcinus orca ) in British Columbia.
Ph.D. Dissertation, University of British Columbia, Vancouver
Ford JKB (1989) Acoustic behaviour of resident killer whales ( Orcinus orca ) off Vancouver
Island, British Columbia. Can J Zool 67:727–745
Ford JKB (1991) Vocal traditions among resident killer whales ( Orcinus orca ) in coastal waters of
British Columbia. Can J Zool 69:1454–1483
Ford JKB (2002) Killer whale Orcinus orca . In: Perrin WF, Wursig B, Thewissen JGM (eds) The
encyclopedia of marine mammals. Academic, San Diego, pp 669–676
Ford JKB (2006) An assessment of critical habitats of resident killer whales in waters off the
Pacifi c coast of Canada. Fisheries and Oceans Canada, Canadian Science Advisory Secretariat,
Ottawa.
http://www.dfo-mpo.gc.ca/csas/
Ford JKB (2009) Killer whales Orcinus orca . In: Perrin WF, Würsig B, Thewissen JGM (eds) The
encyclopedia of marine mammals, 2nd edn. Academic, San Diego, pp 650–657
J.K.B. Ford and G.M. Ellis
95
Ford JKB, Ellis FM, Nichol LM (1992) Killer whales of the Queen Charlotte Islands: a prelimi-
nary study of the abundance, distribution, and population identity of Orcinus orca in the waters
of Haida Gwaii. Unpublished report, South Moresby/Haida Gwaii Haanas National Parks
Reserve, Canadian Parks Service
Ford JKB, Ellis GM (1999) Transients: mammal-hunting killer whales of British Columbia,
Washington, and Southeastern Alaska. UBC Press, Vancouver
Ford JKB, Ellis GM (2006) Selective foraging by fi sh-eating killer whales Orcinus orca in British
Columbia. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 316:185–199
Ford JKB, Ellis GM, Balcomb KC (2000) Killer whales: the natural history and genealogy of
Orcinus orca in the waters of British Columbia and Washington. UBC and University of
Washington Press, Vancouver
Ford JKB, Ellis GM, Barrett-Lennard LG, Morton AB, Palm RS, Balcomb KC III (1998) Dietary
specialization in two sympatric populations of killer whales ( Orcinus orca ) in coastal British
Columbia and adjacent waters. Can J Zool 76:1456–1471
Ford JKB, Ellis GM, Durban JW (2007) An assessment of the potential for recovery of West Coast
transient killer whales using coastal waters of British Columbia. Fisheries and Oceans Canada,
Canadian Science Advisory Secretariat, Ottawa.
http://www.dfo-mpo.gc.ca/csas/
Ford JKB, Ellis GM, Matkin DR, Balcomb KC, Briggs D, Morton AB (2005) Killer whale attacks
on minke whales: prey capture and antipredator tactics. Mar Mamm Sci 21:603–618
Ford JKB, Ellis GM, Matkin CO, Wetklo MH, Barrett-Lennard LG, Withler RE (2011) Shark
predation and tooth wear in a population of northeastern Pacifi c killer whales. Aquat Biol
11:213–224
Ford JKB, Reeves RR (2008) Fight or ight: antipredator strategies of baleen whales. Mammal
Review, 38:50–86
Forney KA, Wade PR (2006) Worldwide distribution and abundance of killer whales. In: Estes JA,
Demaster DP, Doak DF, Williams TM, Brownell RL Jr (eds) Whales, whaling, and ocean eco-
systems. University of California Press, Berkeley, pp 145–173
Futuyma DJ, Moreno G (1988) The evolution of ecological specialization. Annu Rev Ecol Syst
19:207–233
Guinet C (1990) Sympatrie des deux categories d’orques dans le detroit de Johnstone, Columbie
Britannique. Rev Ecol (Terre Vie) 45:25–34
Guinet C (1992) Hunting behaviour of killer whales ( Orcinus orca ) around Crozet Islands. Can J
Zool 70:1656–1667
Guinet C, Domenici P, de Stephanis R, Barrett-Lennard LG, Ford JKB, Verborgh P (2007) Killer
whale predation on bluefi n tuna: exploring the hypothesis of the endurance-exhaustion tech-
nique. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 347:111–119
Hauser DDW, Logsdon MG, Holmes EE, VanBlaricom GR, Osborne RW (2007) Summer distribu-
tion patterns of southern resident killer whales Orcinus orca : c o r e a r e a s a n d s p a t i a l s e g r e g a t i o n
of social groups. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 351:301–310
Heimlich-Boran J (1986) Fishery correlations with the occurrence of killer whales in greater Puget
Sound. In: Kirkevold BC, Lockard JS (eds) Behavioral biology of killer whales. Liss, New
York, pp 113–131
Heimlich-Boran JR (1988) Behavioural ecology of killer whales ( Orcinus orca ) in the Pacifi c
Northwest. Can J Zool 66:565–578
Heise K, Barrett-Lennard LG, Saulitis E, Matkin C, Bain D (2003) Examining the evidence for
killer whale predation on Steller sea lions in British Columbia and Alaska. Aquat Mamm
29:325–334
Herman DP, Burrows DG, Wade PR, Durban JW, Matkin CO, LeDuc R, Barrett-Lennard LG,
Krahn MM (2005) Feeding ecology of eastern North Pacifi c killer whales Orcinus orca from
fatty acid, stable isotope, and organochlorine analyses of blubber biopsies. Mar Ecol Prog Ser
302:275–291
Hoelzel AR (1991) Killer whale predation on marine mammals at Punta Norte, Argentina: food
sharing, provisioning and foraging strategy. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 29:197–204
4 You Are What You Eat: Foraging Specializations and Their Infl uence
96
Hoelzel AR, Dahlheim ME, Stern SJ (1998) Low genetic variation among killer whales ( Orcinus
orca ) in the eastern North Pacifi c, and differentiation between foraging specialists. J Hered
89:121–128
Hoelzel AR, Natoli A, Dahlheim ME, Olavarria C, Baird RW, Black NA (2002) Low worldwide
genetic diversity in the killer whale: implications for demographic history. Proc R Soc Lond
Ser B 269:1467–1473
Jefferson TA, Stacey PF, Baird RW (1991) A review of killer whale interactions with other marine
mammals: predation to co-existence. Mamm Rev 21:151–180
Jones IM (2006) A northeast Pacifi c offshore killer whale ( Orcinus orca ) feeding on a Pacifi c hali-
but ( Hippoglossus stenolepis ). Mar Mamm Sci 22:198–200
Krahn MM, Herman DP, Matkin CO, Durban JW, Barrett-Lennard L, Burrows DG, Dahlheim ME,
Black N, Leduc RG, Wade PR (2007) Use of chemical tracers in assessing the diet and foraging
regions of eastern North Pacifi c killer whales. Mar Environ Res 63:91–114
Krahn MM, Pitman RL, Burrows DG, Herman DP, Pearce RW (2008) Use of chemical tracers to
assess diet and persistent organic pollutants in Antarctic type C killer whales. Mar Mamm Sci
24:643–663
LeDuc RG, Robertson KM, Pitman RL (2008) Mitochondrial sequence divergence among
Antarctic killer whale ecotypes is consistent with multiple species. Biol Lett 4:426–429
Lopez JC, Lopez D (1985) Killer whales ( Orcinus orca ) of Patagonia, and their behavior of inten-
tional stranding while hunting nearshore. J Mammal 66:181–183
Martinez DR, Klinghammer E (1970) The behavior of the whales, Orcinus orca : a review of the
literature. Z Tierpsychol 27:828–839
Matkin C, Ellis G, Saulitis E, Barrett-Lennard L, Matkin D (1999) Killer whales of Southern
Alaska. North Gulf Oceanic Society, Homer
Matkin CO, Barrett-Lennard LG, Yurk H, Ellifrit D, Trites AW (2007a) Ecotypic variation and
predatory behavior among killer whales ( Orcinus orca ) off the eastern Aleutian Islands. Alaska
Fish Bull 105:74–87
Matkin DR, Straley JM, Gabriele CM (2007b) Killer whale feeding ecology and non-predatory
interactions with other marine mammals in the Glacier Bay region of Alaska. In: Piatt JF,
Gende SM (eds) Proceedings of the Fourth Glacier Bay Science Symposium, pp 155–158.
http://pubs.usgs.gov/sir/2007/5047/
McKinnon JS, Mori S, Blackman BK, David U, Kingsley DM, Jamieson L, Chou J, Schluter D
(2004) Evidence for ecology’s role in speciation. Nature (Lond) 429:294–298
Miller PJO, Shapiro AD, Tyack PL, Solow AR (2004) Call-type matching in vocal exchanges of
free-ranging resident killer whales, Orcinus orca . Anim Behav 67:1099–1107
Miller PJO (2006) Diversity in sound pressure levels and estimated active space of resident killer
whale vocalizations. J Comp Physiol A 192:449–459
Mizroch SA, Rice DW (2006) Have North Pacifi c killer whales switched prey species in response
to depletion of the great whale populations? Mar Ecol Prog Ser 310:235–246
Morin PA, Leduc RG, Robertson KM, Hedrick NM, Perrin WF, Etnier M, Wade P, Taylor BL
(2006) Genetic analysis of killer whale ( Orcinus orca ) historical bone and tooth samples to
identify western U.S. ecotypes. Mar Mamm Sci 22:897–909
Morin PA, Archer FI, Foote AD, Vilstrup J, Allen EE, Wade P, Durban J, Parsons KL, Pitman RL,
Li L, Bouffard P, Abel Nielsen SC, Rasmussen M, Willerslev E, Gilbert MTP, Harkins T (2010)
Complete mitochondrial genome phylogeographic analysis of killer whales ( Orcinus orca )
indicates multiple species. Genome Res 20:908–916
Morton AB (1990) A quantitative comparison of the behavior of resident and transient forms of the
killer whale off the central British Columbia coast. Rep Int Whal Comm Spec Issue
12:245–248
Nichol LM, Shackleton DM (1996) Seasonal movements and foraging behaviour of northern resi-
dent killer whales ( Orcinus orca ) in relation to the inshore distribution of salmon ( Oncorhynchus
spp.) in British Columbia. Can J Zool 74:983–991
J.K.B. Ford and G.M. Ellis
97
Olesiuk PF, Ellis GM, Ford JKB (2005) Life history and population dynamics of northern resident
killer whales ( Orcinus orca ) in British Columbia. Canadian Science Advisory Secretariat,
Fisheries and Oceans, Canada.
http://www.dfo-mpo.gc.ca/csas/
Osborne RW (1999) A historical ecology of Salish Sea “resident” killer whales ( Orcinus orca ),
with implications for management. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Victoria, British Columbia
Pike GC, MacAskie IB (1969) Marine mammals of British Columbia. Fish Res Board Can Bull
71:1–54
Pitman RL, Ensor P (2003) Three forms of killer whales ( Orcinus orca ) in Antarctic waters.
J Cetacean Res Manag 5:131–139
Pitman RL, Perryman WL, LeRoi D, Eilers E (2007) A dwarf form of killer whale in Antarctica.
J Mammal 88:43–48
Reeves RR, Notarbartolo Di Sciara G (2006) The status and distribution of cetaceans in the Black
Sea and Mediterranean Sea. IUCN Centre for Mediterranean Cooperation, Malaga, Spain.
http://cmsdata.iucn.org/downloads/status_distr_cet_blac_med.pdf
Robinson BW, Wilson DS, Shea GO (1996) Trade-offs of ecological specialization: an intraspe-
cifi c comparison of pumpkinseed sunfi sh phenotypes. Ecology 77:170–178
Saulitis E, Matkin C, Barrett-Lennard L, Heise K, Ellis G (2000) Foraging strategies of sympatric
killer whale ( Orcinus orca ) populations in Prince William Sound. Alaska Mar Mamm Sci
16:94–109
Schluter D (2001) Ecology and the origin of species. Trends Ecol Evol 16(7):372–380
Similä T (1997) Behavioral ecology of killer whales in Northern Norway. Norwegian College of
Fisheries Science. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Tromso, Tromso
Similä T, Ugarte F (1993) Surface and underwater observations of cooperatively feeding killer
whales in northern Norway. Can J Zool 71:1494–1499
Simon M, Ugarte F, Wahlberg M, Miller LA (2005) Icelandic killer whales Orcinus orca use a
pulsed call suitable for manipulating the schooling behaviour of herring Clupea harengus .
Bioacoustics 16:57–74
Springer AM, Estes JA, van Vliet GB, Williams TM, Doak DF, Danner EM, Forney KA, Phister B
(2003) Sequential megafaunal collapse in the North Pacifi c Ocean: an ongoing legacy of indus-
trial whaling? Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 100:12223–12228
Steiger GH, Calambokidis J, Straley JM, Herman LM, Cerchio S, Salden DR, Urbán J, Jacobsen
RJK, von Ziegesar O, Balcomb KC, Gabriele CM, Dahlheim ME, Uchida S, Ford JKB, Ladron
de Guevara P, Yamaguchi M, Barlow J (2008) Geographic variation in killer whale attacks on
humpback whales in the North Pacifi c: implications for predation pressure. Endang Species
Res 4:247–256
Stephens DW, Krebs JR (1986) Foraging theory. Princeton University Press, Princeton
Stevens TA, Duffi led DA, Asper ED, Hewlett KG, Bolz A, Gage LJ, Bossart GD (1989) Preliminary
ndings of restriction fragment differences in mitochondrial DNA among killer whales. Can J
Zool 67:2592–2595
Strager H (1995) Pod-specifi c call repertoires and compound calls of killer whales, Orcinus orca
Linnaeus, 1758, in the waters of northern Norway. Can J Zool 73:1037–1047
Temte JL, Bigg MA, Wiig O (1991) Clines revisited: the timing of pupping in the harbour seal
( Phoca vitulina ). J Zool (Lond) 224:617–632
Ternullo R, Black N (2002) Predation behavior of transient killer whales in Monterey Bay,
California. In: Fourth International Orca Symposium and Workshop, CEBC-CNRS, France, pp
156–159
Trites AW, Deecke VB, Gregr EJ, Ford JKB, Olesiuk PF (2007) Killer whales, whaling, and
sequential megafaunal collapse in the North Pacifi c: a comparative analysis of the dynamics of
marine mammals in Alaska and British Columbia following commercial whaling. Mar Mamm
Sci 23:751–765
Via S (2001) Sympatric speciation in animals: the ugly duckling grows up. Trends Ecol Evol
16:381–390
4 You Are What You Eat: Foraging Specializations and Their Infl uence
98
Visser IN, Smith TG, Bullock ID, Green GD, Carlsson OGL, Imberti S (2008) Antarctic peninsula
killer whales ( Orcinus orca ) hunt seals and a penguin on fl oating ice. Mar Mamm Sci
24:225–234
Wade PR, Barrett-Lennard LG, Black NA, Burkanov VN, Burdin AM, Calambokidis J, Cerchio S,
Dahlheim ME, Ford JKB, Friday NA, Fritz LW, Jacobsen JK, Loughlin TR, Matkin CO,
Matkin DR, McCluskey SM, Mehta AV, Mizroch SA, Muto MM, Rice DW, Robe P, Clapham
P (2007) Killer whales and marine mammal trends in the North Pacifi c: a re-examination of
evidence for sequential megafauna collapse and the prey-switching hypothesis. Mar Mamm Sci
23:766–802
Yurk H, Barrett-Lennard LG, Ford JKB, Matkins C (2002) Cultural transmission within maternal
lineages: vocal clans in resident killer whales in southern Alaska. Anim Behav 63:1103–1119
J.K.B. Ford and G.M. Ellis
... This quantification of noise in the frequency ranges used for communication and echolocation by SRKW demonstrated the potential for masking from vessel noise. Other studies suggest that prey detection and foraging efficacy could be reduced between 38-100% when a killer whale is in close proximity to a vessel Holt et al., 2013), either through direct masking of acoustic signals or reduced ability to coordinate prey location and capture (Ford and Ellis, 2014), and prey sharing (Ford and Ellis, 2006;Wright et al., 2016). The SRKW population has been characterized as nutritionally stressed, as evidenced by reduced survival and reproduction (Ward et al., 2009;Ford et al., 2010;Wasser et al., 2017), and an observed decline in body condition (Fearnbach et al., 2011(Fearnbach et al., , 2018. ...
... A reduction in range that SRKW can contact conspecifics using communication calls could impact group cohesion, and hinder the coordination of foraging or prey sharing (Ford and Ellis 2014;Wright et al., 2016). ...
Technical Report
Full-text available
Primary threats to the endangered Southern Resident Killer Whale (SRKW) population are reduced prey availability, acoustic and physical disturbance, contaminants, and vessel strike. Successful threat mitigation is contingent on knowledge of spatiotemporal overlap of whale presence with areas of elevated risk. A co-occurrence framework was used to illustrate areas within the critical habitat where SRKW are at higher risk for physical and acoustic disturbance and strike from vessels from May to October. The SRKW sightings data analyses indicate that the locations with the highest SRKW frequency of occurrence are Swiftsure Bank, Haro Strait, the Fraser River estuary. Individual and group follow data were used to add behavioural context to sightings data and identify Swiftsure Bank and Haro Strait as key foraging locations, while Juan de Fuca Strait is mainly used for travel. Collision risk was determined using a combination of Automatic Information System (AIS) vessel data to assess presence of large commercial ships, and aerial surveys adding to records of the presence of smaller or recreational vessels. The acoustic implications of vessel presence were explored by examining the noise additions in frequency ranges used for SRKW communication (500 Hz to 15 kHz) and echolocation (15-100 kHz). A reference level of ‘minimum ambient’ noise was derived from the 1% quietest conditions obtained from acoustic mooring recordings in the study area. The ranges at which calls and clicks could travel in minimum ambient conditions were then calculated. Loss of communication and echolocation ranges were expressed as percent reductions from minimum ambient. Noise from AIS Class A vessels resulted in significant loss of both echolocation and communication range in SRKW critical habitat. Echolocation range loss of greater than 50% was identified in key foraging locations, and range loss increased with foraging depth. Echolocation range loss from small vessels was also observed, with increased impacts occurring on weekends. These analyses will inform recovery measures to reduce acoustic impacts on SRKW in critical habitat, and provide a framework for future investigations on threats to recovery. Moving forward, inclusion of prey and contaminants data into the co-occurrence analysis will inform our understanding of cumulative effects, and will support management actions for the survival and recovery of the population.
... Dotted lines indicate the orcas' movements during the encounters in which predation behavior was documented: A1-A2, A3-A4, A6-A7, and A8-A9. bony and cartilaginous fish, and cephalopods (Fertl et al., 1996;Nichol & Shackleton, 1996;Dahlheim & Heyning, 1999;Ford & Ellis, 2014;Vargas-Bravo et al., 2020). Although as a species they display a generalist foraging behavior, local groups or ecotypes of orcas typically specialize on a small range of available prey (Ford et al., 1998). ...
Article
“Field observations are a fundamental method to increase our knowledge of orcas in the Gulf of California, for which little is known about their feeding habits and behavior. Before this paper orca predation on rays and sharks had already been recorded within the Gulf of California, but only anecdotally and without descriptions or information of hunting tactics. Here we present a graphical and behavioral description of orcas used to techniques to feed on four different species of rays. The fact that the same individuals were involved in different predation events could suggest that some orcas specialize in preying on rays in the Gulf of California."
... Dotted lines indicate the orcas' movements during the encounters in which predation behavior was documented: A1-A2, A3-A4, A6-A7, and A8-A9. bony and cartilaginous fish, and cephalopods (Fertl et al., 1996;Nichol & Shackleton, 1996;Dahlheim & Heyning, 1999;Ford & Ellis, 2014;Vargas-Bravo et al., 2020). Although as a species they display a generalist foraging behavior, local groups or ecotypes of orcas typically specialize on a small range of available prey (Ford et al., 1998). ...
Article
Field observations are a fundamental method to increase our knowledge of orcas in the Gulf of California, for which little is known about their feeding habits and behavior. Orcas have been observed feeding on sharks and rays in different parts of the world, and specific ray hunting tactics have been documented in New Zealand, including the use of the ocean bottom as a physical barrier, tossing rays, and releasing bubbles, perhaps to dislodge the prey (Visser, 1999). In the Gulf of California, there are records of orcas preying on both marine mammals and elasmobranchs. However, there are only opportunistic records of the latter, without descriptions of the hunting strategies, behaviors displayed, or identification of the prey species (Guerrero Ruiz et al., 2007). Before this paper, orca predation on rays and sharks had already been recorded within the Gulf of California, but only anecdotally and without descriptions or information of hunting tactics. Herein, we provide graphical and behavioral descriptions of predation events by orcas on elasmobranch species in the southern Gulf of California. Our observations suggest that the group of orcas we documented in the Gulf of California may be exhibiting a foraging specialization in feeding on rays, a behavior that had not been previously documented in this area, an additional ecotype may be using this area, and continued studies will be important to help define this.
... Although a detailed analysis of killer whale call types was not conducted for this study, the vocalizations included all ecotypes: resident (fish-eating), west coast transient (Bigg's; www.nature.com/scientificreports/ mammal-eating), and offshore (largely shark-eating) killer whales 86 , which have all been documented in this region previously [87][88][89][90] , but visual observations of Bigg's killer whales are the most commonly reported in HG waters (L. Lee, pers. ...
Article
Full-text available
Cetaceans spend most of their time below the surface of the sea, highlighting the importance of passive acoustic monitoring as a tool to facilitate understanding and mapping their year-round spatial and temporal distributions. To increase our limited knowledge of cetacean acoustic detection patterns for the east and west coasts of Gwaii Haanas, a remote protected area on Haida Gwaii, BC, Canada, acoustic datasets recorded off SG̱ang Gwaay (Sep 2009–May 2011), Gowgaia Slope (Jul 2017–Jul 2019), and Ramsay Island (Aug 2018–Aug 2019) were analyzed. Comparing overlapping periods of visual surveys and acoustic monitoring confirmed presence of 12 cetacean species/species groups within the study region. Seasonal patterns were identified for blue, fin, humpback, grey and sperm whale acoustic signals. Killer whale and delphinid acoustic signals occurred year-round on both coasts of Haida Gwaii and showed strong diel variation. Cuvier’s, Baird’s, beaked whale and porpoise clicks, were identified in high-frequency recordings on the west coast. Correlations between environmental factors, chlorophyll-a and sea surface temperature, and cetacean acoustic occurrence off Gwaii Haanas were also examined. This study is the first to acoustically monitor Gwaii Haanas waters for an extended continuous period and therefore serves as a baseline from which to monitor future changes.
... Intrinsic traits shown to modify social organisation include socially learned, group specific behaviours, i.e., animal culture (Laland and Hoppitt 2003;Laland and Galef 2009). For example, sympatric killer whale (Orcinus orca) ecotypes differing in their prey preferences showed distinct sociality (Baird 2000;Saulitis et al. 2000;Ford and Ellis 2013). The evolution of these divergent ecotypes appeared to be a consequence of stable cultural differences (Riesch et al. 2012). ...
Article
Full-text available
Social structuring from assortative associations may affect individual fitness, as well as population-level processes. Gaining a broader understanding of social structure can improve our knowledge of social evolution and inform wildlife conservation. We investigated association patterns and community structure of female Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus) in Shark Bay, Western Australia, assessing the role of kinship, shared culturally transmitted foraging techniques, and habitat similarity based on water depth. Our results indicated that associations are influenced by a combination of uni- and biparental relatedness, cultural behaviour and habitat similarity, as these were positively correlated with a measure of dyadic association. These findings were matched in a community level analysis. Members of the same communities overwhelmingly shared the same habitat and foraging techniques, demonstrating a strong homophilic tendency. Both uni- and biparental relatedness between dyads were higher within than between communities. Our results illustrate that intraspecific variation in sociality in bottlenose dolphins is influenced by a complex combination of genetic, cultural, and environmental aspects.
Article
Full-text available
Представлены результаты по суточному и годовому потреблению пищи косатками при круглогодичном содержании в сетевых вольерах в бухте Средняя. Рацион питания состоял из двух видов рыб: горбуши и сельди. Для самок суточное потребление рыбы варьировало от 9 до 50,9 кг при среднем 32,6 кг, для самцов от 6 до 45,1 кг при среднем 34,1 кг. Годовое минимальное количество рыбы для содержания одной молодой особи в неволе составляет от 11,5 до 12,1 тонн горбуши, что соответствует энергетическим потребностям в размере 15,9 Гкал/год для самок и 16,7 Гкал/год для самцов. Установлено возрастание необходимого количества пищи для животного с понижением температуры воды. По результатам фактического материала по питанию рассчитано годовое потребление разновозрастной находящейся в естественных условиях группы из десяти косаток. Эта величина в пересчёте на горбушу оценена на уровне 200 тонн корма в год. Приведён теоретический расчёт годового потребления различных видов гидробионтов косатками. По результатам исследования и анализа современных литературных источников в сравнении с историческими данными по питанию косаток предложено не разделять этот вид в Охотском море на «плотоядный» и «рыбоядный» экотипы. По нашему мнению, следует рассматривать проявление разных видов поведения (резидентный тип, транзитный тип) у особей косаток в зависимости от климатических условий и наличия объектов питания. В местах охоты у особей косаток преобладает резидентный тип, который изменяется на транзитный тип поведения при смене района нагула или сезонной миграции.
Article
Full-text available
The soundscape of critical habitat for southern resident killer whale (SRKW) Orcinus orca in the Salish Sea, the waters around southern British Columbia, Canada, and northern Washington State, USA, is shaped by wind and wave noise as well as heavy commercial and recreational vessel traffic loads. First, we used recordings from 6 passive acoustic moorings to characterize the acoustic landscape experienced by SRKW in this region, focusing on the frequencies used for communication and echolocation. Mid-frequency wind noise was prevalent in winter sound fields, whereas higher-frequency noise levels associated with increased numbers of recreational vessels increased during summer. Commercial vessel presence was consistent, with acoustic inputs prevalent in the western part of the study area. The potential implications of these additions on SRKW acoustics use were then explored for the frequency band 1-40 kHz to represent communication calls and at 50 kHz to consider echolocation. The inputs of wind were extrapolated from modelled hourly wind speed measures and commercial shipping noise. The noise impact was expressed as a proportional reduction of communication and echolocation extent compared to maximum acoustic ranges at ‘minimum ambient’ levels, void of vessel and abiotic noise. The reductions calculated were substantial, with the presence and impact of vessel noise greater than wind-derived additions and the greatest impacts around shipping lanes. Impacts were found for SRKW foraging areas, with implications for group cohesion and feeding success. This interpretation of the influence of natural and vessel noise more clearly demonstrates the potential implications of altered soundscapes for SRKW.
Article
The inland waters of Washington State are inhabited regularly by 10 species of marine mammals. Updated and comprehensive information regarding marine mammal occurrence, distribution, and behavior in the area is critical when addressing local anthropogenic impact, management, and conservation concerns. We conducted a total of 16,198 km of observation effort during systematic line-transect surveys from a high-wing, twin-engine airplane throughout Puget Sound in all 4 seasons during 2013 to 2016, and in the Strait of Juan de Fuca and San Juan Islands area during summer 2014 and spring 2015. Ten marine mammal species were confirmed across 5772 groups of an estimated 10,673 individuals sighted, including some seasonal patterns of occurrence. The most commonly sighted species in terms of individuals were Harbor Seals (n = 8012), Harbor Porpoises (n = 2168), California Sea Lions (n = 238) and Steller Sea Lions (n = 77). The most common behavior states documented across species among the total 5670 groups were rest (73%) and medium travel (16%); probable foraging was observed among 13 groups (0.2%) and included Gray Whale foraging pits in tidal mudflats. Potential behavioral reactions to the aircraft were rare (0.3% of total individuals), primarily by Harbor Seals and Harbor Porpoises. Results of these surveys represent the most comprehensive up-to-date information available on marine mammal occurrence, distribution, and behavior across seasons in the Puget Sound region.
Article
Full-text available
The Pacific Offshore killer whale population is currently listed as data deficient on the IUCN Red List and Threatened in Canada. The population is estimated at 300 individuals with a range extending from Southern California to the Aleutian Islands in Alaska. Only 157 encounters with this ecotype have been photo-documented between 1988 and 2014; consequently, movement and behavioral data are limited and restricted to areas commonly surveyed. To better understand movements, habitat use, and diving behavior, we deployed seven dart-attached satellite tags during two encounters with Offshores off California and one encounter off Washington State in 2013. Group size estimates were 6, 9, and 30 whales, respectively. Transmission durations ranged from 6.3 to 147.4 days providing a combined 2,469 location estimates. Whales tagged in Southern California travelled from 30.7°N to 59.3°N degrees latitude, covering a larger latitudinal range in 75 days than all previous sightings (33.5°N to 60.0°N). Within most of the California Current (southern extent of locations up to 48.5°N), Offshores typically used waters deeper than the 200 m isobath. As they approached the northern extent of the California Current and travelled into British Columbia and Alaska, locations were more common near or inside the 200 m isobath. Individuals tagged in the same group disassociated and re-associated within the tracking duration, with animals tagged together separating by as much 1,339 km. Two of the tags also reported summarized diving behavior, and tags captured 1,110 total dives with median dive depths of 41 m and 100 m for each tagged whale; the maximum dive depth was 480 m. Dives were typically short (median = 3.9 and 4.1 min respectively, max = 12.3). A comparison of dive depths and bathymetry suggests that whales typically dove to or near the seafloor in continental shelf habitat. Despite the small number of tag deployments, these data provide new information on social structure, individual ranges, diving behavior, and habitat use of this seldom encountered killer whale ecotype.
Article
Full-text available
The social organization of mammal-eating "transient" killer whales (Orcinus orca) was studied off southern Vancouver Island from 1985 through 1996. Strong and long-term associations exist between individual transients, so sets of individuals with consistently high association levels, termed pods, can be delineated. Pods consist of individuals of mixed ages and sexes, and typically contain an adult female and one or two offspring (averaging 2.4 individuals). The mother-offspring bond remains strong into adulthood for some male (and less often for female) offspring. Other males disperse from their maternal pod and appear to become "roving" males, spending some of their time alone, and occasionally associating with groups that contain potentially reproductive females. These males appear to have no strong or long-term relationships with any individuals, and adult male - adult male associations occur significantly less often than expected by chance. Females that disperse from their natal pod appear to be gregarious (having high average association rates) but socially mobile (having low maximum association rates). Differences in social organization from the sympatric fish-eating "resident" killer whales (where no dispersal of either sex occurs) likely relate to differences in foraging ecology. Transient killer whales maximize per capita energy intake by foraging in groups of three individuals, whereas no such relationship has been documented for resident killer whales.
Article
Full-text available
Two forms of killer whale (Orcinus orca), resident and transient, occur sympatrically in coastal waters of British Columbia, Washington State, and southeastern Alaska. The two forms do not mix, and differ in seasonal distribution, social structure, and behaviour. These distinctions have been attributed to apparent differences in diet, although no comprehensive comparative analysis of the diets of the two forms had been undertaken. Here we present such an analysis, based on field observations of predation and on the stomach contents of stranded killer whales collected over a 20-year period. In total, 22 species of fish and 1 species of squid were documented in the diet of resident-type killer whales; 12 of these are previously unrecorded as prey of O. orca. Despite the diversity of fish species taken, resident whales have a clear preference for salmon prey. In field observations of feeding, 96% of fish taken were salmonids. Six species of salmonids were identified from prey fragments, with chinook salmon (Oncorhynchus tshawytscha ) being the most common. The stomach contents of stranded residents also indicated a preference for chinook salmon. On rare occasions, resident whales were seen to harass marine mammals, but no kills were confirmed and no mammalian remains were found in the stomachs of stranded residents. Transient killer whales were observed to prey only on pinnipeds, cetaceans, and seabirds. Six mammal species were taken, with over half of observed attacks involving harbour seals (Phoca vitulina). Seabirds do not appear to represent a significant prey resource. This study thus reveals the existence of strikingly divergent prey preferences of resident and transient killer whales, which are reflected in distinctive foraging strategies and related sociobiological traits of these sympatric populations. 1471
Article
«Transient» and «resident» pods of killer whales were studied from August 1985 to July 1986 in the Johnstone Strait area, British Columbia. The occurrence of the «resident» killer whales appears to be seasonal and closely linked to salmon runs from July to November. Quite the contrary, the «transient» killer whales occur year round along the coast feeding mostly on marine mammals. Different types and size of prey therefore appear to determine differences in social organisation and behaviour in killer whales, as in some others top predators. The present study suggests that the terms «Transient» and «Resident» are inaccurate and that new terms must be coined to distinguish the two types of killer whales, taking into account their behavioural ecology, especialy their feeding strategies and yearly patterns of movements.
Article
Signal source intensity and detection range, which integrates source intensity with propagation loss, background noise and receiver hearing abilities, are important characteristics of communication signals. Apparent source levels were calculated for 819 pulsed calls and 24 whistles produced by free-ranging resident killer whales by triangulating the angles-of-arrival of sounds on two beamforming arrays towed in series. Levels in the 1-20 kHz band ranged from 131 to 168 dB re 1 mu Pa at 1 m, with differences in the means of different sound classes (whistles: 140.2 +/- 4.1 dB; variable calls: 146.6 +/- 6.6 dB; stereotyped calls: 152.6 +/- 5.9 dB), and among stereotyped call types. Repertoire diversity carried through to estimates of active space, with "long-range" stereotyped calls all containing overlapping, independently-modulated high-frequency components (mean estimated active space of 10-16 km in sea state zero) and "short-range" sounds (5-9 km) included all stereotyped calls without a high-frequency component, whistles, and variable calls. Short-range sounds are reported to be more common during social and resting behaviors, while long-range stereotyped calls predominate in dispersed travel and foraging behaviors. These results suggest that variability in Sound pressure levels may reflect diverse social and ecological functions of the acoustic repertoire of killer whales.
Article
Resident Orcinus orca were seen most often in the summer-fall whereas transient occurrence peaked in the spring and fall. Transient groups were smaller than resident groups. Transients spent more time foraging and dived for longer periods. Residents spent more time playing and resting, and were more vocal. Transients travelled more erratic routes and appeared to have less specific ranges. Only transients were observed eating warm-blooded prey. The 2 races were not seen to mix but did avoid each other. -from Author
Chapter
This chapter discusses the characteristics, taxonomy, distribution, abundance, and ecology of the killer whale or Orcinus orca. With its striking black and white markings and cosmopolitan range, the killer whale, or orca, is one of the most easily recognized and widely distributed of all cetaceans and is unlikely to be confused with any other species. It is a large dolphin, attaining maximum body lengths of 9.0 m in males and 7.7 m in females. Maximum measured weights are 6600 kg for a 7.65-m male and 4700 kg for a 6.58-m female. In addition to sexual dimorphism in size, mature males develop disproportionately larger appendages than females. This includes the pectoral flippers, tail flukes (the tips of which curl downward in males), and dorsal fin, which is erect in shape and may attain a height of 1.8 m in males. At birth, neonate killer whales are approximately 2–2.5 m long and weigh approximately 200 kg. The killer whale is second only to humans as the most widely distributed mammal in the world. It has a cosmopolitan distribution, being found in all oceans and most seas, but is most common in coastal, temperate waters, particularly in areas of high marine productivity.