ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Subordinate ant species utilise different tactics to reduce competition with the stronger, larger and more aggressive individuals of a dominant species. In our experimental study, we assessed the behavioural response of individual workers of four subordinate ant species during their co-occurrence with workers of a single dominant species. Contrary to most classical experiments focused on aggressive interactions, we assessed workers' speed as a crucial factor in the outcome of co-occurrence. Generally, there was a large intraspecific variation in the speed of the studied species - each had slow and fast individuals. Workers of all studied species moved faster just after interaction, suggesting that contact between two hostile workers is a stressful stimulus, generating a behavioural reaction of increasing speed. Also, the number of aggressive contacts experienced by a given individual positively affected its speed. Moreover, workers which were fast when exploring territory were also fast after interspecific interactions. The duration of aggression was significantly reduced by the speed and body size of a subordinate species worker - the more quickly a worker reacted and bigger it was, the shorter was the time of cumulative aggression. To our knowledge, this is the first study of this type to be conducted on ants and we conclude that speed is an overlooked and important characteristic of species and also individuals, therefore it should be considered as a driver of patterns of co-occurrence in ant assemblages. This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Insect Science (2017) 24, 842–852, DOI 10.1111/1744-7917.12354
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Changes in the speed of ants as a result of aggressive
interactions
Piotr ´
Slipi´
nski1and Michał ˙
Zmihorski2,3
1Laboratory of Social and Myrmecophilous Insects, Museum and Institute of Zoology, Polish Academy of Sciences, Warsaw, Poland;
2Department of Ecology, Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences, Uppsala, Sweden and 3Institute of Nature Conservation, Polish
Academy of Sciences, Krak´
ow, Poland
Abstract Subordinate ant species utilize different tactics to reduce competition with the
stronger, larger and more aggressive individuals of a dominant species. In our experimental
study, we assessed the behavioral response of individual workers of 4 subordinate ant
species during their co-occurrence with workers of a single dominant species. Contrary to
most classical experiments focused on aggressive interactions, we assessed workers’ speed
as a crucial factor in the outcome of co-occurrence. Generally, there wasa large intraspecif ic
variation in the speed of the studied species—each had slow and fast individuals. Workers
of all studied species moved faster just after interaction, suggesting that contact between
2 hostile workers is a stressful stimulus, generating a behavioral reaction of increasing
speed. Also, the number of aggressive contacts experienced by a given individual positively
affected its speed. Moreover, workers which were fast when exploring territory were also
fast after interspecific interactions. The duration of agg ression was significantly reduced by
the speed and body size of a subordinate species worker—the more quickly a worker reacted
and bigger it was, the shorter was the time of cumulative aggression. To our knowledge,
this is the first study of this type to be conducted on ants and we conclude that speed is an
overlooked and important characteristic of species and also individuals, therefore it should
be considered as a driver of patterns of co-occurrence in ant assemblages.
Key words hierarchy; reaction; response; speed; survival; velocity
Introduction
To dominate is to have priority in accessing the crucial
resources necessary to live and reproduce (Clutton-Brock
& Parker, 1995). The physical supremacy of a dominant
species often leads to establishing a spatial and tempo-
ral order in terms of access to vital niche resources. Di-
verse inter- and intraspecific hierarchies are especially
common among social animals, such as wasps, ants, and
bumblebees (Wilson, 1975; Van Honk & Hogeweg, 1981;
Th´
eraulaz et al., 1992; Parr & Gibb, 2010). Among ants,
Correspondence: Piotr ´
Slipi´
nski, Laboratory of Social and
Myrmecophilous Insects, Museum and Institute of Zoology,
Polish Academy of Sciences, Wilcza 64, 00–679 Warsaw,
Poland. Tel: +48 502 309 722; fax: +48 22 629 63 02; email:
piotrs@miiz.waw.pl
such hierarchical interspecific organization is especially
well studied in Euro-Siberian and North American bo-
real forest, as well as in tropical forests (Savolainen
& Veps¨
al¨
ainen, 1988; Andersen, 1995; Davidson, 1997;
Bluethgen & Fiedler, 2004; Philpot, 2010; Parr & Gibb,
2010; Dejean et al., 2015).
In accordance with the competition hierarchy theory,
red wood ants (RWA e.g., Formi c a r u fa Linnaeus, F.
polyctena F¨
orst) are typical dominants of multispecies
ant assemblages (Savolainen & Veps¨
al¨
ainen, 1988). A
mature colony of red wood ants dominates as a soci-
ety, influencing subordinate species’ colonies inhabiting
a common niche (Punttila et al., 1996; Czechowski et al.,
2013). Single workers of red wood ants also dominate
over workers of subordinate species due to their physical
superiority (Kaspari & Weiser, 1999; Czechowski et al.,
2012). Red wood ants are active and aggressive animals
that fight all kinds of intruders encroaching their territory
C2016 Institute of Zoology, Chinese Academy of Sciences
842
Aggression in ants 843
(Reznikova & Dorosheva, 2004; Domisch et al., 2005;
V¨
a¨
an¨
anen et al., 2010). They affect the distribution and
behavior of subordinate ant species as well as of other in-
vertebrates (Reznikova & Dorosheva, 2004; Sorvari et al.,
2014; ˙
Zmihorski & ´
Slipi´
nski, 2016). Despite this, subor-
dinate species nest and forage in the territories of stronger
competitors (Czechowski & Mark´
o, 2006; Mark´
oet al.,
2013; ´
Slipi´
nski et al., 2014).
Subordinate ant species avoid direct interactions with
dominant species by utilizing different spatiotemporal
fragments of the common niche (Arnan et al., 2012; Cerd´
a
et al., 2013). However, interactions occur anyway, forcing
the smaller and weaker individuals of subordinate species
to flee or fight (Tanner & Adler, 2009). In Euro-Siberian
boreal forests, many species of Myrmicinae and Formici-
nae subfamilies co-occur in space and time with territorial
and aggressive red wood ants (V¨
a¨
an¨
anen et al., 2010;
˙
Zmihorski, 2011). The morphological and behavioral
features of species belonging to the above-mentioned
subfamilies differ significantly, which can potentially
determine their behavior during interactions with stronger
competitors (Tanner & Adler, 2009; Czechowski et al.,
2012). While workers from the Formicinae subfamily
(subgenus Serviformica) are generally fast moving, Myr-
mica workers are rather slow. The latter are protected by a
thick cuticle, which may increase their chances of survival
during antagonistic interactions. It is not clear however
how these differences in behavior (i.e., speed) and the
morphological characteristics of subordinate species af-
fect their interactions with dominants (i.e., red wood ants).
In this paper, we focused on the variation in speed
of 4 ant species’ workers of different size and ecology
during their co-occurrence with the workers of a domi-
nant species. The whole study was inspired by a common
(among many myrmecologists) conviction that workers
of F.fusca can forage in close vicinity to large groups
of red wood ants because they (F.fusca) are “dexterous
and agile” ants. Workers of both species usually inter-
act repeatedly when, for example, they locate a common
food source, yet in most situations, F. f u s c a workers are
able to “outrun” RWA workers just after an aggressive
interaction and escape. There are many similar examples
of relatively fast subordinate species in ant assemblages,
however, not many studies have tried to quantify the speed
of subordinate ant species after interactions as part of a
possible “defense reaction.” Therefore, we aimed to test 3
hypotheses based on current knowledge concerning inter-
specific interactions. First, given the obvious morpholog-
ical differences between subordinate species, we expected
to observe clear interspecific differences in the speed of
subordinate species’ workers after interactions with work-
ers of the dominant species. Second, we assume that an
increase in speed as a response to aggression—triggered
by interactions with workers of dominant species—is uni-
versal for all considered subordinate species, despite ob-
vious morphological and behavioral differences between
them. We assumed that such universal behavioral reac-
tions (i.e., speed increase) may potentially increase the
survival probability of subordinate species’ workers when
they are forced to compete with the larger and stronger
workers of dominant species. Third, we hypothesized that
the speed of subordinate species’ workers, after the in-
teraction with workers of a dominant species affects the
physical contact duration with the workers of the domi-
nant species. The reduction of contact time may be im-
portant for increasing the survival of subordinate species.
To verify the proposed hypotheses, we hosted artificial
colonies and performed a set of laboratory experiments,
video-recording the interactions among the ants.
Materials and methods
Ant species in the experiment
Four subordinate and 1 dominant ant species were used
in the experiments.
Formica polyctena was selected as a typical, territorial
species with large, strong and aggressive individuals. F.
polyctena creates large (up to several million individuals)
colonies, shaping the location, density, and fitness of
subordinate species’ colonies (Punttila et al., 1996). F.
polyctena workers permanently patrol their territory,
excluding subordinate species’ workers, as well as
influencing the size, location and fitness of subordinate
species’ colonies. Individuals of subordinate species are
excluded from food sources, injured and killed during
encounters (Savolainen & Veps¨
al¨
ainen, 1988; Pisarski
& Veps ¨
al¨
ainen, 1989). Because of the aggressive status
of this species and its physical superiority (compared to
the subordinate species in the experiment), we expected
workers from this species to be relatively fast when ex-
ploring territory and also to react quickly after aggressive
interactions for the purpose of attacking the opponent.
F.fusca Linnaeus is a subordinate species living in
F.polyctena territories. Colonies of F.fusca nesting in
F.polyctena territories are small, well concealed and in-
conspicuous (Savolainen, 1990). Shy, but fast and agile
workers forage mostly singly, do not engage in aggres-
sive interactions and avoid contact (De Biseau, 1997). We
expect F.fusca workers to be faster after the interactions
than F.polyctena workers.
A subsequent subordinate species used in the experi-
ment was F.rufibarbis Fabricius. Compared to F.fusca,
its colonies are more numerous and not so well concealed.
C2016 Institute of Zoology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, 24, 842–852
844 P. ´
Slipi´
nski & M. ˙
Zmihorski
Workers are much more predaceous and aggressive com-
pared to F. f u s c a . Because of its aggressiveness, F.ru-
fibarbis nests on the borders of F.polyctena territories
(Savolainen & Veps¨
al¨
ainen, 1988). We expect that work-
ers of F.rufibarbis may behave in a similar manner to the
workers of F.fusca.
Two species were chosen from the Myrmicinae sub-
family: Myrmica ruginodis Nylander and M.scabrinodis
Nylander. Colonies of these species are of various sizes,
up to 2 thousand individuals. Both species may show
limited aggression towards other ants during interactions
(Czechowski, 1979). Both species—as with most of the
Myrmica sp.—are stocky, move rather slowly and are
well protected by a hard cuticle (Czechowski et al., 2012,
p. 73). We hypothesized that these 2 species might
increase their speed as a response to aggression, but it
is rather unlikely that they would be able to outrun F.
polyctena, so alternative strategies could be also expected
for this species.
Colonies in the experiment
Five colonies were used (1 per each species) in the
experiment; they were taken from an area adjacent to
the protected region of the Mazovian Landscape Park
near the city of Warsaw (central Poland 52°1026N,
18°519E). Entire colonies were taken (a partial colony
in the case of F.polyctena) from the field with all
possible nest material and kept in large plastic contain-
ers (200 mm ×200 mm ×330 mm for F.polyctena,
360 mm ×230 mm ×230 mm for F.rufibarbis,
260 mm ×120 mm ×120 mm for F.fusca, and
180 mm ×180 mm ×210 mm for both species of Myr-
mica). The colonies in the field were spatially isolated, so
there was no possibility that workers from 1 colony had
had contact with worker from another colony in field con-
ditions. Such an arrangement excludes the occurrence of
the “dear-enemy phenomenon” (Pfennig & Reeve, 1989).
Each artificial nest was connected by a silicone tube to
a plastic feeding arena of 140 mm ×140 mm ×50 mm
(supplied with dead crickets, honey, and water), where
ants (foragers) were collected for the aggression test. For-
aging workers are usually more experienced comparing
to workers confined to tasks inside the colony, therefore
the aggression tests were performed only with the use of
foraging workers.
Aggression tests
Thirty aggression tests were conducted with F.fusca,
30 with F.rufibarbis, and 30 with Myrmica workers
(21 with M.scabrinodis and9withM.ruginodis); in all
cases the subordinate species interacted with different
F.polyctena individuals (total 90). After the aggression
test, ants were separated from the colony to prevent the
same individual from performing the test twice.
Each test lasted 5 min and 20 sec. Encounters were per-
formed on a Petri dish arena (9 cm in diameter). Before the
test, each Petri dish arena was placed in the container with
F.polyctena (for 5 min), allowing the workers to walk on
it and mark it with their scent. In this way, subsequent ag-
gression tests between dominant and subordinate species
were performed on the “territory” of the former. An F.
polyctena worker was always placed first into the arena—
also to ensure the effect of territorial “ownership”—the
subordinate individual was placed later (after 5–10 sec).
Workers of both species were transported with the use of
a plastic vial aspirator and placed randomly on the arena.
The temperature during the aggression test was stable (23–
24 ºC). All workers taking part in the aggression tests were
measured (from the tip of the head to the end of the ab-
domen) in Photoshop based on photographs made of live
individuals (in 1280 ×720 resolution). As body length
is associated with species identity (e.g., Fo rmic a were in
general larger than Myrmica), body length measurements
were standardized for each species independently. More
specifically, we centered the measurements by subtract-
ing the mean and scaled the measurements by dividing the
centered value by its standard deviation. As a result, the
standardized body length of individuals belonging to each
species had a mean =0 and SD =1, thus providing a mea-
sure for larger and smaller individuals of a given species
independent of between-species differences. These stan-
dardized values of body length were further used in the
statistical analyses.
Each 5-min aggression test was video recorded (Mi-
crosoft LifeCam Studio) in high resolution (1280 ×720,
15 frames per sec). Each recorded film was later broad-
casted in slow motion mode using BehaView software
(Boguszewski, 2011) in order to calculate: general speed
(dependent variable in model 1), mean postcontact speed
(model 2) and aggressive contact duration (model 3). Gen-
erally, the speed of workers was expressed as the distance
in centimeters travelled by an ant during 2 sec. The dis-
tance travelled by an ant was assessed manually by vi-
sually tracking the ant on a 1 cm ×1cmgrid(visible
through the transparent base of the Petri dish) and ex-
pressed as the total number of the grid cells that the ge-
ometrical center of the ant’s body passed during 2 sec.
We measured workers’ speed in 2 different situations: (1)
when they were exploring territory (so called exploration
speed) and (2) just after any kind of physical interaction
between workers (postcontact speed; see description of
C2016 Institute of Zoology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, 24, 842–852
Aggression in ants 845
Tab le 1 Set of explanatory variables used in the modeling.
Abbreviation Description
ANTSPEC Ant species. Categorical variable with 5 (in the case of model 1) or 4 (model 2) levels. Expectation:
different species may behave differently.
SPEEDCONTEXT Speed context. Categorical variable with 2 levels: exploration or postcontact. Used only in model 1,
where both types of speed were considered. Expectation: movement speed may differ between
exploration and after contact with F.polyctena.
Exploration speed: speed of a worker measured every 20 sec (each time for 2 sec). Represents the speed
of a subordinate species worker while “exploring” F.polyctena territory and is, at present, devoid of
any interspecific interactions. F.polyctena exploration speed is the speed of the dominant species
“exploring” its own territory. If there was any interaction between workers, the measurement of
exploration speed was omitted and measured again 20 sec later.
Postcontact speed: Speed of a worker (subordinate and dominant species) measured just after any type
of physical contact (interaction) between the 2 individuals. Speed was measured each time for 2 sec,
separately for the subordinate species’ workers and for F.polyctena.
TIMEINTERV Time interval. Continuous variable. Refers to the time intervals (20 sec) in which a given measurement
was taken, from 1 (beginning of aggression test) to 16 (end). Expectation: ants may change their
movement speed over time.
AGGRCONT Aggressive contacts. Continuous variable. Cumulative number of aggressive contacts between the 2
ants participating in a given aggression test. Each aggressive contact started with visible aggression
between the ants and ended when the ants became spatially separated, the number of such aggression
contacts was summarized for each individual ant. Expectation: number of former contacts, that is,
experience of a given individual, may change its behavior.
EXPLSPEEDRWA Exploration speed of Red Wood Ant. Continuous variable. Mean exploration speed of an F.polyctena
individual participating in a given aggression test. Expectation: exploration speed of RWA may force
workers of a subordinate species to behave in a different way (e.g., move slower/faster).
POSTCONTSPEEDRWA Postcontact speed of Red Wood Ant. Continuous variable. Mean postcontact speed of a F.polyctena
individual participating in a given aggression test. Expectation: postcontact speed of F. polyctena
may force workers of a subordinate species to behave in a different way (e.g., move slower/faster).
EXPLSPEED Exploration speed. Continuous variable. Mean exploration speed of a subordinate species’ individual
participating in a given aggression test. Expectation: specific exploration speed for a given individual
of a subordinate species may somehow affect its postcontact speed, contact duration, etc.
POSTCONTSPEED Postcontact speed. Continuous variable. Mean postcontact speed of a subordinate species’ individual
participating in a given aggression test. Expectation: specific postcontact speed for a given individual
of a subordinate species may somehow affect its exploration speed, contact duration, etc.
BODYLENGTHRWA Body length of Red Wood Ant worker. Continuous variable. F. polyctena body length.
BODYLENGTHSUB Body length of subordinate worker. Continuous variable. F.fusca,F.rufibarbis,M.ruginodis,andM.
scabrinodis body length.
variables used in the modeling in Table 1). The 2-sec
period used during preliminary observations showed that
the behavior (i.e., speed) of subordinate species’ individ-
uals after contact with F.polyctena distinctly changed
during the following 2 sec.
Statistical analysis
Speed differences between individuals We investi-
gated the differences in speed between individuals within
a given species. For this purpose, we used the Kruskal–
Wallis test implemented independently for each species
and speed context (exploration and postcontact).
Drivers of general speed (model 1) The f irst analy-
sis was performed to verify the effect of the 4 selected
factors on the average speed of a worker (both explo-
ration and postcontact speed) of all studied species. We
considered the 4 explanatory variables: species of ant
(ANTSPEC), time elapsed since the beginning of the exper-
iment (TIMEINTERV), number of aggressive contacts that
C2016 Institute of Zoology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, 24, 842–852
846 P. ´
Slipi´
nski & M. ˙
Zmihorski
took place between the 2 individuals (AGGRCONT), and
type of a given speed measure (SPEEDCONTEXT: postcon-
tact vs. exploration). Moreover, we considered the inter-
actions between ANTSPEC and the 3 explanatory variables
(TIMEINTERV,AGGRCONT,SPEEDCONTEXT), assuming that
the effect of these 3 variables can be different for a par-
ticular species of ant. The explanatory variables showed
no strong collinearity (r<0.42 in all cases). We used a
general linear mixed model (GLMM) with Gaussian error
distribution and identity link on all available measures of
exploration and postcontact speed for 172 individuals of
all 5 species (n=2241, i.e., single speed measurement
was a unit in this model). As we usually had more than 1
measurement for a given individual, we used the ID of the
ant individual within a given species as a random categor-
ical factor. We used the lme4 package (Bates et al., 2014)
implemented in R environment (R Core Team, 2015).
Drivers of the mean postcontact speed (model 2)
This analysis was performed to examine which factors
influence the speed of subordinate species’ workers after
contact with the dominant F.polyctena workers. We in-
vestigated the variability of the individual-specific mean
postcontact speed (i.e., average values from all measure-
ments taken among 73 subordinate and 73 dominant in-
dividuals) for all individuals of 4 subordinate species.
We considered the 6 explanatory variables: ant species
(ANTSPEC), mean exploration and mean postcontact speed
of F. polyctena in a given aggression test (EXPLSPEEDRWA
and POSTCONTSPEEDRWA, respectively), mean explo-
ration speed of a given ant (EXPLSPEED) and standard-
ized body length of F.polyctena and subordinate species’
workers (n=146) (BODYLENGTHRWA and BODYLENGTH-
SUB, respectively). The explanatory variables showed no
strong collinearity (r<0.44 in all cases). We used a gen-
eral linear model (GLM) implemented in R with Gaus-
sian error distribution and identity link for the investi-
gation of the effects of ANTSPEC as well as the effects
of the 3 explanatory variables (EXPLSPEEDRWA , P OST-
CONTSPEEDRWA , EXPLSPEED), and all the interactions be-
tween these variables and ANTSPEC.
Drivers of the aggressive contact duration (model 3)
The last analysis was performed for the purpose of ex-
amining how the speed of subordinate species workers
and their body length influence the duration of aggres-
sive contacts with workers of the dominant species. We
investigated the pooled duration time in seconds of all
aggressive contacts between 2 ants (n=73 individuals of
subordinate species and 73 of F.polyctena) occurring in a
given aggression test in relation to 6 potential predictors:
mean exploration and postcontact speed of F. polyctena in
a given test (EXPLSPEEDRWA and POSTCONTSPEEDRWA ,
respectively), mean exploration and postcontact speed of a
given subordinate ant (EXPLSPEED and POSTCONTSPEED,
respectively), standardized body length of F.polyctena
and subordinate species workers (BODYLENGTHRWA and
BODYLENGTHSUB, respectively) as well as interactions
between ANTSPEC and the 4 explanatory variables (EX-
PLSPEEDRWA , P OSTCONTSPEEDRWA , E XPLSPEED,POST-
CONTSPEED). The explanatory variables showed no strong
collinearity (r<0.55 in all cases). We used a general
linear model (GLM) with Gaussian error distribution and
identity link implemented in R (R Core Team, 2015).
Model selection We used an information-theoretic ap-
proach to infer the importance of particular predictors
for the variability of the observed speed and aggressive
contact duration. For this purpose, we used Akaike’s infor-
mation criterion (AIC). For each model (1–3), all possible
models were compared based on AIC scores. Model av-
eraging was conducted within AIC 4 (Burnham &
Anderson, 2002) with the help of the MuMIn package
(Barto´
n, 2013) in R (R Core Team, 2015) and presented
in Table S1. For each model, we used averaged parameter
estimates to infer the biological importance of the vari-
ables for the observed ant behavior. Averaged parameter
estimates with 95% confidence intervals not overlapping
zero were considered “significant” and important for ex-
plaining the dependent variables.
Results
Speed differences between individuals We recorded
significant differences in the mean exploration speed be-
tween individuals belonging to the same species. The dif-
ferences were recorded for M.ruginodis,M.scabrinodis
and F.polyctena, showing that speed variance between
individuals was greater than the variance within individu-
als. There were no such differences in the case of For m i ca
fusca, and F. rufibarbis (the latter on the verge of signif-
icance, see Fig. S1). A similar pattern was observed for
postcontact speed, but in this case, the differences between
individuals were significant for all species (Fig. S1).
General speed (model 1) In general, ants were mov-
ing faster after the contact than during exploration (ef-
fect of SPEEDCONTEXT, Table 2, Fig. 1). However, the
change of speed after the contact differed between the
studied species (interaction between ANTSPEC and SPEED-
CONTEXT). Moreover, the number of aggressive contacts
between 2 individuals positively affected the speed of both
ants (effect of AGGRCONT, Fig 2A). But the speed of both
ants showed a decreasing trend over time (i.e., during a
C2016 Institute of Zoology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, 24, 842–852
Aggression in ants 847
Tab le 2 Averaged parameter estimates explaining the speed of ants (models 1–2) and aggressive contact duration between ants (model
3) in response to experimental conditions.
Response variable Predictor Estimate 95% CI
Model 1 TIMEINTERV –0.040 –0.056; –0.025
General speed AGGRCONT 0.074 0.040; 0.1087
SPEEDCONTEXT =EXPLORATION SPEED –0.919 –1.072; –0.766
ANTSPEC =F.rufibarbis 0.676 0.375; 0.977
=F.fusca 0.241 0.030; 0.513
=M.ruginodis 0.098 0.527; 0.329
=M.scabrinodis –1.549 –1.868; –1.230
ANTSPEC =F.rufibarbis:SPEEDCONTEXT –0.570 –0.913; –0.228
=F.fusca:S
PEEDCONTEXT –0.366 –0.686; –0.046
=M.ruginodis:S
PEEDCONTEXT 0.035 0.401; 0.471
=M.scabrinodis:S
PEEDCONTEXT 0.910 0.600; 1.221
Model 2 ExplSpeed 0.209 0.045; 0.372
Mean postcontact PostContSpeedRWA –0.183 –0.313; –0.053
speed ExplSpeedRWA 0.131 0.063; 0.327
BodyLengthRWA 0.448 2.041; 2.938
BodyLengthSub 0.154 0.349; 0.040
ANTSPEC =M.ruginodis 0.422 1.076; 0.232
=M.scabrinodis –1.768 –2.287; –1.248
=F.rufibarbis 0.409 0.108; 0.928
Model 3 POSTCONTSPEED –0.135 –0.229; –0.042
Aggressive contact EXPLSPEED 0.040 0.229; –0.042
duration POSTCONTSPEEDRWA 0.031 0.599; 5.227
EXPLSPEEDRWA 0.101 0.319; 0.115
BodyLengthRWA 2.314 0.599; 5.227
BodyLengthSub –0.323 –0.561; –0.085
Note: Averaging was conducted on the basis of ωAIC. Estimates that do not overlap zero are typed in bold. In the explanatory variable
ANTSPEC,theFormica polyctena species was used as the reference level in model 1, For m ica f u s ca in model 2, whereas in SPEEDCONTEXT
level, POSTCONTACT was used as the reference level and parameter estimates for these levels are not provided, as it was denoted as 0.
5-min period, the effect of TIMEINTERV). The speed of ants
differed significantly among species. M. scabrinodis was
the slowest species, whereas F.rufibarbis was the fastest,
F.fusca was also very fast.
Mean postcontact speed (model 2) The mean post-
contact speed of an ant was positively correlated with
the exploration speed of this individual, which means
that workers exploring with high speed are also fast af-
ter the interaction with a worker of the dominant species.
The mean postcontact speed of a subordinate ant was
negatively affected by the postcontact speed of the F.
polyctena individual in a given aggression test (POSTCON-
TSPEEDRWA, Fig. 2B). Moreover, we recorded differences
among the subordinate species—M. scabrinodis was the
slowest after the contact whereas M. rufibarbis was the
fastest (Table 2).
Aggressive contact duration (model 3) The duration
of all aggressive contacts between 2 ants in a given ag-
gression test was negatively influenced by the postcontact
speed (POSTCONTSPEED) of the subordinate individual and
its size (BODYLENGTHSUB)—the slower and smaller the
subordinate worker, the longer the total time of aggres-
sion experienced by that worker (Figs. 2C and D).
Discussion
The experiments revealed that the speed of subordinate
ants differs distinctly, not only between species but also
between individuals of a given species. Each species has
slow and fast individuals, but workers of the majority
of species move faster after a contact than during explo-
ration. We recorded that the postcontact speed of subordi-
nates was affected negatively by the speed of F.polyctena,
C2016 Institute of Zoology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, 24, 842–852
848 P. ´
Slipi´
nski & M. ˙
Zmihorski
Fig. 1 Exploration and postcontact speed of the 5 ant species
expressed with kernel density estimation (irregular polygons)
and boxplots (white dot, median; black box, interquartile range).
but positively by aggression between individuals. More-
over, the slower the ant, the longer was the aggression
time. Subordinate workers of F.rufibarbis and F.fusca
are in general faster compared to the dominant workers
of F.polyctena, while Myrmica workers are slower. The
whole study was based on the use of only 1 nest for each
study species, so more replicates are needed to confirm
the patterns; however, the differences between species
were large and most likely robust in spite of the lack
of replication. Moreover, using different time window for
the postaggression speed measurements (in our study: 2
sec) may affect the obtained results as ants change behav-
ior continuously over time that elapsed since aggression.
This should be kept in mind when comparing our results
to other investigations in the future. Clear methodological
recommendations in this issue are lacking which could be
the reason for why so few studies examine speed of ants.
However, basing on the preliminary observations we as-
sessed that 2-sec period covers the visible ant behavioral
response to aggression and thus we recommend using sim-
ilar time window for the future studies. Below we discuss
the role of workers’ speed as a driver of patterns of ant
co-occurrence.
Speed as an individual response
Each species has individuals exploring at limited
speeds, which we may generally term “slow” (see Fig.
S1). On the other hand, each species has individuals ex-
ploring at a high speed, which can be called “fast” (up
to ca. 9 cm/sec in the case of Formicinae workers). A
similar pattern is observed for postcontact speed variabil-
ity within each studied species (Fig. S1). The observation
that each species has slow and fast individuals is also con-
firmed by model 2: we recorded that subordinate workers
which are fast during exploration are also fast after di-
rect contact with dominant workers (Table 2, model 2).
This suggests that the speed of a given individual is—
at least to some extent—repeatable in different situations
(exploration and postcontact). The existence of, for ex-
ample, bold and shy explorers has been well recognized,
mainly in birds (Drent et al., 2002). Our studies, however,
were not devoted to addressing the issue of differences
between individuals in intrinsic motivation, willingness
to explore and physiological state (Trullas & Skolnick,
1993; Schowalter, 2011; Hui & Pinter-Wollman, 2014;
Kraij-Fiˇ
ser & Schuett, 2014), thus leaving questions con-
cerning personality open.
Speed as result of co-occurrence
The analysis revealed significant differences in speed
between species, with workers of both Myrmica species
being the slowest,subordinate Formicinae (F.fusca and F.
rufibarbis) the fastest, whereas the workers of territorial F.
polyctena were moving at moderate speed (Fig. 1). Inter-
estingly, speed after contact was negatively related to the
mean postcontact speed of F.polyctena—the faster the F.
polyctena workers, the slower the workers of subordinate
species (Fig. 2B). In the laboratory conditions of a closed
Petri dish, workers usually detect the presence of other
workers at a distance of about 1–2 cm. This observation
is consistent with the literature, for example, Camponotus
sp. recognize a nest-mate from a distance of 1 cm (Brand-
staetter et al., 2008). At this distance, workers often stop
or slow down, probably to compare the hydrocarbon pro-
file with their own profile (Howard & Blomquist, 2005).
After such a comparison, F.polyctena workers usually
decide to attack. This strategy is utilized by most of the
behaviourally dominant species, most probably because
initiating a fight can increase the likelihood of winning a
contest (Mcauley et al., 1998; Tsutsui et al., 2003; Tan-
ner & Adler, 2009). The seconds following a contact are
crucial—if the subordinate worker decides to flee—then
reaction time and speed within the first few seconds of the
interaction are vital in terms of survival (De la Mora et al.,
2008). An attacking individual needs time during physical
contact to find a suitable place to bite the opponent. Work-
ers of a similar size usually cannot directly grab the other
ant on the head, thorax or abdomen and they usually try
to grasp the opponent on the leg or antennae (Dietrich &
Wehner, 2003). Also, our results (model 3) specified that
C2016 Institute of Zoology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, 24, 842–852
Aggression in ants 849
Fig. 2 Visualization of the relationship between speed and aggressive contact duration of ants and the most important explanatory
variables used in models 1–3 (see Table 2). Dots are partial residuals obtained by dropping the given explanatory variable from the
model while including all other variables. Lines are given to help with the visual interpretation of the relationships while significance
of the relationships and parameter estimates are given in Table 2.
the duration of all aggressive contacts occurring between
2 ants was negatively influenced by the individual size
of the subordinate—the smaller the subordinate worker,
the longer the total time of aggression. Contact between 2
hostile workers is a stressful stimulus, generating a behav-
ioral reaction of increasing speed by both dominant and
subordinate workers. This is unexpected, as most studies
of behavioral stress measured the stress effect in subor-
dinate species or individuals, only a few suggested that
the dominant species is also significantly stressed (Creel
et al., 1996; Kotrschal et al., 1998; Summers, 2002).
Speed as a response to aggression
The nature of the interactions among competing ant
workers from different species may create a series of
stressful situations, including aggression (Cerd´
a, 2001;
Summers, 2002; Czechowski & Mark´
o, 2006). There-
fore, during the analysis, we also considered the effect
of the number of aggressive contacts and time elapsed
from the beginning of the experiment (i.e., TIMEINTERV in
Table 1). Both factors significantly influenced ant speed
(model 1). An increased number of aggressive contacts
resulted in increased speed, which may indicate that a
worker already engaged in an aggressive interaction is
more stressed and disturbed after each subsequent con-
tact (Fig. 2A). On the other hand, the speed of observed
individuals decreased over time, which may be related to
increasing tiredness of co-occurring ants or other factors.
Strong competitive interactions between ecologically
similar species lead to asymmetric niche compression
with the most suitable localities occupied by the domi-
nant species. This rule can be observed, for example, in
guilds of avian predators, where the subordinate species
shifts its habitat use to avoid contact with the domi-
nant species (Vrezec & Tome, 2004). Similarly, strong
intraguild competition was noted for many epigeic inver-
tebrates, for example, survival of 1 species of spiders was
affected by the density of another species (Denno et al.,
2004). One may assume that in the case of ants, subor-
dinate species try to shorten the duration of aggressive
contacts with F. polyctena as much as possible, as fight-
ing with a stronger competitor decreases their chances
for survival. Model 3 showed clearly that the duration of
aggression was reduced by the speed of a subordinate in-
dividual after contact (Fig. 2C). In other words, the more
C2016 Institute of Zoology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, 24, 842–852
850 P. ´
Slipi´
nski & M. ˙
Zmihorski
quickly a worker reacts and flees after contact, the shorter
the cumulative time of aggression. A similar behavior was
observed in Carabidae beetles exploring the territory of F.
polyctena (Reznikova & Dorosheva, 2004). This experi-
ment showed that different Carabids use sets of behavioral
tactics to avoid aggression with ants in ant-dominated ter-
ritory. The largest individuals, such as Carabus regalis,
increased their speed of movement when sighting ants.
Another species, Pterostichus oblongopunctatus, actively
maneuvered among ants, while individuals of P.magus
preferred to stop and wait until the ants passed by, which
may suggest that this species uses a different strategy
in contacts with F. Polyctena (Reznikova & Dorosheva,
2004). A similar strategy may be used by Myrmica
which was the slowest species in our experiment. Sub-
ordinate workers of Myrmica rugulosa usually “freeze”
during an interaction or in some cases, bend their legs
inward under the body (the so called subordinate pose or
transport pose) and are passively transported, which may
potentially reduce the aggression (Czechowski, 1979).
Conclusions
Considering the 3 hypotheses declared in the introduction,
we confirmed clear between-species differences in terms
of speed, reflecting the differences in the various species’
morphology and behavior. However, at the same time, we
observed differences in the speed of subordinate species’
workers after contact with the workers of the dominant
species. Contrary to our predictions, M. scabrinodis does
not increase its speed after the contact, showing that
particular subordinate species may use different tactics to
avoid interaction with a worker of the dominant species.
We also recorded that fast individuals have shorter
cumulative aggression time with dominant workers of red
wood ants. We find this result to be especially important,
as it directly shows that a worker’s speed ability can
benefit it directly, and thus should be considered in future
studies as an important adaptation to co-occurrence with
dominant species.
Acknowledgments
We are grateful for the helpful comments of 3 anonymous
referees, which greatly improved the quality of this article.
We are also indebted to Barbara Przybylska for linguis-
tic revision. This research was financed by a grant from
Iuventus Plus (IP2011 064771) funded by the Ministry
of Science and Higher Education and also by an Internal
Grant for Young Scientists from the Museum and Institute
of Zoology, Polish Academy of Sciences.
Disclosure
All authors declare no conflict of interest.
References
Andersen, A.N. (1995) A classification of Australian ant com-
munities, based on functional groups which parallel plant
life-forms in relation to stress and disturbance. Journal of
Biogeography, 22, 15–29.
Arnan, X., Cerd´
a, X. and Retana, J. (2012) Distinctive life traits
and distribution along environmental gradients of dominant
and subordinate Mediterranean ant species. Oecologia, 170,
489–500.
Barto´
n, K. (2013) MuMIn: multi-model inference. RPackage
Version 1.9.13. http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=MuMIn
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B. and Walker,S. (2014) _lme4:
Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4_. RPackage
Version 1.1. http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4>7
Bluethgen, N. and Fiedler, K. (2004) Competition for composi-
tion: Lessons from nectar-feeding ant communities. Ecology,
85, 1479–1485.
Boguszewski, P. (2011) BehaView 0.0.11c. http://www.
pmbogusz.net/software/behaview/
Brandstaetter, A.S., Endler, A. and Kleineidam, C.J. (2008)
Nestmate recognition in ants is possible without tactile in-
teraction. Naturwissenschaften, 95, 601–608.
Burnham, K.P. and Anderson, D.R. (2002) Model Selection and
Multimodel Inference: A Practical Information–Theoretic Ap-
proach. 2nd edn. Springer, New York.
Cerd´
a, X. (2001) Behavioural and physiological trails to thermal
stress tolerance in two Spanish desert ants. Etologia, 9, 15–27.
Cerd´
a, X., Arnan, X. and Retana, J. (2013) Is competition a sig-
nificant hallmark of ant (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) ecology?
Myrmecological News, 18, 131–147.
Clutton-Brock, T.H. and Parker, G.A. (1995) Punishment in
animal societies. Nature, 373, 209–216.
Creel, S., Creel, N.M. and Monfort, S.L. (1996) Social stress
and dominance. Nature, 379, 212.
Czechowski, W. (1979) Competition between Lasius niger (L.)
and Myrmica rugulosa Nyl. (Hymenoptera, Formicidae). An-
nales Zoologici, 34, 437–451.
Czechowski, W. and Mark´
o, B. (2006) Uncomfortable protec-
tion: Formica polyctena F¨
orst. shelters Form i c a fus c a L. from
Formica sanguinea Latr. (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). An-
nales Zoologici, 56, 539–548.
Czechowski, W., Radchenko, A., Czechowska, W. and
Ve ps ¨
al¨
ainen K. (2012) The Ants of Poland with the
Reference to the Myrmecofauna of Europe, 496 pp. Fauna
Poloniae, Warsaw.
Czechowski, W., Mark´
o, B., Radchenko, A. and ´
Slipi´
nski,
P. (2013) Long-term partitioning of space between two
C2016 Institute of Zoology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, 24, 842–852
Aggression in ants 851
territorial species of ants (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) and
their effect on subordinate species. European Journal of En-
tomology, 110, 327–337.
Davidson, D.W. (1997) The role of resource imbalances in the
evolutionary ecology of tropical arboreal ants. Biological
Journal of the Linnean Society, 61, 153–181.
De Biseau, J.C., Quinet, Y., Deffernez, L. and Pasteels, J.M.
(1997) Explosive food recruitment as a competitive strategy
in the ant Myrmica sabuleti (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). In-
sectes Sociaux, 44, 59–73.
Dejean, A., Az´
emar, F., C ´
er´
eghino, R., Leponce, M., Corbara,
B., Orivel, J. and Compin, A. (2015) The dynamics of ant
mosaics in tropical rainforests characterized using the self-
organizing map algorithm. Insect Science. doi: 10.1111/1744-
7917.12208.
De la Mora, A., P´
erez-Lachaud, G. and Lachaud, J.P.
(2008) Mandible strike: the lethal weapon of Odontomachus
opaciventris against small prey. Behavioural Processes, 78,
64–75.
Denno, R.F., Mitter, M.S., Langellotto, G.A., Gratton, C. and
Finke, D.L. (2004) Interactions between a hunting spider and
a web-builder: consequences of intraguild predation and can-
nibalism for prey suppression. Ecological Entomology, 29,
566–577.
Dietrich, B. and Wehner, R. (2003) Sympatry and allopatry in
two desert ant sister species: how do Cataglyphis bicolor and
C.savignyi coexist? Oecologia, 136, 63–72.
Domisch, T., Fin´
er, L. and Jurgensen, M.F. (2005) Red wood ant
mound densities in managed boreal forests. Annales Zoologici
Fennici, 42, 277–282.
Drent, P.J., van Oers, K. and van Noordwijk, A.J. (2002) Re-
alized heritability of personalities in the great tit (Paru s ma -
jor). Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 270,
45–51.
Howard, R.W. and Blomquist, G.J. (2005) Ecological, behav-
ioral, and biochemical aspects of insect hydrocarbons. Annual
Review of Entomology, 50, 371–393.
Hui, A. and Pinter-Wollman, N. (2014) Individual variation in
exploratory behaviour improves speed and accuracy of col-
lective nest selection by Argentine ants. Animal Behaviour,
93, 261–266.
Kaspari, M. and Weiser, M.D. (1999) The size-grain hypothesis
and interspecific scaling in ants. Functional Ecology, 13, 530–
538.
Kotrschal, K., Hirschenhauser, K. and M¨
ostl, E. (1998) The
relationship between social stress and dominance is seasonal
in greylag geese. Animal Behaviour, 55, 171–176.
Kraij-Fiˇ
ser, S. and Schuett, W. (2014) Studying personality vari-
ation in invertebrates: why bother? Animal Behaviour, 91,
41–52.
Mcauley, D.G., Clugston, D.A. and Longcore, J.R. (1998) Out-
come of aggressive interactions between American black
ducks and mallards during the breeding season. Journal of
Wildlife Management, 62, 134–141.
Mark´
o, B., Czechowski, W. and Radchenko, A. (2013) Com-
bining competition with predation: drastic effect of Lasius
fuliginosus (Latr.) on subordinate ant species at the north-
ern limit of its distribution. Annales Zoologici, 63, 107–
111.
Parr, C.L. and Gibb, H. (2010) Competition and the role of dom-
inant ants. Ant Ecology (eds. L. Lach, C. Parr & K. Abbot), pp.
77–96. Oxford University Press, Oxford, United Kingdom.
Pfennig, D.W. and Reeve, H.K. (1989) Neighbour recognition
and context-dependent aggression in a solitary wasp, Sphecius
speciosus (Hymenoptera: Sphecidae). Ethology, 80, 1–18.
Philpott, S.M. (2010) A canopy dominant ant affects twig-
nesting ant assembly in coffee agroecosystems. Oikos, 119,
1954–1960.
Pisarski, B. and Veps¨
al¨
ainen, K. (1989) Competitive hierarchy
in ant communities (Hymenoptera, Formicidae). Annales Zo-
ologici, 42, 321–329.
Punttila, P., Haila, Y. and Tukia, H. (1996) Ant communities
in taiga clearcuts: habitat effects and species interactions.
Ecography, 19, 16–28.
R Core Team (2015) R: A Language and Environment for Sta-
tistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing,
Vienna, Austria. http://www.R-project.org/.
Reznikova, Z. and Dorosheva, H. (2004) Impacts of red wood
ants Formica polyctena on the spatial distribution and be-
havioural patterns of ground beetles (Carabidae). Pedobiolo-
gia, 48, 15–21.
Savolainen, R. and Veps¨
al¨
ainen, K. (1988) A competition hier-
archy among boreal ants: impact on resource partitioning and
community structure. Oikos, 51, 135–155.
Savolainen, R. (1990) Colony success of the submissive ant
Form i c a fus c a within territories of the dominant Formica
polyctena.Ecological Entomology, 15, 79–85.
Schowalter, T.D. (2011) Insect Ecology: An Ecosystem Ap-
proach, 633 pp. Academic Press, San Diego.
Sorvari, J., Huhta, E. and Hakkarainen, H. (2014) Survival
of transplanted nests of the red wood ant Formica aquilo-
nia (Hymenoptera: Formicidae): The effects of intraspecific
competition and forest clear-cutting. Insect Science, 21, 486–
492.
Summers, C.H. (2002) Social interaction over time, implica-
tions for stress responsiveness. Integrative and Comparative
Biology, 42, 591–599.
´
Slipi´
nski, P., Czechowski, W., Mark ´
o, B., Rzeszowski, K.
and Babik H. (2014) Lasius fuliginosus (Hymenoptera:
Formicidae) shapes local ant assemblages drastically. North-
Western Journal of Zoology, 10, 404–412.
Tanner, C.J. and Adler, F.R. (2009) To fight or not to fight:
context-dependent interspecific aggression in competing ants.
Animal Behaviour, 77, 297–305.
C2016 Institute of Zoology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, 24, 842–852
852 P. ´
Slipi´
nski & M. ˙
Zmihorski
Th´
eraulaz, G., Gervet, J., Thon, B., Pratte, M. and Semenoff-
Tian-Chansky, S. (1992) The dynamics of colonyorganization
in the primitively eusocial wasp Polistes dominulus (Christ).
Ethology, 91, 177–202.
Trullas, R. and Skolnick, P. (1993) Differences in fear motivated
behaviors among inbred mouse strains. Psychopharmacology,
111, 323–331.
Tsutsui, N.D., Suarez, A.V. and Grosberg, R.K. (2003) Ge-
netic diversity, asymmetrical aggression, and recognition in
a widespread invasive species. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100,
1078–1083.
Van Honk, C. and Hogeweg, P. (1981) The ontogeny of the social
structure in a captive Bombus terrestris colony. Behavioral
Ecology and Sociobiology, 9, 111–119.
V¨
a¨
an¨
anen, S., Veps¨
al¨
ainen, K. and Savolainen, R. (2010) In-
direct effect in boreal ant assemblages: territorial wood ants
protect potential slaves against enslaving ants. Annales Zoo-
logici, 60, 57–67.
Vrezec, A. and Tome, D. (2004) Habitat selection and patterns
of distribution in a hierarchic forest owl guild. Ornis Fennica,
81, 109–118.
Wilson, E.O. (1975) Sociobiology: The New Synthesis, 697 pp.
Harvard University Press, Cambridge.
˙
Zmihorski, M. (2011) Does the decline of red wood ants after
clear-cutting favour epigeic arthropods? European Journal of
Entomology, 108, 425–430.
˙
Zmihorski, M. and ´
Slipi´
nski, P. (2016) The importance of diurnal
and nocturnal activity and interspecific interactions for space
use by ants in clear-cuts. Ecological Entomology, 41, 276–
283.
Revised February 21, 2016 Accepted March 3, 2016
Supporting Information
Additional Supporting Information may be found in the
online version of this article at the publisher’s web-site:
Tabl e S1 Three sets of competing models included in
model averaging based on delta AICc.
Fig. S1 Mean exploration and postcontact speed of
ant individuals in increasing order (with 95% confidence
intervals) and results of Kruskal–Wallis test of the dif-
ferences in mean speed between individuals of a given
species.
C2016 Institute of Zoology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, 24, 842–852
... Callows and mature individuals differ strongly in locomotion. Young workers primarily engage in activities within the nest, while mature workers have to efficiently locate and exploit food resources, as well as evade predation risks (Fewell 1988, Hurlbert et al. 2008, Ślipiński and Żmihorski 2017. Previous studies have demonstrated an increase in locomotory performance during individual development (Trillmich et al. 2003, Kirkton and Harrison 2006, Bernadou and Heinze 2013. ...
Article
Full-text available
In many species of eusocial Hymenoptera, workers exhibit a division of labour based on age, wherein young workers remain protected within the nest before transitioning to foraging activities. These changes in tasks and in external stress exposure are accompanied by physiological and anatomical modifications. Although pigmentation changes in the exoskeleton are well documented in ants, limited information is available on muscle morphology and developmental changes in the adult cuticle. Here, we investigated the alterations in the exoskeleton and thoracic muscles across different age groups of the clonal ant Platythyrea punctata, in which reproduction is monopolized by one or a few dominant workers. Our findings revealed a significant thickening of the cuticle and muscle fibres in young workers during the intranidal period, reaching a final stage in forager individuals. We further tested the influence of reproductive status on body development and found variations only in muscle fibres between reproductive and nonreproductive individuals. This suggests that dominant individuals allocate resources in egg-laying by minimizing other metabolic costs. These findings shed light on how eusociality profoundly alters selection pressures and results in temporal shifts in individual development. This strategy probably provides advantages to colonies by minimizing nutritional waste and optimizing resource allocation.
... A behavioral adaptation to moving fairly quickly and more erratically outside of the nest has the advantage of preventing exposure to both the elements and predation. Conversely, the very aggressive fire ant had the lowest trailing velocity, which may be related to its high degree of aggressivity [15]. ...
Article
Full-text available
A comparison of trail-following movement parameters of six major urban pest ants, Nylanderia fulva (Forel) (Hymenoptera: Formicidae), Pheidole megacephala, Linepithema humile (Mayr), Solenopsis invicta Buren, Paratrechina longicornis (Forel), and Technomyrmex albipes (Smith) demonstrated several differences in velocity of movement, amplitude of the deviations from a marked trail, percent fidelity to the trail, number of curves per unit of trail, and trail-following accuracy. Paratrechina longicornis and N. fulva had the largest deviations from the marked trails and moved three times faster (25–30 mm/s) along the trail than the slowest ant, S. invicta (< 10 mm/s), with other ants following between these extremes. Species differences in relation to going toward or returning from food were observed in a few cases, especially with Pa. longicornis for which velocity, amplitude, and trail fidelity differed between the foraging and return trails. Quantification of ant trail-following movement parameters can be useful in understanding the mechanics of ant movement and may be important in testing specific strategies and products that disrupt trail-following behavior.
... По нашим данным, при увеличении количества участников происходит также и увеличение неагрессивных реакций, таких как изменение траектории движения. Взаимодействие при контакте между рабочими доминанта и подчиненных видов чаще всего приводит к ускорению движения последних, чтобы избежать агрессии (Slipinski, Zmihorski, 2017). ...
Article
Full-text available
In 2013–2014, in Crimea, interspecies relationships between potentially invasive ant species Crematogaster subdentata and native ant species has been studied. It is shown that C. subdentata do not cause a negative impact on dynamic density of other ant species as compared with native dominant Myrmica bergi. Subordinate ant species have spread over biotope territories, both occupied by dominants and unoccupied ones, according to the preferences of the species closest in hierarchy. In 2014, C. subdentata increased their workers’ dynamic density and destroyed peripheral nests of M. bergi in the vicinity of their new trails and nests. While workers of M. bergi have qualitative advantage in fighting (according to the linear type of the Osipov–Lanchester law), C. subdentata have quantitative advantage (according to the quadratic type of the Osipov–Lanchester law). Interactions between M. bergi and C. subdentata colonies take place at the different levels of social organization: secondary federation (or supercolony) in C. subdentata, and polycaly with breeding nests in M. bergi.
... Ants are easy targets for studying the role of competition, since they can be found in high abundance in a wide variety of terrestrial habitats. Competition among ants is studied on many ecological levels, starting from the direct interactions between individuals and colonies (e.g., CzeChowski 1999, 2004, MaRkó & CzeChowski 2004, CzeChowski & MaRkó 2005, CzeChowski & al. 2009, Maák & al. 2014, vepsäläinen & CzeChowski 2014, Ślipiński & Żmihorski 2017 to the role of competition in the formation of multispecies ant assemblages (e.g., savolainen & vepsäläinen 1989, väänänen & al. 2010, tRiGos peRal & al. 2016, Żmihorski & Ślipiński 2016; for a review, see AdAms 2016). ...
Article
Full-text available
There are almost no hard data on the competitive status of Formica sanguinea, and previous studies focus mainly on its spectacular lifestyle as a slave-maker. It is usually described as an aggressive and territorial species, the latter due to its raiding behaviour and morphological resemblance to species of the subgenus Formica s. str. (typical territorial species and top dominants). A series of field observations using baits was carried out across the longitudinal extent of F. sanguinea’s range in Europe, from Finland through Poland to Romania. The main objective of the present study was to determine the competitive abilities of F. sanguinea and its status in the interspecific ant hierarchy. We also investigated the changes in the competitive abilities of F. sanguinea along the meridional axis of the species’ range. Based on our results, the distribution of F. sanguinea foragers around the nest showed a distance dependent pattern, with fewer individuals present in distant arenas and at distant baits. While in the absence of baits, other ant species were seemingly not influenced by the abundance of F. sanguinea, this changed in the presence of baits, showing that F. sanguinea has a negative effect on other species. Still, many baits were exploited by other ant species around the studied nests. There were also clear differences among the study regions in terms of bait utilisation, with Finnish colonies exploiting most of baits, while Romanian colonies mostly neglecting them. In light of the present study and due to the complexity of the competitive strategy of the species, we believe that F. sanguinea does not fall into the conventionally arranged three level hierarchy of interspecific ant competition. In addition, F. sanguinea’s competitive ability may depend on local ecological conditions as revealed by the comparison of different populations across Europe.
... While the observer of the behaviour encounters principally had access to the information of the origin of workers, we consider it unlikely that an observer bias had been introduced (van Wilgenburg & Elgar 2013). Among the authors of recent behaviour studies, there were both observers blind (Jongepier et al. 2015;Purcell et al. 2016;Yagound et al. 2016) and observers not blind about the origin of workers (Kleeberg & Foitzik 2016;Parmentier et al. 2016;Ślipiński & Żmihorski 2016). The aggression indices AI and MMAI yielded different values (Figs 2A-B and 3). ...
Article
Full-text available
Social structure influences animal societies on various levels (e.g., relatedness, behaviour). In ants, both the number of matings per queen and the number of queens per colony can vary strongly. While workers from both monogynous and polygynous colonies often fight fiercely, in supercolonies (an extreme form of polygyny comprising thousands of queens in spatially separated but interconnected nests), non-nestmates interact peacefully. Studies on social and behavioural polymorphism within ant species can help elucidate their influence on genetic diversity and behaviour and the factors triggering variation in social structure and behaviour. Here, we reveal a behavioural and social polymorphism comprising monogyny with and without internest aggression in Tetramorium alpestre sampled in Tyrol, Austria. The social polymorphism is based on genetic and behavioural evidence and contrasts with the supercolonial organisation known from another location in Austria (Carinthia), 150 km away. Microsatellite genotyping using eight polymorphic loci revealed monogyny-monandry and high intranest pairwise relatedness. Interestingly, various experimental one-on-one worker encounters revealed only occasional aggressive behaviour between monogynous colonies, and thus a behavioural polymorphism. Mantel tests revealed a significant negative correlation between spatial distance and relatedness, while worker behaviour was not correlated with relatedness or spatial distance. These results indicate that behaviour might be influenced by other factors – for example, the experience of workers, ecological, chemical, and/or genetic factors not characterised in this study. However, workers distinguished nestmates from non-nestmates also when aggression was lacking. We hypothesise an adaptive value of reduced aggression. We speculate that the non-aggressive and partly aggressive encounters observed represent different options in the social structure of T. alpestre, the non-aggressiveness possibly also promoting supercolony development. The social and behavioural polymorphisms observed offer opportunities to identify the factors triggering these changes and thus further explore the behavioural and social polymorphism of this ant species.
Article
Статья посвящена разработке системы распознавания, отслеживания и оценки количественных характеристик поведения муравьев на видеопоследовательностях. Подобное ПО необходимо для биологических исследований, а конкретно – для изучения муравьев (мирмекология). Текущая версия системы ориентирована на расчет динамической плотности особей на территории – количества рабочих муравьев в заданной области за минуту. Рассматриваются видеозаписи, сделанные в полевых условиях на искусственной размеченной поверхности, для определения динамической плотности особей на территории. Для распознавания отдельных особей использован нейросетевой детектор, определяющий положение и ключевые точки муравья, соответствующие его голове и брюшку. По ключевым точкам рассчитывается ориентация объекта в пространстве. Точность определения границ изображений муравьев составила 92 %, а ошибка определения ориентации – 0.4 радиана. За счет определения ориентации объекта для решения задачи отслеживания используется расширенный фильтр Калмана, который учитывает ее в прогнозировании движения. Для решения задачи соотношения данных был применен критерий минимального времени перемещения муравья. Траектории движения муравьев, полученные в процессе работы системы, используются для расчета динамической плотности в произвольной области. Для отсеивания ложноположительных срабатываний детектора в системе отслеживания применяются разработанные эвристики. Благодаря используемым алгоритмам удалось добиться автоматического подсчета динамической плотности с точностью, не уступающей обработке видеозаписи человеком. Полученные результаты позволят значительно облегчить работу мирмекологов с видеозаписями, заменив человеко-часы более дешевыми машино-часами. В дальнейшем система может быть дополнена блоками определения других количественных характеристик движения животных.
Article
Full-text available
Foraging is one of the main reasons for ants to walk. Foraging speed is mainly dependent on morphological traits, but also on the surface structure. The size-grain hypothesis (SGH) suggests that (1) relative leg length should increase allometrically with body size, and (2) smaller, shorter legged species have an advantage with increasing habitat complexity in comparison to larger and longer legged species. In general, it is thought that leg length is a good predictor for running speed. We performed morphological analyses and running experiments with differently sized ant species along an artificial gradient of surface rugosity. We measured running speed, number of steps, body size and leg length to answer the question, if morphological traits can be used to predict the running speed of ants in complex habitats, as predicted by the SGH. We found that (1) leg length increased allometrically with body size, and that (2) the largest species were among the fastest and could sustain their speed with increasing surface rugosity. The smallest species with the relatively shortest legs were the fastest on the flattest surface, but their speed decreased rapidly with increasing rugosity. Leg length was not a good predictor for running speed. Similar-sized species responded idiosyncratically to high surface rugosity, which might be related to species-specific habitat preferences. Species-specific behavior or stride frequency influenced running speed strongly, which hampers precise predictions on their running speed, based on morphological traits exclusively.
Article
Parasite infection often results in alterations in host behaviour. These changes vary greatly in their magnitude, from slight shifts in the time spent by the host performing a given activity to the appearance of novel behaviours. The effects of parasites can differ with the age and the physiological condition of the host. Rickia wasmannii is an ectoparasitic fungal symbiont in Myrmica ants that covers the whole body surface of the host and reduces its lifespan. The fungus is present in both young and old individuals, making it an optimal subject for the study of age-related parasitic effects. We tested the effect of fungal infection on the locomotory activity of the Myrmica scabrinodis ant in different age categories. The fat content of workers was measured as a proxy for their physiological status. Based on our findings, old workers bore more thalli and were leaner than young individuals, while they tended to move at higher speeds and with a lower degree of meandering. Young individuals covered smaller distances, at slower speeds and with a higher degree of meandering. Contrary to our expectations, the infection intensity of R. wasmannii affected neither the fat content nor the locomotory activity of ant workers. However, the two age classes seem to have different strategies with regards to the relationship between fat content and distance covered. Our results suggest that characteristics of locomotory activity differ between the age classes in many respects, and are also influenced by their physiological status, but parasitism by R. wasmannii does not seem to have a straightforward effect on any of the variables studied
Article
Full-text available
Ants, the most abundant taxa among canopy-dwelling animals in tropical rainforests, are mostly represented by territorially-dominant arboreal ants (TDAs) whose territories are distributed in a mosaic pattern (arboreal ant mosaics). Large TDA colonies regulate insect herbivores, with implications for forestry and agronomy. What generates these mosaics in vegetal formations, which are dynamic, still needs to be better understood. So, from empirical research based on three Cameroonian tree species (Lophira alata, Ochnaceae; Anthocleista vogelii, Gentianaceae; and Barteria fistulosa, Passifloraceae), we used the Self-Organizing Map (SOM, neural network) to illustrate the succession of TDAs as their host trees grow and age. The SOM separated the trees by species and by size for L. alata, which can reach 60 m in height and live several centuries. An ontogenic succession of TDAs from sapling to mature trees is shown, and some ecological traits are highlighted for certain TDAs. Also, because the SOM permits the analysis of data with many zeroes with no effect of outliers on the overall scatterplot distributions, we obtained ecological information on rare species. Finally, the SOM permitted us to show that functional groups cannot be selected at the genus level as congeneric species can have very different ecological niches, something particularly true for Crematogaster spp. which include a species specifically associated with B. fistulosa, non-dominant species and TDAs. Therefore, the SOM permitted the complex relationships between TDAs and their growing host trees to be analyzed, while also providing new information on the ecological traits of the ant species involved. This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved. This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
Article
Full-text available
1. Full sunlight conditions in open clear‐cuts may limit the activity of ants as soil surface temperatures reach lethal levels. Therefore, differences may be expected between the diurnal and nocturnal activity of ants, and in the interactions between ant species. These predictions, however, have been poorly investigated so far. 2. The circadian activity of ants in clear‐cuts in managed forests in P oland was investigated. Repeated counts of ants were performed during the day and the following night at the clear‐cut edge and in the clear‐cut interior. Interspecific interactions and the effect of plant coverage were also considered. 3. Abundances of F ormica fusca L innaeus and red wood ants were higher during the day, whereas M yrmica were more common at night. F ormica fusca , L asius and red wood ants were more common at the clear‐cut edge than in the interior. M yrmica showed the opposite pattern, but at night, its numbers increased at the edge. Plant coverage positively affected F . fusca and red wood ants. 4. Red wood ants tended to be negatively associated with L asius , whereas they were neutral for F. fusca . The negative association of red wood ants and M yrmica was stronger during the day compared to night. 5. The time of day was a strong driver of ant activity in the clear‐cuts, whereas the distribution of red wood ants was of lesser importance. It is concluded that circadian activity may substantially contribute to niche separation between coexisting species, therefore, studies performed exclusively during the day cannot reflect the real structure of the community.
Technical Report
Full-text available
Description Fit linear and generalized linear mixed-effects models. The models and their components are represented using S4 classes and methods. The core computational algorithms are implemented using the 'Eigen' C++ library for numerical linear algebra and 'RcppEigen' ``glue''.
Article
Full-text available
In forest ecosystems in the temperate and boreal zones in Europe, red wood ants (RWA, Formica rufa group) have a significant affect as predators and competitors in communities of ground-dwelling arthropods. Therefore, the spatiotemporal distribution and abundance of RWA affect the distribution of many other species. The hypothesis that a reduction in the abundance of RWA in clear-cut areas enables other arthropods to increase in abundance was tested. The study was conducted in NW Poland in 2007 and 2008. A total of 276 1×1 m plots were sampled and 1,696 individuals recorded. The probability of the occurrence of RWA decreased significantly towards the center of clear-cut areas and increased with increasing plant cover. The frequency of Lasius platythorax, Formica fusca and spiders in the plots significantly increased towards the edge of a clear-cut area. Moreover, the occurrence of L. platythorax was negatively associated with the presence of RWA, while that of the Myrmica species was positively associated. The effect of the distance to the edge of a clear-cut area seems to be much more pronounced than the effect of RWA. This suggests that the arthropods studied prefer habitats close to the edge that are utilized by RWA than RWA-free sites located in the centre of clear-cut areas.
Code
Tools for performing model selection and model averaging. Automated model selection through subsetting the maximum model, with optional constraints for model inclusion. Model parameter and prediction averaging based on model weights derived from information criteria (AICc and alike) or custom model weighting schemes. [Please do not request the full text - it is an R package. The up-to-date manual is available from CRAN].
Chapter
Competition occurs when different species or individuals require the same limiting resources. It can occur within colonies for reproductive rights, between colonies of the same species (intraspecific competition) and between populations of different species (interspecific competition). Evidence for intraspecific competition includes overdispersion of nests, territoriality, and reallocation of castes in response to new neighbours. Evidence for interspecific competition includes spatial ant mosaics, agonistic behaviour, chemical defence and behavioural dominance hierarchies. Experiments in interspecific competition show that it is highly conditional varying with resource quality and quantity, biotic and abiotic conditions. The discovery-;dominance trade-off suggests a possible mechanism for species coexistence. The dominance-impoverishment relationship suggests that species richness is reduced where the abundance of dominant ants is high.
Article
The third edition of Insect Ecology: An Ecosystem Approach provides a modern perspective of insect ecology that integrates two approaches traditionally used to study insect ecology: evolutionary and ecosystem. This integration substantially broadens the scope of insect ecology and contributes to prediction and resolution of the effects of current environmental changes, as these affect and are affected by insects. The third edition includes an updated and expanded synthesis of feedback and interactions between insects and their environment. This updated material and a new chapter on applications of insect ecology to social and environmental issues effectively demonstrates how evolutionary and ecosystem approaches complement each other, with the intent of stimulating further integration of these approaches in experiments that address insect roles in ecosystems. Effective management of ecosystem resources depends on evaluation of the complex, often complementary, effects of insects on ecosystem conditions, as well as insect responses to changing conditions.. Timely revision of a key reference on insect ecology. Full coverage of ecosystem structure and function balanced with essential background on evolutionary aspects. New chapter on applications to issues such as pest management, ecosystem restoration, invasive species and environmental changes. Case studies highlight practical and theoretical applications for topics covered in each chapter.
Article
In Mediterranean open habitats, dominant ant species are heat-intolerant and risk-averser, foraging very far from their critical thermal limits (CTM). Subordinate are heat-tolerant (thermophilic) and risk-proner, foraging very near their CTM, running a high heat mortality risk, but having better performance at high temperatures. Thermal tolerance allows a far greater dominance in the ecosystem by subordinates than might be expected from their relative abundance and fighting abilities. Foraging of subordinates is more influenced by temperature than by competition of dominants. The mutual exclusion between dominant and subordinate species seems the result of physiological specialization to different temperature ranges. Focussing on two Cataglyphis species (subordinate and thermophilic), two alternative mechanisms facing extreme heat are described: C. velox polymorphism (large workers are more resistant than small ones), and physiological and behavioural adaptations of C. rosenhaueri workers (all of them of small size).
Article
During long-term field studies on division of space between the territorial ant species Lasius fuliginosus (Latr.) and Formica polyctena, Först. in southern Finland a severe decrease in the abundance of subordinate ant species was observed within L. fuliginosus territory. As part of this study we analyze the extent of changes in subordinate ant species assemblage in the light of already documented cases of L. fuliginosus predation on colonies of subordinate ants. The results showed that L. fuliginosus had a much stronger negative impact on co-occurring subordinate species, than the neighbouring rival F. polyctena. The hypothesis of hunger-induced myrmecophagy in this species is put forward, and is discussed as a possible competitive mechanism by which L. fuliginosus could shape ant assemblages within its territories.