ArticlePDF AvailableLiterature Review

Intense, Passionate, Romantic Love: A Natural Addiction? How the Fields That Investigate Romance and Substance Abuse Can Inform Each Other

Frontiers
Frontiers in Psychology
Authors:

Abstract

Individuals in the early stage of intense romantic love show many symptoms of substance and non-substance or behavioral addictions, including euphoria, craving, tolerance, emotional and physical dependence, withdrawal and relapse. We have proposed that romantic love is a natural (and often positive) addiction that evolved from mammalian antecedents by 4 million years ago as a survival mechanism to encourage hominin pair-bonding and reproduction, seen cross-culturally today in Homo sapiens. Brain scanning studies using functional magnetic resonance imaging support this view: feelings of intense romantic love engage regions of the brain’s “reward system,” specifically dopamine-rich regions, including the ventral tegmental area, also activated during drug and/or behavioral addiction. Thus, because the experience of romantic love shares reward pathways with a range of substance and behavioral addictions, it may influence the drug and/or behavioral addiction response. Indeed, a study of overnight abstinent smokers has shown that feelings of intense romantic love attenuate brain activity associated with cigarette cue-reactivity. Could socially rewarding experiences be therapeutic for drug and/or behavioral addictions? We suggest that “self expanding” experiences like romance and expanding one’s knowledge, experience and self-perception, may also affect drug and/or behavioral addiction behaviors. Further, because feelings of romantic love can progress into feelings of calm attachment, and because attachment engages more plastic forebrain regions, there is a rationale for therapies that may help substance and/or behavioral addiction by promoting activation of these forebrain systems through long-term, calm, positive attachments to others, including group therapies. Addiction is considered a negative (harmful) disorder that appears in a population subset; while romantic love is often a positive (as well as negative) state experienced by almost all humans. Thus, researchers have not categorized romantic love as a chemical or behavioral addiction. But by embracing data on romantic love, it’s classification as an evolved, natural, often positive but also powerfully negative addiction, and its neural similarity to many substance and non-substance addictive states, clinicians may develop more effective therapeutic approaches to alleviate a range of the addictions, including heartbreak–an almost universal human experience that can trigger stalking, clinical depression, suicide, homicide, and other crimes of passion.
fpsyg-07-00687 May 6, 2016 Time: 16:17 # 1
REVIEW
published: 10 May 2016
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00687
Edited by:
Xiaochu Zhang,
University of Science and Technology
of China, China
Reviewed by:
Ricardo De Oliveira-Souza,
Federal University of the State of Rio
de Janeiro, Brazil
Sabine Vollstädt-Klein,
Heidelberg University, Germany
*Correspondence:
Lucy L. Brown
lucy.brown@einstein.yu.edu
Specialty section:
This article was submitted to
Cognitive Science,
a section of the journal
Frontiers in Psychology
Received: 08 February 2016
Accepted: 25 April 2016
Published: 10 May 2016
Citation:
Fisher HE, Xu X, Aron A and Brown LL
(2016) Intense, Passionate, Romantic
Love: A Natural Addiction? How
the Fields That Investigate Romance
and Substance Abuse Can Inform
Each Other. Front. Psychol. 7:687.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00687
Intense, Passionate, Romantic Love:
A Natural Addiction? How the Fields
That Investigate Romance and
Substance Abuse Can Inform Each
Other
Helen E. Fisher1, Xiaomeng Xu2, Arthur Aron3and Lucy L. Brown4*
1The Kinsey Institute, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA, 2Department of Psychology, Idaho State University,
Pocatello, ID, USA, 3Department of Psychology, The State University of New York Stony Brook, Stony Brook, NY, USA,
4Department of Neurology, Albert Einstein College of Medicine, Bronx, NY, USA
Individuals in the early stage of intense romantic love show many symptoms of
substance and non-substance or behavioral addictions, including euphoria, craving,
tolerance, emotional and physical dependence, withdrawal and relapse. We have
proposed that romantic love is a natural (and often positive) addiction that evolved
from mammalian antecedents by 4 million years ago as a survival mechanism to
encourage hominin pair-bonding and reproduction, seen cross-culturally today in Homo
sapiens. Brain scanning studies using functional magnetic resonance imaging support
this view: feelings of intense romantic love engage regions of the brain’s “reward
system,” specifically dopamine-rich regions, including the ventral tegmental area, also
activated during drug and/or behavioral addiction. Thus, because the experience of
romantic love shares reward pathways with a range of substance and behavioral
addictions, it may influence the drug and/or behavioral addiction response. Indeed, a
study of overnight abstinent smokers has shown that feelings of intense romantic love
attenuate brain activity associated with cigarette cue-reactivity. Could socially rewarding
experiences be therapeutic for drug and/or behavioral addictions? We suggest that “self
expanding” experiences like romance and expanding one’s knowledge, experience and
self-perception, may also affect drug and/or behavioral addiction behaviors. Further,
because feelings of romantic love can progress into feelings of calm attachment, and
because attachment engages more plastic forebrain regions, there is a rationale for
therapies that may help substance and/or behavioral addiction by promoting activation
of these forebrain systems through long-term, calm, positive attachments to others,
including group therapies. Addiction is considered a negative (harmful) disorder that
appears in a population subset; while romantic love is often a positive (as well
as negative) state experienced by almost all humans. Thus, researchers have not
categorized romantic love as a chemical or behavioral addiction. But by embracing
data on romantic love, it’s classification as an evolved, natural, often positive but
also powerfully negative addiction, and its neural similarity to many substance and
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 1May 2016 | Volume 7 | Article 687
fpsyg-07-00687 May 6, 2016 Time: 16:17 # 2
Fisher et al. Romantic Love As an Addiction
non-substance addictive states, clinicians may develop more effective therapeutic
approaches to alleviate a range of the addictions, including heartbreak–an almost
universal human experience that can trigger stalking, clinical depression, suicide,
homicide, and other crimes of passion.
Keywords: romantic love, addiction, ventral tegmental area, caudate
INTRODUCTION
We propose that romantic love is a natural addiction (Frascella
et al., 2010) that evolved from mammalian antecedents (Fisher
et al., 2006). Brain scanning studies show that feelings of intense
romantic love engage regions of the brain’s “reward system,
specifically dopamine pathways associated with energy, focus,
learning, motivation, ecstasy, and craving, including primary
regions associated with substance addiction, such as the ventral
tegmental area (VTA), caudate and accumbens (Breiter et al.,
1997;Bartels and Zeki, 2000, 2004;Fisher et al., 2003, 2005, 2006,
2010;Aron et al., 2005;Ortigue et al., 2007;Acevedo et al., 2011;
Xu et al., 2011). Several of these reward regions of the mesolimbic
system associated with romantic love and substance addiction
are also activated during non-substance or behavioral addiction,
including viewing images of appealing food (Wang et al., 2004),
shopping (Knutson et al., 2007), playing video games (Hoeft
et al., 2008), and gambling (Breiter et al., 2001). Indeed, several
researchers have taken the position that “addiction is a disease
of the reward system” (Rosenberg and Feder, 2014). Moreover,
men and women who are passionately in love and/or rejected in
love show the basic symptoms of substance-related and gambling
addiction listed in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
Mental Disorders-5, including craving, mood modification,
tolerance, emotional and physical dependence and withdrawal.
Relapse is also a common problem for those suffering with a
substance and/or behavioral addiction, as well as among rejected
lovers.
Because passionate romantic love is regularly associated with
a suite of traits linked with all addictions, several psychologists
have come to believe that romantic love can potentially become
an addiction (Peele, 1975;Tennov, 1979;Hunter et al., 1981;
Halpern, 1982;Schaef, 1989;Griffin-Shelley, 1991;Mellody
et al., 1992). However, many define addiction as a pathological,
problematic disorder (Reynaud et al., 2010); and because
romantic love is a positive experience under many circumstances
(i.e., not harmful), researchers remain hesitant to officially
categorize romantic love as an addiction. But even when romantic
love can’t be regarded as harmful, it is associated with intense
craving and can impel the lover to believe, say and do dangerous
and inappropriate things.
All forms of substance abuse, including alcohol, opioids,
cocaine, amphetamines, cannabis, and tobacco activate reward
pathways (Breiter et al., 1997;Melis et al., 2005;Volkow et al.,
2007;Frascella et al., 2010;Koob and Volkow, 2010;Diana, 2013),
as do several of the behavioral addictions (see Cuzen and Stein,
2014); and several of these same reward pathways are also found
to be activated among men and women who are happily in
love, as well as those rejected in love (Bartels and Zeki, 2000,
2004;Fisher et al., 2003, 2010;Aron et al., 2005;Ortigue et al.,
2007;Acevedo et al., 2011;Xu et al., 2011). So regardless of
its official diagnostic classification, we propose that romantic
love should be considered as an addiction (Fisher, 2004, 2016):
a positive addiction when one’s love is reciprocated, non-toxic
and appropriate, and a negative addiction when one’s feelings of
romantic love are socially inappropriate, toxic, not reciprocated
and/or formally rejected (Fisher, 2004;Frascella et al., 2010).
Romantic love may have evolved at the basal radiation of
the hominin clade some 4.4 million years ago in conjunction
with the evolution of serial social monogamy and clandestine
adultery–hallmarks of the human reproductive strategy (Fisher,
1998, 2004, 2011, 2016). Its purpose may have been to motivate
our forebears to focus their mating time and energy on a single
partner at a time, thus initiating the formation of a pair-bond to
rear their young together as a team (Fisher, 1992, 1998, 2004,
2011, 2016;Fisher et al., 2006;Fletcher et al., 2015). Thus, as
products of human evolution, the neural systems for romantic
love and mate attachment could be considered as survival systems
among humans.
ADDICTION-LIKE BEHAVIORS IN EARLY
STAGE, PASSIONATE LOVERS:
EUPHORIA, OBSESSION, RISKY
BEHAVIOR
Men and women in the early stage of intense passionate romantic
love express many of the basic traits associated with all addiction
(Tennov, 1979;Liebowitz, 1983;Hatfield and Sprecher, 1986;
Harris, 1995;Lewis et al., 2000;Meloy and Fisher, 2005;American
Psychiatric Association, 2013). Like all addicts, they focus on
their beloved (salience); and they yearn for the beloved (craving).
They feel a “rush of exhilaration when seeing or thinking about
him or her (euphoria/intoxication). As their relationship builds,
the lover seeks to interact with the beloved more and more
frequently (tolerance). If the beloved breaks off the relationship,
the lover experiences the common signs of drug withdrawal,
too, including protest, crying spells, lethargy, anxiety, insomnia,
or hypersomnia, loss of appetite or binge eating, irritability
and chronic loneliness. Like most addicts, rejected lovers also
often go to extremes, even sometimes doing degrading or
physically dangerous things to win back the beloved (Meloy,
1998;Lewis et al., 2000;Meloy and Fisher, 2005). Romantic
partners are willing to sacrifice, even die for the other. Romantic
jealousy is particularly dangerous and can lead to major crimes
including homicide, and/or suicide. Lovers also relapse the way
drug addicts do: long after the relationship is over, events,
people, places, songs, and/or other external cues associated with
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 2May 2016 | Volume 7 | Article 687
fpsyg-07-00687 May 6, 2016 Time: 16:17 # 3
Fisher et al. Romantic Love As an Addiction
their abandoning sweetheart can trigger memories and initiate
renewed craving, obsessive thinking and/or compulsive calling,
writing or showing up in hopes of rekindling the romance–
despite what they suspect may lead to adverse consequences.
Passionate lovers also express strong sexual desire for the
beloved; yet their yearning for emotional union tends to
overshadow their craving for sexual union with him or her
(Tennov, 1979). Most characteristic, the lover thinks obsessively
about the beloved (intrusive thinking). Besotted lovers may
also compulsively follow, incessantly call, write or unexpectedly
appear, all in an effort to be with their beloved day and night
(Tennov, 1979;Lewis et al., 2000;Meloy and Fisher, 2005).
Paramount to this experience is intense motivation to win
him or her. All these behaviors are common to those with
substance addictions. However, not everyone exhibits these types
of behaviors after a breakup, just as not everyone who uses
a substance exhibits dependency and withdrawal effects (e.g.,
Shiffman, 1989;Shiffman et al., 1995;Shiffman and Paty, 2006;
Haney, 2009).
THE BRAIN SYSTEMS ASSOCIATED
WITH ROMANTIC LOVE
Neuroimaging studies of intense, passionate romantic love reveal
the physiological underpinnings of this universal or near-
universal human experience, and they all show activation of the
VTA (Fisher et al., 2003, 2010;Bartels and Zeki, 2004;Aron
et al., 2005;Ortigue et al., 2007;Zeki and Romaya, 2010;Acevedo
et al., 2011;Xu et al., 2011). In our first experiment (Fisher et al.,
2003;Aron et al., 2005), we used functional magnetic resonance
imaging (fMRI) to study 10 women and 7 men who had recently
fallen intensely and happily in love. All scored high on the
Passionate Love Scale (Hatfield and Sprecher, 1986), a self-report
questionnaire that measures the intensity of romantic feelings; all
participants also reported that they spent more than 85% of their
waking hours thinking of their beloved.
Participants alternately viewed a photograph of their
sweetheart and a photograph of a familiar individual,
interspersed with a distraction-attention task. Group activation
occurred in several regions of the brain’s reward system,
including the VTA and caudate nucleus (Fisher et al., 2003;Aron
et al., 2005), regions associated with pleasure, general arousal,
focused attention and motivation to pursue and acquire rewards
and mediated primarily by dopamine system activity (Delgado
et al., 2000;Schultz, 2000;Elliott et al., 2003). These regions of
the reward system are directly associated with addiction in many
studies of drugs of abuse (Breiter et al., 1997;Panksepp et al.,
2002;Melis et al., 2005;Volkow et al., 2007;Frascella et al., 2010;
Koob and Volkow, 2010;Diana, 2013) and behavioral addictions
(see Cuzen and Stein, 2014).
These data from several studies indicate that individuals who
are happily in the early stages of passionate love express activity
in neural regions associated with drug and some behavioral
addictions.
There is also a difference between “wanting” and
“liking/pleasure” suggested by Berridge et al. (2009). As in
substance addiction, “wanting” the romantic partner is different
from “liking” a pretty face and finding pleasure in a beautiful
sight. We found that brain activation to an attractive face
(“liking”) was different from activation to the beloved partner
(“wanting”): the former activated the left VTA, while the
latter activated the right VTA (Aron et al., 2005). The result
suggests the addictive aspects of romantic love are mediated
through the right VTA, and that pleasure, or “liking” is
different.
ADDICTION-LIKE BEHAVIORS
ASSOCIATED WITH ROMANTIC
REJECTION: CRAVING, RELAPSE AND
DESTRUCTIVE BEHAVIOR
Cross-culturally, few men or women avoid suffering from
romantic rejection at some point across their lives. In one
American college community, 93% of both sexes queried
reported that they had been spurned by someone whom they
passionately loved; 95% reported they had rejected someone who
was deeply in love with them (Baumeister et al., 1993). Romantic
rejection can cause a profound sense of loss and negative affect
(although this is not always the case e.g., Lewandowski and
Bizzoco, 2007). Like many addictions, romantic rejection can
also jeopardize one’s health, because abandonment rage stresses
the heart, raises blood pressure and suppresses the immune
system (Dozier, 2002). It can also induce clinical depression,
and in extreme cases lead to suicide and/or homicide. Some
broken-hearted lovers even die from heart attacks or strokes
caused by their depression (Rosenthal, 2002). The suite of
negative phenomena associated with rejection in love, including
protest, the stress response, frustration attraction, abandonment
rage, and jealousy, in conjunction with craving and withdrawal
symptoms, most likely also contribute to the high worldwide
incidence of crimes of passion (see Meloy, 1998;Meloy and
Fisher, 2005).
One pathology is also regularly associated with romantic love,
stalking. There are two common types of stalkers: those who
sustain pursuit of a former sexual/romantic intimate who has
rejected them; and those who pursue a stranger or acquaintance
who has failed to return the stalker’s romantic overtures (Meloy
and Fisher, 2005). In both cases, the stalker exhibits several
of the characteristic components of all addictions, including
focused attention on the love object, increased energy, following
behaviors, and obsessive thinking about and impulsivity directed
toward the victim, suggesting that stalking also activates aspects
of the reward system in the brain (Meloy and Fisher, 2005) and
may be akin to addiction. Another pathology, de Clerambault’s
syndrome, also known as erotomania, has not been associated
with addiction. This syndrome is characterized by the patient’s
delusional notion that another person is madly in love with
him or her; generally it is a young woman who believes
that she is the love object of a man of higher social or
professional standing. But because this syndrome has no direct
association with reward system activity and may be a form
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 3May 2016 | Volume 7 | Article 687
fpsyg-07-00687 May 6, 2016 Time: 16:17 # 4
Fisher et al. Romantic Love As an Addiction
of paranoid schizophrenia or other delusional disorder (Jordan
and Howe, 1980;Kopelman et al., 2008) rather than addiction,
discussion of this syndrome is beyond the scope of this
paper.
It appears, however, as if evolution has overdone the negative
response to romantic abandonment. But romantically rejected
individuals have wasted precious courtship time and metabolic
energy; they have lost essential economic and financial resources;
their social alliances have been jeopardized; their daily rituals
and habits have been altered; they may have lost property; and
they have most likely experienced damage to their personal
happiness, self-esteem and reputation (see Leary, 2001;Fisher,
2004). Most important, rejected lovers of reproductive age are
likely to have lost breeding opportunities or a parenting partner
for the offspring they have already produced—forms of reduced
future genetic viability (Fisher, 2004). Thus, romantic rejection
can have severe social, psychological, economic, and reproductive
consequences.
ROMANTIC REJECTION ALSO
ACTIVATES BRAIN REGIONS
ASSOCIATED WITH DRUG CRAVING
To identify some of the neural systems associated with this
natural craving state elicited by romantic rejection, we used
fMRI to study 10 women and 5 men who had recently been
rejected by a partner, but reported that they were still intensely
“in love” (Fisher et al., 2010). The average length of time since the
initial rejection and the participants’ enrollment in the study was
63 days. All scored high on the Passionate Love Scale (Hatfield
and Sprecher, 1986); all reported that they spent most of their
waking hours thinking about the person who rejected them;
and all yearned for their abandoning partner to return to the
relationship.
Participants alternately viewed a photograph of their rejecting
partner and a photograph of a familiar, emotionally neutral
individual, interspersed with a distraction-attention task. Their
responses while looking at their rejecter in the scanner included
feelings of romantic passion, despair, joyous, and painful
memories, rumination about why this had happened, and mental
assessments of their gains and losses from the experience. Brain
activations coupled with viewing the rejecter occurred in several
regions of the brain’s reward system. Included were: the VTA
associated with feelings of intense romantic love; the ventral
pallidum associated with feelings of attachment; the insular
cortex and the anterior cingulate associated with physical pain
and the distress associated with physical pain; and the nucleus
accumbens and orbitofrontal/prefrontal cortex associated with
assessing one’s gains and losses, as well as craving and addiction
(Fisher et al., 2010). Activity in several of these brain regions has
been correlated with craving for cocaine and other drugs of abuse
(Melis et al., 2005;Frascella et al., 2010;Koob and Volkow, 2010;
Diana, 2013).
To understand the impact of right VTA activations associated
with happy early stage relationships and romantic rejection, it
is important to consider both “liking” (hedonic impact) and
“wanting” (e.g., incentive salience) aspects of reward. That is,
approach behavior and desired interaction with a person or a
substance may or may not involve actual pleasurable experiences.
In the context of addiction, it is often the case that a strong desire
for the substance or a behavioral addiction, approach motivation
and use, occurs even when the stimuli no longer provides a “high”
and the reward-seeking behavior is associated with negative
outcomes (e.g., the addiction is detrimental to the individual’s
health, career, social relationships etc.). Those who are rejected
in love still “want” the ex-partner and experience approach
motivation (e.g., desiring to contact the ex-partner) even when
contact with the ex may be accompanied by negative outcomes
and not pleasurable (e.g., experiences of sadness and pain).
A distinction between hedonic impact and incentive salience has
been explored in animal studies (Berridge et al., 2009). We also
found that looking at the partners face activated the right VTA
while left VTA activation correlated with the attractiveness of
faces in the study (Aron et al., 2005).
ATTACHMENT
For those who stay in a relationship beyond the early stage,
intense romantic phase, an important second constellation of
feelings sets in, associated with attachment (Acevedo et al., 2011).
In our studies of individuals who are happily in love (Fisher
et al., 2003;Aron et al., 2005), we found that those in longer
partnerships (8–17 months as opposed to 1–8 months) began to
show activity in the ventral pallidum, associated with attachment
in animal studies (Insel and Young, 2001), while continuing to
show activity in the VTA and caudate nucleus associated with
passionate romantic love. Thus, with time, feelings of attachment
begin to accompany feelings of passionate romantic love (Fisher,
2004;Acevedo et al., 2011). Working in conjunction, these two
basic neural systems for romantic love and attachment may
constitute the biological foundation of human pair-bonding—
and provide the context for the evolution of love addictions
(Insel, 2003;Burkett and Young, 2012;Fisher, 2016).
EVOLUTION OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND
ATTACHMENT
It has been proposed that the neural systems associated with
feelings of intense romantic love and partner attachment evolved
in conjunction with the evolution of the human predisposition
for pair-bonding, serving as mechanisms to stimulate mate choice
and motivating individuals to remain with a mate long enough to
breed and rear their offspring through infancy as a team (Fisher,
2004, 2011, 2016;Fisher et al., 2006). This hypothesis suggests
that the neural systems for romantic love and attachment are
survival systems with evolutionary roots (Frascella et al., 2010).
Pair-bonding is a hallmark of humanity. Data from the
Demographic Yearbooks of the United Nations on 97 societies
canvassed in the 1980s indicate that approximately 93.1% of
women and 91.8% of men in that decade married by age 49
(Fisher, 1989, 1992). Worldwide, marriage rates have declined
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 4May 2016 | Volume 7 | Article 687
fpsyg-07-00687 May 6, 2016 Time: 16:17 # 5
Fisher et al. Romantic Love As an Addiction
since then; but today 85 to 90% of men and women in the
United States are projected to marry (Cherlin, 2009). Cross-
culturally, most individuals are monogamous; they form a sexual
and socially sanctioned partnership with one person at a time.
Polygyny (many females) is permitted in 84% of human societies;
but in the vast majority of these cultures, only 5 to 10% of
men actually have several wives simultaneously (Van den Berghe,
1979;Frayser, 1985). Moreover, because polygyny in humans
is regularly associated with rank and wealth, monogamy (i.e.,
pair-bonding) may have been even more prevalent in the pre-
horticultural, unstratified societies of our long human hunting-
gathering past (Daly and Wilson, 1983), when the neural systems
for intense early stage romantic love and partner attachment most
likely evolved.
Data suggest that the human predisposition for pair-bonding
(often preceded by romantic attraction) also has a biological
basis. The investigation of human attachment began with Bowlby
(1969, 1973) and Ainsworth et al. (1978) who proposed that,
to promote the survival of the young, primates have evolved
an innate attachment system designed to motivate infants to
seek comfort and safety from their primary caregiver, generally
the mother. Since these early studies, extensive research has
been done on the behaviors, feelings and neural mechanisms
associated with this attachment system in adult humans and
other animals (Fraley and Shaver, 2000;Eisenberger et al., 2003;
Panksepp, 2003a,b;Bartels and Zeki, 2004;MacDonald and Leary,
2005;Tucker et al., 2005;Noriuchi et al., 2008). Currently,
researchers believe that this biologically based attachment system
remains active throughout the human life course, serving as the
foundation for attachment between pair-bonded partners for the
purpose of raising offspring (Hazan and Shaver, 1987;Hazan and
Diamond, 2000).
Pair-bonding could have evolved at any point in hominin
evolution; and with it, various love addictions (Fisher, 2016).
However, two lines of data suggest that the neural circuitry for
human pair-bonding may have evolved at the basal radiation of
the hominin stock (Fisher, 1992, 2011, 2016), in tandem with the
hominin adaptation to the woodland/savannah eco-niche some
time prior to 4 million years B.P. Ardipithecus ramidus, currently
dated at 4.4 million years B.P., displays several physical traits that
have been linked with pair-bonding in many species (Lovejoy,
2009); so Lovejoy (2009) proposes that human monogamy had
evolved by this time. Anthropologists have also re-measured
Australopithecus afarensis fossils for skeletal variations; and they
report that by 3.5 million years B.P. hominins exhibited roughly
the same degree of sexual dimorphism in several physical traits
that the sexes exhibit today. Thus, some have proposed that
these hominins were “principally monogamous” (Reno et al.,
2003).
The emergence of bipedalism may have been a primary
factor in the evolution of the neural circuitry for hominin
pair-bonding (Fisher, 1992, 2011, 2016) and the concomitant
evolution of romantic love (and possibly attachment) addiction.
While foraging and scavenging in the woodland/savannah eco-
niche, bipedal Ardipithecine females were most likely obliged to
carry infants in their arms instead of on their backs, thus needing
the protection and provisioning of a mate while they transported
nursing young. Meanwhile, Ardipithecine males may have had
considerable difficulty protecting and providing for a harem of
females in this open woodland/savannah eco-niche. But a male
could defend and provision a single female with her infant as
they walked near one another, within the vicinity of the larger
community.
So the exigencies of bipedalism in conjunction with hominin
expansion into the woodland/savannah eco-niche may have
pushed Ardipithecines over the “monogamy threshold, selecting
for the neural system for attachment to a pair-bonded partner.
And along with the evolution of pair-bonding and the neural
system for attachment may have emerged the brain system for
intense positive romantic addiction—serving to motivate males
and females to focus their mating energy on a single partner and
remain together long enough to trigger feelings of attachment
necessary to initiate and complete their co-parenting duties of
highly altricial young (Fisher, 1992, 2004, 2011, 2016).
HUMAN ROMANTIC LOVE AS A
DEVELOPED FORM OF A MAMMALIAN
COURTSHIP MECHANISM
Considerable data suggest that the human brain system for
romantic love arose from mammalian antecedents. Like
humans, all birds and mammals exhibit mate preferences; they
focus their courtship energy on favored potential mates and
disregard or avoid others (Fisher, 2004;Fisher et al., 2006).
Moreover, most of the basic traits associated with human
romantic love are also characteristic of mammalian courtship
attraction, including increased energy, focused attention,
obsessive following, affiliative gestures, possessive mate guarding,
goal-oriented behaviors and motivation to win and keep a
preferred mating partner for the duration of one’s species-specific
reproductive and parenting needs (Fisher et al., 2002, 2006;
Fisher, 2004).
The brain system for human romantic love shows biological
similarities with mammalian neural systems for courtship
attraction. When a female laboratory–maintained prairie vole
is mated with a male, she forms a distinct preference for him,
associated with a 50% increase of dopamine in the nucleus
accumbens (Gingrich et al., 2000). When a dopamine antagonist
is injected into the nucleus accumbens, the female no longer
prefers this partner; and when a female is injected with a
dopamine agonist, she begins to prefer the conspecific who
is present at the time of the infusion, even if she has not
mated with this male (Wang et al., 1999;Gingrich et al.,
2000). An increase in the activities of central dopamine is also
associated with courtship attraction in female sheep (Fabre-
Nys et al., 1997). In male rats, increased striatal dopamine
release has also been shown in response to the presence of a
receptive female rat (Robinson et al., 2002;Montague et al.,
2004).
Because human romantic love shares many behavioral and
biological characteristics with mammalian courtship attraction,
it is likely that human romantic love is a developed form
of this mammalian neural courtship mechanism (Fisher, 1998,
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 5May 2016 | Volume 7 | Article 687
fpsyg-07-00687 May 6, 2016 Time: 16:17 # 6
Fisher et al. Romantic Love As an Addiction
2004, 2011,2016;Fisher et al., 2006). However, in most species
courtship attraction is brief, lasting only minutes, hours, days,
or weeks; while in humans, intense, early stage romantic love
can last 12–18 months (Marazziti et al., 1999) or much longer
(Acevedo et al., 2011). So in early hominin prehistory, activity
in this mammalian neural system for courtship attraction
may have become intensified and prolonged as pair-bonding
evolved, eventually becoming the positive (or negative) romantic
addictions experienced by men and women cross-culturally
today.
ROMANTIC LOVE MAY ACT AS A
REWARD REPLACEMENT FOR OTHER
ADDICTIONS
High quality social relationships (including romantic
relationships) can be extremely beneficial to those recovering
from an addiction (e.g., Hänninen and Koski-Jännes, 1999).
One potential mechanism for this benefit comes from the
therapeutic approach to drug addiction of reward replacement.
That is, when quitting one addictive substance or behavior,
the addicted individual replaces this addiction with another
form of rewarding behavior, often without prompting from an
outside source, such as a clinician (Donovan, 1988;Marks, 1990;
DiNardo and Lemieux, 2001;Haylett et al., 2004;Alter et al.,
2006). Because of this, clinicians who treat addictions are known
to effectively engage patients in new reinforcers (see Bickel et al.,
2014), specifically healthy replacement reinforcers such as sports
activities, new hobbies and more or new social interactions (e.g.,
Vaillant, 1983;Salvy et al., 2009;Liu et al., 2011).
Could early stage romance provide a replacement reward for
those engaged in substance abuse (or a behavioral addiction)? To
explore this question, Xu et al. (2012) put 18 Chinese overnight
nicotine-deprived smokers who had just fallen madly in love
into a brain scanner, using fMRI. These men and women looked
at side-by-side photos, one of a hand holding either a lighted
cigarette (cue) or a pencil (control) and one of their newly
beloved or a familiar acquaintance (non-smokers so they were not
cigarette-cues). Among those who were moderately addicted to
nicotine, when the cigarette cue was presented next to the image
of the beloved (compared to the acquaintance), less activation
was observed in regions associated with cigarette cue-reactivity.
Additionally, more activation in the caudate was observed during
trials that included the beloved’s pictures (compared to the
acquaintance’s).
These preliminary data provide more evidence that romantic
love could be considered a powerful and primordial natural
addiction because it can, under some circumstances, modify
brain activations associated with a more contemporary addiction,
nicotine.
“Self-expansion” and “incorporation of others into one’s sense
of self may also act as reward substitutes for addictions,
including love addiction.
First proposed by Aron and Aron (1986), the self-expansion
model proposes that a basic human motivation is the desire to
increase one’s self-concept by engaging in novel, interesting,
challenging and/or other exciting pursuits in order to gain
resources and perspectives that can enhance one’s self
concept and capabilities (for review see Aron et al., 2013),
as well as garner positive emotions and reward feelings
(Aron et al., 1995, 2000;Strong and Aron, 2006). They
propose that rapid self-expansion occurs during early stage
romance.
This self-expansion, which is rooted in approach motivation
(see Mattingly et al., 2012), may be beneficial when attempting to
quit or reduce use of a substance or behavioral addiction because
it offers a replacement and distracting rewarding experience. Self-
expansion in the context of romantic love has been shown to
attenuate perceptions of physical pain (Younger et al., 2010) via
a reward mechanism (rather than distraction), which suggests
that it might assist with the painful process of withdrawal
after romantic rejection. Further, self-expansion may also be
beneficial in the context of quitting any addiction because it
facilitates self-concept change (e.g., starting to think of oneself as
a writer, musician, bird watcher or whatever the self-expanding
experience may be) into a new and healthier direction, and
away from one’s identity as a “user” (Kellogg and Kreek,
2005). In addition to providing distraction, replacement and
redirection, engaging in self-expanding (i.e., novel, interesting,
and/or challenging) activities may be biologically beneficial,
because any form of novelty activates the dopamine system in
the brain to facilitate energy and optimism, thereby potentially
providing a replacement reward.
Indeed, three studies have directly investigating self-expansion
in the context of nicotine addiction, each finding quite
positive results. Ex-smokers reported that significantly more
self-expanding experiences had occurred directly before they
successfully quit smoking than did current smokers who reported
on their unsuccessful attempts to quit (Xu et al., 2010). Even
among the current smokers who relapsed, the number of
self-expanding experiences occurring directly before their quit
attempt was significantly positively correlated with how long
they were able to abstain from smoking (Xu et al., 2010).
Two fMRI studies of overnight abstinent smokers suggest that
self-expansion via activities with a romantic partner attenuates
cigarette cue-reactivity in the brain (Xu et al., 2012, 2014). These
data suggest that when smokers engage in self-expansion, they are
less responsive to smoking cues.
Another cognitive phenomenon that may play a role in
attenuating romantic addiction is “inclusion of the other in
the self (IOS). This occurs when representations of the self
change to incorporate aspects of a romantic partner. A scale
has been developed to measure this cognitive process (Aron
et al., 1992). Over time the partner’s perspectives, identities, and
resources become incorporated into the person’s own sense of self
and the distinction between self and partner blur. For example,
people transition to more use of plural pronouns like “we” and
“us”(Agnew et al., 1998), and become slower at distinguishing a
partner’s belongings or traits from one’s own (Aron et al., 1991;
for a review, see Aron et al., 2004). This growth of the self-concept
can provide positive outcomes (e.g., additional resources, positive
feelings), which may be effective in a therapeutic situation.
Indeed, activation of the reward system through the VTA was
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 6May 2016 | Volume 7 | Article 687
fpsyg-07-00687 May 6, 2016 Time: 16:17 # 7
Fisher et al. Romantic Love As an Addiction
correlated with a lover’s IOS scores (Acevedo et al., 2011),
which suggests that a moderate amount of positive identification
with another person or group could be therapeutic–by boosting
a positive self-image and providing a reward substitute for a
substance or behavioral addiction that a person has given up.
IMPLICATIONS FOR TREATMENT OF
ROMANTIC REJECTION AND
ADDICTION
Clinicians have a host of strategies for helping lovers and
drug addicts. However, when data on romantic love and
substance abuse are considered together, some approaches have
a particularly strong rationale.
Perhaps most important, like giving up a drug, rejected
lovers should remove all reasonable evidence of their abandoning
sweetheart, such as cards, letters, songs, photos, and memorabilia,
as well as avoid contact with their rejecting partner, because
reminders and partner contact can act as cues that induce craving
and are likely to sustain the activity of brain circuits associated
with romantic passion and thus interfere with the healing process.
Self-expansion research also finds that positive outcomes such
as personal growth and positive emotions are possible (even
likely) following a break-up if the relationship had offered few
self-expanding opportunities and if the newly single person
engages in rediscovery of the self (Lewandowski and Bizzoco,
2007).
Close, positive contact with a friend or friends is rewarding
and may also help to replace the craving for substances or
a rejecting partner, because looking at a photo of a close
friend activates the nucleus accumbens, associated with reward
(Acevedo et al., 2011). Looking at a photo of a close friend
also activates the periaqueductal gray, associated with oxytocin
receptors and the calm of attachment. This suggests that group
therapies, such as Alcoholics Anonymous and other 12 step
programs, are successful because these group dynamics engage
the brain’s reward and attachment systems. Participating in group
programs may be important for rejected lovers as well as for those
addicted to substances like alcohol or those with a behavioral
addiction, such as gambling.
Data suggest that rejected lovers should also stay busy to
distract themselves (Thayer, 1996;Rosenthal, 2002). Physical
exertion may be especially helpful as it elevates mood (Rosenthal,
2002), triggering dopamine activity in the nucleus accumbens
to bestow pleasure (Kolata, 2002). Exercise also increases levels
of β-Endorphin and endocannabinoids which reduces pain and
increases feelings of calm and well-being (Goldfarb and Jamurtas,
1997;Dietrich and McDaniel, 2004). Also, engaging in a new
form of exercise can be a self-expanding experience (see Xu et al.,
2010). Because of these benefits of exercise, some psychiatrists
believe that exercise (aerobic or anaerobic) can be as effective
in healing depression as psychotherapy or antidepressant drugs
(Rosenthal, 2002).
Self-expanding activities (e.g., hobbies, sports, spiritual
experiences) can be helpful both in the context of addiction
and heartbreak as they offer reward, benefits to the self-concept,
and distraction. It is recommended that a person has more
than one source of self-expansion in their life, thus should one
no longer become available (e.g., a partner leaves), the other
sources can help buffer the impact of that loss. It would also be
helpful to have multiple and diverse sources of self-expansion in
various domains of life (e.g., hobby, workplace, friends, family,
volunteer organization, spiritual group, and academic interest
etc.) and to have strong social networks to which one can
turn for support in times of need (e.g., breakup, attempting
to quit). It is important, however, to note that self-expansion
should be pursued in a healthy manner with caution about
potentially risky behaviors (e.g., seeking to fall in love with a
new person immediately after the loss of a partner, picking up
unhealthy habits or becoming an addict of another substance
when quitting).
Similarly, it is important to remember that relationships and
addictions can co-exist and influence each other and it may
be especially difficult to have a strong and positive romantic
relationship when issues of addiction need to be dealt with.
As addiction often leads to less desire for and response to
alternative rewards, it may be especially difficult for those
dealing with addiction to engage in pro-relationship behaviors,
and thus increase the risk of rejection. In addition, romantic
rejection increases the risk of relapse, so close attention to
romantic relationships during substance abuse withdrawal may
be important.
Furthermore, smiling utilizes facial muscles that activate nerve
pathways in the brain that can stimulate feelings of pleasure
(Carter, 1998). Focusing on the positive may be effective too.
A study by Lewandowski (2009) found that writing for 20 min on
three consecutive days about a recent relationship break-up was
beneficial when people wrote about positive feelings as opposed
to when they wrote about negative feelings or wrote without
expressing any feelings. Perhaps most important, time attenuates
the attachment system. In our study of rejected men and women,
the greater the number of days since rejection, the less the activity
in a brain region (the ventral pallidum) associated with feelings of
attachment (Fisher et al., 2010).
As disappointed lovers use strategies originally developed
to quit a substance addiction, their love addiction is likely to
eventually subside.
CONCLUSION
Researchers have long discussed whether the compulsive pursuit
of non-substance rewards, such as uncontrolled gambling,
eating, sex, exercise, Internet use, compulsive buying disorder
and other obsessive behavioral syndromes can be classified
as addictions (Frascella et al., 2010;Rosenberg and Feder,
2014). All can lead to salience, obsession, tolerance, emotional,
and physical dependence, withdrawals, relapse and other traits
common to substance abuse. Moreover, several of these non-
substance rewards have been shown to produce specific activity
in dopamine pathways of the reward system similar to drugs of
abuse (see Frascella et al., 2010; see Rosenberg and Feder, 2014).
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 7May 2016 | Volume 7 | Article 687
fpsyg-07-00687 May 6, 2016 Time: 16:17 # 8
Fisher et al. Romantic Love As an Addiction
This suggests that uncontrolled use of these non-substances can
be considered addictions. Romantic love is likely to be a similar
addiction, with one exception. Unlike other addictions (that
afflict only a percentage of the population), some form of love
addiction is likely to occur to almost every human being that
lives now and in our human past; few avoid the pain of romantic
rejection either.
Romantic love appears to be a natural addiction, “a normal
altered state” experienced by almost all humans (Frascella
et al., 2010, p. 295) that evolved during human evolution
to motivate our ancestors to focus their mating energy on a
specific partner, thereby conserving mating time and energy,
initiating reproduction, triggering feelings of attachment and
subsequent mutual parenting, and assuring the future of their
mutual DNA (Fisher, 2004, 2011, 2016;Fisher et al., 2006).
Romantic love may be a positive addiction when the relationship
is reciprocated, non-toxic and appropriate; but a harmful,
negative addiction when unreciprocated, toxic, inappropriate
and/or formally rejected.
To alleviate the negative symptoms of love addiction, addicted
lovers are advised to remove the cues that fan their ardor,
follow some advisories of a 12-step program, build new daily
habits, meet new people, take up new interests, find the
appropriate medication and/or therapist, and wait out the days
and nights of intrusive thinking and craving, because feelings
of attachment to a former romantic partner decrease over
time (Fisher et al., 2010). Moreover, therapies that increase
self expansion and incorporate new individuals into one’s
sense of self may also be useful in alleviated love addiction.
Self expansion approaches may help drug and other negative
addiction therapies, also.
If the public and the therapeutic, medical and legal
communities come to understand that passionate early stage
romantic love is an evolved drive (Fisher, 2004) and a natural
addiction (Frascella et al., 2010) that can have profound
social, economic, psychological, and genetic consequences (both
beneficial and adverse), clinicians and researchers might develop
more effective procedures for dealing with this powerful and
primordial neural mechanism for mate preference and initial
partner attachment, romantic love.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
HF wrote half the text based on her ideas and data from previous
studies and edited the final version. XX wrote twenty percent of
the text based on her ideas and data from previous studies. AA
contributed to the text based on his ideas and previous studies.
LB wrote thirty percent of the text based on her ideas and data
from previous studies and edited the final version.
REFERENCES
Acevedo, B., Aron, A., Fisher, H., and Brown, L. (2011). Neural correlates of
long-term intense romantic love. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 7, 145–159. doi:
10.1093/scan/nsq092
Agnew, C. R., Van Lange, P. A. M., Rusbult, C. E., and Langston, C. A. (1998).
Cognitive interdependence: commitment and the mental representation of
close relationships. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74, 939–954. doi: 10.1037/0022-
3514.74.4.939
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., and Wall, S. N. (1978). Patterns
of Attachment: A Psychological Study of the Strange Situation. Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Alter, R. J., Lohrmann, D. K., and Greene, R. (2006). Substitution of marijuana for
alcohol: the role of perceived access and harm. J. Drug. Educ. 26, 335–355. doi:
10.2190/2780-G96W-J17N-R3H1
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual
of Mental Disorders, 5th Edn. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric
Publishing.
Aron, A., and Aron, E. (1986). Love and the Expansion of self: Understanding
Attraction and Satisfaction. New York, NY: Hemisphere.
Aron, A., Aron, E. N., and Smollan, D. (1992). Inclusion of other in the self scale
and the structure of interpersonal closeness. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 63, 596–612.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.63.4.596
Aron, A., Aron, E. N., Tudor, M., and Nelson, G. (1991). Close relationships as
including other in the self. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 60, 241–253. doi: 10.1037/0022-
3514.60.2.241
Aron, A., Fisher, H. E., Mashek, D. J., Strong, G., Li, H. F., and Brown, L. L.
(2005). Reward, motivation, and emotion systems associated with early-stage
intense romantic love: an fMRI study. J. Neurophysiol. 94, 327–337. doi:
10.1152/jn.00838.2004
Aron, A., Lewandowski, G., Mashek, D., and Aron, E. N. (2013). “The self-
expansion model of motivation and cognition in close relationships, in Oxford
Handbook of Close Relationships, eds J. A. Simpson and L. Campbell (New York:
Oxford), 90–115.
Aron, A., Norman, C. C., Aron, E. N., McKenna, C., and Heyman, R. (2000).
Couples shared participation in novel and arousing activities and experienced
relationship quality. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 78, 273–283. doi: 10.1037/0022-
3514.78.2.273
Aron, A., Paris, M., and Aron, E. N. (1995). Falling in love: prospective studies
of self-concept change. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 69, 1102–1112. doi: 10.1037/0022-
3514.69.6.1102
Aron, A. P., Mashek, D. J., and Aron, E. N. (2004). “Closeness as including other
in the self, in The Handbook of Closeness and Intimacy, eds D. Mashek and A.
Aron (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), 27–41.
Bartels, A., and Zeki, S. (2000). The neural basis of romantic love. Neuro Report.
11, 3829–3834. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200011270-00046
Bartels, A., and Zeki, S. (2004). The neural correlates of maternal and romantic
love. Neuroimage 21, 1155–1166. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.11.003
Baumeister, R. F., Wotman, S. R., and Stillwell, A. M. (1993). Unrequited love: on
heartbreak, anger, guilt, scriptlessness and humiliation. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 64,
377–394. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.64.3.377
Berridge, K. C., Robinson, T. E., and Aldridge, J. W. (2009). Dissecting components
of reward: ‘Liking’, ‘wanting’, and learning. Curr. Opin. Pharmacol. 9, 65–73. doi:
10.1016/j.coph.2008.12.014
Bickel, W. K., Johnson, M. W., Koffarnus, M. N., MacKillop, J., and Murphy, J. G.
(2014). The behavioral economics of substance use disorders: reinforcement
pathologies and their repair. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 10, 641–677. doi:
10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032813-153724
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and Loss: Vol. 1 Attachment. New York: Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and Loss: Vol. 2. Separation. New York: Basic Books.
Breiter, H. C., Aharon, I., Kahneman, D., Dale, A., and Shizgal, P. (2001).
Functional imaging of neural responses to expectancy and experience
of monetary gains and losses. Neuron 30, 619–639. doi: 10.1016/S0896-
6273(01)00303-8
Breiter, H. C., Gollub, R. L., Weisskoff, R. M., Kennedy, D. N., Makris, N., Berke,
J. D., et al. (1997). Acute effects of cocaine on human brain activity and emotion.
Neuron 19, 591–611. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80374-8
Burkett, J. P., and Young, L. J. (2012). The behavioral, anatomical and
pharmacological parallels between social attachment, love and addiction.
Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 224, 1–26. doi: 10.1007/s00213-012-2794-x
Carter, R. (1998). Mapping the Mind. Los Angeles, CA: University of California
Press.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 8May 2016 | Volume 7 | Article 687
fpsyg-07-00687 May 6, 2016 Time: 16:17 # 9
Fisher et al. Romantic Love As an Addiction
Cherlin, A. J. (2009). The Marriage-Go-Round: the State of Marriage and the Family
in America today. New York: Alfred A. Knopf.
Cuzen, N. L., and Stein, D. J. (2014). “Behavioral addiction: the nexus of impulsivity
and compulsivity, in Behavioral Addictions: Criteria, Evidence and Treatment,
eds K. R. Rosenberg and L. C. Feder (London: Elsevier), 19–34.
Daly, M., and Wilson, M. (1983). Sex, Evolution and Behavior, 2nd Edn. Boston:
Willard Grant.
Delgado, M. R., Nystrom, L. E., Fissel, C., Noll, D. C., and Fiez, J. A. (2000).
Tracking the hemodynamic responses to reward and punishment in the
striatum. J. Neurophysiol. 84, 3072–3077.
Diana, M. (2013). The addicted brain. Front. Psychiatry 4:40. doi:
10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00040
Dietrich, A., and McDaniel, W. F. (2004). Endocannabinoids and exercise. Br. J.
Sports. Med. 38, 536–541. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.2004.011718
DiNardo, J., and Lemieux, T. (2001). Alcohol, marijuana, and American youth:
the unintended consequences of government regulation. J. Health. Econ. 5,
991–1010. doi: 10.1016/S0167-6296(01)00102-3
Donovan, J. M. (1988). “Assessment of addictive behaviors for relapse prevention,
in Assessment of Addictive Behaviours, eds D. M. Donovan and G. A. Marlatt
(New York: Guilford), 3–48.
Dozier, R. W. (2002). Why We Hate: Understanding, Curbing, and Eliminating hate
in Our Selves and our World. New York: Contemporary books.
Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., and Williams, K. D. (2003). Does
Rejection Hurt? An FMRI study of social exclusion. Science 302, 290–292. doi:
10.1126/science.1089134
Elliott, R., Newman, J. L., Longe, O. A., and Deakin, J. F. W. (2003). Differential
response patterns in the striatum and orbitofrontal cortex to financial reward
in humans: a parametric functional magnetic resonance imaging study.
J. Neurosci. 23, 303–307.
Fabre-Nys, C., Ohkura, S., and Kendrick, K. M. (1997). Male faces and odors evoke
differential patterns of neurochemical release in the mediobasal hypothalamus
of the ewe during estrus: an insight into sexual motivation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 9,
1666–1677. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1997.tb01524.x
Fisher, H., Aron, A., and Brown, L. L. (2005). Romantic love: an MRI study
of a neural mechanism for mate choice. J. Comp. Neurol. 493, 58–62. doi:
10.1002/cne.20772
Fisher, H., Aron, A., and Brown, L. L. (2006). Romantic love: a mammalian brain
system for mate choice. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 361, 2173–2186.
doi: 10.1098/rstb.2006.1938
Fisher, H., Aron, A., Mashek, D., Strong, G., Li, H., and Brown, L. L. (2002).
Defining the brain systems of lust, romantic attraction and attachment. Arch.
Sex. Behav. 31, 13–19. doi: 10.1023/A:1019888024255
Fisher, H., Aron, A., Mashek, D., Strong, G., Li H., and Brown L.L. (2003). Early
stage intense romantic love activates cortical-basal-ganglia reward/motivation,
emotion and attention systems: an fMRI study of a dynamic network that varies
with relationship length, passion intensity and gender. Poster presented at the
Annual Meeting of the Society for Neuroscience, New Orleans.
Fisher, H. E. (1989). Evolution of human serial pair-bonding. Am. J. Phys.
Anthropol. 78, 331–354. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.1330780303
Fisher, H. E. (1992). Anatomy of Love: The Natural History of Monogamy, Adultery,
and Divorce. New York, NY: W. W. Norton.
Fisher, H. E. (1998). Lust, attraction, and attachment in mammalian reproduction.
Hum. Nat. 9, 23–52. doi: 10.1007/s12110-998-1010-5
Fisher, H. E. (2004). Why we Love: the Nature and Chemistry of Romantic Love.
New York: Henry Holt.
Fisher, H. E. (2011). “Serial monogamy and clandestine adultery: evolution and
consequences of the dual human reproductive strategy, in Applied Evolutionary
Psychology, ed. S. C. Roberts (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 96–111.
Fisher, H. E. (2016). Anatomy of Love: a Natural History of Mating, Marriage and
Why we Stray, 2nd Edn. New York: WW Norton.
Fisher, H. E., Brown, L. L., Aron, A., Strong, G., and Mashek, D. (2010). Reward,
addiction, and emotion regulation systems associated with rejection in love.
J. Neurophysiol. 104, 51–60. doi: 10.1152/jn.00784.2009
Fletcher, G. J. O., Simpson, J. A., Campbell, L., and Overall, N. C. (2015). Pair
bonding, romantic love, and evolution: the curious case of Homo sapiens.
Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 10, 20–36. doi: 10.1177/1745691614561683
Fraley, R. C., and Shaver, P. R. (2000). Adult romantic attachment: theoretical
developments, emerging controversies, and unanswered questions. Rev. Gen.
Psychol. 4, 132–154. doi: 10.1037//1089-2680.4.2.132
Frascella, J., Potenza, M. N., Brown, L. L., and Childress, A. R. (2010). Shared brain
vulnerabilities open the way for nonsubstance addictions: caving addiction
at a new joint? Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1187, 294–315. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-
6632.2009.05420.x
Frayser, S. (1985). Varieties of Sexual Experience: An Anthropological Perspective of
Human Sexuality. New Haven: HRAF Press.
Gingrich, B., Liu, Y., Cascio, C. Z., and Insel, T. R. (2000). Dopamine D2 receptors
in the nucleus accumbens are important for social attachment in female prairie
voles (Microtus ochrogaster). Behav. Neurosci. 114, 173–183. doi: 10.1037/0735-
7044.114.1.173
Goldfarb, A. H., and Jamurtas, A. Z. (1997). Beta-endorphin response to exercise.
An update. Sports. Med. 24, 8–16. doi: 10.2165/00007256-199724010-00002
Griffin-Shelley, E. (1991). Sex and Love: Addiction, Treatment, and Recovery.
Westport, CT: Praeger.
Halpern, H. M. (1982). How to Break your Addiction to a Person. New York:
McGraw-Hill.
Haney, M. (2009). Self-administration of cocaine, cannabis and heroine in
the human laboratory: benefits and pitfalls. Addict. Biol. 14, 9–21. doi:
10.1111/j.1369-1600.2008.00121.x
Hänninen, V., and Koski-Jännes, A. (1999). Narratives of recovery from
addictive behaviors. Addiction 94, 1837–1848. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-
0443.1999.941218379.x
Harris, H. (1995). “Rethinking heterosexual relationships in polynesia: a case study
of mangaia, cook island, in Romantic Passion: A Universal Experience?, ed. W.
Jankowiak (New York, NY: Columbia University Press), 95–127.
Hatfield, E., and Sprecher, S. (1986). Measuring passionate love in intimate
relationships. J. Adolesc. 9, 383–410. doi: 10.1016/S0140-1971(86)80043-4
Haylett, S. A., Stephenson, G. M., and Lefever, R. M. H. (2004). Covariation
in addictive behaviours: a study of addictive orientations using the Shorter
PROMIS Questionnaire. Addict. Behav. 29, 61–71. doi: 10.1016/S0306-
4603(03)00083-2
Hazan, C., and Diamond, L. M. (2000). The place of attachment in human mating.
Rev. Gen. Psychol. 4, 186–204. doi: 10.1037//1089-2680.4.2.186
Hazan, C., and Shaver, P. R. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment
process. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 52, 511–524. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.52.3.511
Hoeft, F., Watson, C. L., Kesler, S. R., Bettinger, K. E., and Reiss, A. L. (2008).
Gender differences in the mesocorticolimbic system during computer game-
play. J. Psychiatr. Res. 42, 253–258. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2007.11.010
Hunter, M. S., Nitschke, C., and Hogan, L. (1981). A scale to measure love
addiction. Psychol. Reports 48, 582. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1981.48.2.582
Insel, T. R. (2003). Is social attachment an addictive disorder? Physiol. Behav. 79,
351–357. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(03)00148-3
Insel, T. R., and Young, L. J. (2001). The neurobiology of attachment. Nat. Rev.
Neurosci. 2, 129–136. doi: 10.1038/35053579
Jordan, H. W., and Howe, G. (1980). De clerambault syndrome (Erotomania): a
review and case presentation. J. Natl. Med. Assoc. 72, 979–985.
Kellogg, S. H., and Kreek, M. J. (2005). Gradualism, identity, reinforcements, and
change. Int. J. Drug Policy 16, 369–375. doi: 10.1016/j.drugpo.2005.08.001
Knutson, B., Rick, S., Wimmer, G. E., Prelec, D., and Loewenstein, G.
(2007). Natural predictors of purchases. Neuron 53, 147–156. doi:
10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.010
Kolata, G. (2002). Runner’s High? Endorphins? Fiction, some scientist say. Sci.
Times 21, F1–F6.
Koob, G. F., and Volkow, N. D. (2010). Neurocircuitry of addiction.
Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 217–238. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.110
Kopelman, M. D., Guinan, E. M., and Lewis, P. D. R. (2008). Delusional memory,
confabulation, and frontal lobe dysfunction: a case study of De Clerambault’s
Syndrome. Neurocase 1, 71–77. doi: 10.1080/13554799508402348
Leary, M. R. (2001). Interpersonal Rejection. New York, NY: Oxford University
Press.
Lewandowski, G. W. Jr. (2009). Promoting positive emotions following
relationship dissolution through writing. J. Pos. Psychol. 4, 21–31. doi:
10.1080/17439760802068480
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 9May 2016 | Volume 7 | Article 687
fpsyg-07-00687 May 6, 2016 Time: 16:17 # 10
Fisher et al. Romantic Love As an Addiction
Lewandowski, G. W. Jr., and Bizzoco, N. (2007). Addition through subtraction:
growth following the dissolution of a low quality relationship. J. Pos. Psychol. 2,
40–54. doi: 10.1080/17439760601069234
Lewis, T., Amini, F., and Lannon, R. (2000). A General theory of Love. New York,
NY: Random House.
Liebowitz, M. R. (1983). The Chemistry of Love. Boston: Little Brown.
Liu, Y., Young, K. A., Curtis, J. T., Aragona, B. J., and Wang, Z. (2011).
Social bonding decreases the rewarding properties of amphetamine through a
dopamine D1 receptor-mediated mechanism. J. Neurosci. 31, 7960–7966. doi:
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1006-11.2011
Lovejoy, O. C. (2009). Reexamining human origins in light of Ardipithecus
ramidus.Science 326, 74–78. doi: 10.1126/science.1175834
MacDonald, G., and Leary, M. R. (2005). Why does social exclusion hurt? The
relationship between social and physical pain. Psychol. Bull. 131, 202–223. doi:
10.1037/0033-2909.131.2.202
Marazziti, D., Akiskal, H. S., Rossi, A., and Cassano, G. B. (1999). Alteration of the
platelet serotonin transporter in romantic love. Psychol. Med. 29, 741–745. doi:
10.1017/S0033291798007946
Marks, I. (1990). Behavioural (non-chemical) addictions. Br. J. Addict. 85, 1389–
1394. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1990.tb01618.x
Mattingly, B. A., McIntyre, K. P., and Lewandowski, G. W. Jr. (2012). Approach
motivation and the expansion of self in close relationships. Pers. Rel. 19,
113–127. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-6811.2010.01343.x
Melis, M., Spiga, S., and Diana, M. (2005). The dopamine hypothesis of drug
addiction: hypodopaminergic state. Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 63, 101–154. doi:
10.1016/S0074-7742(05)63005-X
Mellody, P., Miller, A. W., and Miller, J. K. (1992). Facing Love Addiction.
New York, NY: Harper Collins Publishers.
Meloy, J. R. (1998). The Psychology of Stalking: Clinical and Forensic Perspectives.
New York, NY: Academic Press.
Meloy, J. R., and Fisher, H. E. (2005). Some thoughts on the neurobiology of
stalking. J. Forensic. Sci. 50, 1472–1480. doi: 10.1520/JFS2004508
Montague, P. R., McClure, S. M., Baldwin, P. R., Phillips, P. E., Budygin, E. A.,
Stuber, G. D., et al. (2004). Dynamic gain control of dopamine delivery in freely
moving animals. J. Neurosci. 24, 1754–1759. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4279-
03.2004
Noriuchi, M., Kikuchi, Y., and Senoo, A. (2008). The functional neuroanatomy
of maternal love: mother’s response to infant’s attachment behaviors. Biol.
Psychiatry 63, 415–423. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.05.018
Ortigue, S., Bianchi-Demicheli, F., Hamilton, A. F., and Grafton, S. T. (2007).
The neural basis of love as a subliminal prime: an event-related functional
magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 1218–1230. doi:
10.1162/jocn.2007.19.7.1218
Panksepp, J. (2003a). At the interface of the affective, behavioral, and cognitive
neurosciences: decoding the emotional feelings of the brain. Brain Cogn. 52,
4–14. doi: 10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00003-4
Panksepp, J. (2003b). Neuroscience. Feeling the pain of social loss. Science 302,
237–239. doi: 10.1126/science.1091062
Panksepp, J., Knutson, B., and Burgdorf, J. (2002). The role of brain emotional
systems in addictions: a neuro-evolutionary perspective and new ‘self-
report’ animal model. Addiction 97, 459–469. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2002.
00025.x
Peele, S. (1975). Love and Addiction. New York, NY: Taplinger Publishing
Company.
Reno, P. L., Meindl, R. S., McCollum, M. A., and Lovejoy, C. O. (2003).
Sexual dimorphism in Australopithecus afarensis was similar to that
of modern humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 100, 9404–9409. doi:
10.1073/pnas.1133180100
Reynaud, M. L., Karila, L., Blecha, L., and Benyamina, A. (2010). Is love
passion an addictive disorder? Am. J. Drug Alcohol. Abuse 36, 261–267. doi:
10.3109/00952990.2010.495183
Robinson, D. L., Heien, M. L., and Wightman, R. M. (2002). Frequency of
dopamine concentration transients increases in dorsal and ventral striatum of
male rats during introduction of conspecifics. J. Neurosci. 22, 10477–10486.
Rosenberg, K. P., and Feder, L. C. (2014). “Forward to: behavioral addictions, in
Criteria, Evidence and Treatment, eds K. R. Rosenberg and L. C. Feder (London:
Elsevier), 13.
Rosenthal, N. E. (2002). The Emotional Revolution: How the New Science of Feelings
can Transform Your Life. New York: Citadel Press Books.
Salvy, S., Nitecki, L. A., and Epstein, L. H. (2009). Do social activities substitute for
food in youth? Ann. Behav. Med. 38, 205–212. doi: 10.1007/s12160-009-9145-0
Schaef, A. W. (1989). Escape from Intimacy: The Pseudo-Relationship Addictions.
San Francisco: Harper & Row.
Schultz, W. (2000). Multiple reward signals in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 1,
199–207. doi: 10.1038/35044563
Shiffman, S. (1989). Tobacco “chippers” individual differences in tobacco
dependence. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 97, 539–547. doi: 10.1007/BF00439561
Shiffman, S., and Paty, J. (2006). Smoking patterns and dependence: contrasting
chippers and heavy smokers. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 115, 509–523. doi:
10.1037/0021-843X.115.3.509
Shiffman, S., Paty, J. A., Gnys, M., Kassel, J. D., and Elash, C. (1995). Nicotine
withdrawal in chippers, and regular smokers: subjective, and cognitive effects.
Health Psychol. 14, 301–309. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.14.4.301
Strong, G., and Aron, A. (2006). “The effect of shared participation in novel and
challenging activities on experienced relationship quality: is it mediated by high
positive affect?, in Connecting Intrapersonal and Interpersonal Processes, eds K.
Vohs and E. Finkel (New York, NY: Guilford), 342–359.
Tennov, D. (1979). Love and Limerence: the Experience of Being in Love. New York:
Stein and Day.
Thayer, R. E. (1996). The Origin of Everyday Moods: Managing Energy, Tension and
Stress. New York: Oxford University Press.
Tucker, D. M., Luu, P., and Derryberry, D. (2005). Love hurts: the evolution of
empathic concern through the encephalization of nociceptive capacity. Dev.
Psychopathol. 17, 699–713. doi: 10.1017/S0954579405050339
Vaillant, G. (1983). The Natural History of Alcoholism. Cambridge: Harvard
University Press.
Van den Berghe, P. L. (1979). Human Family Systems: An Evolutionary View.
Westport, CT: Greenwood Press.
Volkow, N. D., Fowler, S. J., Wang, G. J., Swanson, J. M., and Telang, F.
(2007). Dopamine in drug abuse and addiction: results of imaging
studies and treatment implications. Arch. Neurol. 64, 1575–1579. doi:
10.1001/archneur.64.11.1575
Wang, G. J., Volkow, N. D., Telang, F., Jayne, M., Ma, J., Rao, M., et al. (2004).
Exposure to appetitive food stimuli markedly activates the human brain.
Neuroimage 21, 1790–1797. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.11.026
Wang, Z., Yu, G., Cascio, C., Liu, Y., Gingrich, B., and Insel, T. R. (1999). Dopamine
D2 receptor-mediated regulation of partner preferences in female prairie voles
(Microtus ochrogaster): a mechanism for pair bonding? Behav. Neurosci. 113,
602–611. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.113.3.602
Xu, X., Aron, A., Brown, L. L., Cao, G., Feng, T., and Weng, X. (2011). Reward and
motivation systems: a brain mapping study of early-stage intense romantic love
in Chinese participants. Hum. Brain Mapp. 32, 49–57. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21017
Xu, X., Aron, A., Westmaas, J. L., Wang, J., and Sweet, L. H. (2014). An fMRI study
of nicotine-deprived smokers’ reactivity to smoking cues during novel/exciting
activity. PLoS ONE 9:e94598. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0094598
Xu, X., Floyd, A. H. L., Westmaas, J. L., and Aron, A. (2010). Self-
expansion and smoking abstinence. Addict. Behav. 35, 295–301. doi:
10.1016/j.addbeh.2009.10.019
Xu, X., Wang, J., Lei, W., Aron, A., Westmaas, L., and Weng, X. (2012). Intense
passionate love attenuates cigarette cue-reactivity in nicotine-deprived smokers:
an fMRI study. PLoS ONE 7:e42235. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0042235
Younger, J., Aron, A., Parke, S., Chatterjee, N., and Mackey, S. (2010). Viewing
pictures of a romantic partner reduces experimental pain: involvement of neural
reward systems. PLoS ONE 5:e13309. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0013309
Zeki, S., and Romaya, J. P. (2010). The brain reaction to viewing faces
of opposite and same sex romantic partners. PLoS ONE 5:e15802. doi:
10.1371/journal.pone.0015802
Conflict of Interest Statement: The authors declare that the research was
conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could
be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Copyright © 2016 Fisher, Xu, Aron and Brown. This is an open-access article
distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY).
The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the
original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this
journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution
or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 10 May 2016 | Volume 7 | Article 687
... This second incorrect sentence, found in Paragraph 2, previously stated: "Whereas Fisher et al. (2016) and others (e.g., Marazziti et al., 2014) believe that romantic love precedes a period of pair bonding, I assert that part of romantic love is the process of pair bonding (i.e., pair bond formation)." ...
... The corrected sentence appears below: "Whereas Fisher et al. (2016) and others (e.g., Marazziti et al., 2021) believe that romantic love precedes a period of pair bonding, I assert that part of romantic love is the process of pair bonding (i.e., pair bond formation)." Bode . ...
... The second factor refers to NMW's sexual needs. Humans are basically social beings (35), and sexuality is an integral part of psychological health in an adult's life (36)(37)(38)(39). The sexual activity of couples usually signifies exclusivity, intimacy, and bondreinforcing behavior, and sexual impairment is regarded as both distressing and disturbing for the individual quality of life. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective: The purpose of the present study was to design the Farsi Scale of Sexual-Reproductive Needs and Concerns of never-married women over 35 and to assess its psychometric properties. Materials and methods: This mixed method research had two phases. The first phase was qualitative (Conventional Content Analysis). We interviewed never-married Iranian women over 35; from their responses, we extracted specific statements which to be used as questionnaire items. To validate the questionnaire psychometrically, we tested its validity (face, content, and structure) and reliability (internal consistency and stability). For qualitative face validity, 15 never-married women and 5 experts commented on the style of sentences. For quantitative face validity, we used the impact score. For qualitative content validity, 15 experts commented on the items based on their appropriateness, and for quantitative content validity, we performed the Content Validity Ratio and Content Validity Index. Exploratory Factor Analysis was used to Construct Validity. To evaluate structural validity, a cross-sectional sample of 240 never-married women over the age of 35 completed the questionnaire. Cronbach's Alpha was used for internal consistency. In addition, the test-retest method and Intraclass Correlation Coefficient were used to ensure stability. Results: The Sexual-Reproductive Needs and Concerns Scale of Iranian Never-Married Women over 35 was developed with 15 items and three dimensions (Emotional burden; Sexual needs; Stigma). Cronbach's alpha for the instrument was 0.81 and the Intraclass Correlation Coefficient was 0.98. Conclusion: The results of the study showed that the developed scale has acceptable validity and reliability, and thus can be used to assess the sexual-reproductive needs and concerns of never-married Iranian women of 35 and over.
... Affective addicts tend to enter relationships with abusive, abusive, violent, manipulative and detached persons [3]. A peculiar feature of affective dependence is that, despite suffering, the dependent subject cannot end the relationship and causes the behavior of the partner to be attributed to the traumas of his childhood [53]. The "women who love too much" have the dysfunctional belief that it is only by sacrificing and dedicating themselves totally to the other, cancelling sexless, that they can help the partner. ...
... One rationale for this rejection exposure 'treatment' is the relationship between an LI's desire and the increased dopamine levels that develop as a consequence of minimal interaction between the LO and LI, regardless of how limited or incidental this contact is, alongside the LI's neurological response to this chemical release (Fisher et al. 2002). Intense romantic feelings have been found to trigger the same physiological responses as gambling addictions (Fisher et al. 2016) with neurological activity centred in the same mesolimbic region (reward centre) of the brain (Wyant 2021). Moments of positive interaction with the LO can result in extreme highs that fuel the behaviour, but in contrast to this, the LI may also experience extreme lows if their affection is rejected (Wolf 2017). ...
Article
Full-text available
This systematic scoping review explores the behavioural state of limerence and the relationship it has with rumination as part of a precursory phase to stalking, for the purpose of identifying a trajectory in harmful human behaviour. The review also considers how limerence impacts those who experience it, as well as factors which serve as accelerants to this cognitive state. It examines cognitive disorders identifiable on the DSM-5, such as obsessive–compulsive disorders, autism spectrum disorders, and erotomania, and applies them to studies which conceptualise limerence as an obsessive behaviour. At present, there is extraordinarily limited literature focusing on this state of unrequited and intense human emotion towards another person—a phenomenon characterised by limited by self-awareness and restraint, yet also a state that involves obsession and fixation, sharing cognitive characteristics and behaviours intrinsically tied to stalking behaviours. This review argues that behaviours exhibited in a state of limerence can serve as the building blocks upon which more harmful, externally focused stalking behaviours could develop. This review identified that the emerging literature on limerence offers new and important insights into the psychology of obsessive desire as a precursor to other, more proximal forms of violence which warrant greater attention, as they do not fit into existing psychological classifications of obsession because these behaviours are motivated by a lack of reciprocation and rejection. The objective is not to label individuals experiencing limerence as deviant but, rather, to better understand how fixation and obsessive desire can be maintained in the absence of approach behaviours.
Article
Full-text available
In this article, we wish to show that the experience of addictive desires is similar to the experience of limerence, which can be defined as the state of being in love. In other words, we want to argue that an inherent part of the experience of addiction is found in the experience of romantic love. Many in the literature have tried to show that one can be addicted to love in the way that love constitutes an addiction. Yet, none seem to have taken the opposite route, which rather takes love as a constituent of the addiction itself. This is odd, as such a perspective is warranted by first-person accounts of addiction. In this regard, in our view, addiction is like love, and not the other way around. Nevertheless, it is not addiction as a whole that is similar to love; love in itself cannot be compared to addiction: it is the experience of addictive cravings in addiction that is like limerence. In order to put forward the similarities between experiences of cravings and experiences of limerence, we will go through the basic components of limerence as described by Dorothy Tennov (1979), and prove that they are all found in the experience of cravings.
Article
This paper offers a study of the foodstuffs used by speakers of Peninsular Spanish when referring to an attractive individual. Two online forums were scanned to automatically search for linguistic realizations of the metaphor an attractive individual is appetizing food . A corpus of 860 short texts was compiled, which contains the food terms used in context. The frequency distribution of the type of food and the variable of gender was measured. Results indicate a preference to exploit the metaphor through salty food products and that the foodstuffs used are highly dependent on the variable of gender. Contrary to previous studies (e.g., Caitlin Hines 2000 ; Irene López Rodríguez 2007 , 2008 ), the frequency of sweets and desserts is not higher with female referents. The findings do not agree with prior assumptions about the foodstuffs used in Peninsular Spanish to conceptualize attractive people and the cultural stereotypes associated with them.
Article
Full-text available
Using a newspaper questionnaire, a door-to-door survey, and 3 laboratory experiments, the authors examined a proposed effect of shared participation in novel and arousing activities on experienced relationship quality. The questionnaire and survey studies found predicted correlations of reported shared “exciting” activities and relationship satisfaction plus their predicted mediation by relationship boredom. In all 3 experiments, the authors found predicted greater increases in experienced relationship quality from before to after participating together in a 7-min novel and arousing (vs. a more mundane) task. Comparison with a no-activity control showed the effect was due to the novel–arousing task. The same effect was found on ratings of videotaped discussions before and after the experimental task. Finally, all results remained after controlling for relationship social desirability. Results bear on general issues of boredom and excitement in relationships and the role of such processes in understanding the typical early decline of relationship quality after the honeymoon period.
Article
Full-text available
Book
The National Institute of Health has identified sexual addiction as a research priority. This volume provides a detailed definition of sex and love addiction as well as an explicit outline of treatment and recovery. It uniquely integrates sex and love in its formulation and presents a two level approach to recovery. This presentation provides in-depth examples and suggestions for change and supports the growing involvement of Twelve-Step programs in mental health. Professionals will find this volume an excellent resource in their clinical practice. People can be addicted to sex and/or love and recovery is possible. More than ten years ago the National Institute of Health identified sexual addiction as a research priority. Experts now conservatively estimate a prevalence rate of 5 percent of the American population. Eric Griffin-Shelley provides a detailed definition of sex and love addiction as well as an outline of treatment and recovery. Unique to this work, Griffin-Shelley integrates sex and love in its formulation and also presents a two-level approach to recovery. This presentation provides in-depth examples and suggestions for change and supports the growing involvement of Twelve-Step programs in mental health. Professionals can use this resource in their clinical practice to identify and assist sex and love addicts. Griffin-Shelley clearly describes the behavior of sex and love addicts and the emotions they may be experiencing. Problems such as multiple addictions (to drugs, alcohol, food, work) are examined. The book's two-layer approach to recovery focuses initially on the establishment of sobriety and then outlines an outer layer of protection that the sex and love addict can develop to sustain long-term recovery. Griffin-Shelley's meticulous description of the role of psychotherapy in aiding the recovery process is clearer than any book published to date on either sex or love addiction.
Book
Interpersonal rejection ranks among the most potent and distressing events that people experience. Romantic refusal, ostracism, betrayal, stigmatization, job termination, and other kinds of denial have the power to compromise the quality of people's lives. As a result, individuals are highly motivated to avoid social rejection, and, indeed, much of human behavior appears to be designed to prevent such experiences. With the widespread effects of real, anticipated, and even imagined refutations, psychologists have devoted their efforts on dissecting this topic under different psychological subspecialties (e.g. social, clinical, developmental, and personality). The goal of this book is to consolidate all related literatures to further understand the influences of interpersonal rejection on behavior and emotion, and also, to have identifiable areas for future research. Other topics covered include sensitivity, emotional responses, and personality moderators of reactions to rejection.
Book
Behavioral Addictions is the first and most authoritative text ever written on the subject of behavioral addictions. This comprehensive work explains the criteria used to determine addiction, the evidence for identifying assorted behaviors as addictions, and the evidence-based treatment for each. DSM-V broke new ground in May of 2013, designating a new disorder called "behavioral addiction." Clinicians immediately wanted to know: how is a behavioral addiction different from an impulse control disorder? What are the criteria for determining that some behaviors are addictions rather than impulses? What, if anything, does this mean in terms of effective treatment? With contributions from preeminent experts covering an exhaustive list of behavioral addictions, this book is unique in its coverage of behavioral addictions, their criteria, and treatment. It is a valuable and timely resource for any clinician treating addictions.
Article
Impulsivity and compulsivity have sometimes been viewed as lying at opposite ends of a single dimension. However, convergent evidence from research that informs clinical practice suggests that these are complex phenomena with some overlapping and some distinctive psychobiological mechanisms. The co-occurrence of impulsive and compulsive features may be relevant to understanding a number of disorders, including behavioral and substance addictions as well as obsessive-compulsive disorder and related disorders. Kleptomania is a diagnosis that elucidates the convergences between models of addiction, impulsivity, and compulsivity, and may provide a framework to understand these behaviors. A deeper understanding of the cognitive-affective neuroscience of compulsive-impulsive behaviors may have implications for the assessment and treatment of these conditions.
Article
Considerable data suggest that humans have evolved a dual reproductive strategy: life long and/or serial monogamy in conjunction with clandestine adultery. This paper explores the underlying biochemical and genetic mechanisms likely to contribute to this flexible, yet specific human reproductive system, and explores some of the implications of this dual human reproductive strategy for contemporary partnerships. Critics of evolutionary psychology fail to find the profound value of this budding discipline, yet it yields important insights that could be of use to medical and legal professionals, researchers and therapists.