ArticlePDF Available

Using Experimental Survey Designs to Support Pricing Decisions

Authors:
  • VARI (Value Research Institute)

Abstract and Figures

Due to a changing marketing environment (more volatile, uncertain, complex and ambiguous framework, digitization) and the lack of a consistent pricing approach (challenge to harmonize neoclassical and behavioral elements), managers are increasingly questioning established ways to determine price levels and display price structures. Experimental testing represents a possibility to provide a decision support to pricing managers in the short run. The article presents three different projects, which quantitatively examine key elements of behavioral pricing: (1) the “Center-stage Effect” (electricity contract), (2) the impact of context on price perception (promotional campaigns for train tickets) and (3) offering of price guarantees as a means for improving the overall image as well as customer loyalty of the supplier (gas station). The results are partly unexpected as they contradict often-cited rules of thumb in behavioral pricing. Hence, it can be shown that experimental testing based on online surveys is a valuable and cost-efficient method to optimize price structures, price levels as well as price communication.Keywords: VUCA, Price strategy, Behavioral pricing, Experimental testing, Online survey
Content may be subject to copyright.
Business and Management Horizons
ISSN 2326-0297
2016, Vol. 4, No. 1
22
Using Experimental Survey Designs to Support Pricing
Decisions
Andreas Krämer
BiTS - Business and Information Technology School GmbH Iserlohn, Germany
Tel: 49-178-256-2241 E-mail: andreas.kraemer@bits-laureate.de
and
exeo Strategic Consulting AG
Wittelsbacherring, 24, 53115 Bonn, Germany
E-mail: andreas.kraemer@exeo-consulting.com
Received: April 1, 2016 Accepted: April 18, 2016 Published: April 27, 2016
doi:10.5296/bmh.v4i1.9355 URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.5296/bmh.v4i1.9355
Abstract
Due to a changing marketing environment (more volatile, uncertain, complex and ambiguous
framework, digitization) and the lack of a consistent pricing approach (challenge to
harmonize neoclassical and behavioral elements), managers are increasingly questioning
established ways to determine price levels and display price structures. Experimental testing
represents a possibility to provide a decision support to pricing managers in the short run. The
article presents three different projects, which quantitatively examine key elements of
behavioral pricing: (1) the “Center-stage Effect” (electricity contract), (2) the impact of
context on price perception (promotional campaigns for train tickets) and (3) offering of price
guarantees as a means for improving the overall image as well as customer loyalty of the
supplier (gas station). The results are partly unexpected as they contradict often-cited rules of
thumb in behavioral pricing. Hence, it can be shown that experimental testing based on online
surveys is a valuable and cost-efficient method to optimize price structures, price levels as
well as price communication.
Keywords: VUCA, Price strategy, Behavioral pricing, Experimental testing, Online survey
Business and Management Horizons
ISSN 2326-0297
2016, Vol. 4, No. 1
23
1. Introduction: Do We Know How Pricing Really Works?
“The more I know, the more I know that I know nothing” (Albert Einstein). At least partially
this also applies to price management. The more information and tools for optimizing pricing
structures are available, the more it becomes obvious how limited the current understanding
of causal relationships in pricing actually is. This is mainly due to the fact that (1) digitization
and Big Data arouse the hope to be able to automate the “right” price decision, (2)
behavior-based pricing elements nowadays represent the mainstream in pricing research
(Somervuori, 2014), but still leave open questions when optimizing price levels or price
structures and (3) different factors in combination built an environment characterized by the
keywords Volatility, Uncertainty, Complexity, and Ambiguity - known by the acronym VUCA
- for decision-making. These trends are discussed in the following.
1.1 Pricing in the Age of Digitization and Big Data
Basically the increasing commercialization of products and services via the Internet and
linking different data sources improve the possibilities, to carry out cause-effect analyses with
the help of experimental testing. Nevertheless Thomke & Manzi (2014) note that such
methods are not widespread in management practice and ask: “Why don’t more companies
conduct rigorous tests of their risky overhauls and expensive proposals? Because most
organizations are reluctant to fund proper business experiments and have considerable
difficulty executing them.” If it is not possible for companies to carry out experiments in
practice (for example, to change the product placement in individual stores or to
systematically vary prices for a product in a given time period) there is at least the option for
experimental testing based on a survey. The ability to carry out research online, to define
structure-identical test groups and target these by experimental factors (for example changes
in price levels, presentation of different advertisements or price displays etc.) offers a wide
range of applications.
1.2 Mainstream: Behavioral Pricing
Typically, firms have a relatively good knowledge of the available economic inputs to the
pricing decision such as cost of goods sold, price of substitutes and competitors, but at the
same time not fully understand and anticipate a consumers response to a change in pricing
(price level, price communication). The majority of economists nowadays agree with the
statement that a purely analytical approach to price optimization is rarely successful.
Consumers do not always react in the “rational” way that traditional microeconomics predict.
Simon (2013) notes that since the award of the Nobel Prize to Kahneman & Tversky for their
work on Prospect Theory “the number of authors and publications dedicated to behavioral
economic problems explosively swells.” However, the key problem still remaining for the
practical price management is, that there is no consistent model for behavior-based pricing.
Finally, there are no clear and pragmatic rules to implement behavioral pricing in practice.
1.3 Pricing in a VUCA World
Managers increasingly face an environment in which information has no long-term
prognostic value anymore since conditions change rapidly, market determinants are becoming
Business and Management Horizons
ISSN 2326-0297
2016, Vol. 4, No. 1
24
increasingly complex and drivers of future trends are changing. The consequence is Volatility,
Uncertainty, Complexity and Ambiguity: VUCA. This also leads to a scenario where
companies are frequently thinking about alternative price models and question the way prices
were traditionally set (Krämer, 2015a).
2. Background and Methodology
2.1 Background
Out of a wide range of effects that are discussed in behavioral pricing, three elements will be
examined more closely in the following. All of them are particularly relevant to the price
image. The first relates to the preference of price offers placed in the center of a
price-/product range, secondly the context influence of price displays and thirdly, the impact
of price guarantees on the overall image of the supplier.
2.1.1 Consumer Preference for the Mean Offer (“Center-stage effect”)
A common learni ng effect from behavioral economics is that consumers change their choices,
depending on whether two alternatives are presented (with a lower and a higher price) or
three alternatives. In the case of three alternatives there is a stronger preference for the middle
price range. There are different explanations. Some researchers argue that a changed set of
reference prices might be responsible, others state consumers feel more comfortable with a
medium priced product (Simon, 2013; Rodway, Schepman, & Lambert, 2012). Nevertheless,
the consumers’ price sensitivity appears to change simply by adjusting the assortment of
products that are available. However, prior research examining the effect of the physical
position of products in an array has found inconsistent effects ranging from preferences for
the last option (Nisbett & Wilson, 1978), the middle option (Christenfeld, 1995; Shaw, 2000),
either the first or the middle option (Dreze, Hoch, & Purk, 1994), or no position effect at all
(Chandon et al., 2007). Valenzuela & Raghubir (2009) summarize their studies and conclude
“that there is a center-stage effect: a product in a central position is preferred over one at
either end of the array” and “the center advantage is due to beliefs that the product in the
center is the most popular one rather than due to greater attention paid to the product”.
Although, not all choices experiments confirm center-stage effect there is an assumption that
consumers believe that options placed in the center of a simultaneously presented array are
usually the most popular.
2.1.2 The Impact of Context on Price Perception
When consumers translate price information into categories as expensive or inexpensive, this
requires a comparison with an internal standard price, typically referred to as a reference
price (Helson, 1964). Janiszewski & Lichtenstein (1999) enhance this perspective by
differentiating between internal (i.e., price paid in the past) and external reference price (for
example where the price of a competitor’s product is known). In addition, an often discussed
phenomenon in pricing is the price-ending-effect: If a price is $9.99 whereas another is $10,
although the real difference is only one Cent, the fact that the number 10 has two digits, its
perceived value is a lot higher than it really is. This is described as the “left-digit-effect”
(Thomas & Morwitz, 2005), when the leftmost digit of the compared prices differ. Further
Business and Management Horizons
ISSN 2326-0297
2016, Vol. 4, No. 1
25
pricing tactics widely used emphasize limited availability as framing-effect (“Scarcity effect”)
as well as hints on absolute and relative savings. If consumers are promotion-focused (either
from product attributes or marketing messages), supply-based scarcity (e.g., purchase limits)
can motivate more purchases (Ku, Kuo, & Kuo, 2012).
2.1.3 Feel of Safety: the Usage of Price Guarantees to Influence the Overall Image of the
Supplier
As Bodea & Ferguson (2012) point out, the price image of a company is often driven by its
perceived fairness; the perceived fairness can be related to the sellers profit, to past and
future prices (price changes over time) or to what other customers get (value to the customer).
One possibility to communicate fairness in pricing is that a company decides on a marketing
strategy that emphasizes low prices as a general company policy (for example, everyday low
prices at Wal-Mart). The strategy of “Every Day Low Prices” (EDLP) is useful when
consumers try to maximize savings, and therefore choose retailers they believe offer the
lowest prices the majority of the time (Danziger, Hadar, & Morwitz, 2014).
Another possibility is to offer price guarantees, sometimes as a tool to strengthen the
EDLP-approach. The customer receives assurance that he has paid a lower price for a product
at current purchase than if he had bought from a competitor. Andersen & Simester (2003)
argue that customers find these price-matching policies reassuring, although there is evidence
that a price matching used by the main players in the market might finally lead to higher price
in the market. There are also ways to combine price fairness elements with customer loyalty.
Amazon Prime is one example (the customer receives free and preferred delivery while
paying a subscription fee); another example is the BahnCard, a loyalty and discount card in
Germany, which guarantees a discount (25 % or 50 %, depending on the type of card) on the
full fare train tickets (Krämer, 2015b).
2.2 Methodology
In order to test and document the performance of experimental research designs subsequently
three different independently conducted studies in Germany are presented, each related to a
different industry. Core elements of online studies are experiments as part of online surveys
in which test groups were presented different price advertisements or price structures. A
precondition for the experiment is firstly, that only selected experimental factors have impact
while interference effects are avoided and, secondly, the test groups are equally structured
(randomized). If the experiment is designed and executed properly, the only thing
consistently different between the two variants is the change between the control and
treatment (Weiss, 1997, p. 215). Therefore it is assumed that the observed change in the
dependent variable is, in fact, due to the independent variable (internal validity).
Online studies offer particularly good possibilities to implement a methodically reliable
testing environment for projects with limitations concerning time and budget. The design
fulfills the preconditions for a laboratory experiment, in which the independent variable is
manipulated and measures of the dependent variable are taken in an artificial setting for the
purpose of controlling the many possible extraneous variables that may affect the dependent
Business and Management Horizons
ISSN 2326-0297
2016, Vol. 4, No. 1
26
variable. During the last years options to conduct experiments integrated into surveys are
enhanced due to progress in developing more elaborated survey software and due to
simplified survey recruiting. Modern web-based survey platforms now allow researchers to
expose participants to a variety of multi-media stimuli such as advertisements or individual
sales offers.
The first study is focused on the optimal number of price bundles that are offered to the
consumer (electricity supplier). The second study examines price perception effects in price
displays (rail industry). The third study examines the influence of a price guarantee on the
company’s image and customer loyalty (fuel station). All studies were conducted between
September 2015 and January 2016.
3. Empirical Studies Using Experimental Survey Designs
For all cases as a first step the market environments are discussed briefly. Thereafter, the key
questions for the study are presented and the background is explained. In the last step
questions are answered based on the results of the experimental survey designs.
3.1 Study on Bundle Offers of an Electricity Provider
In Germany, there are about 1,000 electricity providers; the number of providers is increasing
slightly over time. In about 80% of electricity-grid areas household customers can choose
from more than 50 providers. The intensity of competition has significantly strengthened, last
but not least because the standardized change process to a different supplier has been
facilitated for the consumer. Since 2006 more than 15 million electricity customers changed
their suppliers (excluding those who changed their supplier after relocation) - corresponding
to one third of all households with electricity purchases. This trend continues: an additional
15 million people (16+ years) indicate that they consider changing their electricity supplier in
the future (Krämer & Bongaerts, 2015). This result is also confirmed in the empirical study:
about a quarter of respondents intend to change their electricity provider in the near future.
60% have switched suppliers in the past.
3.1.1 Key Focus: Efficient Presentation of Bundle Offers for Private Households
The experimental study was preceded by first quantitative studies (conjoint measurement) to
determine relevant service components, followed by qualitative research (focus groups) and
quantitative studies to determine the best way to structure offers (bundles vs. service modules
that can be selected by the potential buyer). Before the final decision on the launch, however,
there were two questions unanswered. The first question concerned the number of bundles
that should be offered. Here the variants two or three bundles were discussed. The second
question relates to the type of price and product presentation. The rising performance level of
the bundle set (from “Small” to “Medium” to “Large”) should be communicated and guided
through core services, either the term of the contract or the type of price guarantee.
3.1.2 Experimental Design
In the empirical study (Germany, online) about 631 people (age 18+ years) were surveyed,
which are responsible for the purchase of electricity in their household. Focus of the survey
Business and Management Horizons
ISSN 2326-0297
2016, Vol. 4, No. 1
27
was an experimental design, which divided the subjects into four equally structured groups (A
to D, Table 1). Respondents were asked to indicate the postcode of residence and the power
consumption of the household. Based on these parameters individual prices were calculated
and displayed in the experiment.
Table 1. Experimental design for bundle offers (power contract for private households)
Product bundles
offered to
interviewees
A: 3 bundle offers (S,
M, L) contract period
unchangeable
B: 2 bundle offers
(M, L) contract period
unchangeable
C: 3 bundle offers (S,
M, L) price guarantee
unchangeable
D: 2 bundle offers
(M, L) price
guarantee
unchangeable
Bundle “S” Service low (1 month
contract period)
----- Service low (no price
guarantee)
-----
Bundle “M” Service medium (12
months contract period)
Service medium (12
months contract
period)
Service medium
(reduced price
guarantee)
Service medium
(reduced price
guarantee)
Bundle “L” Service Large (24
months contract period)
Service Large (24
months contract
period)
Service Large (full
price guarantee)
Service Large (full
price guarantee)
Each group received a specific simulation surface (individual prices were presented) with the
option to add or eliminate certain service elements. Respondents were asked to indicate
which of the product bundles were eligible for selection, which offer would be chosen and
how satisfactory the offer was rated overall. In addition, image items as clarity,
comprehensibility and interest were evaluated after respondents made their choice. In order to
keep the choice set realistic, there was always a “non-option”, provided respondents did not
like or accept the bundle offers presented in the experiment.
3.1.3 Main Results
In Table 2 the shares of selected bundle offers out of a set of two bundles (group B and D) as
well as three bundles (group A and C) are presented.
Table 2. Choices according to experimental group
Choices A: 3 bundle offers (S,
M, L) contract period
changeable (n=157)*
B: 2 bundle offers (M,
L) contract period
changeable (n=158)**
C: 3 bundle offers (S,
M, L) price guarantee
changeable (n=158)*
D: 2 bundle offers (M, L)
price guarantee
changeable (n=158)**
Bundle “S” 26% ----- 17% -----
Bundle “M” 33% 37% 8% 11%
Bundle “L” 18% 34% 56% 67%
Do not select 23% 29% 18% 22%
Note. * Differences between A / C are significant (p=0.000; Chi-square), ** Differences between B / D are
significant (p=0.000, Chi-square).
Business and Management Horizons
ISSN 2326-0297
2016, Vol. 4, No. 1
28
The results of the choice experiment can be summarized as follows:
The proportions of subjects, who did not opt for one of the bundled products offered, are
between 18% (group C) and 29% (group B). The majority of respondents were able and
willing to select one offer.
The choice structure strongly differs between the bundles S, M and L (group A and C). In
particular, the presentation style in group C leads to high preference shares for the package
“L”. A tendency towards the center - often quoted in the literature (Simon, 2013) - can be
observed in the group A (in this case the bundle offer “M” cumulates 33% of the choices), but
not in the group C (here the medium bundle “M” amounts to 8%, a lower percentage than for
“S” and “L”).
Reducing the variants from three to two options (group B vs. A and group D vs. C) also
leads to inconsistent effects: the shares for non-option increases slightly, in Group D the
highest quality bundle offer (L) accounts for 67% of the choices simulated in the survey.
Through the experimental design it could be clearly demonstrated that the basic structure of
the three product bundles is best explained by using the frame of a price guarantee. This is
also supported by the image review. The groups C and D achieve better results in terms of
clarity of the offer as well as overall attractiveness than the groups A and B (although
differences are not significant). About three-quarters of respondents in the trade-off (two vs.
three offers, direct question) decided for three bundle deals. Nevertheless, opportunities arise
for suppliers to influence customers by a reduced offer. In order to determine the best strategy
for the supplier, the incremental costs between the bundles have to be considered as well.
3.2 Study on Price Perception of Ticket Promotion for Passenger Rail
The current competitive situation for rail passenger transport in Germany could hardly be
more dynamic: the business model of low-cost airlines has become prevailing in the domestic
market. Cost reductions of the airlines are passed on to the market. Therefore the price
pressure increases. Additional pressure on prices is resulting from the announcement of
Ryanair to displace competitor Air Berlin from rank No. 2 in domestic air travel in medium
term. In addition, dynamic developments concerning intercity buses and car sharing are
considerable. After the liberalization of the market for long distance bus connections in
Germany (2013), the market for bus trips boosted. One determinant was the low price level
offered by suppliers of bus trips (Krämer & Jung, 2014). For 2014, the German Federal
Statistical Office estimated the market size of 16 million trips, followed by 20 million trips in
2015. In relation to the approximately 130 million train rides (2014), there is a ratio of 0.12:1
(bus trips vs. train trips). German Rail is aiming to counter these tendencies with a service
campaign and with very low promotional rates from 19 for a saver ticket (Krämer, Jung, &
Burgartz, 2016).
3.2.1 Key Focus: Measuring Framing Effects on Price Promotions
With the special fare of 19-tickets German Rail (DB) was breaking new ground in 2015 as
such low rates have not been offered before. Therefore, the campaign was promoted with the
Business and Management Horizons
ISSN 2326-0297
2016, Vol. 4, No. 1
29
slogan “The lowest fare ever”. The offer was valid on longer journeys. Including additional
discounts effective prices of about 14 were generated which correspond to yield of about 3
cents per km (medium distances were approximately 450 km). Within the advertising banner
on the website of DB the sold number of tickets has been presented, a novelty, since sales
figures have not been included in DB advertising displays before. The price promotion has
been repeated in 2016. However, it is yet unclear how attractive the communicated price level
evaluated by individual market segments is and whether there are additional positive effects
due to the indication of the volume of tickets sold. Since rail ads are also competing with ads
from intercity bus connections their influence on the rail price image has to be questioned as
well.
3.2.2 Experimental Design
The experimental study was conducted in two steps: the first study was conducted in
September 2015 (n=547 respondents, Germany, 16+ years), a modified second study
followed in January 2016 (similar questionnaire, recruiting process and sample size). In both
cases, a systematic controlled variation of influencing factors was ensured. In the first group
(group 1.1 and group 2.1 in Table 3) in addition to the original display of sales ad the subjects
received more information about the prices of the competitor (price advertising with lower
absolute price point was shown first). To the second group only the original price advertising
was presented. In group 3 and 4 the prices and details were changed to the original display
(see Table 1). For example, instead of 19 the price was modified to 18.99 (group 1.3), to
20 (group 2.3) and to 29 (group 2.4).
Table 3. Evaluation of perceived cheapness of rail tickets
Experimental design #1(September 2015)
Evaluation
“Offer … is
cheap”
(1.1) Base sales offer
(19 per trip, 2.4
million tickets sold) +
offer of competitor (5
offer, Postbus)
(1.2) Base sales offer
(19 per trip, 2.4
million tickets sold)
(1.3) Sales offer
variation (18.99
per trip, 2.4
million tickets
sold)
(1.4) Sales offer
variation (19 per
trip, tickets sold not
indicated)
Mean 2.72 (46%)* 2.79 (47%) 2.98 (35%) 2.74 (51%)
Significance p=0.79 Control group p=0.38 p=0.83
Experimental design #2 (January 2016)
Evaluation
“Offer … is
cheap”
(2.1) Base sales offer
(19 per trip, 2.4
million tickets sold) +
offer of competitor (1
offer, Megabus)
(2.2) Base sales offer
(19 per trip, 2.4
million tickets sold)
(2.3) Sales offer
variation (20 per
trip, 2.4 million
tickets sold)
(2.4.) Sales offer
variation (29 per
trip, 2.4 million
tickets sold)
Mean 2.74 (49%) 2.48 (60%) 3.32 (35%) 2.74 (51%)
Significance p=0.32 Control group p=0.00 p=0.43
Note. * Value in ( ) indicate top-2-ratings (1=“I fully agree” and 2=“I agree”); scale from 1=“I fully agree” to
6=“I fully disagree”
Business and Management Horizons
ISSN 2326-0297
2016, Vol. 4, No. 1
30
3.2.3 Main Results
While 47% (top-2-values) of respondents who saw the original advertising, in the first study
rated the train tickes as cheap the corresponding values in the second study increases to 60%.
Correspondingly mean values are lower in control group 2.2 (average of 2.48) compared to
control group 2.1 (average of 2.79). Since there is no significant difference in sampling
structure it is likely that environmental effects might be responsible, for example an increased
price sensitivity in the market (Table 3).
As Figure 1 illustrates, overall the assessment of price ads for rail tickets are relative positive.
49% of respondents agree with the statement that rail prices are cheap (15% disagreement).
Those who frequently use trains (63% agreement vs. 10% disagreement) and those who used
intercity busses in the last 12 months (57% agreement vs. 4% disagreement), provide an
above-average rating. Consequently, it is easier for Deutsche Bahn to address existing
customers compared with non-users (Krämer, 2016). Also the low price is obviously an
argument for bus users to travel (again) by train.
Figure 1. Perceived cheapness and purchase intent for promotional train tickets
3.3 Study on Image Perception of Gas Stations
On one hand the market development at petrol stations is characterized by a strong structural
change, on the other hand by a strong concentration of supply. The five major companies Aral
/ BP, Shell, Jet, Esso and Total dominate the market with a total share of around 70 percent.
This has consequences for the price setting of gas prices that traditionally have been carefully
observed by competition authorities and the public media with great interest. Again and again
the suspicion of illegal price fixing is expressed, which, however, yet could not be proven
(Dewenter, Haucap, & Heimeshoff, 2012). As the German Monopoly Commission stated
(Bundeskartellamt Markttransparenzstelle für Kraftstoffe, 2015) “in essence, average gas
Business and Management Horizons
ISSN 2326-0297
2016, Vol. 4, No. 1
31
prices show a robust wave-shaped function over of the day, with a significant increase in the
evening and on the following day until the evening and then again falling prices. This curve
is repeating on any day of the week”.
3.3.1 Key Focus: Improve Customer Loyalty by Offering a Price Guarantee
In mid 2015, Shell (ranking No. 2 in terms of market share in Germany) introduced a
modified loyalty scheme in its 2000 stations in Germany (slogan: “Life is too short to
compare gasoline prices”). Key element is a new pricing and discount model that is based on
data from the official market transparency unit. At checkout, the currently valid prices of the
respective Shell station are compared with the prices of the ten nearest stations. In case there
is a cheaper competitor Shell’s customer is charged at maximum 2 cents per liter more than at
the cheapest gas station. “We want to offer customers the guarantee that they pay fair and low
prices,” underlined Shell when introducing the price model. The offer is targeted to
approximately six million owners of Shell discount card Clubsmart. As Simon-Kucher &
Partners (SKP, 2015) stated: “The new pricing model not only fits perfectly to the customer
settings, it also strengthens the brand and increases customer loyalty”.
Yet, it remains unclear whether consumers are truly convinced of the fairness and price safety
promised by the supplier. Overall, the study reveals an ambivalent image. Only about one out
of three respondents is interested in Shell’s pricing model (top-2 rating), for 51%, however,
the pricing model is not interesting (low-2 rating). The impressions and comments of
respondents (open question) demonstrate a strong polarization. Particularly positive is the
review for Shell customers currently holding a Clubsmart card - the loyalty scheme of Shell.
In this group, about 65% of customers are clear supporters of the price guarantee.
Admittedly, the situation is different for Shell customers without the Clubsmart loyalty card.
In this group the proportion of critical consumers prevails. Thus, the new pricing model also
polarizes and splits existing Shell customers into proponents and opponents (Figure 2).
Tot al
1) in Germany, Shell offers the following price guarantee: Regi stered Shell Club Smart members pay no mor e than 2 cents / liter than at the cheapest
competitor gas station close t o the Shell station and collect the same Shell Club Smart points. How interesting t his price guarantee is for you
personally?
Shell-non-
customers
This is very interesting
(=1)
2
3
4
This is not interesting
at all (=5)
Interest in the
price guarantee
1)
Shell-customers /
without Clubsmart
Shell-customers /
with Clubsmart
10%
19%
21%
15%
36%
8%
18%
21%
17%
37%
3%
17%
23%
11%
46%
36%
29%
14%
14%
7%
Figure 2. Consumers’ interest in the Shell price guarantee according to customer segments
Business and Management Horizons
ISSN 2326-0297
2016, Vol. 4, No. 1
32
This also corresponds to a differentiated willingness to participate in Clubsmart (higher for
Clubsmart customers and lower for Shell customers without Clubsmart card).
3.3.2 Experimental Design
To measure the relative performance of Shell against major rivals, seven performance
indicators for gas station companies were evaluated (scale from 1=very good to 6=very bad).
Only interviewees who have visited a gas station in the last two months were considered.
Overall, the results for Shell and its main competitors show little differentiations: in most
brand-image dimensions only slight differences in the perception of the provider are obvious.
Significant disadvantages are found for the market leaders ARAL and Shell in terms of
prices.
To measure the effect of the Shell price guarantee on the company’s image, the interviewees
were classified randomly into two groups. One group of subjects received specific questions
about Shell’s price guarantee during the interview (among others questions awareness of
Shell’s media campaign, awareness of Shell’s price guarantee, degree of interest in
participating in Clubsmart, use of intention, etc. were measured), another group did not
receive any questions about Shell (control group).
3.3.3 Main Results
The ratings of the two groups are compared in Figure 3. Furthermore, the differences in the
top-2-values (level of agreement) are shown (scale point “good” and “very good”). In
addition, the significances of the mean values of both groups have been checked. Basically, it
should be noted that the hypothesis that offering a price guarantee and thereby increasing
price transparency for the consumer positively impacts the corporate image, could not be
confirmed.
Figure 3. Rating of Shell gas stations according to test groups (Germany 2016)
Business and Management Horizons
ISSN 2326-0297
2016, Vol. 4, No. 1
33
On the contrary, the figures almost consistently indicate poorer results in the group of
subjects who were involved in questions concerning the topic of Shell’s price guarantee.
Particularly strong effects apply to the dimensions price of gas and quality of the gas.
In principle the influence of Shell’s price guarantee on the company image could also be
determined by a pre-post measurement. However, overlapping effects in the market have to
be considered. For example, the price level for fuel at petrol stations in Germany has fallen
sharply since mid 2015. Alone, this effect can in principle influence the corporate image
significantly and overcompensate the effects of the new price model.
4. Conclusion and Recommendations
Basically, it should be noted that important elements of behavioral pricing contradict a
decision-making according to the “homo oeconomicus”. Unfortunately the effects are
difficult to generalize and can hardly translate into guidelines for decision making in pricing.
The first study (price bundles for electricity contracts) shows a clear preference for three
offers when respondents had to trade-off between two or three choice options. However, as
the choice experiment demonstrates, the specific choice of product bundles essentially
depends on which product or service elements are determined by the supplier to communicate
and emphasize different performance levels (small, medium, large). Unexpectedly, there is no
clear tendency towards the offer in the center (choice option). One explanation is that not
only the number of options, but also the similarity of the product bundles plays a crucial role.
A detailed analysis of the prices displayed in the simulation tool during the choice experiment
(individual prices vary according to power consumption, postcode of residence and the
selected components of the product bundle) showed relatively small price differences
between the bundle options S, M and L. Therefore the customers sacrifice for receiving a
higher performance level is limited. Based on a classic experiment in relation to a
subscription decision for the Economist magazine Ariely (2010) showed that the difference
between two or three options can have a tremendous impact on consumer choices even when
the alternative that is added is not chosen.
The evaluation of the experiment on the perceived cheapness of train tickets was relatively
good for the base ad with a price of 19 (group 1.2, respectively group 2.2) but lower for the
displayed price of 18.99 (group 2.1). Here the slightly lower price had no positive impact on
the perceived cheapness and a so-called “Left Digit Effect” cannot be confirmed. Additionally,
there is a significant drop in perceived attractiveness when prices of 20 per train ticket are
displayed (group 2.3), while the price of 29 (group 2.4) is rated much better. Customers
react favorably when they see prices that end in 9. Anderson & Simester (2003) report a
similar finding when testing prices in women’s clothing catalog: “For instance, when a
national women’s clothing catalog raised the price of one of its dresses from $34 to $39, sales
jumped up.” In our study possibly the transcription of the pricing information works
differently. Hence, one could assume that it is easier for the consumer to transcript 19 as
inexpensive compared to the price of 18.99 (for rail prices in Germany price endings
with .99 are rather uncommon). The weak evaluation of the 18.99 priced train ticket
compared to the base offer of 19 might also be explained due to a different quality
Business and Management Horizons
ISSN 2326-0297
2016, Vol. 4, No. 1
34
perception (Balan, 2013). Overall, the results for prices with 9-endings are confirmed by
other studies that suggest that consumers favor round numbers when paying for goods and
services (Lynn, Flynn, & Helion, 2013) and adopting round-pound prices may be more
effective (Bray & Harris, 2006).
The third project example illustrates that the offer of a price guarantee must convey a
customer benefits that is solid and easy to understand. If this requirement is not fulfilled, the
assessment of the consumer is ambivalent. The German car drivers are to a large extend
aware of Shell’s advertising explaining the new price model and a significant share is able to
recall this. However, consumers are not fully convinced of the price model, since it offers a
relative price guarantee - not an absolute price guarantee. One explanation is that the fuel
prices in Germany are partly changed hourly. Throughout the day prices per liter of fuel may
vary by more than 20 cents in cities like Cologne, Munich or Berlin (Bundeskartellamt
Markttransparenzstelle für Kraftstoffe, 2015). The benefit of a relative price guarantee
therefore is limited for the consumer. Based on the results of the survey experiments the
following recommendations are given:
(1) In the illustrated experiments only a few factors influencing the price image are tested. In
his overview article Larson (2014) describes more than fifty of these principles of behavioral
pricing dividing them into four categories: framing, congruency, context, and signaling.
Under a cost-benefit perspective the management should focus on the most important effects.
Therefore it is crucial to formulate concrete hypotheses and accurately narrow the subject
matter, to be able to provide concrete results based on the experimental design in a field or
laboratory environment. As the three studies presented in this paper show online surveys with
experimental designs can provide useful results when pricing manager face uncertainty. Due
to its simplicity, clarity of the results and the cost-efficiency such methods are recommended
to support pricing decisions. This approach is generally not competing with other pricing
research methods as conjoint measurement or Price Sensitivity Measurements (PSM). Test
designs are rather helpful at the end of a pricing process; for example, if key questions on the
implementation of promotional offers need clarification.
(2) Although, it seems intuitively more reasonable to decide for a Web-Testing as a field
experiment (if practically feasible), there are also limitations compared with an experimental
survey design (Table 4). One critical issue is that even if quantitative metrics are provided,
there is usually a lack of explanations. As Kohavi et al. (2008) state: “It is possible to know
which variant is better, and by how much, but not why.” One option is to introduce usability
labs in order to augment and complement controlled experiments (Nielsen, 2005). Another
problem is consistency. Users may notice that they are getting a different variant than their
friends and family. It is also possible that the same user will see multiple variants when using
different computers (with different cookies). Since the price is often a key parameter for web
users, variations in pricing may become a concern (for example if consumers misunderstand
the experiment as a way to discriminate prices by the supplier, as shown by Kalka & Krämer,
2016). In 2000, Amazon.com Inc. infuriated many customers when it sold DVDs to different
people for different prices. Amazon angered customers, who discussed DVDs at the website
DVDTalk.com and noticed that they were charged as much as 40% more than other
Business and Management Horizons
ISSN 2326-0297
2016, Vol. 4, No. 1
35
consumers (Streitfeld, 2000; Taylor, 2004) leading to the public discussion on price fairness.
Amazon called it merely a test and ultimately refunded the price difference to people who
paid more. (Valentino-Devries, Singer-Vine, & Soltani, 2012). Jeff Bezos said in a news
release: “We have never tested and we never will test prices based on customer
demographics,” founder Amazon.com spokesman Bill Curry said the tests were useful in
determining a price point-the right balance between how much Amazon.com could charge
and still maintain a good sales volume. Nevertheless, because of the consumer outcry,
Amazon.com ended up refunding 6,896 customers an average of $3.10 each, or a total of
$21,377.60. Similar risks of perceive unfairness when testing price levels (Li, 2014) can be
avoided when experimental survey designs are used instead of real-time web-testings.
Table 4. Comparison of Web-Testing and experimental study design
Characteristics Web-Testing Experimental Survey Design
Research method Observation / tracking Interviews / questionnaire
Typ o f ex per imen t Fi el d (“ real life”) Laboratory (“artificial”)
Focus Behavior (purchase, response) ... Perception, purchase intent …
Scope E-commerce (Internet) All sales channels
(3) While access to consumer data is no real limitation for decision making anymore, the
ability to draw the right conclusions is remaining a crucial factor. Yet, companies receive
more, detailed and real-time data about products and services. However, there is always a
retrospective view on the data (unless data are provided in real time). This is true for data
internally available for firms as well as externally acquired. Because the uncertainty in
pricing continuously increases and decisions must be taken and proved more frequently and
faster, experimental research designs in surveys are increasingly becoming relevant. As
behavioral pricing shows, small differences in communication or display of prices can have a
large effect on the price image and hence on sales volumes. Unfortunately, the direction of
the effect can hardly be predicted. Testing under realistic conditions is possibly one way to
systematically and specifically prepare pricing decisions.
Acknowledgement
The author is grateful to Rogator AG, Nuremberg, for the provision of survey software, the
programming of the questionnaire and the complete data management including access to the
online panel. Many thanks also Dr. Gerd Wilger, Dr. Robert Bongaerts and Martin Jung (exeo
Strategic Consulting AG) for valuable suggestions and substantive input to the study.
References
Anderson, E., & Simester, D. (2003). Mind Your Pricing Cues. Harvard Business Review
81(9), 96-103.
Ariely, D. (2010). Predictably Irrational, Revised and Expanded Edition: The Hidden Forces
Business and Management Horizons
ISSN 2326-0297
2016, Vol. 4, No. 1
36
That Shape Our Decisions. New York: Harper Collins.
Balan, C. (2013). Research on Odd Prices. In A. Hinterhuber & S. Liozu (Eds.), Innovation in
Pricing (pp. 376-392). London Abingdon: Routledge.
Bodea, T., & Ferguson, M. (2012). Pricing Segmentation and Analytics. Boston: Harvard
Business Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.4128/9781606492581
Bray, J. P., & Harris, C. (2006). The Effect of 9-Ending Prices on Retail Sales: A Quantitative
UK Based Field Study. Journal of Marketing Management, 22(5-6), 601-617,
http://dx.doi.org/10.1362/026725706777978631
Bundeskartellamt Markttransparenzstelle für Kraftstoffe. (2015). Das 2. Jahr
Markttransparenzstelle für Kraftstoffe (MTS-K). Bonn, December (17).
Chandon, P., Hutchinson, J. W., Bradlow, E. T., & Young, S. (2007). Measuring the value of
point-of-purchase marketing with commercial eye-tracking-data. In M. Wedel & R. Pieters
(Eds.), Visual Marketing: From Attention to Action. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Christenfeld, N. (1995). Choices from identical options. Psychological Science, 6, 50-54.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1995.tb00304.x
Dewenter, R., Haucap, J., & Heimeshoff, U. (2012). Maßnahmen zur Steigerung des Wett-
bewerbs auf den Kraftstoffmärkten in Deutschland. Munich: Allgemeiner Deutscher
Automobil-Club e.V. (ADAC).
Danziger, S., Hadar, L., & Morwitz, V. G. (2014). Retailer Pricing Strategy and Consumer
Choice under Price Uncertainty. Journal of Consumer Research, 41(3), 761-774.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/677313
Drèze, X., Hoch, S. J., & Purk, M. E. (1994). Shelf management and space elasticity. Journal
of Retailing, 70(Winter), 301-326. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-4359(94)90002-7
Helson, H. (1964). Adaptation-level theory. New York: Harper & Row.
Janiszewski, C., & Lichtenstein, D. R. (1999). A Range Theory Account of Price Perception.
Journal of Consumer Research, 25(4), 353-368. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/209544
Kalka, R., & Krämer A. (2016). Dynamic Pricing - verspielt Amazon das Vertrauen seiner
Kunden? [Online] Available:
http://www.absatzwirtschaft.de/dynamic-pricing-verspielt-amazon-das-vertrauen-seiner-kund
en-75271/
Kohavi, R., Longbotham, R., Sommerfield, D., & Henne, R. M. (2008). Controlled
experiments on the web: survey and practical guide. Data Min Knowl Disc (2009), 18,
140-181. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10618-008-0114-1
Krämer, A. (2015a). Pricing in a VUCA World - How to Optimize Prices, if the Economic,
Social and Legal Framework Changes Rapidly. In O. Mack et al. (Eds.), Managing in a
VUCA World (pp. 115-128). New York: Springer.
Business and Management Horizons
ISSN 2326-0297
2016, Vol. 4, No. 1
37
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16889-0
Krämer, A. (2015b). Rabatt- und Kundenbindungskarten im Personenverkehr -
Länderübergreifende Analyse zu den Bahn-Rabattkarten in der DACH-Region. ZEVrail,
139(9), 341-347.
Krämer, A., & Jung, M. (2014). Zwischen Preiswettbewerb und Preiskampf - Das
Spannungsfeld zwischen Nachfrageboom und Preiserosion bei Reisen mit Fernlinienbussen.
Internationales Verkehrswesen, 66(4), 58-60
Krämer, A., & Bongaerts, R. (2015). Mit der doppelten Wertperspektive zum Erfolg - Einsatz
der Value-to-Value-Segmentierung im Strommarkt. ew Magazin für die Energiewirtschaft,
114(9), 39-42.
Krämer, A., Jung, M., & Burgartz, T. (2016). A Small Step from Price Competition to Price
War - Understanding Causes, Effects and Possible Countermeasures. International Business
Research, 9(3), 1-13. http://dx.doi.org/10.5539/ibr.v9n3p1
Krämer, A. (2016). Robustness of Price Perception: How Strong are Anchoring-, Left-Digit-
and Framing-Effects when Promoting Sales Offers? Business and Management Studies, 2(1),
S. 35-43. http://dx.doi.org/10.11114/bms.v2i1.1137
Ku, H., Kuo, C., & Kuo, T. (2012). The Effect of Scarcity on the Purchase Intentions of
Prevention and Promotion Motivated Consumers. Psychology and Marketing, 29(8), 541-548.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/mar.20541
Larson, R. B. (2014). Psychological Pricing Principles for Organizations with Market Power.
Journal of Applied Business and Economics, 16(1), 11-25
Li, K. J., & Sanjay, J. (2016). Behavior-Based Pricing: An Analysis of the Impact of
Peer-Induced Fairness. Management Science, 1.
Lynn, M., Flynn, S. M., & Helion, C. (2013). Do consumers prefer round prices? Evidence
from pay-what-you-want decisions and self-pumped gasoline purchases. Journal of Economic
Psychology, 36(June), 96-102. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2013.01.010
Nielsen, J. (2005). Putting A/B testing in its place. Useit.com Alertbox. [Online] Available:
http://www. useit.com/alertbox/20050815.html
Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on
mental processes. Psychological Review, 84(3), 231-259.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.84.3.231
Rodway, P., Schepman, A., & Lambert, J. (2012). Preferring the One in the Middle: Further
Evidence for the Centre-stage Effect. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26(2), 215-222.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.1812
Simon, H. (2013). Preisheiten - Alles, was Sie über Preise wissen müssen. Frankfurt:
Campus.
Business and Management Horizons
ISSN 2326-0297
2016, Vol. 4, No. 1
38
Simon-Kucher & Partners. (2015). An die Spitze mit TopLine Power. [Online] Available:
https://www.simon-kucher.com/sites/default/files/SimonKucher_Imagebroschuere_DE.pdf
Somervuori, S. (2014). Profiling behavioral pricing research in marketing. Journal of Product
& Brand Management, 23(6), 462-474. http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/JPBM-06-2014-0653
Shaw, J. I. (2000). Centrality preferences in choices among similar options. Journal of
General Psychology, 127(2), 157-164.
Streitfeld, D. (2000). On the Web Price Tags Blue: What You Pay Could Depend on WhoYou
Are. The Washington Post, September (27).
Taylor, C. (2004). Consumer Privacy and the Market for Customer Information. RAND
Journal of Economics, 35(4), 631-650. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1593765
Thomas, M., & Morwitz, V. (2005). Penny Wise and Pound Foolish: The Left-Digit Effect in
Price Cognition. Journal of Consumer Research, 32(1), 54-64.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/429600
Thomke, S., & Manzi, J. (2014). The Discipline of Business Experimentation. Harvard
Business Review, 92(12), 70-79.
Valenzuela, A., & Raghubir, P. (2009). Position-based beliefs: The center-stage effect.
Journal of Consumer Psychology, 19(2), 185-196.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2009.02.011
Va l en t i n o- De vr ie s, J. , S in g e r -V in e, J. , & So lt ani, A. (2012). Websites Vary Prices, Deals
Based on Users’ Information. Wall Street Journal, December (24).
Weiss, C. H. (1997). Evaluation: methods for studying programs and policies (2nd ed.). New
Jersey: Prentice Hall.
Copyright Disclaimer
Copyright for this article is retained by the author(s), with first publication rights granted to
the journal.
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative
Commons Attribution license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/).
... However, some previous researches hypothesized that price sensitivity can be dynamic and influenced by other channel's price [10]. Kramer [11] studied an application of experimental design approach in pricing. However, the solution finding in the previous research did not consider a Nash Equilibrium solution, a win-win solution scenario for each channel, and price sensitivity deep study. ...
... To accommodate, this study conducted pricing experiment to know the price effect on customers' preferences by using response surface methods. Experiment is one of the methodological approaches to accommodate pricing effect on customers [11]. ...
Article
Manufacturers utilize dual-channel supply-chain to cope with customer preferences. This structure allows manufacturer's online facility and conventional retailer to deliver their products more efficiently. However, this mechanism also brings up anew internal competition between manufacturers and retailers. To observe the price effect within this system, response surface methodology is used to model the customer preference. The independent variables are online and retailer prices, while the dependent variables are price and cross-price sensitivities. To simulate proposed model into real-practice, game theory is employed. The strategies are the priority of each channel price and cross-price sensitivities. Based on this game model, a Nash equilibrium is obtained. This research proposes anovelty by means of the usage of response surface results as game theory payoffs to represent the uncertainty in a competitive environment. The integration of response surface model and game matrix can be used as managerial implication in predicting competitors' behaviour.
... One of the methodologies that consists of hypotheses testing is an experimental design (Elbert, Bogusch, and Özsucu 2014). The experimental approach can be used to support pricing decision by considering uncertainty (Kramer 2016). Pricing experiment can perceive the effect of price to demand quantity of product (Kiefer, Kelly, and Burdett 1994). ...
... The experimental design was employed to analyse the type of uncertainty in DCSC because pricing experiment can analyse the customer behaviour towards the product price (Kiefer, Kelly, and Burdett 1994;Kramer 2016). The reason is that the retail and online price factors were included in the continuous variable. ...
Article
Companies apply dual-channel supply-chain (DCSC) by allocating online facilities without leaving conventional retailers. As a result, carbon emissions from production activities have increased in the company structure. Nowadays, DCSC researchers have a tendency to set their models in deterministic settings. These models cannot cope with the structure complexity. This research used Response Surface Methodology to analyse uncertainty in DCSC. The decision variables were the retailers and online prices, while the responses were price channel sensitivities (βo and βr), cross-channel sensitivities (γor and γro), profit estimation, and carbon emission estimation. In illustrating the real-world competition under oligopoly games, the optimisation phase used nonlinear programming under cooperative and non-cooperative scenarios. The result showed that in each channel, price combination had a significant effect on the response variables. Price differences on each channel could confirm the changes of customer preference channel. This study finds that the application of a response surface methodology can describe the significance of price to the DCSC system.
... Aber 85 auch hier bestehen Risiken -von einer zwingend positiven Wirkung kann nicht gesprochen werden [16]. Um den Einfluss von Ankerpreisen oder Kosteninformationen auf die Zahlungsbereitschaft zu bestimmen, wurden im Rahmen der eigenen Studie randomisiert drei Gruppen gebildet [23]. Die erste Gruppe enthielt keine Zusatzinformationen (Kontrollgruppe), in der zweiten Gruppe (Experimentalgruppe A) wurden Informationen zu den Trassenkosten bereitgestellt ("26 EUR pro Person") in der dritten Gruppe (Experimentalgruppe B) wurde auf den aktuellen Eckpreis der DB ("ab 12 Die in den Experimentalgruppen identifizierten Dämpfungseffekte betreffen unterschiedliche Zielgruppen, sind also sehr robust. ...
Article
Knowing the level of willingness to pay and the levers for influencing this is of key importance for price management. In spring 2022, there are good opportunities for long-distance rail travel to build on the demand momentum at the beginning of 2020. The reason: favorable offer prices in conjunction with an improved perception of value for long-distance rail travel. The article shows how the willingness to pay for long-distance rail travel can be measured, what the main drivers are and how much they can be influenced, e.g. by price anchors.
Article
Purpose-This study proposes and demonstrates a novel approach to analyzing customer channel preferences and willingness-to-pay (WTP) in the dual sales channel (DSC) system involving direct online channels and conventional offline retailers, and to how the pricing decisions are made under specific game competition. Design/methodology/approach-Questionnaire survey based on central composite experiment design was utilized to obtain primary data. The model for customer channel preferences and WTP was then built by using multinomial logistic regression. The propensity of a customer to make purchases in either channel estimated by using the logit model was inserted in the bilevel programming model to formulate and solve for the Stackelberg competition where the conventional retailer acted as a leader. Findings-The study found that channel prices have nonlinear impacts on WTP and channel preference. The empirical results complement the mathematical formulation well where high-order own-price and cross-price effects on channel selection are generally not analytical tractable. Under the Stackelberg competition, the traditional retailer (as the leader) still achieves higher profits than the online facility. Practical implications-The proposed framework provides an empirical approach that can easily address the competition model in the sales channel when complicated own-price or cross-price effects are present. Originality/value-The present work provides a novel approach to analyze customer preference and WTP of the DSC systems. This alternative method simplifies the procedure for investigating and estimating price sensitivity, especially when the online and offline prices affect customer WTP and channel preferences nonlinearly. This model is also utilized in the game competition to facilitate data-driven price decision making to better formulate and understand real-world DSC problems.
Article
Full-text available
Due to the half-open system, an average load factor that rose sharply until the begin of Corona crisis and a large proportion of flexible tickets, the efficiency of revenue management in terms of avoiding overload of trains is reaching its limits. For this reason, greater attention is being paid to further measures that go beyond objective price levels. Here, instruments of price optics play a role, as does non-price information. Based on empirical results it can be shown, that the load factor display introduced by Deutsche Bahn in 2019 is an efficient and effective way to better distribute demand for a given seat capacity and thus reduce the problem of peak load. In times of the corona crisis, the importance of this non-price demand management has even increased.
Chapter
Der Einfluss der Digitalisierung auf die Unternehmenspraxis und auf die Entstehung neuer Geschäftsmodelle ist eines der Top-Themen im Management. Erst in letzter Zeit wird diskutiert, welche Einflüsse sich durch die veränderten Rahmenbedingungen auf das Pricing ergeben. Eine Wirkungsrichtung dabei ist die Diskussion innovativer Preismodelle („Pay-what-you-want“, „Name your own price“ etc.), eine andere die stärkere Fokussierung auf verhaltenswissenschaftliche Aspekte des Preismanagements und eine weitere die Übertragung bekannter Preismodelle auf neue Branchen (z. B. Dynamic Pricing, Abo-Systeme) sowie deren Weiterentwicklung. In der Konsequenz ergeben sich damit auch Veränderungen hinsichtlich der Erwartungen und Anforderungen an die Preiskommunikation, die Prozesse und letztlich die strategische Relevanz der Art und Weise, wie Unternehmen über Preise informieren, diese darstellen und Veränderungen kommunizieren.
Chapter
In den letzten zwei Jahrzehnten ist eine immer größer werdende Kluft zwischen Neoklassischer Theorie (mit einem Menschenbild des rational handelnden Verbrauchers) und der Verhaltensökonomie (die Entscheidungen des Menschen sind durch dessen Unzulänglichkeiten bestimmt) entstanden. Teilweise wird dies noch extremer dargestellt, wenn das Bild eines überwiegend irrational agierenden Menschen gezeichnet wird. Vor diesem Hintergrund werden die Kernaspekte des Behavioral Pricing allgemein beschrieben und spezielle Ansatzpunkte zur Beeinflussung der Preiswahrnehmung untersucht. Eigene Studienergebnisse belegen die Wirksamkeit dieser Instrumentarien nur bedingt und zeigen stattdessen eine insgesamt eingeschränkte Beeinflussbarkeit der Preiswahrnehmung auf. Die Irrationalität des Verbrauchers ist offensichtlich begrenzt („Bounded Irrationality“).
Article
Full-text available
Price communication is increasingly relevant in practice but is not sufficiently appreciated in research. Changed framework conditions, e.g. due to new price models in the context of digitalization or changed consumer behaviour, mean on the one hand that price communication has become more complex and on the other hand that new challenges on price communication have arisen. Overall, price communication should not be viewed as an isolated but as a pivotal part of overall pricing strategy.
Article
Full-text available
The first part of this paper describes the characteristics of price wars, pointing to recent examples that have caused a stir among the public as well as in the respective industries. A new, concise definition of the term price war is suggested. In the second part drivers for price wars are discussed and explained based on behavioral economics (understanding the competitor's strategy as well as a company's own cost situation). Particularly in industries that are characterized by a high proportion of costs that are unchangeable in the medium-term and low variable costs there is a substantial risk for unintended price competition possibly ending in a price war. Even slight price reductions can have fatal consequences when decision makers mistakenly estimate the price elasticities too high. In the third part a case study of a price war is presented by focusing on the market of long-distance bus journeys in Germany. Since the market for intercity bus connections was liberalized in 2013, the newly created market segment faces a very strong growth and intensive competition. Using a multi-source-multi-method-approach it is shown how the market entry of UK-based company Megabus affected price levels for bus journeys und initiated competitive reactions of the German railway operator Deutsche Bahn. The interaction of various parameters (low barriers to enter the market; high similarity of products/services; fixation on market share and capacity utilization) leads to a ruinous price competition and leaves few chances for a sustainable profitability. Measures to avoid an impending or to terminate an ongoing price war are presented.
Article
Full-text available
While the large number of publications on behavioral pricing and partly spectacular results suggest that it is easily possible to influence parameters in the consumers ́ price perception (for example, by providing price anchors as competitive prices, 99-price endings, information on a relative or absolute saving or information on availability of the prices by the suppliers), this empirical study based on experimental designs comes to a contrary conclusion: it turns out that the assessment of perceived value for money and cheapness for different sales promotion campaigns in Germany (train ticket, smartphone flat rate, filter coffee) is amazingly robust and that psychological factors tested in different experiments have a relatively low impact. The theoretical, practical and research implications of these findings are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Die stärkere Fokussierung auf Marketing und Vertrieb bei den Energieversorgern steht vor der Herausforderung, einerseits die Bedürfnisse der Kunden abzubilden, anderseits einen Re-turn on Invest nachzuweisen. Im Folgenden wird für den Bereich Haushaltsstrom der Value-to-Value-Segmentierungsansatz vorgestellt und aufgezeigt, wie Effizienzgewinne im Marke-ting und Vertrieb erreicht werden können.
Article
Full-text available
Firms tracking consumer purchase information often use behavior-based pricing (BBP), i.e., price discriminate between consumers based on preferences revealed from purchase histories. However, behavioral research has shown that such pricing practices can lead to perceptions of unfairness when consumers are charged a higher price than other consumers for the same product. This paper studies the impact of consumers’ fairness concerns on firms’ behavior-based pricing strategy, profits, consumer surplus, and social welfare. Prior research shows that BBP often yields lower profits than profits without customer recognition or behavior-based price discrimination. By contrast, we find that firms’ profits from conducting BBP increase with consumers’ fairness concerns. When fairness concerns are sufficiently strong, practicing BBP is more profitable than without customer recognition. However, consumers’ fairness concerns decrease consumer surplus. In addition, when consumers’ fairness concerns are sufficiently strong, they reduce inefficient switching and improve social welfare. This paper was accepted by J. Miguel Villas-Boas, marketing.
Article
Full-text available
This research examines how consumers choose retailers when they are uncertain about store prices prior to shopping. Simulating everyday choice, participants made successive retailer choices where on each occasion they chose a retailer and only then learned product prices. The results of a series of studies demonstrated that participants were more likely to choose a retailer that offered an everyday low pricing strategy (EDLP) or that offered frequent small discounts over a retailer that offered infrequent large discounts. This choice advantage for the retailer that was cheaper more often manifested even when its average price was judged to be higher. The same results were obtained when choices were made a day apart, when price feedback was only given for the chosen retailer, and when price feedback was given for both retailers. Participant’s expectations of future prices but not their judgments of retailer’s past average prices predicted their subsequent retailer choice.
Article
Variations in the pricing approaches firms employ may partially explain why observed industry prices appear inconsistent with economic theory. Some firms may use principles developed from psychology that do not fit traditional economic models to enhance their profits beyond the basic solutions from economic theory. This paper describes more than fifty of these principles, dividing them into four categories: framing, congruency, context, and signaling. By studying these principles from psychology, researchers and policy makers can better understand the prices they observe in the marketplace. By following more of these principles, firms may be able to enhance their performance. Journal of Applied Business and Economics http://www.na-businesspress.com/JABE/LarsonRB_Web16_1_.pdf
Article
Evidence is reviewed which suggests that there may be little or no direct introspective access to higher order cognitive processes. Subjects are sometimes (a) unaware of the existence of a stimulus that importantly influenced a response, (b) unaware of the existence of the response, and (c) unaware that the stimulus has affected the response. It is proposed that when people attempt to report on their cognitive processes, that is, on the processes mediating the effects of a stimulus on a response, they do not do so on the basis of any true introspection. Instead, their reports are based on a priori, implicit causal theories, or judgments about the extent to which a particular stimulus is a plausible cause of a given response. This suggests that though people may not be able to observe directly their cognitive processes, they will sometimes be able to report accurately about them. Accurate reports will occur when influential stimuli are salient and are plausible causes of the responses they produce, and will not occur when stimuli are not salient or are not plausible causes.
Book
This book examines volatility, uncertainty, complexity and ambiguity (VUCA) and addresses the need for broader knowledge and application of new concepts and frameworks to deal with unpredictable and rapid changing situations. The premises of VUCA can shape all aspects of an organization. To cover all areas, the book is divided into six sections. Section 1 acts as an introduction to VUCA and complexity. It reviews ways to manage complexity, while providing examples for tools and approaches that can be applied. The main focus of Section 2 is on leadership, strategy and planning. The chapters in this section create new approaches to handle VUCA environments pertaining to these areas including using the Tetralemma logics, tools from systemic structural constellation (SySt) approach of psychotherapy and organizational development, to provide new ideas for the management of large strategic programs in organizations. Section 3 considers how marketing and sales are affected by VUCA, from social media's influence to customer value management. Operations and cost management are highlighted in Section 4. This section covers VUCA challenges within global supply chains and decision-oriented controlling. In Section 5 organizational structure and process management are showcased, while Section 6 is dedicated to addressing the effects of VUCA in IT, technology and data management. The VUCA forces present businesses with the need to move from linear modes of thought to problem solving with synthetic and simultaneous thinking. This book should help to provide some starting points and ideas to deal with the next era. It should not be understood as the end of the road, but as the beginning of a journey exploring and developing new concepts for a new way of management. © Springer International Publishing Switzerland 2016. All rights reserved.
Article
Purpose – The purpose of this paper is to provide an overview of behavioral pricing research, including the identification of the primary areas studied and a summary of the core findings in each based on previous literature. Design/methodology/approach – This research examines 613 articles on the ISI Web of Science database and focuses on marketing journals that discuss behavioral pricing. The reviews of these articles use traditional literature review and research profiling methods. Findings – The main subareas in behavioral pricing this study identifies are the price–quality relationship, reference price, price awareness, price elasticity estimation and price fairness. In general, the behavioral pricing field is relatively new, and all subareas would benefit from additional research. Originality/value – For pricing researchers, this study offers integrative insights into the field based on previous literature and identifies the main contribution and main topic of each. The study also offers suggestions for new research ideas.