ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Unlabelled: Uncontrollable, compared with controllable, painful stimulation can lead to increased pain perception and activation in pain-processing brain regions, but it is currently unknown which brain areas mediate this effect. When pain is controllable, the lateral prefrontal cortex (PFC) seems to inhibit pain processing, although it is unclear how this is achieved. Using fMRI in healthy volunteers, we examined brain activation during controllable and uncontrollable stimulation to answer these questions. In the controllable task, participants self-adjusted temperatures applied to their hand of pain or warm intensities to provoke a constant sensation. In the uncontrollable task, the temperature time courses of the controllable task were replayed (yoked control) and participants rated their sensation continuously. During controllable pain trials, participants significantly downregulated the temperature to keep their sensation constant. Despite receiving the identical nociceptive input, intensity ratings increased during the uncontrollable pain trials. This additional sensitization was mirrored in increased activation of pain-processing regions such as insula, anterior cingulate cortex, and thalamus. Further, increased connectivity between the anterior insula and medial PFC (mPFC) in the uncontrollable and increased negative connectivity between dorsolateral PFC (dlPFC) and insula in the controllable task were observed. This suggests a pain-facilitating role of the mPFC during uncontrollable pain and a pain-inhibiting role of the dlPFC during controllable pain, both exerting their respective effects via the anterior insula. These results elucidate neural mechanisms of context-dependent pain modulation and their relation to subjective perception. Significance statement: Pain control is of uttermost importance and stimulus controllability is an important way to achieve endogenous pain modulation. Here, we show differential effects of controllability and uncontrollability on pain perception and cerebral pain processing. When pain was controllable, the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex downregulated pain-evoked activation in important pain-processing regions. In contrast, sensitization during uncontrollable pain was mediated by increased connectivity of the medial prefrontal cortex with the anterior insula and other pain-processing regions. These novel insights into cerebral pain modulation by stimulus controllability have the potential to improve treatment approaches in pain patients.
Neurological pain signature (NPS) response. A, A priori-defined pattern of the NPS. The insets show examples of the pattern distribution of voxel weights within certain brain areas; ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; SII, secondary somatosensory cortex; pINS, posterior insula. (Figure provided by Dr. Wager, University of Colorado, Boulder, Colorado). B, Estimated values of mixed-model ANOVA analysis of the NPS responses; depicted are post hoc comparisons of the main effect " stimulation intensity " (levels: pain, warm), the interaction " stimulation intensity " " task " (levels: controllable, uncontrollable), and the interaction " stimulation intensity " " regressor type " (levels: constant, time–rating/temperature). The main effect of " pain " had a significantly higher NPS response than " warm. " There was no main effect of " task " (F (1,53) 0.13, p 0.720), indicating that correspondence to the NPS was comparable for the uncontrollable and controllable tasks. The significant interaction " stimulation intensity " " regressor type " with the respective post hoc tests indicates that a higher NPS response was associated with the condition regressors (i.e., constant regressors) compared with the time course regressors of ratings and temperature for painful stimulation. This was not the case for warm stimulation. The nonsignificant interaction " stimulation intensity " " task " indicates that the NPS correspondence for pain or warmth was not differentially influenced by un/controllability ( post hoc comparison uncontrollable pain versus controllable pain: p 0.592). Bars represent the mean scalar values expressing the NPS across subjects, error bars the SEM. Scaling of the NPS values depends on many factors such as voxel size, contrast weight, field strength, etc. Because only a within-study comparison was of interest here, we did not attempt to equate scaling of the NPS values with previous studies. *p 0.05; **p 0.01.
… 
Content may be subject to copyright.
Behavioral/Cognitive
Different Brain Circuitries Mediating Controllable and
Uncontrollable Pain
Anne-Kathrin Bra¨scher,
1,2
*XSusanne Becker,
1,3
* Marie-Eve Hoeppli,
1,5
and Petra Schweinhardt
1,4,5
1
Alan Edwards Centre for Research on Pain, Faculty of Dentistry, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec H3A 1G1, Canada,
2
Department of Clinical
Psychology, Psychotherapy and Experimental Psychopathology, Johannes Gutenberg University Mainz, Mainz 55122, Germany,
3
Department of Cognitive
and Clinical Neuroscience, Central Institute of Mental Health, Mannheim 68159, Germany,
4
Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery, Faculty of
Medicine, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec H3A 2B4, Canada, and
5
Faculty of Dentistry, McGill University, Montreal QC H3A 0C7, Canada
Uncontrollable, compared with controllable, painful stimulation can lead to increased pain perception and activation in pain-processing
brain regions, but it is currently unknown which brain areas mediate this effect. When pain is controllable, the lateral prefrontal cortex
(PFC) seems to inhibit pain processing, although it is unclear how this is achieved. Using fMRI in healthy volunteers, we examined brain
activation during controllable and uncontrollable stimulation to answer these questions. In the controllable task, participants self-
adjusted temperatures applied to their hand of pain or warm intensities to provoke a constant sensation. In the uncontrollable task, the
temperature time courses of the controllable task were replayed (yoked control) and participants rated their sensation continuously.
During controllable pain trials, participants significantly downregulated the temperature to keep their sensation constant. Despite
receiving the identical nociceptive input, intensity ratings increased during the uncontrollable pain trials. This additional sensitization
was mirrored in increased activation of pain-processing regions such as insula, anterior cingulate cortex, and thalamus. Further, in-
creased connectivity between the anterior insula and medial PFC (mPFC) in the uncontrollable and increased negative connectivity
between dorsolateral PFC (dlPFC) and insula in the controllable task were observed. This suggests a pain-facilitating role of the mPFC
during uncontrollable pain and a pain-inhibiting role of the dlPFC during controllable pain, both exerting their respective effects via the
anterior insula. These results elucidate neural mechanisms of context-dependent pain modulation and their relation to subjective
perception.
Key words: controllability; dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC); insula; medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC); pain; pain modulation
Introduction
Would you rather remove a splinter from your finger yourself or
let somebody else do it? Many people would choose the former,
illustrating the empowerment of having control over pain (Bow-
ers, 1968). The clinical relevance of pain controllability has been
shown for acute (Tinti et al., 2011) as well as chronic pain
(Ha¨rka¨pa¨a¨ et al., 1991;Jensen and Karoly, 1991) and experimen-
tal work confirms that controllable pain stimuli are perceived as
less intense than uncontrollable stimuli (Arntz and Schmidt,
Received May 20, 2015; revised March 14, 2016; accepted March 17, 2016.
Author contributions: A.-K.B., S.B., and P.S. designed research; A.-K.B. and S.B. performed research; M.-E.H.
contributed unpublished reagents/analytic tools; A.-K.B. and S.B. analyzed data; A.-K.B., S.B., and P.S. wrote the
paper.
This work was supported by the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (Operating Grant to P.S.). A.-K.B. was
supported by a scholarship from the German National Academic Foundation. S.B. was supported by an International
Association for the Study of Pain International Trainee Fellowship funded by the ScanDesign Foundation BY INGER
& JENS BRUUN. We thank Tor Wager for help with the NPS analysis.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
*A.-K.B. and S.B. contributed equally to this work.
Correspondence should be addressed to Anne-Kathrin Bra¨scher, Department of Clinical Psychology, Psychother-
apy and Experimental Psychopathology, Johannes Gutenberg University Mainz, Wallstra
e 3, Mainz 55122, Ger-
many. E-mail: abraesch@uni-mainz.de.
DOI:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1954-15.2016
Copyright © 2016 the authors 0270-6474/16/365013-13$15.00/0
Significance Statement
Pain control is of uttermost importance and stimulus controllability is an important way to achieve endogenous pain modulation.
Here, we show differential effects of controllability and uncontrollability on pain perception and cerebral pain processing. When
pain was controllable, the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex downregulated pain-evoked activation in important pain-processing
regions. In contrast, sensitization during uncontrollable pain was mediated by increased connectivity of the medial prefrontal
cortex with the anterior insula and other pain-processing regions. These novel insights into cerebral pain modulation by stimulus
controllability have the potential to improve treatment approaches in pain patients.
The Journal of Neuroscience, May 4, 2016 36(18):5013–5025 • 5013
1989;Mu¨ller, 2011). Imaging studies have extended these find-
ings by demonstrating that activity in pain-processing brain re-
gions, including the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), insula,
thalamus, primary and secondary somatosensory cortex (SI, SII),
as well as in pain-modulatory regions such as the amygdala, peri-
aqueductal gray (PAG), and the prefrontal cortex (PFC) varies
with pain stimulation being controllable or uncontrollable (Sa-
lomons et al., 2004;Mohr et al., 2005;Helmchen et al., 2006;
Wiech et al., 2006;Salomons et al., 2007). Human and animal
studies point to an important role of the lateral PFC, particularly
the anterior and dorsolateral PFC (dlPFC), in mediating the
pain-inhibitory effects of (perceived) control over pain (Amat et
al., 2005;Wiech et al., 2006;Borckardt et al., 2011), predomi-
nantly in individuals who do not believe that the outcomes of
their behavior is determined by their decisions and efforts (i.e.,
who have low internal control beliefs; Wiech et al., 2006). How-
ever, it is currently unclear how the lateral PFC affects pain-
processing regions to achieve pain inhibition when pain is
controllable. In addition, although implicitly assumed in previ-
ous studies, it is has not been shown whether uncontrollability
facilitates pain processing and, if so, which brain areas might
drive pain facilitation by uncontrollability. Candidate regions for
such a facilitation are the pain-modulatory regions medial PFC
(mPFC), amygdala, and PAG, which show increased activation
when pain is uncontrollable (Salomons et al., 2004;Mohr et al.,
2005;Wiech et al., 2006). The mPFC, including the cingulofron-
tal cortex, is a pain-modulatory area that has been shown to
facilitate pain in different contexts (Ploghaus et al., 2001;Mayer
et al., 2005;Schweinhardt et al., 2008;Berna et al., 2010). The
PAG plays an important role in descending pain-modulatory
pathways, including facilitation (Porreca et al., 2002;Gwilym et
al., 2009;Yoshida et al., 2013). Finally, the amygdala plays a key
role in fear and threat processing, also in the context of pain
(Neugebauer et al., 2004;Phelps and LeDoux, 2005), and can be
considered a candidate region because it has been hypothesized
that a lack of control increases the threatening value of pain
(Bowers, 1968;Arntz and Schmidt, 1989).
Here, we tested in healthy volunteers in an fMRI) experiment
whether uncontrollability of pain stimuli facilitates pain percep-
tion and if such facilitation is reflected by increased activation in
pain-processing regions such as insula, thalamus, ACC, SI, and
SII. Univariate analysis and comparison with a multivariate cere-
bral pattern of pain described previously (Wager et al., 2013)
were used to test whether uncontrollable pain is associated with
more activation in pain-processing regions than controllable
pain. We also investigated whether the functional connectivity of
an important pain-processing region, the insula, with mPFC,
amygdala, or PAG is increased during uncontrollability and
whether the insula is inversely connected to the lateral PFC dur-
ing controllability, indicating potentially downregulation.
Materials and Methods
Subjects
Inclusion criteria included age between 18 and 40 years and good health.
Exclusion criteria were the presence or history of significant neurological
or psychiatric disease, chronic pain, any significant medical condition or
sleep disorders; regular consumption of alcohol or recreational drugs;
recent use of any pain medication; or regular or frequent night shift work.
The study was approved by the local ethics committee and written in-
formed consent was obtained from all subjects. Subjects were compen-
sated with $50 US for their participation.
Three subjects were excluded because they did not rate the painful
stimulus as painful during 50% of the pain trials. The final sample
consisted of 23 volunteers (13 males; 1 left-handed) age 19 –30 years
(mean 24.2 3.57 years).
Experimental design
The study followed a within-subject yoked-control design with two
within-subjects factors with two levels each (task “controllable” vs “un-
controllable” and stimulation intensity “pain” vs “warm”). Warm inten-
sities were used to identify pain-specific effects in the behavioral
responses and brain activation. The sequence of stimulation intensities
was pseudorandomized with the same order across participants. In each
functional scan, the first 12 trials were controllable, followed by 12 un-
controllable trials to avoid frequent switching between the two tasks so
that subjects would not be confused which task was to be performed in a
specific trial (Fig. 1). Because of the yoked design, uncontrollable trials
had to be preceded by controllable trials (see below). In the controllable
task, subjects performed a temperature regulation task (Kleinbo¨hl et al.,
1999;Ho¨lzl et al., 2005;Becker et al., 2011). After the thermode had
reached the target temperature, the subject had to keep his/her sensation
constant by antagonizing any perceived temperature change using a re-
sponse unit. Therefore, subjects had full instrumental control (Miller,
1979) over the physical stimulus intensity. The response unit allowed
regulating the temperature down or up with a left or right button, respec-
tively. If the sensation had not changed, subjects had to press a middle
button to control for motor responses. Because the temperature only
changed when the subject regulated it, any change perceived by the sub-
ject was due to sensitization or habituation. A flashing arrow displayed
on a screen visible to the subject reflected the subject’s response and
served as visual feedback to minimize unspecific differences to the un-
controllable task. After 20 s, the temperature returned to baseline. In the
uncontrollable task, the temperature profiles of the previous controllable
trials were replayed unbeknownst to the subject. Now, the subject rated
his or her sensation on a visual analog scale (VAS; see below) projected
onto the screen. The right mouse button increased the rating; the left
mouse button decreased the rating. If the sensation had not changed, the
subject had to press the middle mouse button. Due to the yoked design,
the nociceptive input was identical in both tasks.
During both tasks, a green square flashed every 2 s, prompting the
subject to give a response. The intertrial interval was 20 s.
General procedure
Each subject underwent two sessions: a familiarization session and an
fMRI session, 1–3 d apart.
Familiarization session
The study was explained to the subject and, after obtaining informed
consent, participants were familiarized with the stimuli, the tasks, and
the rating scales. Cutaneous heat stimuli were delivered using a 30 30
mm
2
contact thermode (Pathway; Medoc Advanced Medical Systems).
Stimulus intensities were individually determined (see below), but tem-
peratures 50°C were not allowed for safety reasons. A vertically ori-
ented VAS anchored with 0 (“no sensation”), 40 (“just painful,” defined
as the pain threshold), and 100 (“most intense pain tolerable”) was used
to rate nonpainful and painful sensations. This VAS has been shown to
possess linear properties (Lautenbacher et al., 1992). Pain tolerance test-
ing was performed so that the subjects experienced a sensation that cor-
responded to the upper end of the VAS, thereby providing a perceptual
anchor. For this, the thermode was applied to the subject’s forearm and
the temperature slowly increased (rise rate 0.5°C/s) from 32°C until the
subject stopped it by pressing a button on the response unit when the
sensation became intolerable. This procedure was repeated twice with a
rise rate of 1.5°C/s.
To assess subjects’ pain threshold, a series of stimuli was applied to the
testing site to be used during the fMRI: the subject’s nondominant thenar
eminence. After each stimulus, the subject indicated their most intense
sensation during the stimulation on the VAS. The baseline temperature
was 36°C and the first target temperature was 39°C (stimulus duration
5 s). The subsequent target temperatures each increased by 1°C until the
participant rated a stimulus as “just painful.” Based on this temperature,
5 more temperatures around this initial painful temperature (1°C,
0.5°C, /0°C, 1°C, 0.5°C) were applied and the one closest to the
5014 J. Neurosci., May 4, 2016 36(18):5013–5025 Bra¨scher, Becker et al. Neural Correlates of Un/Controllable Pain
“just painful” anchor was used as an estimate of the pain threshold. The
resulting average pain threshold was 44.2°C (SD 1.42°C), consistent
with the literature (Rolke et al., 2006).
The temperature to be used as initial temperature in the controllable
pain trials was the temperature of the individual’s pain threshold plus
1.5°C. Adjustments were made if this temperature was not rated as mod-
erately painful or did not fall between 45.5°C and 48°C (mean tempera-
ture 47°C, SD 0.78°C). For the controllable warm trials, the initial
temperature was 39°C or 40°C, depending on the participant’s rating
(mean temperature 39.5°C, SD 0.5°C).
FMRI session
At the beginning of the fMRI session, subjects were reminded of the tasks
and stimulus intensities were adjusted if necessary to achieve moderately
painful and warm sensations. FMRI data acquisition was performed in
two functional scans separated by an anatomical scan (Fig. 1).
Stimulus presentation was controlled by a laptop computer using
Eprime software (Psychology Software Tools). The display was back-
projected onto a screen and was visible via a mirror that was mounted on
the head coil of the MRI scanner. A computer mouse with three buttons,
modified in-house to ensure MR compatibility, served as response unit
so that the subjects could perform the experimental tasks.
Questionnaires
The internal– external control (IPC) scale (Levenson, 1981) was used to
identify the locus of control beliefs (subscales: “internal,” “powerful oth-
ers,” and “chance”). Twenty subjects completed this questionnaire. All
subjects completed the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (Spielberger et al.,
1970) indicating their level of state anxiety before the scan and their level
of trait anxiety after the scan.
FMRI data acquisition
Imaging data were acquired ona3TSiemens TRIO MRI scanner at the
McConnell Brain Imaging Center, Montreal Neurological Institute
(MNI), using a 32-channel head coil. A gradient echo planar imaging
(EPI) sequence covering the whole brain was used for functional scans
[TR 2.62 s, TE 30 ms, flip angle 90 degree, 44 interleaved, 3.5 mm
thick axial slices (parallel to the AC–PC line), field of view (FoV) 224
mm 224 mm, matrix 64 64, resulting in an in-plane resolution of
3.5 3.5 mm
2
, 441 image volumes]. The first two images were discarded
to allow steady-state magnetization. Field maps were obtained using a
gradient echo sequence (TE 20 ms, 0.47 ms dwell time, FoV and matrix
identical to EPI). High-resolution, anatomical T1-weighted images (RF
spoiled, prescan normalized MPRAGE sequence, TR 2300 ms, TE
2.98 ms, TI 900 ms, flip angle 9 degree, FoV 192 mm 256 mm
256 mm, matrix 192 256 256, resulting in a voxel size of 1 mm
3
)
were acquired for all subjects for coregistration purposes.
Statistical analysis of behavioral data
Mean values and SDs were calculated for the self-adjusted temperature
changes and intensity ratings. Two-sided ttests were used to test whether
the temperatures or VAS ratings at the end of the trial differed from those
at the beginning of the trial. A linear mixed model was used to test
whether VAS ratings changed over the course of the experiment. IPC
scores and anxiety scores were correlated with the psychophysical data
using Pearson’s correlations (uncorrected for multiple comparisons).
Alpha 0.05 was used as the significance level. Statistical tests were
performed with PASW Statistics 17.0.3.
Statistical analysis of fMRI data
All image processing and statistical analysis was performed using the
software package FSL 5.0.8 (FMRIB’s Software Library; Smith et al.,
2004). Of 1104 trials in total, 19 trials were excluded due to missing data
caused by technical problems such as thermode malfunction. Eleven
uncontrollable trials were shortened due to technical failure.
Subject-level analysis. To eliminate scanner artifacts, denoising was
performed on the raw images, before preprocessing, using MELODIC
(Multivariate Exploratory Linear Optimized Decomposition into Inde-
pendent Components; Beckmann and Smith, 2004) within FEAT (FMRI
Expert Analysis Tool). The number of independent components was
estimated from the data. Components were individually inspected and
Figure 1. Experimental procedure. Twelve controllable trials (CON) were followed by 12 uncontrollable trials (UNCON) during the first functional scan. Participants saw instructions for the
respective trials and visual feedback of their responses on a screen, indicated here by the insets. After an anatomical scan (10 min), a second functional scan was performed during which 12
controllable trials were again followed by 12 uncontrollable trials. The sequence of stimulation intensities (painful, solid line; warm, dashed line) was pseudorandomized.
Bra¨scher, Becker et al. Neural Correlates of Un/Controllable Pain J. Neurosci., May 4, 2016 36(18):5013–5025 • 5015
removed using regression if the spatial map or the associated time series
was clearly abnormal, such as the spatial map being striped or the time
series showing spikes 5 SDs. Then, the following preprocessing steps
were applied to each functional dataset: spatial smoothing (Gaussian
kernel, full width at half-maximum: 5 mm), motion correction, and
temporal high-pass filtering (Gaussian-weighted least-squares straight
line fitting with
100 s). Susceptibility-related distortions were cor-
rected using FSL field map correction routines.
The time courses of CSF and white matter masks were extracted to be
used as nuisance variables. To generate CSF and white matter masks, the
individual structural images were segmented into CSF, white matter, and
gray matter using FAST (Zhang et al., 2001), thresholded at 0.8 (Biswal et
al., 2010). CSF and white matter masks were transformed into functional
space by applying the inverse transformation matrix of the individual
motion-corrected EPI to the structural image using FLIRT. The masks
were rebinarized and partial volumes edges excluded after interpolation.
Finally, the time series averaged across the voxels of the respective mask
was extracted.
A general linear model (GLM) was applied to each functional dataset
with the following regressors of interest: four condition regressors (con-
trollable pain, controllable warm, uncontrollable pain, uncontrollable
warm), the time–rating curves (i.e., the time courses of the intensity
ratings) of uncontrollable trials, and time–temperature curves of con-
trollable trials (excluding the stimulus rise time; time bins of 2000 ms).
Temperature rise and fall times, motion outliers, time series for CSF and
white matter, and time–temperature curves in the uncontrollable condi-
tions were included in the model as nuisance variables. In addition, the
first4softhe20sstimulation period were modeled as nuisance regres-
sors to account for the time subjects needed to “catch up” with their VAS
ratings with the initial temperature increase of the thermode in the un-
controllable task (Fig. 2). Regressors were convolved with a gamma he-
modynamic response function and the first temporal derivatives were
included. This model allowed identifying the main effects and interaction
of task (controllable or uncontrollable) and stimulation intensity (pain
or warm), as well as brain activation correlating with sensitization or
habituation in the respective tasks (i.e., with self-adjusted temperature
changes in the controllable tasks and with subjective ratings in the un-
controllable tasks).
Voxelwise parameter estimates were derived using the appropriate
contrasts. Individual’s functional images were registered to their own
structural scan using linear transformation (FLIRT; Jenkinson et al.,
2002), followed by registration to the International Consortium for Brain
Mapping (ICBM) 152 nonlinear sixth-generation symmetric template in
MNI standard space using nonlinear transformations (FNIRT, warp res-
olution 10 mm). The parameter estimates and the corresponding es-
timates of the variance from the two functional scans of each subject were
merged in a second-level fixed-effects analysis.
Group level analysis. Third-level analyses were performed using a
mixed-effects model implemented in FLAME (Beckmann et al., 2003).
Statistical inference was based on a voxel-based threshold of Z2.3
cluster corrected across the whole brain at p0.05.
Neurological Pain Signature. We tested the correspondence of the dif-
ferent stimulation intensities and tasks with the so-called “Neurological
Pain Signature” (NPS; Wager et al., 2013), which was provided by its
authors. The NPS is a distributed pattern of fMRI activations defined by
multivariate pattern analysis, which sensitively and specifically tracks
changes in perceived pain intensity achieved by altering nociceptive in-
put (Wager et al., 2013). The rationale for testing any modulatory influ-
ences on the NPS was its high sensitivity and the fact that it does not
correspond to nonpainful emotional events that otherwise activate sim-
ilar brain areas. Modulation of the NPS by un/controllability would be
consistent with the interpretation that pain facilitation by uncontrolla-
bility occurs in pain intensity coding regions. Lack of effects on the NPS,
in conjunction with no change in the magnitude of pain-related activa-
tion in the univariate GLM analysis, would point to un/controllability-
induced pain modulation by other cerebral systems.
For calculating the strength of expression of the NPS response in the
different conditions, the a priori defined pattern of regression weights for
each voxel (Wager et al., 2013) was used to calculate the scalar product
with the brain activation maps (maps of parameter estimates obtained
from the subject-level fMRI analysis) of each contrast of interest (con-
trollable pain, uncontrollable pain, controllable warm, uncontrollable
warm, the time–rating curves of uncontrollable trials, and the time–
temperature curves of controllable trials), resulting in one scalar value for
each subject for each contrast. These values were analyzed for differences
with a repeated-measuremes ANOVA design using mixed-model proce-
dures with the factors “stimulation intensity” (pain or warm), “task”
(controllable or uncontrollable), and “regressor type” (i.e., constant re-
gressors coding the four conditions and the time–rating/time–tempera-
ture regressors).
Connectivity analyses. To investigate the connectivity between pain
processing and potential pain-modulatory brain regions, depending on
un/controllability, psychophysiological interaction analyses (PPIs; Fris-
ton et al., 1997) were performed. PPI analyses provide a model of how a
psychological context (i.e., uncontrollability) changes the influence of
one area on another area and is regarded as a measure of effective con-
nectivity (Friston et al., 1997). The anterior insula served as seed region
because it is a core region implicated in pain processing (Apkarian et al.,
2005) and is consistently modulated by psychological interventions in-
cluding controllability (Salomons et al., 2004;Wiech et al., 2006). For the
seed, an ellipsoid was created that comprised the peak coordinates for the
contrasts associated with significant insula activation (controllable pain,
uncontrollable pain, controllable warm, uncontrollable warm, time–rat-
ing curve of uncontrollable pain). Four PPI regressors were computed,
Figure 2. Psychophysical data of painful (A) and warm (B) trials. Illustrated are the temperature courses in blue, averaged across trials and participants, and the VAS ratings, equally averaged
across trials and participants, in red. For better illustration, we downsampled the displayed data (interval length 250 ms) and do not display the temperature rise in the beginning of the trial; that
is, illustrated are the 20 s after reaching the target temperature. Gray ribbons represent the SEM. Because increasing ratings at the beginning of the uncontrollable trials do not reflect sensitization
(thesubjectsneeded time to adjust theirratingafter the thermode hadreachedthe target temperature), thefirst4softheuncontrollabletrialswere excluded. The red barsindicatethe time segment
that was included in the comparison of intensity ratings between end and beginning of the trial, as well as as pain-rating regressors in the fMRI analysis of the uncontrollable trials (see Materials and
Methods). The red dotted line depicts the “just painful” anchor of the VAS. The blue dotted line marks habituation (values 0) and sensitization behavior (values 0) for the controllable trials. The
initial variations in temperature were due to fluctuations of the thermode.
5016 J. Neurosci., May 4, 2016 36(18):5013–5025 Bra¨scher, Becker et al. Neural Correlates of Un/Controllable Pain
each as the scalar product of the time series of the activity averaged across
the voxels in the anterior insula seed and a vector coding for the control-
lable pain, uncontrollable pain, controllable warm, and uncontrollable
warm conditions, respectively. All PPI regressors were included in the
same GLM to model the full space of the conditions of interest for this
analysis. Commonly, two conditions are subtracted from each other and
modeled as one PPI regressor. However, such an approach makes as-
sumptions about the interrelationship between the conditions and does
not allow comparisons of similarities between the PPI contrasts. Evi-
dence demonstrates also a better single-subject model fit and higher
sensitivity for true positive findings when modeling the full space of
conditions (McLaren et al., 2012). In addition to the PPI regressors,
regressors coding all four conditions (controllable painful, controllable
warm, uncontrollable painful, uncontrollable warm), the average time
course extracted from the seed, and the same nuisance variables as in the
GLM were included to ensure that the variance explained by the PPI
regressors was not confounded by main effects of the conditions or other
factors (O’Reilly et al., 2012). Therefore, variance explained by the PPI
regressors represents specifically variance explained over and above the
variance explained by the conditions, the time series, and error variance
explained by the nuisance regressors, indicating which brain regions are
functionally connected to the seed dependent on the experimental con-
dition. The regions of interest (ROIs) were the lateral PFC (controllable
task) and the mPFC, amygdala, and PAG (uncontrollable task). To test
the specificity of the effects, the PPI analyses were repeated with the
lateral PFC serving as ROIs for the uncontrollable tasks and the mPFC,
amygdala, and PAG serving as ROIs for the controllable tasks. Lateral
PFC was defined as Brodmann areas 9, 10, and 46. The amygdalae were
defined with the Harvard–Oxford Subcortical Structural Atlas imple-
mented in FSL (signal intensity minimum at 20%). The ROI for the PAG
was manually drawn on the ICBM 152 template and comprised 112
voxels. The ROI for mPFC was generated by combining the cingulate
cortex, paracingulate cortex, frontal medial cortex, frontal pole, and sub-
callosal cortex as defined in the Harvard–Oxford Cortical Structural At-
las implemented in FSL. The resulting area was restricted in the x
dimension by planes at x⫽⫺16 and x16 and in the ydimension by a
plane extending from the vertical ramus of the Sylvian fissure ( y20;
Kates et al., 2002) to obtain the final mPFC ROI. Statistical inference was
based on a voxel-based threshold of Z2.3 cluster corrected across the
ROIs at p0.05.
Regions that were identified in the PPI as pain-modulatory areas were
used as seeds in a “reversed” PPI analysis. This was done to test whether
pain-processing brain regions other than anterior insula showed in-
creased connectivity to the regions identified in the first PPI (i.e., dlPFC
for the controllable pain and controllable warm conditions, two clusters
in mPFC for the uncontrollable pain condition and one cluster in
mPFC for the uncontrollable warm condition). For the “reversed”
PPI analyses, a mask of pain-processing brain regions served as ROIs
(composed of bilateral insula, SI, SII, thalamus, and ACC derived
from the Harvard–Oxford Cortical Structural Atlas and Harvard–
Oxford Subcortical Structural Atlas implemented in FSL) and statis-
tical inference was based on a voxel-based threshold of Z2.3 cluster
corrected across the mask at p0.05.
Localization of activation was achieved by inspection of group activa-
tion maps overlaid on the nonlinear ICBM-152 template. Images are
displayed in radiological convention; that is, the right side of the brain is
on the left. Coordinates are given in MNI space.
Additional analyses. Several additional analyses were performed,
mainly for control purposes. A GLM containing only the four condition
regressors (controllable pain, uncontrollable pain, controllable warm,
uncontrollable warm) and the nuisance variables was performed to con-
firm the main effects of condition.
To test the specificity of brain activation associated with additional
sensitization in the uncontrollable pain condition, a “control GLM” was
performed using the pain ratings recorded during the uncontrollable trials as
additional regressor for the controllable pain condition. The other regressors
and nuisance variables were the same as in the original GLM.
In the uncontrollable pain condition, subjects pressed the button more
frequently than at every cue, reflecting the increased sensitization in this
condition. We therefore repeated the GLM analysis as well as the PPI
analyses with an additional regressor of no interest coding the button
presses. This analysis is not ideal because the variance cannot be unam-
biguously explained due to multicollinearity of the button press regressor
with the time–intensity and time–temperature curves. It was nevertheless
performed to gain insight into the potential confound caused by the
button presses.
Last, the modeled time courses of signal change were extracted for all
contrasts with significant anterior insula activation; 5 mm spheres were
created around the anterior insula peaks of the significant contrasts and
the time courses of the percentage signal change extracted.
Results
Behavioral data
In the controllable task during painful trials, subjects regulated
the temperature down to keep their sensation constant (Fig. 2A),
which indicates that sensitization (as a specific form of pain
facilitation) occurred over the course of the 20-s-long trials (tem-
perature difference between end and beginning of the trial: M
0.7°C, SD 0.398°C; T 8.41, p0.001). During warm trials,
subjects on average increased the temperature; that is, they habit-
uated to the stimulation (M 0.19°C, SD 0.305°C; T ⫽⫺3.00,
p0.007; Fig. 2B).
In the uncontrollable task, subjects received the identical no-
ciceptive input as in the controllable task by being administered
the self-adjusted temperature time courses of the controllable
trials. Consistent with the notion that uncontrollability facilitates
pain, they rated the sensation as continuously getting more pain-
ful during pain trials (rating difference between end and begin-
ning of trial: M 23.2, SD 9.22, T 10.95, p0.001; Fig. 2A),
although they had regulated the temperature time courses to pro-
duce constant sensations in the controllable task. During warm
trials, the ratings remained stable throughout the trial (M 0.38,
SD 1.06, T 0.51, p0.62; Fig. 2B) and thus matched the
perceived sensation in the controllable task, indicating that sub-
jects were able to perform the temperature regulation task. These
results indicate that the effects of uncontrollability on the percep-
tion of thermal stimuli are specific for painful sensations.
Although the yoked design required that the uncontrolla-
ble trials were presented after the controllable trials, sensitiza-
tion over the course of the experiment can be ruled out as an
alternative explanation for the increasing pain ratings in the
uncontrollable trials because pain ratings did not increase
across trials (mixed-model analysis, interaction effect trial
intensity: F
(5,514)
0.7, p0.626).
Locus of control and anxiety scores
The magnitude of self-regulated temperature changes in the
controllable painful condition correlated positively with the
“powerful others” subscale of the IPC locus of control sc-
ale (r0.59, p0.012). The more a subject believed that his/her
life was controlled by others, the more he/she sensitized in re-
sponse to the painful stimulation. Neither trait (M 36.4, SD
9.74) nor state anxiety scores (M 29.1, SD 8.07) correlated
with the self-regulated temperature in the controllable task or the
subjective ratings in the uncontrollable task (all p-values 0.4).
Pain-related brain activation
The uncontrollable pain and controllable pain conditions (Fig.
3A) revealed largely overlapping activations of typical pain-
processing brain regions (i.e., regions commonly activated dur-
ing experimental heat in fMRI studies in healthy volunteers;
Apkarian et al., 2005;Farrell et al., 2005), including bilateral in-
sula, thalamus, SII, ACC, PFC, premotor cortex, cerebellum, and
Bra¨scher, Becker et al. Neural Correlates of Un/Controllable Pain J. Neurosci., May 4, 2016 36(18):5013–5025 • 5017
brainstem. The main effects of the uncontrollable warm and con-
trollable warm conditions showed similar, yet less expanded pat-
terns (Fig. 3B). For both tasks, painful stimulation was associated
with significantly more activation in pain-processing brain
regions than warm stimulation (Table 1). This finding was
corroborated by testing the correspondence of the experimen-
tal conditions with the NPS, a brain system that sensitively
scales with perceived pain (Fig. 4A): the NPS response in-
creased significantly for painful compared with warm stimu-
lation intensities (main effect “stimulus intensity”: F
(1,61)
5.03, p0.029; Fig. 4B).
Brain activation during uncontrollable stimulation
The increases in pain perception over the course of a trial in the
uncontrollable task were reflected by increasing activation in typ-
ical pain-processing regions, including bilateral insula, ACC, bi-
lateral medial thalamus, and pallidum. In addition, activation
scaling with intensity ratings was detected in the lateral orbito-
frontal cortex (OFC) and occipital lobe (Fig. 5,Table 2). Note
that this activation is controlled for activation unrelated to sub-
jective perception by the constant condition regressor.
The GLM was used to contrast brain responses in the uncon-
trollable and controllable tasks for the painful stimulation, con-
trolling for warm stimulation [contrast (uncontrollable pain
uncontrollable warm) (controllable pain controllable
warm)], masked with a ROI consisting of pain-processing brain
regions. This contrast only revealed activation in SI/primary mo-
tor cortex (Table 3). Consistent with this result, the comparison
with the NPS did not reveal an interaction between “stimulation
intensity,” “task” (uncontrollable or controllable), and “regres-
sor type” (F
(1,62)
0.52, p0.473) nor between “stimulation
intensity” and “task” (F
(1,62)
0.08, p0.783). The results from
the NPS and the GLM indicate that the uncontrollable pain con-
dition was not associated with more pain-related activation per
se. Importantly, this was also the case when activation related to a
constant regressor as well as to a regressor reflecting the increas-
ing pain ratings was taken into account, as evidenced by the non-
significant interaction for the NPS analysis between “stimulation
intensity” and “task.”
The PPI demonstrated that, during uncontrollable painful
stimulation, connectivity was increased between the anterior
insula seed (Fig. 6A) and two clusters in mPFC, one in peri-
genual ACC (pgACC) (10, 38, 8, Z3.22) and one in the
anteromedial PFC (amPFC) (8, 58, 10, Z3.14) (Fig. 6B,
Table 4). Neither the amygdala nor the PAG showed signifi-
cantly increased connectivity with the anterior insula, but 36
contiguous voxels above a voxel-based threshold of Z1.6,
uncorrected for spatial extent, were detected in the amygdala
(peak: 12, 2, 20, Z2.72). In the uncontrollable warm
condition, increased connectivity of the insula seed with a
cluster in dorsal ACC (8, 28, 30, Z3.40) was observed (Table
4). No increased connectivity was observed with the lateral
PFC (hypothesized ROI for controllable task) in the uncon-
trollable task (painful or warm stimulation intensity). In the
“reversed” PPIs of the uncontrollable pain condition with
pgACC (Fig. 6C) and amPFC as seeds (Fig. 6D), increased
connectivity with the anterior insula, ACC, and inferior pari-
etal lobe was detected (Table 4). In the “reversed” PPI of the
Figure 3. Pain-related brain responses. Shown are uncontrollable pain (A, blue) and controllable pain (A, red) and uncontrollable warm (B, blue) and controllable warm (B, red) conditions of the
GLM analysis (main effects). INS, Insula; BSG, basal ganglia; THA, thalamus; NCA, nucleus caudatus; PFC, prefrontal cortex; PMC, premotor cortex.
Table 1. Brain responses to pain versus warm stimulation for the uncontrollable
and controllable tasks
Brain region
Cluster size
(no. of voxels)
Z-score
peak
MNI peak coordinates (mm)
xyz
Uncontrollable condition
Cluster spanning 27081 6.49 28 66 28
Cerebellum 6.49 28 66 28
4.60 24 58 28
Central opercular
cortex
6.16 38 8 8
Thalamus 6.12 14 20 10
5.95 18 20 14
ACC 3.59 6 20 30
Insula 5.91 36 14 6
5.04 32 16 4
SII 4.78 56 20 22
Cluster spanning 3241 5.19 36 10 64
SMA/premotor
cortex
5.19 36 10 64
5.52 22 254
SI 4.07 42 36 54
MI 4.06 36 26 56
Cluster spanning 693 4.31 16 64 42
Precuneus 4.31 16 64 42
Superior parietal
cortex
3.97 10 62 60
3.62 16 62 58
Controllable condition
Cluster spanning 29104 6.59 42 12 14
SII 6.59 42 12 14
Insula 6.32 38 14 16
5.41 38 12 4
Central opercular
cortex
6.25 56 0 4
ACC 4.54 4 12 44
Thalamus 5.50 12 66
5.41 14 18 16
SMA 4.12 6 18 60
Cerebellum 5.26 26 62 22
Three big clusters spanning several areas were significantly more activated by painful compared with nonpainful
stimulationin the uncontrollabletask and onebig cluster forthe controllable task(significant on awhole brain-level,
voxel-based threshold Z2.3 and cluster-based threshold p0.05). Local maxima within the clusters are given
for individual anatomical areas. SMA, Supplementary motor cortex; MI, primary motor cortex; ACC, anterior cingu-
late cortex; SII, secondary somatosensory cortex; SI, primary somatosensory cortex.
5018 J. Neurosci., May 4, 2016 36(18):5013–5025 Bra¨scher, Becker et al. Neural Correlates of Un/Controllable Pain
uncontrollable warm condition with ACC as seed, increased
connectivity was detected only with left SI (Table 4).
Brain activation during controllable stimulation
In the controllable pain condition, activation in pain-processing
regions was not correlated with the downregulation of the tem-
perature, consistent with the interpretation that subjects success-
fully kept their sensation constant. The PPI analysis revealed
increased inverse connectivity between the anterior insula seed
(Fig. 7A) and two clusters in the dlPFC (34, 58, 22; Z3.51; 42,
16, 34, Z3.16; Fig. 7B,Table 4) in the painful condition and,
partly overlapping, one cluster in the dlPFC in the warm condi-
Figure4. Neurologicalpainsignature (NPS) response. A, Apriori-definedpatternof the NPS. The insets showexamplesof the pattern distribution of voxelweightswithin certain brain areas; ACC,
anterior cingulate cortex; SII, secondary somatosensory cortex; pINS, posterior insula. (Figure provided by Dr. Wager, University of Colorado, Boulder, Colorado). B, Estimated values of mixed-model
ANOVA analysis of the NPS responses; depicted are post hoc comparisons of the main effect “stimulation intensity” (levels: pain, warm), the interaction “stimulation intensity” “task” (levels:
controllable, uncontrollable), and the interaction “stimulation intensity” “regressor type” (levels: constant, time–rating/temperature). The main effect of “pain” had a significantly higher NPS
response than “warm.” There was no main effect of “task” (F
(1,53)
0.13, p0.720), indicating that correspondence to the NPS was comparable for the uncontrollable and controllable tasks. The
significant interaction “stimulation intensity” “regressor type” with the respective post hoc tests indicates that a higher NPS response was associated with the condition regressors (i.e., constant
regressors) compared with the time course regressors of ratings and temperature for painful stimulation. This was not the case for warm stimulation. The nonsignificant interaction “stimulation
intensity” “task” indicates that the NPS correspondence for pain or warmth was not differentially influenced by un/controllability ( post hoc comparison uncontrollable pain versus controllable
pain: p0.592). Bars represent the mean scalar values expressing the NPS across subjects, error bars the SEM. Scaling of the NPS values depends on many factors such as voxel size,
contrast weight, field strength, etc. Because only a within-study comparison was of interest here, we did not attempt to equate scaling of the NPS values with previous studies. *p0.05;
**p0.01.
Bra¨scher, Becker et al. Neural Correlates of Un/Controllable Pain J. Neurosci., May 4, 2016 36(18):5013–5025 • 5019
tion (34, 58, 22; Z3.51). No significant connectivity was ob-
served with the mPFC, amygdala, and PAG (hypothesized ROIs
for uncontrollable task) in the controllable pain or controllable
warm conditions. Using dlPFC as seed, the results of the “re-
versed” PPI analysis for the controllable pain condition revealed
increased inverse connectivity with bilateral thalamus (8, 22,
16; Z3.87; Table 4). The “reversed” PPI for the controllable
warm condition did not reveal increased connectivity with any
region.
Additional analyses
Repeating the GLM with only the four condition regressors as
regressors of interest yielded virtually identical results.
No activation was significantly associated with the time course
of the pain ratings in the controllable task (control GLM). There-
fore, the additional sensitization appears to be specific for pain in
the uncontrollable task.
The GLM and the PPI analyses were repeated including a
regressor coding for the button presses. The GLM results were
similar to the original analysis, albeit partly at a lower statistical
threshold. The PPI results were only slightly different with the
main difference that the anterior insula seed did no longer show
significant inverse connectivity with the dlPFC in the controllable
warm condition when the button presses were included.
The time courses of signal change in the anterior insula fur-
ther support the picture gleaned from the GLM analysis as well as
the comparison with the NPS: the constant regressors for uncon-
trollable pain and controllable pain and the time–intensity curve
for uncontrollable pain were best reflected by the signal change in
the insula. The two warm conditions (uncontrollable and con-
trollable) were also well reflected but showed lower percentage
signal changes (Fig. 8).
Discussion
Here, we identify brain regions important for driving pain facil-
itation by uncontrollability and suggest mechanisms by which
pain is facilitated or inhibited when stimulation is uncontrollable
or controllable. We show that uncontrollable pain is associated
with increased sensitization, reflected by increased activation in
pain-processing regions. At the same time, uncontrollable pain
was not associated with a higher activation magnitude in pain-
processing regions compared with controllable pain, as investi-
gated with univariate GLM analysis as well as by comparison
with the NPS, a multivariate pain activation pattern. Rather,
uncontrollability-induced pain facilitation increased the connec-
tivity of mPFC with anterior insula, ACC, and the inferior pari-
etal lobe. Anterior insula and ACC are important pain-processing
areas and the connectivity to the inferior parietal lobe might in-
clude SII, which is also involved in pain processing. There was
some indication of increased coupling between the dorsal ACC
and insula and/or SI for the uncontrollable warm condition. Al-
though this indicates that the role of the mPFC in the context of
stimulus uncontrollability might not be specific to pain, it should
be noted that the dorsal ACC does not belong to the mPFC re-
gions previously implicated in pain facilitation by negative emo-
tions (Ploghaus et al., 2001;Schweinhardt et al., 2008;Berna et al.,
2010). Consistent with this, we observed behavioral conse-
Figure 5. Brain correlates of sensitization in the uncontrollable condition. The subjective ratings in the uncontrollable painful condition correlated with activation in pain-processing areas over
and above activation related to the condition. Ins, Insula; GP, globus pallidus; THA, thalamus; ACC, anterior cingulate cortex. For details, see Table 3.
Table 2. Brain activation reflecting the additional sensitization in the
uncontrollable pain condition
Brain region
Cluster size
(no. of voxels)
Z-score
peak
MNI peak coordinates
(mm)
xyz
Cluster spanning 1446 3.61 10 28 50
Supplementary motor
area
3.61 10 28 50
ACC 3.59 10 22 32
3.37 2 38 16
Cluster spanning 1011 3.74 46 20 10
OFC 3.74 46 20 10
Inferior frontal gyrus 3.65 50 20 4
Frontal opercular cortex 3.64 44 20 0
Central opercular cortex
(BA44)
3.46 54 8 0
Insula 3.26 44 10 2
Cluster spanning 904 4.41 8 84 10
Visual cortex 4.41 8 84 10
Cluster spanning 507 3.69 44 16 2
Frontal opercular cortex 3.69 44 16 2
OFC 3.68 32 20 14
Insula 3.45 38 10 0
Cluster spanning 455 3.71 4 26
Thalamus 3.71 4 26
Globus pallidus 3.34 14 0 2
Listed are brain areas in which activation was significant on a whole-brain level (voxel-based threshold Z2.3 and
cluster-based threshold p0.05). Please note that local maxima are given as peaks if a significant cluster encom-
passed more than one region. ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; OFC, orbitofrontal cortex.
Table 3. Brain activation for the contrast (uncontrollable pain >uncontrollable
warm) >(controllable pain >controllable warm)
Brain region
Cluster size
(no. of voxels)
Z-score
peak
MNI peak coordinates (mm)
xyz
Cluster spanning 1165 3.95 34 26 54
MI 3.95 34 26 54
SI 3.91 38 30 52
One cluster was significant with a voxel-based threshold Z2.3, cluster-corrected across a mask consisting
of pain processing brain regions at p0.05. Local maxima within the cluster are given for individual
anatomical areas. MI, primary motor cortex; SI, primary somatosensory cortex.
5020 J. Neurosci., May 4, 2016 36(18):5013–5025 Bra¨scher, Becker et al. Neural Correlates of Un/Controllable Pain
quences (i.e., increased sensitization) of uncontrollability only
for painful stimulation. The amygdala appeared to contribute to
uncontrollability-induced pain facilitation, but only at a lenient
statistical threshold. We also confirmed the dlPFC as being an
important region for the pain-inhibitory effects of controllability
and present data suggesting that this inhibition is achieved by
increased inverse connectivity of the dlPFC with the anterior in-
sula and thalamus during controllable pain stimulation.
Dissociation of pain perception in controllable and
uncontrollable tasks
During painful trials in the controllable task, subjects showed
sensitization as a specific form of pain facilitation indexed by
downregulation of the temperature. Such sensitization has been
shown before in similar paradigms (Kleinbo¨hl et al., 1999;Ho¨lzl
et al., 2005;Becker et al., 2011) and related to spinal wind-up
(Eide, 2000;Kleinbo¨hl et al., 2006). Subjects with a high external
locus of control showed more temperature downregulation with
controllable pain, in accordance with theories of learned helpless-
ness (Abramson et al., 1978;Mu¨ller, 2012).
Sensitization with controllable pain was not significantly as-
sociated with increased activation in any pain-processing area,
consistent with the notion that it does not involve supraspinal
mechanisms. In the uncontrollable task, the same nociceptive
inputs were reapplied, thereby accounting for peripheral and spinal
sensitization. Now, these temperature profiles were consistently
Figure 6. Connectivity between insula and medial PFC and connectivity between medial PFC and pain-processing regions. The PPI analysis shows that the connectivity of the anterior insula as
seed (A) is increased during uncontrollable painful stimulation with two clusters in mPFC (pgACC and amPFC) (B). The “reverse” PPI analyses with pgACC and amPFC (B) as seeds show increased
connectivity with right anterior insula and inferior parietal lobe (seed: pgACC) (C; inferior parietal lobe not depicted) and with the bilateral anterior insula and ACC (seed: amPFC) (D) during
uncontrollable painful stimulation. For details, see Table 4. INS, insula; pgACC, perigenual anterior cingulate cortex; amPFC, anteromedial prefrontal cortex.
Bra¨scher, Becker et al. Neural Correlates of Un/Controllable Pain J. Neurosci., May 4, 2016 36(18):5013–5025 • 5021
rated as increasingly painful over the course of the individual trials,
demonstrating the pain-facilitatory effects of uncontrollability. Pre-
vious imaging studies of uncontrollable pain have largely failed to
demonstrate behavioral pain-increasing effects of uncontrollability
(but see Wiech et al., 2006).
Uncontrollability leads to increased activation in pain-
processing brain regions and OFC
Insula, ACC, medial thalamus, and pallidum encoded sensitiza-
tion during uncontrollable pain, confirming indirect evidence
from earlier studies that used uncontrollable pain stimulation as
Table 4. Functional connectivity of the anterior insula with regions of interest during uncontrollable and controllable stimulation and of the “reversed” analyses based on
the prior results
Brain region Cluster size (no. of voxels) Connectivity Z-score peak
MNI peak coordinates (mm)
xyz
Uncontrollable pain; insula seed
amPFC 185 Positive 3.14 8 58 10
Perigenual ACC 243 Positive 3.22 10 38 8
Uncontrollable pain; amPFC seed (“reversed” PPI)
pgACC/dorsal ACC 577 Positive 3.57 2 40 14
Insula/frontal operculum 408 Positive 3.91 34 18 8
Insula/frontal operculum 383 Positive 3.90 34 14 12
Uncontrollable pain; perigenual ACC seed (“reversed” PPI)
Insula/frontal operculum 775 Positive 4.09 36 12 14
Inferior parietal lobule 212 Positive 4.01 62 32 36
Uncontrollable warm; insula seed
ACC 181 Positive 3.40 8 28 30
Uncontrollable warm; ACC seed (“reversed” PPI)
SI/postcentral gyrus 363 Positive 3.91 32 36 40
Controllable pain; insula seed
dlPFC 359 Negative 3.51 34 58 22
dlPFC 193 Negative 3.16 42 16 34
Controllable pain; dlPFC seed (“reversed” PPI)
Thalamus 888 Negative 3.87 8 22 16
Controllable warm; insula (seed)
dlPFC 200 Negative 3.51 34 58 22
Listed are brain areas in which functional connectivity was significant on a voxel-based threshold of Z2.3, corrected for spatial extent across ROIs at a cluster level of p0.05. The ROIs for the PPI analysis with the insula seed were medial
PFC, amygdala, PAG, and dlPFC. The ROIs for the “reversed” PPIs consisted of pain-processing brain regions. amPFC, anteromedial prefrontal cortex; ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; pgACC, perigenual ACC; SI, primary somatosensory cortex;
dlPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.
Figure 7. Connectivity between insula and dlPFC. The PPI analysis shows that the inverse connectivity of the anterior insula (seed; A) with two clusters in the dlPFC (B) is increased during
controllable painful stimulation. INS, insula; dlPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.
5022 J. Neurosci., May 4, 2016 36(18):5013–5025 Bra¨scher, Becker et al. Neural Correlates of Un/Controllable Pain
a control condition (Salomons et al., 2004;Mohr et al., 2005;
Wiech et al., 2006). Each of these regions contains nociceptive
neurons encoding stimulus intensity (Kenshalo et al., 1980;Sikes
and Vogt, 1992;Chudler and Dong, 1995;Frot et al., 2007). The
signal in these regions scales linearly with the perceived intensity
of different stimulus intensities in brain-imaging studies (Coghill
et al., 1999;Seminowicz and Davis, 2007;Loggia et al., 2012),
although nonlinear relationships between applied intensity and
BOLD response have been observed (Johnstone et al., 2012).
Therefore, the result that the additional sensitization in uncon-
trollable pain scales with the activation in these regions (com-
pared with controllable pain) indicates that the loss of control
leads to amplified processing of the nociceptive input.
The lateral OFC also showed increased activation with sensi-
tization during uncontrollable pain. This is not a typical pain-
encoding region, but has been associated with uncontrollable
(Wiech et al., 2006) as well as unpredictable (Carlsson et al., 2006)
pain and the fear of pain (Ochsner et al., 2006). Because the lateral
OFC is implicated in the processing of punishers (O’Doherty et
al., 2001), its activation in the present study may reflect negative
affective processing induced by uncontrollable pain.
mPFC might drive additional sensitization during
uncontrollable pain stimulation
Our results suggest brain regions that mediate pain facilita-
tion during uncontrollable stimulation, namely pgACC and
amPFC as parts of the mPFC. In demonstrating stronger cou-
pling of mPFC to the anterior insula, ACC, and inferior parietal
lobe during uncontrollable pain, we extend previous work that
observed increased activation in these regions during uncontrol-
lable compared with controllable painful stimulation (Salomons
et al., 2004;Wiech et al., 2006). Our results link the activity to
behavioral pain facilitation and suggest that the mPFC drives the
additional sensitization, perhaps via direct connections to the
pain-processing regions anterior insula and ACC (Mesulam and
Mufson, 1982;Mufson and Mesulam, 1982). The mPFC is sug-
gested to play a major role in emotional
processing, with regulatory functions of
the perigenual and subgenual ACC and
mPFC especially for negative emotions
(Etkin et al., 2011). Regarding pain,
mPFC has been shown to mediate pain
facilitation by negative emotions
(Ploghaus et al., 2001;Schweinhardt et al.,
2008;Berna et al., 2010). In subacute back
pain patients, increased functional con-
nectivity between mPFC and nucleus ac-
cumbens predicted transition to chronic
pain (Baliki et al., 2012), suggesting the
clinical significance of this region. Be-
cause the mPFC is a large area, the ques-
tion arises whether different subregions
assume different pain-facilitatory roles.
The studies discussed above have either
described relevant activation in peri-
genual or subgenual ACC (Ploghaus et al.,
2001;Berna et al., 2010) or the amPFC
(Schweinhardt et al., 2008;Baliki et al.,
2012), the latter being two patient studies.
Interestingly, here, both pgACC and
amPFC showed increased connectivity
with the anterior insula during uncontrol-
lable pain. It seems important to probe
specific mPFC subregions in future studies of pain facilitation.
Although below the significance threshold, the amygdala also
showed stronger coupling to the anterior insula during uncontrol-
lable pain, consistent with the notion that a lack of control increases
the threatening value of pain (Bowers, 1968;Arntz and Schmidt,
1989). Contrary to our hypothesis, PAG did not contribute to pain
facilitation. This might indicate that pain facilitation by uncontrol-
lability is not mediated via descending pathways, in contrast to many
pain-inhibitory processes (Basbaum and Fields, 1984). However,
negative findings of course need to be interpreted with caution.
dlPFC might mediate controllability-induced pain inhibition
We confirmed dlPFC activation during controllable pain, consis-
tent with a previous study on pain controllability (Wiech et al.,
2006) and other forms of cognitive– emotional hypoalgesia (Wa-
ger et al., 2004). Furthermore, our study showed an increased
inverse connectivity between insula and dlPFC and between
dlPFC and thalamus during controllable pain. This means that, as
activity in the dlPFC increases, activity in the insula and thal-
amus decreases. Interestingly, although the anterior insula was
inversely connected to the dlPFC also during controllable warm
stimulation, the “reversed PPI” with a dlPFC seed did not reveal
any significantly connected regions for the controllable warm
condition. Therefore, it appears that downregulation of regions
processing pain and warmth occurs predominantly with painful
stimulation. Animal studies provide further insight into possible
mechanisms by showing that having control is conveyed via sig-
naling from vmPFC, the homologous region in the rat to the
human dlPFC (Uylings et al., 2003), to the dorsal raphe nucleus.
During controllable stress, stress-induced activation of the dorsal
raphe nucleus is inhibited by the vmPFC and behavioral conse-
quences of uncontrollable stress (potentiated fear conditioning,
escape deficits) are blocked, which is reversed by pharmacologi-
cal inactivation of vmPFC (Amat et al., 2005).
Figure 8. Time courses of BOLD signal change in the anterior insula. Modeled time courses for the conditions controllable pain,
uncontrollablepain,controllable warm, uncontrollable warm, andtheuncontrollable pain time–rating regressorfromtheanterior
insula; shaded area: stimulation interval after the target temperature was reached. Percentage signal change was calculated
relative to the mean of each time series.
Bra¨scher, Becker et al. Neural Correlates of Un/Controllable Pain J. Neurosci., May 4, 2016 36(18):5013–5025 • 5023
Clinical implications
Chronic pain is in many instances uncontrollable. Experiencing
uncontrollable pain can lead to hypervigilance (Aldrich et al.,
2000), learned helplessness, and depression, resulting in a self-
amplifying vicious circle (Arntz and Schmidt, 1989). Further,
losing control over pain potentially increases fear of pain and
interferes with task performance (Crombez et al., 2008). In con-
trast, perceived control decreases pain and discomfort in acute
pain (Thrash et al., 1982) and is associated with better function-
ing in chronic pain patients (Tan et al., 2002). Understanding the
endogenous mechanisms underlying pain modulation by uncon-
trollability and controllability might inform clinical approaches
of pain management and therapy. Further, the brain regions that
drive pain facilitation or inhibition by uncontrollability or con-
trollability could be targeted with methods such as transmagnetic
stimulation or neurofeedback with the aim to reduce clinical
pain.
The finding that the perceptual sensitization induced by un-
controllability was not reflected by a higher activation magnitude
in pain-processing regions indicates that a lot has to be learned
about pain processing in the brain before pain biomarkers based
on activation patters should be considered (Davis et al., 2012). In
particular, it needs to be established whether different types of
pain augmentation and facilitation (e.g., by increased nociceptive
input or different types of cognitive– emotional pain modula-
tion) are associated with consistent (connectivity) patterns in the
brain or if the brain can construct the sensation of “pain” via
different mechanisms.
References
Abramson LY, Seligman ME, Teasdale JD (1978) Learned helplessness in
humans: critique and reformulation. J Abnorm Psychol 87:49–74.
CrossRef Medline
Aldrich S, Eccleston C, Crombez G (2000) Worrying about chronic pain:
vigilance to threat and misdirected problem solving. Behav Res Ther 38:
457–470. CrossRef Medline
Amat J, Baratta MV, Paul E, Bland ST, Watkins LR, Maier SF (2005) Medial
prefrontal cortex determines how stressor controllability affects behavior
and dorsal raphe nucleus. Nat Neurosci 8:365–371. CrossRef Medline
Apkarian AV, Bushnell MC, Treede RD, Zubieta JK (2005) Human brain
mechanisms of pain perception and regulation in health and disease. Eur
J Pain 9:463–484. CrossRef Medline
Arntz C, Schmidt A (1989) Perceived control and the experience of pain.
In: Stress, personal control and health (Steptoe A, Appels A, eds), pp
131–162. Brussels: Wiley.
Baliki MN, Petre B, Torbey S, Herrmann KM, Huang L, Schnitzer TJ, Fields
HL, Apkarian AV (2012) Corticostriatal functional connectivity
predicts transition to chronic back pain. Nat Neurosci 15:1117–1119.
CrossRef Medline
Basbaum AI, Fields HL (1984) Endogenous pain control systems: brainstem
spinal pathways and endorphin circuitry. Annu Rev Neurosci 7:309 –338.
CrossRef Medline
Becker S, Kleinbo¨hl D, Baus D, Ho¨lzl R (2011) Operant learning of percep-
tual sensitization and habituation is impaired in fibromyalgia patients
with and without irritable bowel syndrome. Pain 152:1408–1417.
CrossRef Medline
Beckmann CF, Smith SM (2004) Probabilistic independent component
analysis for functional magnetic resonance imaging. IEEE Trans Med
Imaging 23:137–152. CrossRef Medline
Beckmann CF, Jenkinson M, Smith SM (2003) General multilevel linear modeling
for group analysis in FMRI. Neuroimage 20:1052–1063. CrossRef Medline
Berna C, Leknes S, Holmes EA, Edwards RR, Goodwin GM, Tracey I (2010)
Induction of depressed mood disrupts emotion regulation neurocircuitry
and enhances pain unpleasantness. Biol Psychiatry 67:1083–1090.
CrossRef Medline
Biswal BB, Mennes M, Zuo XN, Gohel S, Kelly C, Smith SM, Beckmann CF,
Adelstein JS, Buckner RL, Colcombe S, Dogonowski AM, Ernst M, Fair D,
Hampson M, Hoptman MJ, Hyde JS, Kiviniemi VJ, Ko¨tter R, Li SJ, Lin
CP, et al. (2010) Toward discovery science of human brain function.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107:4734 –4739. CrossRef Medline
Borckardt JJ, Reeves ST, Frohman H, Madan A, Jensen MP, Patterson D,
Barth K, Smith AR, Gracely R, George MS (2011) Fast left prefrontal
rTMS acutely suppresses analgesic effects of perceived controllability
on the emotional component of pain experience. Pain 152:182–187.
CrossRef Medline
Bowers KS (1968) Pain, anxiety, and perceived control. J Consult Clin Psy-
chol 32:596 602. CrossRef Medline
Carlsson K, Andersson J, Petrovic P, Petersson KM, Ohman A, Ingvar M
(2006) Predictability modulates the affective and sensory-discriminative
neural processing of pain. Neuroimage 32:1804 –1814. CrossRef Medline
Chudler EH, Dong WK (1995) The role of the basal ganglia in nociception
and pain. Pain 60:3–38. CrossRef Medline
Coghill RC, Sang CN, Maisog JM, Iadarola MJ (1999) Pain intensity pro-
cessing within the human brain: a bilateral, distributed mechanism.
J Neurophysiol 82:1934–1943. Medline
Crombez G, Eccleston C, De Vlieger P, Van Damme S, De Clercq A (2008) Is
it better to have controlled and lost than never to have controlled at all? An
experimental investigation of control over pain. Pain 137:631–639.
CrossRef Medline
Davis KD, Racine E, Collett B (2012) Neuroethical issues related to the use
of brain imaging: can we and should we use brain imaging as a biomarker
to diagnose chronic pain? Pain 153:1555–1559. CrossRef Medline
Eide PK (2000) Wind-up and the NMDA receptor complex from a clinical
perspective. Eur J Pain 4:5–15. CrossRef Medline
Etkin A, Egner T, Kalisch R (2011) Emotional processing in anterior cingu-
late and medial prefrontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci 15:85–93. CrossRef
Medline
Farrell MJ, Laird AR, Egan GF (2005) Brain activity associated with pain-
fully hot stimuli applied to the upper limb: a meta-analysis. Hum Brain
Mapp 25:129 –139. CrossRef Medline
Friston KJ, Buechel C, Fink GR, Morris J, Rolls E, Dolan RJ (1997) Psycho-
physiological and modulatory interactions in neuroimaging. Neuroimage
6:218–229. CrossRef Medline
Frot M, Magnin M, Mauguie`re F, Garcia-Larrea L (2007) Human SII and
posterior insula differently encode thermal laser stimuli. Cereb Cortex
17:610620. Medline
Gwilym SE, Keltner JR, Warnaby CE, Carr AJ, Chizh B, Chessell I, Tracey I
(2009) Psychophysical and functional imaging evidence supporting the
presence of central sensitization in a cohort of osteoarthritis patients.
Arthritis Rheum 61:1226 –1234. CrossRef Medline
Ha¨rka¨pa¨a¨K,Ja¨rvikoski A, Mellin G, Hurri H, Luoma J (1991) Health locus
of control beliefs and psychological distress as predictors for treatment
outcome in low-back pain patients: Results of a 3-month follow-up of a
controlled intervention study. Pain 46:35– 41. CrossRef Medline
Helmchen C, Mohr C, Erdmann C, Binkofski F, Bu¨chel C (2006) Neural
activity related to self-versus externally generated painful stimuli reveals
distinct differences in the lateral pain system in a parametric fMRI study.
Hum Brain Mapp 27:755–765. CrossRef Medline
Ho¨lzl R, Kleinbo¨hl D, Huse E (2005) Implicit operant learning of pain sen-
sitization. Pain 115:12–20. CrossRef Medline
Jenkinson M, Bannister P, Brady M, Smith S (2002) Improved optimization
for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of
brain images. Neuroimage 17:825–841. CrossRef Medline
Jensen MP, Karoly P (1991) Control beliefs, coping efforts, and adjustment
to chronic pain. J Consult Clin Psychol 59:431–438. CrossRef Medline
Johnstone T, Salomons TV, Backonja MM, Davidson RJ (2012) Turning on
the alarm: The neural mechanisms of the transition from innocuous to
painful sensation. Neuroimage 59:1594–1601. CrossRef Medline
Kates WR, Frederikse M, Mostofsky SH, Folley BS, Cooper K, Mazur-
Hopkins P, Kofman O, Singer HS, Denckla MB, Pearlson GD, Kaufmann
WE (2002) MRI parcellation of the frontal lobe in boys with attention
deficit hyperactivity disorder or Tourette syndrome. Psychiatry Res 116:
63–81. CrossRef Medline
Kenshalo DR Jr, Giesler GJ Jr, Leonard RB, Willis WD (1980) Responses of
neurons in primate ventral posterior lateral nucleus to noxious stimuli.
J Neurophysiol 43:1594–1614. Medline
Kleinbo¨hlD,Ho¨lzl R, Mo¨ltner A, Rommel C, Weber C, Osswald PM (1999)
Psychophysical measures of sensitization to tonic heat discriminate
chronic pain patients. Pain 81:35– 43. CrossRef Medline
Kleinbo¨hl D, Trojan J, Konrad C, Ho¨lzl R (2006) Sensitization and habitu-
5024 J. Neurosci., May 4, 2016 36(18):5013–5025 Bra¨scher, Becker et al. Neural Correlates of Un/Controllable Pain
ation of AMH and C-fiber related percepts of repetitive radiant heat stim-
ulation. Clin Neurophysiol 117:118–130. Medline
Lautenbacher S, Mo¨ltner A, Strain F (1992) Psychophysical features of the
transition from pure heat perception to heat pain perception. Percept
Psychophys 52:685– 690. CrossRef Medline
Levenson H (1981) Differentiating among internality, powerful others, and
chance. In: Research with the locus of control construct, Vol 1, Assess-
ment methods (HM Lefcourt, ed), pp 15–63. New York: Academic.
Loggia ML, Edwards RR, Kim J, Vangel MG, Wasan AD, Gollub RL, Harris RE, Park
K, Napadow V (2012) Disentangling linear and nonlinear brain responses to
evoked deep tissue pain. Pain 153:2140 –2151. CrossRef Medline
Mayer EA, Berman S, Suyenobu B, Labus J, Mandelkern MA, Naliboff BD,
Chang L (2005) Differences in brain responses to visceral pain between
patients with irritable bowel syndrome and ulcerative colitis. Pain 115:
398409. CrossRef Medline
McLaren DG, Ries ML, Xu G, Johnson SC (2012) A generalized form of context-
dependent psychophysiological interactions (gPPI): a comparison to standard
approaches. Neuroimage 61:1277–1286. CrossRef Medline
Mesulam MM, Mufson EJ (1982) Insula of the old world monkey. III: Ef-
ferent cortical output and comments on function. J Comp Neurol 212:
38–52. CrossRef Medline
Miller SM (1979) Controllability and human stress: method, evidence and
theory. Behav Res Ther 17:287–304. CrossRef Medline
Mohr C, Binkofski F, Erdmann C, Bu¨chel C, Helmchen C (2005) The ante-
rior cingulate cortex contains distinct areas dissociating external from
self-administered painful stimulation: a parametric fMRI study. Pain 114:
347–357. CrossRef Medline
Mufson EJ, Mesulam MM (1982) Insula of the old world monkey. II: Affer-
ent cortical input and comments on the claustrum. J Comp Neurol 212:
23–37. CrossRef Medline
Mu¨ller MJ (2011) Helplessness and perceived pain intensity: relations to
cortisol concentrations after electrocutaneous stimulation in healthy
young men. Biopsychosoc Med 5:8. CrossRef Medline
Mu¨ller MJ (2012) Will it hurt less if I believe I can control it? Influence of
actual and perceived control on perceived pain intensity in healthy male
individuals: a randomized controlled study. J Behav Med 35:529–537.
CrossRef Medline
Neugebauer V, Li W, Bird GC, Han JS (2004) The amygdala and persistent
pain. Neuroscientist 10:221–234. CrossRef Medline
Ochsner KN, Ludlow DH, Knierim K, Hanelin J, Ramachandran T, Glover
GC, Mackey SC (2006) Neural correlates of individual differences in
pain-related fear and anxiety. Pain 120:69–77. CrossRef Medline
O’Doherty J, Kringelbach ML, Rolls ET, Hornak J, Andrews C (2001) Ab-
stract reward and punishment representations in the human orbitofron-
tal cortex. Nat Neurosci 4:95–102. CrossRef Medline
O’Reilly JX, Woolrich MW, Behrens TE, Smith SM, Johansen-Berg H (2012)
Tools of the trade: psychophysiological interactions and functional con-
nectivity. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 7:604609. CrossRef Medline
Phelps EA, LeDoux JE (2005) Contributions of the amygdala to emotion
processing: from animal models to human behavior. Neuron 48:175–187.
CrossRef Medline
Ploghaus A, Narain C, Beckmann CF, Clare S, Bantick S, Wise R, Mat-
thews PM, Rawlins JN, Tracey I (2001) Exacerbation of pain by anx-
iety is associated with activity in a hippocampal network. J Neurosci
21:9896–9903. Medline
Porreca F, Ossipov MH, Gebhart GF (2002) Chronic pain and medullary
descending facilitation. Trends Neurosci 25:319–325. CrossRef Medline
Rolke R, Baron R, Maier C, To¨lle TR, Treede RD, Beyer A, Binder A,
Birbaumer N, Birklein F, Bo¨tefu¨ r IC, Braune S, Flor H, Huge V, Klug R,
Landwehrmeyer GB, Magerl W, Maiho¨fner C, Rolko C, Schaub C, Sche-
rens A, et al. (2006) Quantitative sensory testing in the German Re-
search Network on Neuropathic Pain (DFNS): standardized protocol and
reference values. Pain 123:231–243. CrossRef Medline
Salomons TV, Johnstone T, Backonja MM, Davidson RJ (2004) Perceived
controllability modulates the neural response to pain. J Neurosci 24:
7199–7203. CrossRef Medline
Salomons TV, Johnstone T, Backonja MM, Shackman AJ, Davidson RJ
(2007) Individual differences in the effects of perceived controllability on
pain perception: critical role of the prefrontal cortex. J Cogn Neurosci
19:993–1003. CrossRef Medline
Schweinhardt P, Kalk N, Wartolowska K, Chessell I, Wordsworth P, Tracey I
(2008) Investigation into the neural correlates of emotional augmenta-
tion of clinical pain. Neuroimage 40:759–766. CrossRef Medline
Seminowicz DA, Davis KD (2007) Interactions of pain intensity and cogni-
tive load: the brain stays on task. Cereb Cortex 17:1412–1422. CrossRef
Medline
Sikes RW, Vogt BA (1992) Nociceptive neurons in area 24 of rabbit cingu-
late cortex. J Neurophysiol 68:1720–1732. Medline
Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Behrens TE,
Johansen-Berg H, Bannister PR, De Luca M, Drobnjak I, Flitney DE,
Niazy RK, Saunders J, Vickers J, Zhang Y, De Stefano N, Brady JM, Mat-
thews PM (2004) Advances in functional and structural MR image anal-
ysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage 23:S208–S219. CrossRef
Medline
Spielberger R, Gorsuch R, Lushene R (1970) STAI manual for the State-
Trait Anxiety Inventory 1970. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists.
Tan G, Jensen MP, Robinson-Whelen S, Thornby JI, Monga T (2002) Mea-
suring control appraisals in chronic pain. J Pain 3:385–393. CrossRef
Medline
Thrash WJ, Marr JN, Box TG (1982) Effects of continuous patient informa-
tion in the dental environment. J Dent Res 61:1063–1065. CrossRef
Medline
Tinti C, Schmidt S, Businaro N (2011) Pain and emotions reported after
childbirth and recalled 6 months later: the role of controllability. J Psy-
chosom Obstet Gynaecol 32:98–103. CrossRef Medline
Uylings HB, Groenewegen HJ, Kolb B (2003) Do rats have a prefrontal cor-
tex? Behav Brain Res 146:3–17. CrossRef Medline
Wager TD, Rilling JK, Smith EE, Sokolik A, Casey KL, Davidson RJ, Kosslyn
SM, Rose RM, Cohen JD (2004) Placebo-induced changes in FMRI in
the anticipation and experience of pain. Science 303:1162–1167. CrossRef
Medline
Wager TD, Atlas LY, Lindquist MA, Roy M, Woo CW, Kross E (2013) An
fMRI-based neurologic signature of physical pain. N Engl J Med 368:
1388–1397. CrossRef Medline
Wiech K, Kalisch R, Weiskopf N, Pleger B, Stephan KE, Dolan RJ (2006)
Anterolateral prefrontal cortex mediates the analgesic effect of expected
and perceived control over pain. J Neurosci 26:11501–11509. CrossRef
Medline
Yoshida W, Seymour B, Koltzenburg M, Dolan RJ (2013) Uncertainty in-
creases pain: evidence for a novel mechanism of pain modulation involv-
ing the periaqueductal gray. J Neurosci 33:5638 –5646. CrossRef Medline
Zhang Y, Brady M, Smith S (2001) Segmentation of brain MR images
through a hidden Markov random field model and the expectation-
maximization algorithm. IEEE Trans Med Imaging 20:45–57. CrossRef
Medline
Bra¨scher, Becker et al. Neural Correlates of Un/Controllable Pain J. Neurosci., May 4, 2016 36(18):5013–5025 • 5025
... Additionally, some basic appetitive and reflexive behaviors such as defensive aggressiveness/violence, feeding, 15 and sex are considered to be "hardwired" in the lower levels of the brain and may be triggered in basic form even when the PFC is not active. 22,23 It has been hypothesized that these behaviors are a type of automatic behavior as well, having been performed over millions of years in the name of survival, until they became reflexive and fast. 12 An additional related phenomenon that is not technically an executive function, but can result in bizarre behaviors, is "dissociative analgesia." ...
... Lopez et al. have noted that 78.7% of sleepwalkers have reported dissociative analgesia during their episodes. 20,22 As with executive functions, pain itself is moderated in different ways via the PFC and integrated with other executive functions. Controllable pain is inhibited via the PFC, although the exact mechanism has not been identified. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction The clinical diagnosis of disorders of arousal (DOA) is based primarily on a clinical history including amnesia for episodes. The presence of amnesia means the patient cannot provide direct evidence. In a forensic setting, when the defendant has been charged criminally with violent actions or sexual related assaults allegedly during sleep, a sleepwalking defense may be presented. As opposed to clinical history, the prosecution generally focuses on the single episode of alleged DOA that resulted in the criminal charges against the defendant. The prosecution will argue that this episode of complex behavior was not consistent with a DOA. A past history of purported episodes is not proof that a recent single episode must be a DOA. However, most sleepwalking defenses rely heavily on standard clinical evaluations despite the fact they have no direct connection with the current criminally charged episode. The International Classification of Sleep Disorders (ICSD-3) General Diagnostic Criteria C for DOAs that states “limited or no associated cognition” should be present. Recent real time studies of DOAs have shown that during DOA episodes the prefrontal cortex (PRC) is deactivated while the motor cortex remains active. Conclusion The PFC is the location of almost all executive functions including inhibition, planning, memory, and many others. Thus, when the PFC is deactivated, these higher cognitive functions are not available. The presence of higher cognitive functions during an alleged episode of DOA would be inconsistent with a deactivated PFC and thus inconsistent with generally accepted brain activity during a NREM parasomnia. This would be direct evidence that the episode could not be a DOA but occurred during wakefulness. Clinical trial No.
... While the S1 reflects sensory discriminative aspects of pain, the PFC is associated with the affective aspect of pain, providing top-down modulation of sensory and affective processes, including inhibition of both sensory and affective pain signals by descending projections to the various brain and spinal cord regions (Ji and Neugebauer, 2014;Bräscher et al., 2016;Kummer et al., 2020). Enhanced functional connectivity during procedural pain has been observed in several areas involved in pain perception: somatosensory cortices, anterior insula, anterior cingulate cortex and thalamus and mPFC (Bräscher et al., 2016;Galambos et al., 2019). ...
... While the S1 reflects sensory discriminative aspects of pain, the PFC is associated with the affective aspect of pain, providing top-down modulation of sensory and affective processes, including inhibition of both sensory and affective pain signals by descending projections to the various brain and spinal cord regions (Ji and Neugebauer, 2014;Bräscher et al., 2016;Kummer et al., 2020). Enhanced functional connectivity during procedural pain has been observed in several areas involved in pain perception: somatosensory cortices, anterior insula, anterior cingulate cortex and thalamus and mPFC (Bräscher et al., 2016;Galambos et al., 2019). Here, we tested whether communication between S1 and mPFC was affected by ELP using synchronization in the u range as a measure of connectivity. ...
Article
Full-text available
Early life pain experience (ELP) alters adult pain behaviour and increases injury induced pain hypersensitivity, but the effect of ELP upon adult functional brain connectivity is not known. We have performed continuous local field potential (LFP) recording in the awake adult male rats to test the effect of ELP upon functional cortical connectivity related to pain behaviour. Somatosensory cortex (S1) and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) LFPs evoked by mechanical hindpaw stimulation were recorded simultaneously with pain reflex behaviour for 10 days after adult incision injury. We show that, post adult injury, sensory evoked S1 LFP delta and gamma energy and S1 LFP delta/gamma frequency coupling are significantly increased in ELP rats compared to controls. Adult injury also induces increases in S1-mPFC functional connectivity but this is significantly prolonged in ELP rats, lasting 4 days compared to 1 day in controls. Importantly, the increases in LFP energy and connectivity in ELP rats were directly correlated with increased behavioural pain hypersensitivity. Thus, early life pain (ELP) alters adult brain functional connectivity, both within and between cortical areas involved in sensory and affective dimensions of pain. The results reveal altered brain connectivity as a mechanism underlying the effects of early life pain upon adult pain perception.Significance StatementPain and stress in early life has a lasting impact upon pain behaviour and may increase vulnerability to chronic pain in adults. Here we record pain-related cortical activity and simultaneous pain behaviour in awake adult male rats previously exposed to pain in early life. We show that functional connectivity within and between the somatosensory cortex and the medial prefrontal cortex is increased in these rats and that these increases are correlated with their behavioural pain hypersensitivity. The results reveal that early life pain alters adult brain connectivity, which may explain the impact of childhood pain upon adult chronic pain vulnerability.
... These findings match the previous reports of increased high-beta activity in different pain syndromes (Vanneste et al., 2017;Wang et al., 2019). Such an increased beta activity could reflect an alteration in the descending pain inhibition pathway linked to lateral prefrontal regions (Bräscher et al., 2016). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
A bstract Phantom limb pain (PLP) is a distressing and persistent sensation that occurs after the amputation of a limb. While medication-based treatments have limitations and adverse effects, neurostimulation is a promising alternative approach whose mechanism of action needs research, including electroencephalographic (EEG) recordings for the assessment of cortical manifestation of PLP relieving effects. Here we collected and analyzed high-density EEG data in three patients (P01, P02, and P03). Peripheral nerve stimulation (PNS) suppressed PLP in P01, but was ineffective in P02. By contrast, transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation (TENS) was effective in P02. In P03, spinal cord stimulation (SCS) was used to suppress PLP. Changes in EEG oscillatory components we analyzed using spatio-spectral decomposition (SSD) and Petrosian fractal dimension (FD). With these methods, changes EEG spatio-spectral components were found in the theta, alpha, and beta bands in all patients, with these effects being specific to each individual. These changes in EEG patterns were found for both the periods when PLP level was stationary and the periods when PLP was gradually changing after neurostimulation was turned on or off. One consistent effect was an increase in EEG fractal dimension on the side contralateral to the amputation when PLP was present, while effective stimulation resulted in a decrease in fractal dimension. Overall, our findings align with the proposed roles of brain rhythms in thalamocortical dysrhythmia, which is has been linked to neuropathic pain. The individual differences in the observed effects could be related to the specifics of treatment in each patient and the unique spectral characteristics in each of them. These findings pave way to the closed-loop systems for PLP management where neurostimulation parameters are adjusted based on EEG-derived markers.
... Perceived control of pain has been shown to be associated with increased activation in the mPFC and reduced activation in the anterior cingulate, insular, and secondary somatosensory cortex (Salomons et al., 2004) and is associated with individual differences in the structure of the central motor output system (Salomons et al., 2012). In addition, a pain-facilitating role of the mPFC during uncontrollable pain and a pain-inhibiting role of the dlPFC during controllable pain was identified, both exerting their respective effects via the anterior insula (Bräscher et al., 2016). Again, this has mainly been studied in the context of acute or short-term pain paradigms, but is of importance for chronic pain (Bushnell et al., 2013;Drake et al., 2021). ...
Article
Full-text available
Chronic, or persistent pain affects more than 10% of adults in the general population. This makes it one of the major physical and mental health care problems. Although pain is an important acute warning signal that allows the organism to take action before tissue damage occurs, it can become persistent and its role as a warning signal thereby inadequate. Although per definition, pain can only be labeled as persistent after 3 months, the trajectory from acute to persistent pain is likely to be determined very early and might even start at the time of injury. The biopsychosocial model has revolutionized our understanding of chronic pain and paved the way for psychological treatments for persistent pain, which routinely outperform other forms of treatment. This suggests that psychological processes could also be important in shaping the very early trajectory from acute to persistent pain and that targeting these processes could prevent the development of persistent pain. In this review, we develop an integrative model and suggest novel interventions during early pain trajectories, based on predictions from this model.
... Our FC findings represent an extension of these results, providing an exploration of further pain modulatory brain areas and demonstrating specific effects related to control-induced analgesia. Another study found bilateral DLPFC to suppress activity in both the thalamus and right insula, thus reducing pain sensitization during uncontrollable pain (Bräscher et al., 2016). The observed FC decreases between DLPFC and key pain-processing areas such as thalamus and insula during selfcontrolled trials paint a more accurate picture of the descending paininhibiting pathways that appear to be disrupted in FM. ...
Article
Full-text available
The perceived lack of control over the experience of pain is arguably-one major cause of agony and impaired life quality in patients with chronic pain disorders as fibromyalgia (FM). The way perceived control affects subjective pain as well as the underlying neural mechanisms have so far not been investigated in chronic pain. We used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to examine the neural correlates of self-controlled compared to computer-controlled heat pain in healthy controls (HC, n = 21) and FM patients (n = 23). Contrary to HC, FM failed to activate brain areas usually involved in pain modulation as well as reappraisal processes (right ventrolateral (VLPFC), dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC)). Computer-controlled (compared to self-controlled) heat revealed significant activations of the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) in HC, whereas FM activated structures that are typically involved in neural emotion processing (amygdala, parahippocampal gyrus). Additionally, FM displayed disrupted functional connectivity (FC) of the VLPFC, DLPFC and dACC with somatosensory and pain (inhibition)-related areas during self-controlled heat stimulation as well as significantly decreased gray matter (GM) volumes compared to HC in DLPFC and dACC. The described functional and structural changes provide evidence for far-reaching impairments concerning pain-modulatory processes in FM. Our investigation represents a first demonstration of dysfunctional neural pain modulation through experienced control in FM according to the extensive functional and structural changes in relevant sensory, limbic and associative brain areas. These areas may be targeted in clinical pain therapeutic methods involving TMS, neurofeedback or cognitive behavioral trainings.
... Additionally, markers of reduced neuronal viability have been observed using magnetic resonance spectroscopy in PFC generally (Siddall et al., 2006) and dlPFC specifically (Grachev et al., 2000) in CBP. Studies of both acute and chronic pain have found the connection between dlPFC and the thalamus to be inhibitory in nature (Bräscher et al., 2016;Grachev et al., 2000;Li et al., 2021a;Lorenz et al., 2003); thus, it follows that this connection would be weaker in CBP. ...
Article
Full-text available
Chronic back pain (CBP) has extensive clinical and social implications for its sufferers and is a major source of disability. Chronic pain has previously been shown to have central neural factors underpinning it, including the loss of white matter (WM), however traditional methods of analyzing WM microstructure have produced mixed and unclear results. To better understand these factors, we assessed the WM microstructure of 50 patients and 40 healthy controls (HC) using diffusion-weighted imaging. The data were analyzed using fixel-based analysis (FBA), a higher-order diffusion modelling technique applied to CBP for the first time here. Subjects also answered questionnaires relating to pain, disability, catastrophizing, and mood disorders, to establish the relationship between fixelwise metrics and clinical symptoms. FBA determined that, compared to HC, CBP patients had: 1) lower fibre density (FD) in several tracts, specifically the right anterior and bilateral superior thalamic radiations, right spinothalamic tract, right middle cerebellar peduncle, and the body and splenium of corpus callosum; 2) higher FD in the genu of corpus callosum; and 3) lower FDC – a combined fibre density and cross-section measure – in the bilateral spinothalamic tracts and right anterior thalamic radiation. Exploratory correlations showed strong negative relationships between fixelwise metrics and clinical questionnaire scores, especially pain catastrophizing. These results have important implications for the intake and processing of sensory data in CBP that warrant further investigation.
... These brain regions are included in the medial, lateral, and rostral pain networks (4). The fMRI-derived NPS is specific to physical pain and is based on repeated and reproducible BOLD signal activations during an evoked heat pain stimulus (24) and its accuracy has been replicated across multiple studies (72)(73)(74)(75)(76). Heat is one of the most common and most reliable (43) evoked pain stimuli used in psychophysical studies of AD and pain. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Pain continues to be underrecognized and undertreated in people with Alzheimer's disease (AD). The periaqueductal gray (PAG) is essential to pain processing and modulation yet is damaged by AD. While evidence exists of altered neural processing of pain in AD, there has not been a focused investigation of the PAG during pain in people with AD.PurposeTo investigate the role of the PAG in sensory and affective pain processing for people living with AD.Methods Participants from a larger study completed pain psychophysics assessments and then a perceptually-matched heat pain task (warmth, mild, and moderate pain) during a functional MRI scan. In this cross-sectional study, we examined blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) responses in the PAG and other pain-related regions in participants with AD (n = 18) and cognitively intact older adults (age- and sex-matched, n = 18). Associations of BOLD percent signal change and psychophysics were also examined.ResultsThere were significant main effects of AD status on the temperature needed to reach each perception of warmth or pain, where people with AD reached higher temperatures. Furthermore, participants with AD rated mild and moderate pain as more unpleasant than controls. PAG BOLD activation was greater in AD relative to controls during warmth and mild pain percepts. No significant differences were found for moderate pain or in other regions of interest. Greater PAG activation during mild pain was associated with higher affective/unpleasantness ratings of mild pain in participants with AD but not in controls.Conclusion Results suggest a role for the PAG in altered pain responses in people with AD. The PAG is the primary source of endogenous opioid pain inhibition in the neuroaxis, thus, altered PAG function in AD suggests possible changes in descending pain inhibitory circuits. People with AD may have a greater risk of suffering from pain compared to cognitively intact older adults.
Article
Full-text available
Purpose The aim of this study was to investigate alterations in the topological organization of whole-brain functional networks in patients with chronic low back pain (CLBP) and characterize the relationship of these alterations with pain characteristics. Methods Thirty-three CLBP patients and 34 matched healthy controls (HCs) underwent fMRI scans. A graph-theoretical approach was applied to identify brain network changes in patients suffering from chronic low back pain given its nonspecific etiology and complexity. Graph theory-based analysis was used to construct functional connectivity matrices and extract the features of small-world networks of the brain in both groups. Then, the whole-brain functional connectivity differences were characterized by network-based statistics (NBS) analysis, and the relationship between the altered brain features and clinical measures was explored. Results At the global level, patients with CLBP showed significantly decreased gamma, sigma, global efficiency, and local efficiency and increased lambda and shortest path length compared with HCs. At the regional level, there were deficits in nodal efficiency within the default mode network and salience network. NBS analysis demonstrated that decreased functional connectivity was present in the CLBP patients, mainly in the frontolimbic circuit and temporal regions. Furthermore, aspects of topological dysfunctions in CLBP were correlated with pain severity. Conclusion This study highlighted the aberrant topological organization of functional brain networks in CLBP, which may shed light on the pathophysiology of CLBP and support the development of pain management approaches.
Article
Objective: The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) has been increasingly used as a neuromodulatory target in pain management. Transcranial random noise stimulation (tRNS) was shown to effectively elevate cortical excitability. Hence, this study aimed to characterize how tRNS over the left DLPFC affects pain expectation and perception, as well as the efficacy of conditioned-pain modulation (CPM) that reflects the function of the endogenous pain-inhibitory pathway. Methods: Using a randomized, double-blinded, and sham-controlled design, healthy participants were randomly recruited to receive tRNS with a direct current offset or sham stimulation. Their expectations and perceptions of painful electrocutaneous stimuli, as well as CPM efficacy were assessed before, immediately after, and 30 min after tRNS. Results: Compared with sham stimulation, perceived-pain ratings to the painful stimuli, and expected-pain ratings before painful stimuli, attenuated immediately after tRNS, whereas this analgesic effect was ineffective 30 min after tRNS. Importantly, the immediate analgesia induced by tRNS could be accounted for by tRNS effect on attenuating expected-pain ratings before certain painful stimuli. However, CPM efficacy was not significantly affected by tRNS. Conclusions: These results demonstrate analgesia immediately after applying tRNS over the left DLPFC. Significance: This study provides evidence for analgesia of DLPFC-tRNS on an experimental pain model.
Article
Full-text available
Background Spinal anesthesia causes short-term deafferentation and alters the crosstalk among brain regions involved in pain perception and pain modulation. In the current study, we examined the effect of spinal anesthesia on pain response to noxious thermal stimuli in non-deafferented skin areas using a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) paradigm. Methods Twenty-two healthy subjects participated in the study. We performed a task-based fMRI study using a randomized crossover design. Subjects were scanned under two conditions (spinal anesthesia or control) at two-time points: before and after spinal anesthesia. Spinal anesthesia resulted in sensory loss up to dermatome Th6. Calibrated heat-pain stimuli were administered to the right forearm (C8-Th1) using a box-car design (blocks of 10s on/25s off) during MRI scanning. Pain perception was measured using a visual analogue scale (1–100) at the beginning and the end of each session. Generalized estimating equations were used to examine the effect of intervention by time by order on pain scores. Similarly, higher-level effects were tested with appropriate general linear models (accounting for within-subject variations in session and time) to examine: (1) Differences in BOLD response to pain stimulus under spinal anesthesia versus control; and (2) Effects of spinal anesthesia on pain-related modulation of the cerebral activation. Results Complete fMRI data was available for eighteen participants. Spinal anesthesia was associated with moderate pain score increase. Significant differences in brain response to noxious thermal stimuli were present in comparison of spinal versus control condition (post—pre). Spinal condition was associated with higher BOLD signal in the bilateral inferior parietal lobule and lower BOLD signal in bilateral postcentral and precentral gyrus. Within the angular regions, we observed a positive correlation between pain scores and BOLD signal. These observations were independent from order effect (whether the spinal anesthesia was administered in the first or the second visit). However, we did observe order effect on brain regions including medial prefrontal regions, possibly related to anticipation of the experience of spinal anesthesia. Conclusions The loss of sensory and motor activity caused by spinal anesthesia has a significant impact on brain regions involved in the sensorimotor and cognitive processing of noxious heat pain stimuli. Our results indicate that the anticipation or experience of a strong somatosensory response to the spinal intervention might confound and contribute to increased sensitivity to cognitive pain processing. Future studies must account for individual differences in subjective experience of pain sensation within the experimental context.
Article
Full-text available
Persistent pain is measured by means of self-report, the sole reliance on which hampers diagnosis and treatment. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) holds promise for identifying objective measures of pain, but brain measures that are sensitive and specific to physical pain have not yet been identified. In four studies involving a total of 114 participants, we developed an fMRI-based measure that predicts pain intensity at the level of the individual person. In study 1, we used machine-learning analyses to identify a pattern of fMRI activity across brain regions--a neurologic signature--that was associated with heat-induced pain. The pattern included the thalamus, the posterior and anterior insulae, the secondary somatosensory cortex, the anterior cingulate cortex, the periaqueductal gray matter, and other regions. In study 2, we tested the sensitivity and specificity of the signature to pain versus warmth in a new sample. In study 3, we assessed specificity relative to social pain, which activates many of the same brain regions as physical pain. In study 4, we assessed the responsiveness of the measure to the analgesic agent remifentanil. In study 1, the neurologic signature showed sensitivity and specificity of 94% or more (95% confidence interval [CI], 89 to 98) in discriminating painful heat from nonpainful warmth, pain anticipation, and pain recall. In study 2, the signature discriminated between painful heat and nonpainful warmth with 93% sensitivity and specificity (95% CI, 84 to 100). In study 3, it discriminated between physical pain and social pain with 85% sensitivity (95% CI, 76 to 94) and 73% specificity (95% CI, 61 to 84) and with 95% sensitivity and specificity in a forced-choice test of which of two conditions was more painful. In study 4, the strength of the signature response was substantially reduced when remifentanil was administered. It is possible to use fMRI to assess pain elicited by noxious heat in healthy persons. Future studies are needed to assess whether the signature predicts clinical pain. (Funded by the National Institute on Drug Abuse and others.).
Article
Full-text available
Predictions about sensory input exert a dominant effect on what we perceive, and this is particularly true for the experience of pain. However, it remains unclear what component of prediction, from an information-theoretic perspective, controls this effect. We used a vicarious pain observation paradigm to study how the underlying statistics of predictive information modulate experience. Subjects observed judgments that a group of people made to a painful thermal stimulus, before receiving the same stimulus themselves. We show that the mean observed rating exerted a strong assimilative effect on subjective pain. In addition, we show that observed uncertainty had a specific and potent hyperalgesic effect. Using computational functional magnetic resonance imaging, we found that this effect correlated with activity in the periaqueductal gray. Our results provide evidence for a novel form of cognitive hyperalgesia relating to perceptual uncertainty, induced here by vicarious observation, with control mediated by the brainstem pain modulatory system.
Article
Full-text available
does perceived control reduce the negative effects of laboratory pain / pain coping strategies and perceived control in the laboratory / acute clinical pain / chronic pain why does perceived control reduce pain / anxiety increases the experience of pain and controllability reduces anxiety / perceiving control promotes endogenous opioid release / controllability changes the meaning of pain or the meaning of the cause of the pain / perceived control is a positive experience per se which can compensate negative experiences / perceiving control allows people to distract themselves from pain (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
The mechanism of brain reorganization in pain chronification is unknown. In a longitudinal brain imaging study, subacute back pain (SBP) patients were followed over the course of 1 year. When pain persisted (SBPp, in contrast to recovering SBP and healthy controls), brain gray matter density decreased. Initially greater functional connectivity of nucleus accumbens with prefrontal cortex predicted pain persistence, implying that corticostriatal circuitry is causally involved in the transition from acute to chronic pain.
Article
Full-text available
The psychophysical features of the transition from the pure heat to the heat pain range were studied in 25 healthy subjects (mean age 28.8 years). Thirty short heat stimuli from −1.6° C to + 1.6° C relative to the pain threshold were applied to the thenar of the left hand with an apparatus containing a Peltier thermode (nine different temperatures at 0.4°C intervals). The subjects rated the sensation intensity on a visual analogue scale. The resulting stimulus/sensation intensity relations could be explained equally well (same goodness of fit) by a model with a power function (PF) and by a model with two linear regression lines (TLR), one for stimulus intensities below and one for those above the pain threshold and intersecting at the pain threshold. The slopes of the TLR model were significantly larger above the pain threshold than below it. The PF model produced exponents between 1.8 and 1.9. We conclude that to describe the transition area, it is sufficient to use simple linear models for both the pure heat and the heat pain ranges.
Article
Full-text available
Psychophysiological interactions (PPIs) analysis is a method for investigating task-specific changes in the relationship between activity in different brain areas, using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data. Specifically, PPI analyses identify voxels in which activity is more related to activity in a seed region of interest (seed ROI) in a given psychological context, such as during attention or in the presence of emotive stimuli. In this tutorial, we aim to give a simple conceptual explanation of how PPI analysis works, in order to assist readers in planning and interpreting their own PPI experiments.
Article
This article discusses general modeling of multisubject and/or multisession FMRI data. In particular, we show that a two-level mixed-effects model (where parameters of interest at the group level are estimated from parameter and variance estimates from the single-session level) can be made equivalent to a single complete mixed-effects model (where parameters of interest at the group level are estimated directly from all of the original single sessions' time series data) if the (co-)variance at the second level is set equal to the sum of the (co-)variances in the single-level form, using the BLUE with known covariances. This result has significant implications for group studies in FMRI, since it shows that the group analysis requires only values of the parameter estimates and their (co-)variance from the first level, generalizing the well-established "summary statistics" approach in FMRI. The simple and generalized framework allows different prewhitening and different first-level regressors to be used for each subject. The framework incorporates multiple levels and cases such as repeated measures, paired or unpaired t tests and F tests at the group level; explicit examples of such models are given in the article. Using numerical simulations based on typical first-level covariance structures from real FMRI data we demonstrate that by taking into account lower-level covariances and heterogeneity a substantial increase in higher-level Z score is possible.
Article
Pain stimuli evoke widespread responses in the brain. However, our understanding of the physiological significance underlying heterogeneous response within different pain-activated and -deactivated regions is still limited. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging, we evaluated brain responses to a wide range of stimulus intensity levels (1 innocuous, 7 painful) in order to estimate region-specific stimulus-response functions, which we hypothesized could illuminate that region's functional relationship to pain. Linear and nonlinear brain responses to pain were estimated through independent Legendre polynomial transformations of pain ratings within a general linear model. This approach identified at least 5 different, regionally specific activity profiles in the brain. Linearly increasing (eg, primary somatosensory/motor cortex, insulae) and intensity-independent (eg, secondary somatosensory cortex) activation was noted in traditional pain-processing areas, potentially reflecting sensory encoding and all-or-none salience responses, respectively. Multiple activity profiles were seen in areas of the default mode network (DMN): intensity-independent deactivation (eg, posterior cingulate cortex), linearly decreasing (eg, contralateral inferior parietal lobule), and quadratic (U-shaped; eg, medial prefrontal cortex). The latter observation suggests that: (1) different DMN subregions exhibit functional heterogeneity and (2) some DMN subregions respond in a percept-related manner to pain, suggesting closer linkage between the DMN and pain processing than previously thought. Future studies should apply a similar approach using innocuous stimuli of multiple intensities to evaluate whether the response profiles reported here can also be generalized to nonpainful somatosensory processing.