ChapterPDF Available

A Paradigm for Understanding Altered Consciousness: The Integrative Mode of Consciousness

Authors:

Abstract

Ritual alterations of consciousness are a virtual universal of human cultures, reflecting a basic human drive generally considered of central importance to religion and spiritual practices. Cross-cultural perspectives show both similarities in the experiences of altered consciousness (AC) that implicate biological factors as the basis for similarities across cultures, time, and space, as well as cultural differences in the manifestations of these potentials that implicate social factors. Individual and group experiences of altered consciousness may vary in many ways, but it is commonal-ities and recurrent patterns, rather than unique differences, that are crucial to understanding AC. This introduction reviews evidence for the universal manifestation of altered consciousness. This universal manifestation is not well explained in the classic paradigms of altered states of consciousness that emphasize their individual nature. In contrast, a biological approach to consciousness helps to situate altered consciousness within human nature. This perspective provides a foundation for an approach that characterizes AC in terms of an integrative mode of consciousness that reflects systemic features of brain functioning. This integrative mode of consciousness is typified in theta wave patterns that synchronize the frontal cortex with discharges from lower brain structures. This integration of ancient brain functions into the frontal cortex explains many of the key features of AC.
CHAPTER 2
A Paradigm for Understanding
Altered Consciousness:
The Integrative Mode of
Consciousness
Michael Winkelman
Ritual alterations of consciousness are a virtual universal of human
cultures, reflecting a basic human drive generally considered of central
importance to religion and spiritual practices. Cross-cultural perspectives
show both similarities in the experiences of altered consciousness (AC)
that implicate biological factors as the basis for similarities across cultures,
time, and space, as well as cultural differences in the manifestations of
these potentials that implicate social factors. Individual and group experi-
ences of altered consciousness may vary in many ways, but it is commonal-
ities and recurrent patterns, rather than unique differences, that are crucial
to understanding AC.
This introduction reviews evidence for the universal manifestation of
altered consciousness. This universal manifestation is not well explained
in the classic paradigms of altered states of consciousness that emphasize
their individual nature. In contrast, a biological approach to consciousness
helps to situate altered consciousness within human nature. This perspec-
tive provides a foundation for an approach that characterizes AC in terms
of an integrative mode of consciousness that reflects systemic features of brain
functioning. This integrative mode of consciousness is typified in theta
wave patterns that synchronize the frontal cortex with discharges from
lower brain structures. This integration of ancient brain functions into
the frontal cortex explains many of the key features of AC.
Culture, Science, and Altered Consciousness
Institutionalized procedures for ritually altering consciousness are found
in virtually all societies (Bourguignon, 1968; Winkelman, 1992), reflecting
an innate drive for altering consciousness (Siegel, 2005). Societies have a
number of adaptations to the capacities of AC, for example, those manifested
in conditions distinguished as soul flight, possession, enlightenment, and
others. As Whitehead points out in Volume 1, culture fundamentally influen-
ces how our capacities are developed. Some cultures extol these experiences
while others vilify and block access to them. Laughlin, McManus, and d’Aquili
(1992) characterized cultural differences in relating to the alteration of con-
sciousness as ranging from “monophasic” cultures, which institutionally value
only waking consciousness, to “polyphasic void” cultures, which encourage
exploration of phases of consciousness beyond phenomenal reality.
Most cultures have traditions designed to enhance the availability of
altered consciousness such as those produced in shamanic and meditative
traditions. In contrast, the dominant cultural ethos of Indo-European soci-
eties generally ignores them or subjects those who seek them to patholog-
ization, social marginalization, or persecution. Some cultures believe that
altering consciousness provides a variety of adaptive advantages through
development of a more objective perception of the external world in
recognizing the illusory and constructed nature of ordinary perception.
For example, many cultures have viewed some drugs as entheogens,
sacred substances that produce a contact with the divine (e.g., Ra
¨tsch,
2005; Schultes & Hofmann, 1979).
In spite of different attitudes, it appears that virtually all societies have
practices that support the ability to function in this mode of altered
consciousness (Laughlin et al., 1992; Winkelman, 2000). This universality
indicates that these manifestations of AC are a part of human biological
potentials. Even when there is cultural repression of altered consciousness,
these experiences are nonetheless manifested because they reflect a
biological basis and its inevitable expression in human experience. Such
a biological basis indicates that we should be able to provide a scientific
explanation of these persistent and significant human phenomena.
Situating Altered Consciousness within Science
The scientific status of studies of altered consciousness has a doubly
stigmatized position—first, by being about consciousness, a field that still
struggles for general scientific recognition, and second, by being marked
as something altered. In spite of many scientific and other inquiries, one
24 Altering Consciousness
might legitimately claim that we still do not have a science of altered con-
sciousness.
Scientific fields, as conceptualized by the historian and philosopher of
science Thomas Kuhn (1970) in his now classic The Structure of Scientific
Revolutions, have regular features called paradigms that guide research
and provide general modes of explanation. In his Postscript—1969 Kuhn
(1970, p. 175) clarified his concepts of paradigm, emphasizing two levels
of use, which he distinguished with new terms:
1) the disciplinary matrix, the common beliefs, values, and techniques of a group
of scientists that provides the universally accepted concepts and practices of
the field and frameworks for most research; and
2) exemplars, an element of the disciplinary matrix, defined as concrete models
used for solutions to research problems.
The main elements of the disciplinary matrix include:
1) symbolic generalizations (exemplified in mathematical expressions, formulas
and laws);
2) metaphysics, analogies, and metaphors that provide acceptable models and
explanations;
3) values (e.g., criteria for research and evaluation of theories); and
4) exemplars (“paradigms”), the concrete problem solutions used to solve the
puzzles confronting the discipline (e.g., use of hydraulic models for explain-
ing electricity, wind, crowds, etc.).
Altered Consciousness within Paradigmatic Frameworks
As is typical of the social sciences in general, consciousness studies are
not characterized by the uniformity and agreement typical of paradigms of
the physical sciences; rather, theoretical diversity and heterodoxy reflect-
ing mutually exclusive paradigms are characteristic. For example, consider
the divergent concepts and theories regarding the nature of consciousness
found in Freudian psychology versus behaviorist psychology. Kuhn
emphasized that paradigms are shared by members of a scientific commu-
nity as a result of common education and training. The diversity of scien-
tific disciplines that study consciousness precludes the uniformity that a
paradigm presumes.
Kuhn proposed that competing schools of thought characterize scien-
tific fields in a preparadigm period, suggesting that consciousness studies
are in a preparadigmatic period. The study of consciousness and AC does
not have accepted symbolic generalizations or laws. Consciousness studies
A Paradigm for Understanding Altered Consciousness 25
in general do not share a metaphysics; instead, one might contend that
diametrically opposed metaphysics are found, ranging from strictly
materialist, reductionist, and dismissive perspectives to idealist and spiri-
tual systems of explanation. Consciousness studies have no core set of val-
ues, with extreme disciplinary diversity and competing camps (e.g.,
qualitative vs. quantitative approaches). Metaphysical and methodological
diversity produces a similar diversity in the exemplars used to elucidate
consciousness. These range from strictly descriptive and phenomenologi-
cal approaches to more systematic cross-cultural concepts (e.g., “soul
flight” and out-of-body experiences), to strictly materialist approaches
exemplified in using brain imaging as a measure of consciousness.
Paradigm Shifts in Science
Consciousness studies and AC may nonetheless be elucidated by
Kuhn’s models. A central feature of Kuhn’s model is the occurrence of sci-
entific revolutions that lead to radically new ways of understanding. Scien-
tific revolutions associated with the impacts of Copernicus, Darwin, and
Einstein involved abandoning one way of viewing the world in favor of a
new incompatible approach that rejected many prior elements and con-
cepts. Kuhn noted these are relatively rare occurrences, but that they are
key to advances in science. Kuhn proposed that significant changes in sci-
entific fields are characterized by a succession of paradigms (exemplars)
involving a reinterpretation of the elements of the disciplinary matrix.
Kuhn characterized scientific revolutions as reinterpretations, a new way
of seeing relationships among theories, concepts, and laws that involve
changes in the meaning of concepts. For instance, in the shift in
astronomy from the geocentric to the heliocentric model of the solar sys-
tem, the elements (the sun, planets, moons, and asteroids) remained the
same although there were changes in relationships among them and the
laws and models that explain their behavior.
Scientific revolutions are not the outcome of normal science but of
impediments to normal science produced by anomalies, findings that are
in contradiction to normal expectation. Anomalies play central roles in the
development of science. Anomalies are findings that provide counterexam-
ples to the assumptions of the dominant paradigm and are generally key ele-
ments in the development of new theoretical frameworks. These crises
produced by anomalies are the “prerequisite to fundamental inventions of
theory” (Kuhn, 1977, p. 208), producing a scientific revolution in the devel-
opment of a new exemplar for the disciplinary matrix [seeBeischel, Rock, &
Krippner, this volume].
26 Altering Consciousness
Altered Consciousness as an Anomaly
Studies of both ordinary consciousness and AC have produced find-
ings that are anomalies for the dominant materialist assumptions of the
physical sciences. But these anomalies have few central roles in any major
field of scientific inquiry, with the very question of consciousness seen by
some as falling outside of scientific inquiry altogether. Anomalies of
altered consciousness are generally seen as exceptions that are best dis-
missed as distorted data rather than novel findings.
Why have these disjunctures between consciousness studies and the rest
of science not provoked a scientific revolution? Kuhn noted that anomalies
must impede the work of normal science and that the significance of an
anomaly is dependent on the development of concepts and methodologies
that make it apparent. Furthermore, what constitutes anomalies and the
need for new paradigms is not strictly the facts but the social context that
dominates a science, including the policies and practices of professional
organizations and their publication organs. This lack of import of the
anomalies of AC for science in general reflects a significant source of these
findings outside of the established sciences in anthropology, folklore,
parapsychology, occult studies, and other marginalized sciences. One can
conclude that whatever anomalies the studies of AC present, they do not
affect the progress of scientific disciplines as currently practiced.
These phenomena of AC may, nonetheless, be the keys to truly novel
developments in science. Philosopher of science Karl Popper chided Kuhn
for his satisfaction with scientific revolutions, considering them to be
“ordinary science.” Scientific revolutions are rather modest affairs in
Kuhn’s (1970) clarifications, the change in the exemplars with other
aspects of the disciplinary matrix (values and metaphysic and symbolic
generalizations) remaining the same; the findings of the past are main-
tained. Popper (1972) argued for a more thorough revision of the bases
of science in ideological revolutions in which the fundamental assumptions
of the paradigm and theories of science are altered.
In assessing the role of AC within the broader psychology of con-
sciousness, a paramount question is whether such findings constitute a
scientific revolution or even a more profound ideological revolution.
Those who have devoted their lives and careers to consciousness studies
often make such claims, pointing to vastly different conceptions of human
nature demanded by these unusual experiences. Their claims, however,
have not swayed the heterodox field of consciousness studies to a unanim-
ity of opinion that would create a common paradigm, nor have the anoma-
lies demanded major changes in metaphysics, methodology, and values of
A Paradigm for Understanding Altered Consciousness 27
other scientific disciplines that are the hallmark of ideological revolutions.
Whatever claims studies of AC have for fomenting scientific or ideological
revolutions, they remain unrealized, perhaps simply premature for a field
still in its infancy.
Nonetheless, Kuhn also emphasized that during the preparadigmatic
period, the community shares paradigmatic elements. In our contribu-
tions here, I foresee the emergence of a paradigm for the study of AC that
may bring the significance of these phenomena to the forefront of other
scientific disciplines. Below I propose a model of AC that constitutes a
neurophenomenological paradigm linking biological features and phe-
nomenological experiences.
A Reconceptualization of ASC: The Integrative Mode of Consciousness
The concept of altered states of consciousness (ASC) as a topic for
legitimate scientific inquiry emerged in the 1960s. A central figure contrib-
uting to this emerging field was Charles Tart (e.g., 1975, 1977). His influen-
tial books and edited volumes contributed to the formation of an implicit
paradigm of ASC that has persisted for decades. This paradigmatic frame-
work for AC was explicitly linked to waking consciousness.
Tart defined states of consciousness (SoC) as conditions that differ qualita-
tively from others by the presence of conditions or characteristics absent in
other states. SoC represent how people judge usual alterations in experience
and are identified by the individual’s assessment of patterns of experience.
Different SoC are determined by personal significance. Tart (1977) concep-
tualized ASC in terms of differences from the baseline state of conscious-
ness, “an active stable overall patterning of psychological functions that,
via multiple (feedback) stabilization relationships among the parts making
it up, maintains its identity in spite of environmental changes” (p. 192).
The ASC paradigm takes the ordinary aspects of consciousness as the frame-
work for conceptualizing the significant aspects of altered consciousness.
The personal significance of a SoC is of less importance to science than
are the underlying biological dynamics that give both similarity to SoC
across people and regular features of ASC across people and cultures.
What is most significant about ASC is not simply that they have personal
significance but that they have a significance that transcends both personal
and cultural factors. These transpersonal and transcultural features point
to their biological foundations. This notion of a biological basis did not
figure significantly in this paradigm of ASC. But Tart’s pioneering work
also pointed to the reality of altered consciousness that involves something
28 Altering Consciousness
transcendental, a set of significant human conditions that must be under-
stood with respect to their own intrinsic properties. Tart’s (1972) concept
of state-specific sciences foreshadowed the perspective suggested below
that characterizes AC in epistemological terms, as specific forms of know-
ing (also see Winkelman, 2010).
To understand altered consciousness requires a framework that goes
beyond personal significance and baseline or waking consciousness.The
neurophenomenological approach proposes a conceptualization of altered
consciousness in relationship to several biologically based functional
modes of operation that we share with other organisms.
Modes of Consciousness
Similarities in manifestations of waking, deep sleep, and dreams across
species and cultures reflect common underlying biological structures.
These biologically structured foundations are discussed as modes of con-
sciousness. A mode of consciousness is a biologically based functional sys-
tem of organismic operation that reflects conditions of homeostatic
balance among brain subsystems to meet global organismic needs (see
Winkelman, 2010, for discussion and details). Different modes of con-
sciousness are revealed in the congruencies in the primary daily patterns
of variation in behavior and experiences of humans and other animals.
We share with other animals the daily cycles of sleep and waking, with
homologous brain structures responsible for these patterns. Similarly,
humans share the dream mode of consciousness with most mammals.
Their presence in other animals indicates the transcendent nature of these
human modes of consciousness. In addition, humans experience altered
consciousness, conceptualized here as the integrative mode of conscious-
ness. Although learning and cultural factors produce variance in these
modes of consciousness in humans, their similar patterns cross-
culturally (and across species) reflect underlying biological functions and
organismic functions and needs:
Waking: learning, adaptation, and food and other survival needs;
Deep sleep: recuperative functions, regeneration, and growth;
Dreaming (REM sleep): memory integration and consolidation and psychosocial
adaptation; and
Integrative: psychodynamic growth and social and psychological integration.
These modes are so basic to organismic operation that they are function-
ally wired in multiple ways into brain structures, as illustrated in the brain’s
A Paradigm for Understanding Altered Consciousness 29
control of dreaming. No single region of the brain is entirely responsible for
the initiation of REM sleep, and dreaming persists or re-emerges even after
excising the supposed dream areas (Graham, 1990; also see Alkire & Miller,
2005; Hobson, 2005; and Kokoszka & Wallace, Volume 2, for general neu-
ral correlates of sleep and dreams).
Tart’s baseline and altered states of consciousness correspond to the
waking and integrative modes of consciousness, respectively. Because
SoCs differ in terms of personal significance or psychological subsystems,
they are a subsidiary level of analysis to that of modes, which are derived
from physiological processes related to organismic needs and homeostatic
balance. SoC occur within modes, with their specific characteristics deter-
mined by the social, cultural, and psychological effects rather than by the
strictly biological needs. Different states of consciousness are found within
each of the modes of consciousness—sleep, dreaming, waking, and inte-
grative. During the modes of deep sleep and dreams, a variety of SoC
may emerge, including pathological states such as somnambulism, noctur-
nal automatisms, sleep drunkenness, sleep terrors, and also “normal”
states such as hypnagogic and hypnopompic transitions between being
awake and being asleep. The integrative mode of consciousness includes
various SoC (e.g., soul flight, possession, samadhi), but would not include
everything that Tart considered ASC since some SoC have disintegrative
and pathological features rather than integrative effects.
Biological Bases of the Integrative Mode of Consciousness
Diverse states of altered consciousness reflect similar brain responses.
Common mechanisms involve manipulation of the autonomic nervous
system through extensive ergotropic (sympathetic) activation leading to a
collapse into a trophotropic (parasympathetic) dominant state with a
slowing of the brain wave discharges into a more synchronized and coher-
ent pattern (see Winkelman, 2010, for review). Altered consciousness epi-
sodes are also characterized by an integration of the various levels of the
brain. This integration is manifested in entrainment of the frontal cortex
by highly coherent and synchronized slow-wave discharges emanating
from the limbic system and related lower-brain structures. These entrain-
ments may occur at a variety of frequencies, but two predominant patterns
are synchronized slow-wave theta bands (3–6 cycles per second) and the
high-frequency gamma oscillations (40+ cps). These synchronized brain
wave patterns justify labeling them as an integrative mode of consciousness
(Winkelman, 2000). The biological foundation for these experiences is
30 Altering Consciousness
reflected in their production by a wide variety of natural agents (i.e., psy-
chedelic drugs) and ritual procedures (i.e., trauma, extreme fasting, and
exertion) that elicit these brain wave patterns.
This paradigm of integrative consciousness originates in the work of
Mandell (1980), who suggested that physiological mechanisms underly-
ing “transcendent states” are based in a common neurochemical
pathway-involving the temporal lobe. Many agents and procedures result
in a loss of serotonin inhibition to the hippocampal cells, producing an
increase in cell activity and the manifestation of hippocampal-septal
slow-wave EEG activity (alpha, delta, and especially theta) that imposes a
synchronous slow-wave pattern across the lobes. Mandell suggested that
the neurobiological basis underlying transcendent states, including their
ineffable and religious components, involves a “biogenic amine-
temporal-lobe limbic neurology . . . the mesolimbic serotonergic pathway
that extends from the median raphe nucleus in the mesencephalon, coex-
istent with part of the mesencephalic reticular formation regulating arousal
. . . to the septum and hippocampus” (pp. 381, 390). This produces hyper-
synchronous discharges across the hippocampal-septal-reticular-raphe
circuit, which links the R(reptilian) complex and paleomammalian brain.
Agents and procedures that invoke this pattern include hallucinogens,
amphetamines, cocaine, marijuana, polypeptide opiates, long-distance run-
ning, hunger, thirst, sleep loss, auditory stimuli such as drumming and
chanting, sensory deprivation, dream states, meditation, and a variety of
psychophysiological imbalances or sensitivities resulting from injury,
trauma, disease, or hereditarily transmitted nervous system conditions (see
Winkelman, 2010, for review).
The underlying psychobiology of many forms of AC involves this
response based in the serotonergic connections between the limbic system
and brain stem regions [see Presti and various chapters on psychoactive
drugs, Volume 2]. While dopamine mechanisms are also involved, sero-
tonin has a significant role as a neuromodulator that regulates the activities
of many other neurotransmitter systems.Mandell proposed that the hippo-
campus is the focal point of the mechanisms that reduce the inhibitory
serotonin regulation of temporal lobe limbic function. The loss of inhibi-
tory regulation by serotonin results in a reduction or loss of the “gating”
of emotional response and an enhancement of dopamine circuitry. This
loss of gating combined with hippocampal-septal-discharges results
in an emotional flooding or ecstasy (cf. Mandell 1980, p. 400). The
hippocampal-septal system is an association area involved in the forma-
tion and the mediation of memory and emotions. The synchronous pat-
terns originating in the hippocampal-septal-reticular raphe circuits
A Paradigm for Understanding Altered Consciousness 31
reflect linkages of the attentional mechanisms in the behavioral brain
regions (reticular formation) and the emotional brain. These synchronous
electrical discharges propagate up the major axon bundles from the base
of the brain into the frontal cortex, integrating activity from ancient levels
of the brain into the frontal lobes.
Psychedelics and Cortico-striato-thalamo-cortical Feedback Loops
This paradigm of integration as a generic feature underlying AC is illus-
trated by Vollenweider’s (1998) research on the mechanisms of action of
psychedelics on the major cortical loops. The frontal-subcortical circuits
provide one of the principal organizational networks of the brain involv-
ing neuronal linkages and feedback loops of the cortical areas of the fron-
tal brain with the thalamus of the brain stem region (Cummings, 1993).
These loops unite specific regions of the frontal cortex with lower brain
regions, specifically the basal ganglia, substantia nigra, and the thalamus.
These circuits are central to brain–behavioral relationships, mediating
motor activity and eye movement, as well as social actions, motivations,
and executive functions. Vollenweider attributes the consciousness-
altering properties of psychedelics to their selective effects on the brain’s
cortico-striato-thalamo-cortical feedback loops that link the information
gating systems of lower levels of the brain with the frontal cortex. These
loops are regulated by the thalamus, which limits the ascending informa-
tion to the frontal cortex from the environment and body. Psychedelics
disable this disinhibition process; this increases access to the flow of infor-
mation that is ordinarily inhibited, overwhelming the frontal cortex and
leading to an alteration of experience of self, other, environment, and the
internal world of psychological structures and projection.
Altered Consciousness as Dysregulation of the Prefrontal Cortex
The notion that alterations of consciousness involve a disruption of the
processes of the frontal cortex is elaborated by Dietrich (2003), who pro-
poses that a variety of conditions of AC involve a temporary dysregulation
of the prefrontal cortex (PFC). He reviews psychological and neurological
studies on the effects of endurance running, dreaming, hypnosis, drug-
induced states, and meditation to illustrate their commonality in the dis-
ruption of the higher-order functions associated with the PFC. The
common effects of this disruption are manifested in the loss of the roles
of the frontal lobes and prefrontal cortex involved in highly integrated
aspects of neural information processing and higher cognitive functions.
32 Altering Consciousness
The downregulation of certain mental faculties from conscious aware-
ness involves a subtraction of certain faculties or experiences, with a vari-
ety of different modules capable of being down-regulated, reducing
awareness to a lower condition within the hierarchy of conscious states.
Greater alterations of consciousness, such as those experienced in dream-
ing or out-of-body experiences (OBE), involve a greater amount of down-
regulation of prefrontal mediated capacities [see Dieguez & Blanke, Vol-
ume 2]. The different kinds of prefrontal capacities and different degrees
of deregulation provide for a variety of phenomenological alterations to
consciousness. The different agents and activities that lead to this dysere-
gulation do so in distinct ways that produce unique phenomenological
characteristics of religious experiences. Dietrich proposes that these differ-
ent forms of hypofrontality involve a general principle of a hierarchical
and progressive disengagement with the more sophisticated cognitive
skills and levels of consciousness involving self-awareness and planning
(e.g., self-reflection, sense of time, planning) being compromised first, fol-
lowed by lower-level systems. The dysregulation of the PFC allows a num-
ber of unusual self-experiences related to our more ancient brain
functions. Dietrich (2003) proposed that the lack of engagement of the
PFC results in the lack of certain frontal qualities—willful action, self-
awareness, the deliberate direction of attention, abstract and creative
thought, and planning. However, many alterations of consciousness, nota-
bly the shamanic soul journey and many meditative states, have those
properties, requiring explanation of how these capacities persist.
Meditation as Integrative and Dissociative Brain Dynamics
The integrative and dissociative or “dysregulated” brain dynamics of
AC are exemplified in meditation. Since the earliest studies, there have
been consistent reports of similarities across meditative disciplines in a
shift toward parasympathetic dominance, an overall decrease in frequency
of the brain waves to alpha and theta ranges, and increases in alpha and
theta amplitude and regularity in the frontal and central regions of the
brain (Taylor, Murphy, & Donovan, 1997). More recent research confirms
that typical brain waves associated with meditation involve an increase in
alpha waves, which then decrease in frequency toward dominant theta
rhythms (Cahn & Polich, 2006; Takahashi et al., 2005). Hebert and
coworkers (2005) implicated alpha EEG during meditation as a form of
“integration in the brain that leads to high-level cognitive processes.
These are the same basic systemic physiological principles underlying the
concept of the integrative mode of consciousness: enhanced synchronization
A Paradigm for Understanding Altered Consciousness 33
of brain wave patterns. An additional level of meditation-induced integration
in the brain is reflected in biphasic hypersynchronous high-frequency
gamma waves (35–44 cps; see Lehmann et al., 2001; Lutz et al., 2004,
Vialatte et al., 2009). The presence of gamma in meditation is a direct confir-
mation of the integrative model because gamma is associated with binding of
diverse signals within the brain; furthermore, gamma synchronization is
modulated by the theta and alpha rhythms (Fries, 2009, p. 217).
Altered consciousness also necessarily involves selective segregation or
deafferentation of input from brain systems. Dissociation may nonetheless
reflect the integration of the principles of lower brain systems and their
imposition on the frontal cortex. This dissociation is specifically seen in
meditation and hypnosis, as well as in more obviously dissociative condi-
tions such as spirit possession [see Sluhovsky, this volume]. D’Aquili and
Newberg (1999) propose that mystical experience involves interference
with the normal functions of tertiary association areas, namely a deafferenta-
tion or functional cutting off of input into a structure. Deafferentation
results in a structure firing independent of normal input from other parts
of the brain. Inhibition of some structures explains the ineffable aspects of
mystical experiences, a result of being cut off from language production
centers. D’Aquili and Newberg propose that deafferentation of limbic stimu-
lation results in specific effects such as relaxation and profound quiescence,
ecstatic and blissful feelings, and a sense of pure space and obliteration of
the self–other distinction. Enhancing the attentional processes characteristic
of meditation results in deafferentation of input from other systems, such as
the environment, that would be distracting to highly focused attention.
Hypnosis as Dissociation
Selection for a biological disposition to these highly focused internal
states of awareness and limbic–frontal integration characterized by theta
wave discharge patterns is illustrated by hypnosis (Crawford, 1994).
Highly hypnotizable people have attentional filtering mechanisms that
provide a concentration with a simultaneous dissociation of some cogni-
tive features [see Carden
˜a & Alvarado, this volume]. Crawford proposed
that hypnosis and its enhanced attention reflect an interaction between
subcortical and cortical brain mechanisms that enable highly hypnotizable
people to sustain attention as well as disattention. A consequence of the
highly hypnotizable individual’s more efficient frontal limbic attentional
systems is the ability to disattend to extraneous stimuli, known as cogni-
tive inhibition, which is associated with enhanced theta-wave production.
34 Altering Consciousness
Enhanced limbic frontal interaction characteristic of highly hypnotizable
individuals is a pattern of brain functioning that typifies the integrative
mode of consciousness, an enhanced interaction between the limbic and
the frontal brain that produces integration across the neuraxis and holistic
information-processing styles (Crawford, 1994)
Dissociation is reflected in the highly susceptible hypnotic subject’s
ability to engage an alternative reality that is demanded by a social rela-
tionship with the hypnotist, expressed in the concept of rapport that
reflects the person’s cooperation with the hypnotist. This feature of hyp-
notic susceptibility also exemplifies integration, epitomized in the ability
to subject oneself to the theory of mind manifested by others in complying
with the requests of the hypnotist. This lower-level brain control is illus-
trated by highly hypnotic subjects’ greater efficiency in implementing the
strategies suggested by others (the hypnotist) while having a general
inability to voluntarily alter their own strategic performance, a predomi-
nance of paleomammalian brain functions over the frontal cortex (also
see Jamieson, 2007; Jamieson & Woody 2007; Ray, 2007).
Hypnosis shifts the interaction between the frontal and limbic systems
to the latter’s evolved capacities for processing of survival-related informa-
tion and feelings (Woody & Szechtman, 2007), allowing social feelings to
take precedence over the sensory world in dictating behavior. Hypnotic
susceptibility engages the motivational systems that manage social hier-
archy, with the individual accepting a subordinate position and the impo-
sition of the will of a dominant other. Highly susceptible hypnotic subjects
experience a dominance of the limbic structures in an enhanced orienta-
tion to the emotional/motivation engagement with the hypnotist, reflect-
ing the survival value of the evolved capacity to subordinate the
individual’s personal perceptions and behaviors to the wishes of the leader.
This reflects the unconscious emotional control of volition characteristic of
hypnosis, where the higher-level ego structures do not identify with the
volitional qualities of behavior generated unconsciously.
This reflects the basic features of the model of the integrative mode of
consciousness, where lower-level structures impose their dynamics on
the overall functional outcomes. The model is supported by the state
approaches to hypnosis, which see it as engaging structures of conscious-
ness and brain networks distinctive from those of waking consciousness.
In contrast to the orientation to the external sensory world, hypnosis and
SoC in the integrative mode of consciousness engage this imagined alter-
native that controls the body, including physiological responses, percep-
tions, emotions, behaviors, and thoughts.
A Paradigm for Understanding Altered Consciousness 35
Spirit Possession as Integration and Dissociation
This interactive dynamic of integration and dissociation is illustrated in
the context of possession and dissociative disorders.Since dissociation is
by definition “a lack of integration of psychological processes that normally
should be integrated” (Carden
˜a & Gleaves, 2003, p. 474), it would seem
that the dissociative interpretation of possession directly contradicts the
fundamental thesis of the integrative mode of consciousness. Without
question, possession involves some forms of dissociation and separation,
such as manifested in amnesia. But what do the symptoms or processes of
possession tell us about the functioning of the individual? These processes
of dissociation have been viewed as defense mechanisms to allow the inte-
grated functioning of the emotional self in the face of self-destroying trauma.
Seligman and Kirmayer (2008) reviewed research on dissociation in
normal populations, where its similarities with pathological forms are
characterized positively in terms of an intense focus of attention, isolation
from the external environment, and absorption, particularly with inter-
nally generated thoughts and images such as daydreams. A dominant
hypothesis is that dissociation evolved as a mechanism to block awareness
and memories in order to escape the stress of interpersonal situations, for
example, protecting oneself from extreme emotional stimulation and asso-
ciated autonomic arousal that comes from an attack or betrayal. The ability
of traumatic stress to block consolidation of conscious narrative memories
provides an explanation based on evolutionary adaptations. Distancing of
self and identity provides emotional numbing mechanisms that can inhibit
the flight-or-fight response, enabling a more objective search for survival
strategies rather than being driven by emotional fears. In order to seek sol-
utions to pressing problems, certain information is compartmentalized,
kept out of consciousness, so that adaptive responses may be made.
Seligman and Kirmayer (2008) showed how we can reconcile the
adaptive paradigm of dissociation with the obvious nonadaptive patho-
logical dimensions by examining how the social context interacts with
psychophysiological bases. Dissociation involves a regulation of atten-
tional mechanisms that allow a selective suppression of perceptions and
memories and a reduction of physiological stress. In shamanistic rituals,
these traumatic dissociative experiences are addressed in processes that
allow the emergence of an integrated sense of self linked to “others” who
provide a variety of attachment functions for self integration. Sar and
Ozturk (2007) propose that dissociation involves a detachment of the
psychological self from the sociological self. In this dissociation, the socio-
logical self becomes amplified. This sociological self, which functions in
36 Altering Consciousness
the interface between the individual and society, undergoes an accelerated
development as a consequence of trauma. This protective dissociation of
the psychological self allows for a reestablishment of connections between
the inner and outer world through the sociological self.
Social roles of possession exemplify this control, where higher-order
representations embodied in the personalities of possession spirits redirect
identity and behavior to adapt to varying social circumstances. Possession
must be seen in light of an extreme tendency to identify with idealized
social norms embodied in the social functions of the paleomammalian
brain.
Allison and Schwarz (1999) provide an understanding of the integra-
tion involved in the possessing “other” in the context of dissociative iden-
tity disorders and the operation of what they called an “inner self helper,”
a source of unconscious inner guidance. This personality structure of
these patients was aware of all of the multiple identities and had a greater
consciousness and control than did the ego. This inner helper also could
help and strengthen the ego in its struggles with the other personalities.
The inner self helper provides a psychological space for control, stability,
clarity, and self-understanding. Comstock (1991) emphasized the ability
of this structure to operate as an organizing force for the person. This
identity provides integrative potentials for the patient and processes of
therapeutic resolution of the dissociated aspects of the ego in the integra-
tion or psychological fusion of the personalities.
Common Denominators in Altered Consciousness
Frecska and Luna (2006) suggested that commonalities in techniques
that alter consciousness involve two opposite processes: overstressing the
frontal-prefrontal circuits of the coping mechanisms or dysregulating
them, pushing them below the functional range. They propose that the
resulting effect is the emergence of a direct intuitive information process-
ing that replaces the ordinary perceptual-cognitive processing. Frecska
and Luna point to support for this model of integrative consciousness in:
(1) the final common pathway of psychedelic drugs’ action in the thala-
mus; (2) the role of thalamic gamma synchronization in binding; (3) the
vertical organization of fronto-subcortico-thalamic feedback loops; and
(4) their relations to the horizontal layering of McLean’s triune brain
model.
This perspective illustrates that integration and dissociation or deaffer-
entation are complementary and co-dependent processes. Typical
A Paradigm for Understanding Altered Consciousness 37
alterations of consciousness occur with an enhanced integration of lower
brain processes that may simultaneously disengage other habitual pro-
cesses of knowing. This also helps illustrate how AC constitutes a special
form of knowing.
Examination of these brain patterns of AC from the perspectives of
MacLean’s (1990) model of the triune brain illustrates the elicitation of
the paleomentation and the emotio-mentation processes of lower brain
structures (R-complex and limbic brain) and their management of emo-
tions, attachment, social relations and bonding, sense of self, and convic-
tions about beliefs.Procedures and conditions that alter consciousness
have an intrinsic potential to produce an integration of information
processing between the R-complex and the limbic system, between the
limbic system and the frontal cortex, and between the hemispheres of
the cortex; hence the labeling as the integrative mode of consciousness.
A primary characteristic of integrative consciousness involves an
ascending integration of brain mechanisms, a limbic-system driving of
the frontal cortex that integrates the preconscious or unconscious func-
tions and material into self-conscious awareness. Key physiological mech-
anisms underlying integrative forms of consciousness are found in
activation of the paleomammalian brain, specifically the hippocampal-
septal circuits, the hypothalamus, and related brain structures that regu-
late emotions (MacLean, 1990). These effects contribute to a second char-
acteristic of integrative forms of consciousness, the ability to act on the
structures of earlier levels of consciousness by engaging operational struc-
tures of the unconscious and dissociated aspects of the self. This engage-
ment allows a differential elicitation of specific aspects of the psyche and
their integration at a higher level of awareness and self-organization.
Conclusions
Explaining AC requires neurophenomenological approaches that link
biological functions and structures to the cultural processes producing
experience. These neurophenomenological approaches (e.g., Laughlin,
McManus, & d’Aquili, 1992; Winkelman, 2010) illustrate that alterations
of consciousness engage special forms of knowing. A prominent manifes-
tation of altered consciousness involves imagetic representations known
as a presentational symbolism (Hunt, 1995). This system of visual symbol-
ism provides knowledge—one might even say wisdom—beyond that of
our rational language-based consciousness, exemplified in the out-of-
body experiences of shamans. Altered consciousness reflects this early
38 Altering Consciousness
level of symbolism through which we know the universe in ways much
like those of other animals through a variety of prelanguage structures
and processes of knowing. Altered consciousness involves special forms
of the integration of consciousness through an elevation of the precon-
scious processes of our ancient reptilian and paleomammalian brains.
These levels of consciousness produce key aspects of AC such as holistic
perceptions and intuition, special forms of pre–self-identification, nonver-
bal knowledge, and manifestations of intense emotional engagement and
detachment.
References
Alkire, M., & Miller, J. (2005). General anesthesia and the neural correlates of
consciousness. Progress in Brain Research, 150, 229–244.
Allison, R., & Schwarz, T. (1999). Minds in many pieces: Revealing the spiritual side
of multiple personality. Los Osos, CA: CIE Publishing.
Bourguignon, E. (1968). Cross-cultural study of dissociational states. Columbus:
Ohio State University Press.
Cahn, B., & Polich, J. (2006). Meditation states and traits: EEG, ERP and neuro-
imaging. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 180–211.
Carden
˜a, E., & Gleaves, D. H. (2003). Dissociative disorders: Phantoms of the
self. In S. M. Turner & M. Hersen (Eds.), Adult psychopathology and diagnosis
(4th ed., pp. 473–503). New York: Wiley.
Comstock, C. (1991). The inner self helper and concepts of inner guidance. Dis-
sociation, 4(3), 165–177.
Crawford, H. (1994). Brain dynamics and hypnosis: Attentional and disatten-
tional processes. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis,
42, 204–232.
Cummings, J. L. (1993). Frontal-subcortical circuits and human behavior.
Archives of Neurology, 50, 873–880.
d’Aquili, E., & Newberg, A. (1999). The mystical mind: Probing the biology of reli-
gious experience. Minneapolis, MN: Fortress Press.
Dietrich, A. (2003). Functional neuroanatomy of altered states of consciousness:
The transient hypofrontality hypothesis. Consciousness and Cognition, 12,
231–256.
Frecska, E., & Luna, E. (2006). Neuro-ontological interpretation of spiritual
experiences. Neuropsychopharmacologia Hungarica VIII, 3, 143–153.
Fries, P. (2009). Neuronal gamma-band synchronization as a fundamental pro-
cess in cortical computation. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 32, 209–224.
Graham, R. (1990). Physiological psychology. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.
Hebert, R., Lehmann, D., Tan, G., Travis, F., & Arenander, A. (2005). Enhanced
EEG alpha-time domain phase synchrony during transcendental meditation:
Implications for cortical integration theory.Signal Processing, 85, 2213–2232.
A Paradigm for Understanding Altered Consciousness 39
Hobson, J. (2005). Sleep is of the brain, by the brain and for the brain. Nature,
437, 1254–1256.
Hunt, H. (1995). On the nature of consciousness. New Haven, CT: Yale University
Press.
Jamieson, G. (2007). Previews and prospects for the cognitive neuroscience of
hypnosis and conscious states. In G. Jamieson (Ed.), Hypnosis and conscious
states: The cognitive neuroscience perspective (pp. 1–14). Oxford, UK: Oxford
University Press.
Jamieson, G., & Woody, E. (2007). Dissociated control as a paradigm for
cognitive neuroscience research and theorizing in hypnosis. In G. Jamieson
(Ed.), Hypnosis and conscious states: The cognitive neuroscience perspective
(pp. 111–131). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Kuhn, T. (1970). The structure of scientific revolutions (2nd ed.). Chicago: Univer-
sity of Chicago Press.
Kuhn, T. (1977). The essential tension. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Laughlin, C., McManus, J., & d’Aquili, E. (1992). Brain, symbol, and experience.
Toward a neurophenomenology of consciousness. New York: Columbia University
Press.
Lehmann, D., Faber, P., Achermann, P., Jeanmonod, D., Gianotti, L., & Pissa-
galli, D. (2001). EEG brain sources of EEG gamma frequency during volition-
ally meditation-induced, altered states of consciousness, and experience of the
self. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 108, 111–121.
Lutz, A., Greischar, L., Rawlings, N., Richard, M., & Davidson, R. (2004). Long-
term meditators self-induced high-amplitude gamma synchronization during
mental practice. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 101, 16369–
16373.
MacLean, P. (1990). The triune brain in evolution. New York: Plenum.
Mandell, A. (1980). Toward a psychobiology of transcendence: God in the brain.
In D. Davidson & R. Davidson (Eds.), The psychobiology of consciousness
(pp. 379–464). New York: Plenum.
Popper, K. (1972). Objective knowledge: An evolutionary approach. Oxford, UK:
Clarendon Press.
Ra
¨tsch, C. (2005). The encyclopedia of psychoactive plants: Ethnopharmacology and
its applications (Trans. J. Baker). Rochester, VT: Park Street Press. (Originally
published 1998, Enzyklopa
¨die der psychoaktiven Pflanzen. Aarau, Switzerland:
AT Verlag.)
Ray, W. (2007). The experience of agency and hypnosis from an evolutionary
perspective. In G. Jamieson (Ed.), Hypnosis and conscious states: The cognitive
neuroscience perspective (pp. 223–240). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Sar, V., & Ozturk, E. (2007). Functional dissociation of the self: A sociocognitive
approach to trauma and dissociation. Journal of Trauma and Dissociation, 8(4),
69–89.
Schultes, E., & Hofmann, A. (1979). Plants of the gods. New York: McGraw-Hill.
40 Altering Consciousness
Seligman, R., & Kirmayer, L. (2008). Dissociative experience and cultural neuro-
science: Narrative, metaphor and mechanism. Medicine & Psychiatry, 32,
31–64.
Siegel, R. K. (2005). Intoxication: The universal drive for mind-altering substances.
Rochester, VT: Park Street Press.
Takahashi, T., Murata, T., Hamada, T., Omori, M., Kosaka, H., Kikuchi, M.,
Yoshida, H., & Wada, Y. (2005). Changes in EEG and autonomic nervous sys-
tem activity during meditation and their association with personality traits.
International Journal of Psychophysiology, 55, 199–207.
Tart, C. (1972). States of consciousness and state-specific sciences. Science, 176,
1203–1210.
Tart, C. (1975). States of consciousness. New York: Dutton.
Tart, C. (1977). Putting the pieces together: A conceptual framework for under-
standing discrete states of consciousness. In N. Zinberg (Ed.), Alternate states
of consciousness (pp. 158–219). New York: Free Press.
Taylor, E., Murphy, M., & Donovan, S. (1997). The physical and psychological
effects of meditation: A review of contemporary research with a comprehensive bib-
liography: 1931–1996. Sausalito, CA: Institute of Noetic Sciences.
Vialatte, F., Bakardjian, H., Prasad, R., & Cichocki, A. (2009). EEG paroxysmal
gamma waves during Bhramari Pranayama: A yoga breathing technique. Con-
sciousness and Cognition 18, 977–988.
Vollenweider, F. (1998). Recent advances and concepts in the search for biologi-
cal correlates of hallucinogen-induced altered states of consciousness. Heffter
Review of Psychedelic Research, 1, 21–32.
Winkelman, M. (1992). Shamans, priests, and witches. A cross-cultural study of
magico-religious practitioners. Anthropological Research Papers #44. Tempe:
Arizona State University.
Winkelman, M. (2000). Shamanism: The neural ecology of consciousness and healing.
Westport, CT: Bergin and Garvey.
Winkelman, M. (2010). Shamanism: A biopsychosocial paradigm of consciousness
and healing. Santa Barbara, CA: ABC-CLIO.
Woody, E., & Szechtman, H. (2007). To see feelingly: Emotion, motivation and
hypnosis. In G. Jamieson (Ed.), Hypnosis and conscious states: The cognitive
neuroscience perspective (pp. 141–256). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
A Paradigm for Understanding Altered Consciousness 41
... While it is true that all ritualists called shamans don't have identical ASC, this does not mean there is no biological bases for ASCs, nor that no similarities in ASC exist for specific types of practitioners. Whitely (2006,2009) has detailed evidence refuting the supposed inadequacies of the neuropsychology models of shamanism, and Winkelman (2010aWinkelman ( , 2011aWinkelman ( , 2013bWinkelman ( , 2017aWinkelman ( , 2017b provides a literature review that shows the robust evidence for a neuropsychological model of ASCs (also see this chapter, The Mystical Stance: Shamanic ASC and the Innate Mind). Winkelman shows common patterns of ASC that involve what he calls an integrative mode of consciousness are characterized by ascending brain wave discharges, an enhanced bottom-up brain dynamics. ...
... The selection for the shaman's role was indicated by spontaneous visions, dreams, and sickness, natural tendencies for entering ASC that reflect an enhanced access to and integration of sub-conscious processes (Winkelman, 2010a(Winkelman, , 2011a. The training was typified in a vision quest involving induction of ASC through practices of isolation in the wilderness, fasting, abstinence, and austerities that stimulate experiences of spiritual encounters. ...
... Singh characterizes shamans as the consequence of such cultural evolution for people who are good at deceiving others into thinking they can control uncontrollable events. Winkelman (2018b) shows the many conceptual and empirical errors in Singh's arguments and why evolutionary models of conspicuous displays (Henrich, 2009;Winkelman 2019c) and biological models of ASC (Winkelman 2011a provide empirically and theoretically grounded characterization of the bases for the evolution of shamanism (also see Winkelman, 2010a). Shamanism involves adaptations for socially beneficial outcomes of psychological, emotional, social, and cognitive integration and selfformation/individuation, not for some general propensity for deception. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
Shamanism is a transcultural concept for understanding roles of ritual and psychedelics in the prehistoric origins of religiosity. The origins of religiosity are revealed by parallels of shamanic and chimpanzee collective ritualizations involving group chorusing and drumming with dramatic bipedal displays. This hominid baseline was expanded with mimetic evolution of song, dance and enactment. Psychedelic substances stimulate innate cognitive dispositions manifested in shamanism such as the human-like qualities of spirits, animal identities and other spiritual and mystical experiences. These structural features of consciousness are stimulated by mimetic performances with song, dancing, and drumming; painful and exhausting austerities; and psychedelic substances. These produce altered experiences of the self which are conceptualized within indigenous psychologies as spirits and one’s soul, spiritual allies, and animal powers that can be incorporated into personal powers (i.e., animal transformation). Cross-cultural manifestation of shamanic features reveal that they are based in biology rather than merely cultural traditions.
... In this context, dissociation allows for a simulation of reality by "reducing" its complexity, a mechanism which is underlined as a neurobiological common ground of creativity in art and science (Kandel ,2016). Perspectives about reality are organized in horizontal and vertical layers., and while an "integrated mode of consciousness" (Winkelman, 2011) is possible temporarily, the "parallel-distinct" structures of internal world and external reality are necessary for maintaining a sense of "actuality". The latter refers to the version of reality that aligns with the minimum requirements of consensus among larger groups of people. ...
... When analysed in the framework of the tripartite self-system, the sociological self takes on the role of the executive organ in negotiating reality, while the psychological self and the internal moderator work to nurture an "integrated mode of consciousness" (Winkelman, 2011) as much as possible. In certain situations, such as hypnosis, the sociological self is more prone to task-focused fragmentation. ...
Article
Dissociative depression is a complex and chronic mood disorder characterized by a combination of persistent depressive symptoms and intermittent major depressive episodes. A key feature of dissociative depression is the existence of dissociative symptoms that are linked to prolonged stress experienced during childhood and infancy. Disturbances of sense of self and agency are core indicators of the disorder. Common symptoms include thoughts of guilt and worthlessness, difficulties with concentration and decision-making, changes in appetite and sleep, and suicidal ideation. Additionally, individuals may exhibit various expressive patterns such as borderline phenomena, psychoticism, trauma-related enactments, and attempts at control through somatization, compensatory narcissism, and obsessions. One challenge is that dissociative depression often does not respond well to biological treatments alone. Psychotherapeutic interventions that do not specifically address the dissociative aspect may also be ineffective. The required comprehensive approach involves working through layers of therapeutic reality to reverse the process that led to the status quo. An intensive psychodynamic practice with a renewed theoretical understanding (Dialectical Dynamic Therapy-DDT) is necessary to identify the leverage points that can bring about radical inner change. The aim is to alleviate the barriers that prevent individuals from realizing their full potential assigned in the beginning of life.
... Mimetic culture Does this mimetic capacity convey the kinds of supernatural meanings and agreements that Dunbar see as essential to qualifying as religion? Drumming, dancing and music are psychotechnologies eliciting the EOS and healing responses which also produce extraordinary ASC universally interpreted as spiritual (Winkelman 2011). The recurrent nature of these experiences reflects biological mechanisms (Winkelman 2010a(Winkelman , 2011. ...
... Drumming, dancing and music are psychotechnologies eliciting the EOS and healing responses which also produce extraordinary ASC universally interpreted as spiritual (Winkelman 2011). The recurrent nature of these experiences reflects biological mechanisms (Winkelman 2010a(Winkelman , 2011. The shamanic capacity for soul flight (aka out-of-body experience) is a widely attested archetypal experience (i.e, near death experiences, astral projection) manifesting innate structures of psyche (Winkelman 2019b). ...
Article
Full-text available
Dunbar emphasizes distinctions between shamanic and doctrinal religions, noting the importance of shamanism for identifying ancient religious features and adaptations. Dunbar proposes religious evolution began with shamanic practices that engage the endogenous opioid system (EOS, endorphins) to enhance social bonding. Doctrinal religions based on higher order mentalizing skills provided a capacity to socially engage a transcendental world but did not replace shamanism which persisted in modified forms. Dunbar notes shamanic universals reflect psychobiological structures but doesn’t provide an empirical framework for characterizing these features or assessing their evolved bases (i.e., Winkelman 1990, 2010a&b, 2021, 2022). These understandings of the deep evolutionary origins of religion are presented as a theory of religious origins based in mimesis.
... ASCs have been experienced and explored in various ways in most traditions and societies throughout history [10,11,61,79,93]. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
There has been increasing interest shown in experiences such as lucid dreams, hallucinations, or awe that arise in HCI. Altered States of Consciousness (ASC) is the umbrella term for these experiences, yet it has been subject to fragmented study, and design knowledge to help individuals working on technology-driven ASCs is lacking. This paper investigates HCI studies involving ASC artefacts through a scoping review. The findings relate to (1) ASC induction methods, (2) ASC experiences through artefacts, (3) ASC artefacts, and (4) the technology of ASC artefacts. The returned literature shows that HCI studies have mainly explored psychologically induced ASCs, and XR technologies and embodied interaction are widely used in ASC research. Meanwhile, physical artefact design including active body movements and the integration of games and play approaches featured as prospective directions. These results will contribute to the knowledge of those studying and designing ASC artefacts.
... Seli et al., 2018;Herbert, 2011a;Becker, 2004). Importantly, it is 12 the commonalities, that are important to understanding ASCs, rather than their differences 13 (Winkelman, 2011). Ultimately, adopting a family-resemblances approach may increase our 14 understanding of the interconnectedness between related phenomena of hypnotic trance, 15 thereby gaining a deeper recognition of the existing variety in entrancing states (Seli et al., 16 2018). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Music has often been linked to the capacity to induce trance-like states, with listeners sometimes describing their experiences as “hypnotic-like”. However, it is still unclear whether music can induce effects similar to those found in response to a hypnotic induction, or whether these references rest on a metaphor. Using matched samples (n=266), this exploratory study compared the subjective experience of hypnosis and music by contrasting phenomenology of consciousness dimension scores in response to listening to self-chosen music and following a hypnotic induction. We estimated regularized Gaussian graphical models and corresponding centrality indices to detect the most central experiential features in the networks for both groups music and hypnosis. We also examined the interconnectedness between communities of nodes, and compared the two networks using the Network Comparison Test (NCT). The intensity of listening to music was found to be slightly higher compared to the hypnotic state, and differed especially in emotions and visual imagery. The NCT revealed that the networks were similar in density, as was the average variance of dimensions explained by the other dimensions included in the network. Although the adjacency matrices correlation was high, the overall network structures for music and hypnosis were nonetheless significantly different. Post-hoc tests further indicated that connections were significantly different for two pairs of dimensions: rationality – volitional control and self-awareness – volitional control. Altogether, these findings suggest that music and an applied hypnotic induction evoke different states of mind. Advocating a family-resemblances approach, more research is required to identify shared mechanisms of trance-like experiences.
... O papel do terapeuta é apenas facilitar o processo terapêutico, produzir e guiar o paciente para a sua autoconsciência. 3,59,60 O termo estado "modificado" de consciência considera estados menos profundos, induzidos por meios mais suaves como a meditação ou abordagens psicoterapêuticas. 61 Estes estados mentais podem ser espontâneos, induzidos (física, fisiológica, psicológica e farmacologicamente) ou patológicos 61 e têm variações socioculturais. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction: Recently, neuropsychiatry has focused almost exclusively on the correction of pathophysiological changes, inevitably neglecting certain psychosomatic factors of chronic diseases in the community. It is thus necessary to develop and apply mental health that uses a more comprehensive approach in the treatment of disabling diseases and in primary health care. Aim: This study aims to review the effect of self-awareness and awareness of morbidity, as described subjectively by patients during psychotherapeutic interventions or by healthy adults in meditative practices. Materials and Methods: To assess the effect described, a conceptual analysis is proposed as a method. Reviewing the current literature on the «state of the art” of brain dynamics and the psychoneuroimmunological axis, the correlation between the patient’s subjective experience and affective and cognitive neuroscience was tested. As a research hypothesis, it was intended to highlight the scientific paradigms used to study the effect described above. Results: The current limitation on the integration of psychotherapeutic approaches in understanding well-being and mental health prevention is presented, as well as the need for its implementation in general and family medicine and in the community psychiatry. Self-awareness and awareness of morbidity promotes physical and mental well-being and contributes to the development of non-pathological emotional traits. Classical psychotherapy, as well as meditation, allows the individual to access his internal representations, providing the mental space necessary to work on affections and cognitions. The clinical access to these internal representations is important not only in the treatment of disabling diseases, but also indispensable in primary health care. Conclusion: The physician, by promoting the physical and mental health of the individual, his family and the community, has a predominant role in the adhesion to the assumptions of good clinical practices. The research hypothesis defended by this conceptual analysis presents ground for the debate of a community medicine more focused on inducing policies for mental wellbeing, for self-awareness and for morbidity awareness. These mental health policies in the community are currently considered to be objectives of the general and family medicine health program, namely the person-centered approach, which aims at training and empowering the patient or healthy individual as the prime agent responsible for his own health.
Article
Full-text available
The cross-cultural practice of shamanism reflects an innate capacity for altered states of consciousness (ASC’s) that are elicited by stress, deliberately sought in shamanic rituals, and associated with psychopathology. Shamanic sickness, animal transformation and death-rebirth experiences specifically resemble psychotic experiences. The triggers of shamanic and psychotic experiences are also related, but while psychotic experiences endure and intensify under uncontrollable circumstances, shamanic sickness is ameliorated by ritualized and controlled engagement of ASC. Shamanic vocation, when and where it is culturally accepted, does not lead to progressive deterioration or dysfunction but rather to an increased functional capacity as expert healer, teacher and ritual guide. Shamanic training methods, or other controlled and ritual methods of engaging ASC, may inform strategies for promoting mental well-being.
Preprint
Full-text available
Context A medium, in the context of these experiments, is a person who receives and transmits information from a hypothesised dead person or spiritual being. During the early stages of mediumship development, mediums often experience difficulty in trusting their true perceptions, believing these to be their imagination. It is important to determine if there is any signatory difference in the brain between the two active states of mediumship and imagination. Objective These experiments test the hypothesis that there is a measurable difference in the brainwave activity of mediumship and imagination. Participants The two medium participants were male and female. They were also the experimenters. The female,(F). aged 73, participated as medium in 33 separate sessions. The male,(M), aged 60, participated as medium in 13 separate sessions. The participants were unrelated and lived in different neighbourhoods, but had been practising mediumship development together for five years. The sessions took place in their normal practice environment. Methods There were 3 sets of experiments, each set involving a different form of mediumship. In each experiment there was electroencephalographic continuous recording, with simultaneous audio recording of the medium, during 30 minutes’ mediumship activity, either followed by, or preceded by, 30 minutes’ imagination/fabrication activity. The data collected was monitored for frequency spectral characteristics and analysed for each cerebral hemisphere. The audio recordings were transcribed and analysed for word count. Results Each of the three experiments showed a significant decrease in the ratio of mean delta to gamma waveband amplitudes, for both cerebral hemispheres, in mediumship when compared with imagination. Conclusion Both mediums showed a consistent significant difference in the delta/gamma ratio in mediumship from that shown in imagination. This supports the hypothesis that there is a difference in the brainwave activity in mediumship as demonstrated in these experiments, from that shown in imagination.
Chapter
Full-text available
The last decades of archaeological work in Portugal showed an increase in the research of post-medieval contexts and an innate development of approaches related to the contacts from the overseas expansion and the process of globalisation. However, theoretical concepts such as given by cognitive archaeology express the lack of archaeological evidence for altered states of consciousness in the early modern period that only recently have started its chaîne operátoire. Thus, the present approach seeks to continue the debate on the altered state of consciousness concept within Archaeology redefining methodological support and to suggest how this area can be applied on early modern archaeological sites and assemblages from Portugal, but susceptible to application in other geographical regions.
Book
Full-text available
Provides a cross-cultural and neurobiological assessment of the nature of shamanism.
Book
Full-text available
This book examines shamanism from evolutionary and biological perspectives to identify the origins of shamanic healing in rituals that enhance individual and group function. What does the brain do during "soul journeys"? How do shamans alter consciousness and why is this important for healing? Are shamans different from other kinds of healers? Is there a connection between the rituals performed by chimpanzees and traditional shamanistic practices? All of these questions and many more are answered in Shamanism, Second Edition: A Biopsychosocial Paradigm of Consciousness and Healing. This text contains crosscultural examinations of the nature of shamanism, biological perspectives on alterations of consciousness, mechanisms of shamanistic healing, as well as the evolutionary origins of shamanism. It presents the shamanic paradigm within a biopsychosocial framework for explaining successful human evolution through group rituals. In the final chapter,"the author compares shamanistic rituals with chimpanzee displays to identify homologies that point to the ritual dynamics of our ancient hominid ancestors.
Chapter
The phenomenon of hypnosis provides a rich paradigm for those seeking to understand the processes that underlie consciousness. Understanding hypnosis tells us about a basic human capacity for altered experiences that is often overlooked in contemporary western societies. Throughout the 200 year history of psychology, hypnosis has been a major topic of investigation by some of the leading experimenters and theorists of each generation. Today hypnosis is emerging again as a lively area of research within cognitive (systems level) neuroscience informing basic questions about the structure and biological basis of conscious states. This book describes the latest advances in understanding hypnosis and similar trance states by researchers within the neuroscience of consciousness. It contains many new and exciting contributions from up and coming researchers and provides a lively debate on methodological and theoretical issues central to the development of emerging research paradigms in the neuroscience of conscious states. The book introduces and describes many of the recent new tools that have become available to researchers in this field. Academics, researchers, and clinicians wanting to develop their knowledge of the latest findings, theories and methods in the scientific study of hypnosis and related states of consciousness will find this an up to date guide to this rapidly advancing field.
Chapter
The phenomenon of hypnosis provides a rich paradigm for those seeking to understand the processes that underlie consciousness. Understanding hypnosis tells us about a basic human capacity for altered experiences that is often overlooked in contemporary western societies. Throughout the 200 year history of psychology, hypnosis has been a major topic of investigation by some of the leading experimenters and theorists of each generation. Today hypnosis is emerging again as a lively area of research within cognitive (systems level) neuroscience informing basic questions about the structure and biological basis of conscious states. This book describes the latest advances in understanding hypnosis and similar trance states by researchers within the neuroscience of consciousness. It contains many new and exciting contributions from up and coming researchers and provides a lively debate on methodological and theoretical issues central to the development of emerging research paradigms in the neuroscience of conscious states. The book introduces and describes many of the recent new tools that have become available to researchers in this field. Academics, researchers, and clinicians wanting to develop their knowledge of the latest findings, theories and methods in the scientific study of hypnosis and related states of consciousness will find this an up to date guide to this rapidly advancing field.
Chapter
The phenomenon of hypnosis provides a rich paradigm for those seeking to understand the processes that underlie consciousness. Understanding hypnosis tells us about a basic human capacity for altered experiences that is often overlooked in contemporary western societies. Throughout the 200 year history of psychology, hypnosis has been a major topic of investigation by some of the leading experimenters and theorists of each generation. Today hypnosis is emerging again as a lively area of research within cognitive (systems level) neuroscience informing basic questions about the structure and biological basis of conscious states. This book describes the latest advances in understanding hypnosis and similar trance states by researchers within the neuroscience of consciousness. It contains many new and exciting contributions from up and coming researchers and provides a lively debate on methodological and theoretical issues central to the development of emerging research paradigms in the neuroscience of conscious states. The book introduces and describes many of the recent new tools that have become available to researchers in this field. Academics, researchers, and clinicians wanting to develop their knowledge of the latest findings, theories and methods in the scientific study of hypnosis and related states of consciousness will find this an up to date guide to this rapidly advancing field.
Chapter
Since the time of atomists like Democritus, forerunner of Plato and Aristotle, two modes of scientific explanation have been used to fill the conceptual space between mind and brain, a dualism more grudgingly resistant to resolution than that of energy and matter. One method assumes a world of hidden realities, impenetrable, to be understood by conjecture and test, observations evaluated for their consistency with hypothetical constructs. The other requires an intuitive grasp of the essence, insightful awareness of the thing itself. The first approach defines a unification of mind and brain out of the possible; the second assumes it. Feelings about these orientations still run strong. In a recent book, the philosopher of science Sir Karl Popper expressed irritation with Plato for intermixing these two thought styles without acknowledging the intermixture, concluding that only the conjectural-test approach is valid; the other kind of knowing Popper dismissed as a “will-o-the-wisp” (Popper & Eccles, 1977).
Chapter
That the cerebral hemispheres are requisite for the spontaneous, directed activities of terrestrial vertebrates has been well known since the last century. As Ferrier (1876) noted, if a decerebrated animal “be left to itself, undisturbed by any form of external stimulus, it remains fixed and immovable on the same spot, and unless artificially fed, dies of starvation....” As has since been repeatedly confirmed, the neuraxis below the level of the hemispheres contains the neural apparatus required for posture and locomotion and the integrated performance of bodily actions involved in self-preservation and procreation. Since the cerebral hemispheres are essential for psychological functions, they may appropriately be referred to as the psychencephalon.
Book
Scitation is the online home of leading journals and conference proceedings from AIP Publishing and AIP Member Societies
Article
Meditation—that great and mysterious subject which in the past has always conjured up the image of the solitary Asian ascetic sitting in deep trance—is fast appearing in unexpected places throughout modern American culture. Secretaries are doing it as part of their daily noon yoga classes. Preadolescent teenagers dropped off at the YMCA by their mothers on a Saturday morning are learning it as part of their karate training. Truck drivers and housewives in the Stress Reduction Program at the University of Massachusetts Medical Center are practicing a combination of Hindu yoga and Buddhist insight meditation to control hypertension. Star athletes prepare themselves for a demanding basketball game with centering techniques they learned in Zen. [1] Dhyana is the generic Sanskrit term for meditation, which in the Yoga Sutras refers to both the act of inward contemplation in the broadest sense and more technically to the intermediate state between mere attention to an object (dharana) and complete absorption in it (samadhi). [2] The earliest known reference to such practice on the Indian subcontinent occurs on one of the seals, a figure seated in the lotus posture, found in the ruins of the pre-Aryan civilizations at Harappa and Mohenjodaro which existed prior to 1500 BCE. Most of the orthodox Hindu schools of philosophy derive their meditation techniques from yoga, but superimpose their own theoretical understanding of consciousness onto the results of the practice. [3] Meditation is also referred to as a spiritual practice in China. Chinese forms of meditation have their origins in the early roots of popular Taoism which existed long before the codification of Taoism as a formal philosophy during the seventh century, B.C.. However, there is no concrete evidence to prove that meditation first arose in Hindu culture and then spread elsewhere. Thus, for the time being the original meditative traditions in China and India should be considered as separate and indigenous. To further complicate the issue, analogies between meditative states and trance consciousness suggest that even earlier precursors to the Asian meditative arts can be found in shamanic cultures such as those in Siberia and Africa. [4] As for modern developments, in trying to formulate a definition of meditation, a useful rule of thumb is to consider all meditative techniques to be culturally embedded. This means that any specific technique cannot be understood unless it is considered in the context of some particular spiritual tradition, situated in a specific historical time period, or codified in a specific text according to the philosophy of some particular individual. [5] Thus, to refer to Hindu meditation or Buddhist meditation is not enough, since the cultural traditions from which a particular kind of meditation comes are quite different and even within a single tradition differ in complex ways. The specific name of a school of thought or a teacher or the title of a specific text is often quite important for identifying a particular type of meditation. Vipassana, or insight meditation, for instance, as practiced in the United States is derived from the Theravada tradition of Buddhism, and is usually associated with the teachings of the Burmese monk Mahasi Sayadaw; Transcendental Meditation is associated exclusively with the teachings of Maharishi Mahesh Yogi, whose tradition is Vedantic Hinduism; and so on.