ChapterPDF Available

Sleep and creativity: a literature review.

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Sleep and creativity have been widely studied in the field of psychology, being essential to understand human behavior. However, the relationship between these variables has not been examined through a systematic literature review. The aim of this study was to realize a review of the studies that related sleep and creativity. Thus, we performed a survey of the studies conducted between 1990 and 2014, which were published in journals indexed in the major electronic databases, in particular EBSCO and Web of Science (WoS). We analyzed 1258 papers that respected the search criteria and were selected by combining the keywords " sleep " and " creativity ". Of these, 94 articles met the criteria concerning the relationship between sleep aspects and types of creativity or creative thinking. From the abovementioned articles, we picked 11 studies respecting the inclusion criteria. The obtained results indicate a positive relationship between sleep and creativity, nevertheless it was observed a significant variability in terms of sleep constructs and creativity dimensions. Although we can affirm that there is a positive relationship between these constructs, we must recognize the limits of this statement.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Advanced Research in Health, Education and Social Sciences: Towards a better practice
131
Chapter XIII
SLEEP AND CREATIVITY: A LITERATURE REVIEW
RUI MARGUILHO1, SAUL NEVES DE JESUS2, JOÃO VISEU3, RITA B.
DOMINGUES4, NATHÁLIA BRANDOLIM BECKER5, RAFAELA DIAS
MATAVELLI6, RAUL QUEVEDO7, GUALBERTO BUELA-CASAL8
1 University of Algarve, Portugal, rui.marguilho@gmail.com
2 University of Algarve, Portugal, snjesus@ualg.pt
3 University of Algarve, Portugal, joaonrviseu@gmail.com
4 University of Algarve, Portugal, rbdomingues@ualg.pt
5 University of Algarve, Portugal, nathalia_brandolim@msn.com
6 University of Algarve, Portugal, rdmpsiche@hotmail.com
7 University of Granada, Spain, rquevedo@ugr.es
8 University of Granada, Spain, gbuela@ugr.es
Note: This paper is supported by FCT (CIEO – Research Centre for Spatial and Organizational Dynamics,
University of Algarve, Portugal)
Abstract: Sleep and creativity have been widely studied in the field of psychology, being
essential to understand human behavior. However, the relationship between these variables has
not been examined through a systematic literature review. The aim of this study was to realize a
review of the studies that related sleep and creativity. Thus, we performed a survey of the
studies conducted between 1990 and 2014, which were published in journals indexed in the
major electronic databases, in particular EBSCO and Web of Science (WoS). We analyzed 1258
papers that respected the search criteria and were selected by combining the keywords “sleep”
and “creativity”. Of these, 94 articles met the criteria concerning the relationship between
sleep aspects and types of creativity or creative thinking. From the abovementioned articles, we
picked 11 studies respecting the inclusion criteria. The obtained results indicate a positive
relationship between sleep and creativity, nevertheless it was observed a significant variability
in terms of sleep constructs and creativity dimensions. Although we can affirm that there is a
positive relationship between these constructs, we must recognize the limits of this statement.
Key-words: creativity, sleep, systematic literature review.
INTRODUCTION
Sleep is a complex behavioral state and one
of the greatest mysteries of modern
neuroscience (Rechtschaffen & Bergmann,
2002). Yet, the latest approaches and
techniques are already able to describe and
interpret many of its’ biological mechanisms
(Carter et al., 2012), providing not only a
description of the sleeping conditions, but
also of the processes that lead to a good
quality of sleep (Miró, Cano-Lozano, &
Buela-Casal, 2005). During sleep, the brain
does not process the information received by
the sense organs, however it is an active
brain state (Pace-Schott & Hobson, 2002).
One of the most important contributions of
sleep is the development of new synapses
and memory consolidation (Ashworth, Hill,
Karmiloff-Smith, & Dimitriou, 2014).
Indeed, sleep consolidates recent memories
and, at the same time, allows implicit
information processing by changing its’
representational structure (Diekelmann &
Born, 2010).
Consolidation theory suggests that, through
sleep, both learning and memory processing
Sibiu, Romania, June 2015
132
benefit creativity, and new memories are
favored by memory representations
reactivated, becoming stronger, more robust,
and preserved in a long period of time
(Marrone, Schaner, McNaughton, Worley, &
Barnes, 2008; Oudiette et al., 2011). Rapid
eye movement (REM) sleep, in particular,
increases the creative process more than any
other sleep phase, helping the formation of
new associative networks in the brain and
useful connections between ideas, working
as an enabler for creativity (Cai, Mednick,
Harrison, Kanady, & Mednick, 2009). Both
scientists and artists have suggested that
sleep facilitates creativity (Ritter, Strick, Bos,
Van Baaren, & Dijksterhuis, 2012).
The direct relationships between sleep and
creativity have rarely been addressed, but
studies relating sleep to cognitive skills, such
as memory and motivation, suggested that
sleep may directly influence learning and the
formation of new concepts, ideas, or
solutions, in other words it leads to the
development of creativity (Marguilho, Jesus,
Viseu, Rus, & Brandolim, 2014).
An individual can state that he is in the
presence of creativity when something new
is produced, whether it is a product, a
decision, a process, or an insight. Wagner,
Gais, Haider, Verleger, and Born (2004)
reinforced this idea. They affirmed that sleep
consolidates recent memories and, in
consequence, develops a new insight over a
problem, allowing a change in the structural
representation of the subject. For instance,
Healey and Runco (2006) observed that
children were able to learn a new task and
improve their performance after a sleep
period. This learning process also involves
other aspects, such as motivation and certain
aspects of personality (Jesus, Rus, Lens, &
Imaginário, 2013).
Nowadays, creativity is recognized as an
urgent transdisciplinar and transcultural
requirement (Kaufman & Sternberg, 2006).
A growing research interest in creativity has
been observed in recent years, most likely
because this concept is the most effective
and natural answer to the world’s increasing
complexity (Runco, 2004). Innovation, new
solutions and decisions, better education, and
high competitive work environments have
turned creativity into a valuable asset and
research theme, despite its’ complex nature
(Starko, 2010).
The main goal of this study was to provide a
literature summary regarding the relationship
between sleep and creativity. To our
knowledge, this is the first literature review
relating these constructs. We also aimed to
suggest paths for future research on sleep
and creativity.
METHOD
Data Sources
The selection of studies was conducted in
January 2015 focused on the papers
published between 1990 and 2014, using the
electronic databases Web of Science (WoS)
and EBSCO. The inclusion criteria defined
were: (a) English, French, Spanish, and
Portuguese as publication languages; (b)
studies published in peer-reviewed scientific
journals; and (c) presence of the necessary
data to analyze what has been studied and
how it was studied. The keywords used to
conduct the search were: (a) “sleep” and
“creativity”; (b) “sleep” and “creative”; and
(c) “sleep intervention” and “creativity”. The
studies included in the literature review and
cited in the present manuscript are identified
with an asterisk (*) in the references section.
Procedure
Search results were coded to facilitate
comparisons using the following criteria: (a)
author; (b) year; (c) sample size; (d) sample
type (i.e., children, adolescents, adults, and
the elderly); (e) type of creativity (i.e.,
product, process, person, and situation); (f)
type of sleep (i.e., quality, insomnia, REM
sleep, and dreams); and (g) type of study.
The taxonomy of Montero and León (2007)
was applied for the studies’ classification. In
the first stage, 1258 articles were selected.
After reading the abstracts, only studies
containing empirical data on the relationship
between sleep and creativity were
considered. Afterwards, a full reading of the
documents allowed choosing those that
contained relevant statistical information. In
Advanced Research in Health, Education and Social Sciences: Towards a better practice
133
Initial sample:
1258 studies
94 studies
13 studies
Final sample:
11 studies
Abstract selection (n = 1164)
Application of the inclusion criteria: (a) type of study; (b) type of
sample; (c) variable of sleep and instrument; (d) variable of
creativity and instrument; and (e) internal consistency analysis and
statistical indicator (n = 81)
Assessment of repeated samples (n = 2)
a third step, we assessed the occurrence of
repeated studies.
RESULTS
Theoretically, we confirmed that the selected
studies were based on identical assumptions.
The characterization of sleep had the same
approach, although there were differences in
relation to: (a) sleep stage analyzed; (b) sleep
quantity/quality; and (c)
morningness/eveningness factors. Also, in
creativity, we found differences in the
studied aspects, some studies focused on the
(a) creative process; (b) creative personality;
and (c) product creation. The selection
criterion was to not discriminate between
these factors, since they all addressed
different perspectives of creativity. The
methodological approach was the same in all
studies. They presented empirical
characteristics and correlation values that
could be subject to objective analysis and
comparison. After the first selection, we
identified 1258 articles. The selection of
abstracts reduced our sample to 94 studies,
which were read taking into consideration
the criteria: (a) type of study; (b) type of
sample; (c) variable of sleep and instrument;
(d) variable of creativity and instrument; and
(e) internal consistency analysis and
statistical indicator. This analysis retrieved
13 articles, of these, 2 were rejected because
they referred to the same sample. A total of
11 articles, identified by an asterisk in the
references section, were the basis for the
present literature review (Figure 1).
Figure 1. Flow of studies in each phase of the systematic literature review. On the left are the studies that
remained after each phase. On the right are the removed studies in each evaluation step
With regard to the studies’ participants, we
registered a total of 6629 participants. Of
those, 2174 were males and 4376 were
females (Table 1). The percentage of males
(32.80%) is lower relatively to the
percentage of females (67.20%). The highest
sample was found in one article (Brand et
al., 2011), which represented 84% of the
total of participants. The age of the
participants varied between 20 and 30 years
old (young-adults), in turn only 9% of the
sample included children and elderly.
Table 1. Characteristics of the Participants Included in the Literature Review (N = 6629)
Sibiu, Romania, June 2015
134
I
D Authors Type of sample
(Age/Status) N
Total M/F
1 Wimmer et al. (1992) All ages/General population 12 12/0
2
Schredl (1995)
Young/Students and
working population
94
62/32
3
Randazzo et al. (1998)
Young/Students and
working population
16
7/9
4
Schredl (2004)
Young/Students and
working population
444
68/376
5 Healey and Runco (2006) Children/Students 60 27/33
6 Giesbrecht and Merckelbach (2006) Young/Students 205 65/140
7
Giampetro and Cavallera (2007)
Young, adult, and
elderly/Students and
working population
120
52/68
8 Cai et al. (2009) Young/Working population 77 a
9 Brand et al. (2011) Young and adults/Students 5580 1869/3711
1
0 Drago et al. (2011) Adults/Working population 8 4/4
1
1 Vartanian et al. (2014) Adults/Working population 13 10/3
Total 6629 2176/4376
Note. M = Male; F = Female; a This study did not discriminate the participants by gender.
Several instruments were used to assess
sleep and creativity, according to the purpose
of each author and taking into account that
both constructs include several factors that
can be analyzed. At an earlier stage of this
review, the instruments most commonly
used to measure creativity and sleep were,
respectively, the Torrance Test of Creative
Thinking and the Pittsburg Quality Sleep
Test. In the final phase of the review,
regarding the sleep variable, the most used
instruments were the Dreamuse
questionnaire and polysomnography (Table
2).
The main conclusions regarding the
relationship between sleep and creativity are
presented in Table 4.
Table 2. Characteristics of the Instruments Used in Studies Included in the Systematic Review
ID Sleep variable Creativity variable
Type Instrument Type Instrument
1 Dream Dreamuse questionnaire Person Self-defined
2 Dream Dream Recall Frequency Person The verbal creativity test of Schoppe (1975)
3 Sleep quality Multiple sleep latency test Person Attitude Towards Creativity
4 Sleep quality SF-B Sleep Questionnaire Person Attitude Towards Creativity
5 Sleep quality Child Depression Process Torrance Test of Creative Thinking
Advanced Research in Health, Education and Social Sciences: Towards a better practice
135
Note. “Type” refers to the type of sleep or creativity considered by each study.
Overall, studies showed a moderate and
statistically significant (p < .05) correlation
between sleep and creativity, with an
arithmetic mean of .27 and weighted average
of .34 (Table 3). Below, we can observe the
correlation results between the variables of
sleep and creativity. Study 11 (Vartanian et
al., 2014) does not possess a correlation
value, but we kept it due to the clear
presentation of its results.
Table 3. Correlation Scores in the Analyzed Studies
ID Authors r
1 Schredl (2004) .19
2 Schredl (1995) .27
3 Randazzo et al. (1998) .33
4 Wimmer et al. (1992) .26
5 Healey and Runco (2006) .31
6 Giesbrecht and Merckelbach (2006) .38
7 Brand et al. (2011) .35
8 Cai et al. (2009) .26
9 Giampetro and Cavallera (2007) .18
10 Drago et al. (2011) .13
11 Vartanian et al. (2014) -
Mean .27
Note. r = correlation coefficient value; All r values were statistically significant (p < .05).
Table 4. Main Results of Each of the Analyzed Studies
ID Results
1
This study suggested that sleep plays a functional role in the discovery of new reasoning
processes to solve problems raised in the previous day. However, the type of sleep that enabled
this process was not analyzed.
2
The sleep variable in this study was the dream recall frequency. The authors concluded that
people with greater creativity have greater recall of their dreams.
3
This study compared the performance, in a cognitive test, of 16 subjects after periods of REM and
NREM (non-rapid eye movement) sleep, and wakefulness. The tests applied after REM sleep
were associated with better results in 32% of cases. The results suggested that REM sleep
facilitates new ways to solve problems, better than NREM sleep and wakefulness.
6 Dissociative
experiences
Dissociative Experiences Scale Person
Creative Experiences Questionnaire
7 Morningness /
eveningness
Reduced Morningness Eveningness
Questionnaire
Process
Torrance Test of Creative Thinking
8 REM sleep Polysomnography
Process
RAT - Related Association Test
9 Sleep
quality/quantity
Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index Person NEO-PI-R, German version
10 Sleep patterns
(REM/NREM)
Polysomnography
Process
Torrance Test of Creative Thinking
11 Sleep quantity Stanford Sleepiness Scale Person Big Five Inventory (BFI)/MRI
Sibiu, Romania, June 2015
136
4
The findings of this research showed a positive relationship between the memory of dreams and
creativity and openness to new experiences.
5
There was a significant difference between the sleeping patterns of children who had higher
results in creativity tests comparatively to those that had lower scores on the same creativity tests.
6
Dissociation and fantasy proneness were related to some types of sleep experiences, such as
narcolepsy or vivid dreams.
7
Data showed that eveningness was correlated with the ability to apply divergent thinking
strategies to visual content. The subjects prone to the night dimension (i.e., sleep pattern in which
the subject has a sleep routine during the morning and wakefulness during the late night) had
higher results in the components of creative thinking, fluidity, flexibility, and originality. We can
assume that the evening type individuals tend to possess more ideas, hypotheses, and memories,
allowing them to easily change their conceptual strategy.
8
REM sleep facilitated the formation of new combinations between ideas and the integration of
new ideas. The study also indicated that sleep develops creative solutions, facilitating the
connection between the information available in the brain through cholinergic and noradrenergic
neuromodulation during REM sleep.
9
This study had the largest sample, therefore the most representative. The results indicated a
significant and positive correlation (r = .21, p = .01) between sleep quality and creativity, and a
moderate and significant correlation (r = .35, p = .01) between the amount of sleep and creativity.
The authors stated that sleep enhanced creative problem solving for items that were primed before
sleep, but this was only true for naps that included REM sleep.
10
NREM sleep is associated with low levels of cortical arousal, which can increase the ability to
access remote associations that are critical for creative innovations. Furthermore, the A1 cycle
(NREM sleep cycles, A1-A4) reflects the frontal lobe activity. The frontal lobes are important for
divergent thinking, a critical aspect of creativity.
11
The results of this study revealed, with high consistency, that openness to experience is related to
working memory processes, decision making, persistence, and concentration occurring in the pre-
frontal cortex.
SLEEP
Studies 9 (Brand et al., 2011), 5 (Healey &
Runco, 2006), 4 (Schredl, 2004), 3
(Randazzo et al., 1998), and 11 (Vartanian et
al., 2014) analyzed the quantity and quality
of sleep. These variables are usually
employed in exploratory studies, given that
the instruments use participants’ responses
and no experimental observation is required.
Studies 8 (Cai et al., 2009) and 10 (Drago et
al., 2011) assessed sleep cycles (i.e., REM
and NREM) which are important factors in
sleep analysis and allowed the understanding
of their relationship with creativity. Studies 2
(Schredl, 1995) and 4 (Schredl, 2004)
evaluated the dream as part of sleep. Study 6
(Giesbrecht & Merckelbach, 2006) measured
dissociative experiences during sleep. Lastly,
study 11 (Vartanian et al., 2014) analyzed
the amount of sleep using subjects that slept
for 8 hours and sleep-deprived subjects after
one night.
Regarding the instruments, the authors used
different options, but we emphasize the
difference between self-response instruments
and the ones’ employed in clinical studies
(polysomnography). This method (i.e.,
polysomnography) is used in medicine and
measures different aspects of sleep and
variables such as electroencephalography
(EEG), eye movement, musculoskeletal
activity, or electrocardiogram (ECG). Within
our selection, the variables analyzed were
REM and NREM sleep.
CREATIVITY
Meusburger, Funke, and Wunder (2009)
found more than 100 definitions in the
literature about this concept. Similarly,
Hilário et al. (2010) identified 239
instruments to assess creativity. So, for this
review, we have not defined any parameters
regarding which instrument was used by the
authors.
The selected researches indicate that studies
5 (Healy & Runco, 2006), 8 (Cai et al.,
2009), 9 (Giampetro & Cavallera, 2007), and
10 (Vartanian et al., 2014) used the
Advanced Research in Health, Education and Social Sciences: Towards a better practice
137
dimension process to assess creativity and
the other studies employed the dimension
person. This option is understandable, given
that the assessment of the dimensions
product or situation would require an
experimental approach, where the subject
would have to create a product or interact in
a situation.
Concerning the measures applied, we
highlight the use of the Torrance Test of
Creative Thinking. This instrument, which
measures creativity as a process, was created
by Torrance, Torrance, Williams, and Horng
(1978) based on the work of Guilford in the
1950’s and originally involved verbal and
non-verbal tasks that assess factors such as
flexibility, fluency, originality, and
elaboration.
NEO-PI-R (German version: Ostendorf &
Angleitner, 1994) was one of the other
instruments used. It measures, essentially,
the person dimension of creativity and is
based on the analysis of five major
personality traits (i.e., neuroticism,
extraversion, openness to experience,
agreeableness, and conscientiousness).
DISCUSSION
Our study showed that sleep has a positive
influence on creativity, confirming the
hypothesis that these two constructs are
related. However, the analysis also showed
the existence of different factors in sleep
processes which should be thoroughly
analyzed, particularly their relationships with
creativity and its’ different dimensions.
Positive relationships between creativity and
dreams have been observed by several
authors (e.g., Brand et al., 2011; Schredl,
1995, 2004). These relationships were
confirmed by the test of creative
performance after periods of REM sleep,
demonstrating that this type of sleep
promoted the formation of associated
memories and integration of non-associated
information (Cai et al., 2009). NREM sleep
may also be important for creativity, given
that this sleep stage is associated with low
levels of cortical arousal, which can increase
the ability to access remote associations that
are critical for creative innovations (Drago et
al., 2011). Likewise, NREM sleep reflects
frontal lobe activity which is important for
divergent thinking, a vital aspect of
creativity (Drago et al., 2011).
Morningness and eveningness are also
important aspects for creativity. Giampetro
and Cavallera (2007) showed that
eveningness was correlated with the ability
to apply different thinking strategies to
visual content. Subjects with an eveningness
sleep pattern were more open to new
experiences and presented greater divergent
thinking. The subjects with a morningness
pattern had higher results in creative
thinking, as fluidity, flexibility, and
originality, while the evening type tended to
create more ideas, hypotheses, and
memories, allowing them to easily change
their conceptual strategy. The question of
sleep schedules may be an important factor
in creativity, in many cases working at night
might have a significant impact on cognitive
and professional performance.
Regarding sleep quality, Healy and Runco
(2006) observed a significant difference, in
sleep quality, between children with
different creativity levels. Indeed, sleep
deprived children had worse results in
creativity tests in relation to well-slept
children (Randazzo et al., 1998).
In the case of creativity, there are also
different perspectives in the published
studies. Only two dimensions of creativity
were studied, person and process. Creativity
associated with personality (i.e., creative
person) refers to an individual characteristic
and process (i.e., creative process) is the
bridge between the person and the product.
Three of the four studies that examined the
process dimension used the Torrance Test of
Creative Thinking. This instrument is a self-
report questionnaire that has been adapted
and perfected over the years, and is widely
used and accepted for creativity assessment.
Findings of the studies which evaluated the
process dimension of creativity (studies 5, 7,
8, and 10) showed a direct relationship
between sleep and forms of creative
thinking, whether verbally, visually, or
verbal fluency. With regard to the person
dimension, it was also observed, in the
Sibiu, Romania, June 2015
138
remaining studies, a positive relationship
with sleep. Subjects submitted to creativity
tests revealed to be more open to new
experiences, have the ability to use divergent
thinking, and motivation for new tasks.
Cai et al. (2009) registered that sleep
enhanced creative problem solving for items
that were primed before sleep, but only for
naps that included REM sleep. Contrasting
with these results, Drago et al. (2011)
observed that subjects with NREM sleep
time produced more remote associations and
divergent thinking, increasing creativity.
Moreover, Wimmer et al. (1992) and
Randazzo et al. (1998) found differences in
creative abilities in sleep restricted
individuals. These individuals had worse
performance in creativity measures. These
conclusions are shared by Vartanian et al.
(2014) which adds a more complete analysis
in aspects such as working memory,
concentration, and resilience through
magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and by
biologically locating these processes.
According to this author, sleep loss has a
negative influence on pre-frontal cortex,
impairing functions like fluency and
divergent thinking.
Few attributes of human performance have
so much impact on our lives as creativity,
which can be improved through training
(Scott et al., 2004). Although there are ways
to facilitate and develop creativity,
unconscious processes have been seldom
addressed and introduced in various contexts
(Ritter & Dijksterhuis, 2014). The benefits
of an incubation period (i.e., performing less
demanding tasks for shorter interleaved
periods) are clear for the development of
creativity (Sio & Ormerod, 2009), and
include, for instance, the improvement of the
verbal component of this concept (Davies,
Gilhooly, Gilhooly, Harries, & Cairns,
2013).
Considering sleep as a multifaceted
phenomenon, we can conclude that there is a
positive relationship between the different
sleep structures and the various dimensions
of creativity. However, research on the
relationships between sleep and creativity is
scarce and these results indicate that sleep
should be considered as a major issue with
regard to creativity and innovation. A good
sleep routine can indeed increase levels of
innovation, creative solutions, and
development of new strategies in education,
management, marketing, research, and other
creative areas.
Nevertheless this relationship is not
straightforward. Future research should
consider possible moderators and mediators,
by evaluating, for instance, the combination
of conscious and unconscious processes, or
verify how practical knowledge, amount of
time, and investment in a task can change
sleep influence on decision making and
creative production. Another critical aspect
in sleep and creativity research is to
understand if individuals with previous
knowledge on a subject exhibit the same
creativity level after sleep than individuals
without any prior knowledge, but benefiting
from sleep. Also, other studies should
deepen some critical issues such as sleep
patterns and cycles, and quality and quantity
of sleep. There are also gaps in studies
dealing with the different dimensions of
creativity, which could open new
perspectives for further research.
Understanding and training creativity and
creative thinking plays an important role in
many different areas, such as education,
management, research, and arts. Therefore, a
continued investment in creativity research is
essential for human and economic
development. We believe that intervention
programs on creativity must take in
consideration different conditions, such as
level of knowledge in the area in question,
motivation, and other cognitive issues (e.g.,
stress, anxiety, resilience, and fluency).
Research in this area must take these factors
into account as mediators in the relation
between sleep and creativity. Despite the
influence of other variables, we may state
that, in general, sleep has a positive
influence on creativity and that this
relationship should be examined in future
studies.
Advanced Research in Health, Education and Social Sciences: Towards a better practice
139
REFERENCES
Ashworth, A., Hill, C. M., Karmiloff-Smith,
A., & Dimitriou, D. (2014). Sleep enhances
memory consolidation in children. Journal
of Sleep Research, 23(3), 302-308. doi:
10.1111/jsr.12119
*Brand, S., Beck., J., Kalak, N., Gerber,
Kirov, V., Puhse, U., Holsboer-Trachsler,
E. (2011). Dream recall and its relationship
to sleep, perceived stress, and creativity
among adolescents. Journal of Adolescent
Health, 49(5), 525–531. doi:
10.1016/j.jadohealth.2011.04.004
*Cai, D. J., Mednick, S. A., Harrison, E. M.,
Kanady, J. C., & Mednick, S. C. (2009).
REM, not incubation, improves creativity by
priming associative networks. Proceedings
of the National Academy of Sciences of the
United States of America, 106(25), 10130–
10134. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0900271106
Carter, M., Brill, J., Bonnavion, P.,
Huguenard, J., Huerta, R., & Lecea, L.
(2012). Mechanism for hypocretin-mediated
sleep-to-wake transitions. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences of the United
States of America, 109(39), 2635-2644. doi:
10.1073/pnas.1202526109
*Drago, V., Foster, P., Heilman, K., Aricò,
D., Williamson, J., Montagna, P., & Ferri, R.
(2011). Cyclic alternating pattern in sleep
and its relationship to creativity. Sleep
Medicine, 12(4), 361–366. doi:
10.1016/j.sleep.2010.11.009
Diekelmann, S., & Born, J. (2010). The
memory function of sleep. Nature Reviews
Neuroscience, 11, 114-126. doi:
10.1038/nrn2762
Davies, M., Gilhooly, M., Gilhooly K.,
Harries P., & Cairns D. (2013). Factors
influencing decision-making by social care
and health sector professionals in cases of
elder financial abuse. European Journal of
Ageing, 10(4), 313-323. doi:
10.1007/s10433-013-0279-3
*Giampietro, M., & Cavallera, M. (2007).
Morning and evening types and creative
thinking. Personality and Individual
Differences, 42, 453–463. doi:
10.1016/j.paid.2006.06.027
*Giesbrecht, T., & Merckelbach, H. (2006).
Dreaming to reduce fantasy? Fantasy
proneness, dissociation, and subjective sleep
experiences. Personality and Individual
Differences, 41(4), 697-706. doi:
10.1016/j.paid.2006.02.015
Guilford, J. P. (1950). Creativity. American
Psychologist, 5, 444–454. doi:
10.1037/h0063487
Hilário, J., Martinho, J., Godinho, P.,
Martins, A., Pacheco, A., Mendonça, S., &
Jesus, S. N. (2010). Instrumentos de
avaliação da criatividade: Revisão da
literatura. VII Simpósio Nacional de
Investigação em Psicologia. Braga:
Universidade do Minho.
*Healey, D., & Runco, M. A. (2006). Could
creativity be associated with insomnia?.
Creativity Research Journal, 18(1) 39-43.
doi: 10.1207/s15326934crj801_5
Jesus, S. N., Rus, C., Lens, W., &
Imaginário, S. (2013). Creativity and
intrinsic motivation: A meta-analysis of the
studies between 1990-2010. Creativity
Research Journal, 25(1), 80-84. doi:
10.1080/10400419.2013.752235
Kaufman, J. C., & Sternberg, R. J. (2006).
The international handbook of creativity.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Marguilho, R., Jesus, S. N., Viseu, J., Rus,
C., & Brandolim, N. (2014). Sleep and
creativity: A quantitative review. In M.
Milcu, H. Krall, & P. Dan (Eds.),
Prospecting interdisciplinarity in health,
education and social sciences (pp. 117-126).
Bucharest: Editura Universitara.
Marrone. D., Schaner J., McNaughton B.,
Worley P., & Barnes C. (2008). Immediate-
early gene expression at rest recapitulates
recent experience. The Journal of
Neuroscience, 28(5), 1030 –1033. doi:
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4235-07.2008
Meusburger, P., Funke, J., & Wunder, E.
(2009). Introduction: The spatiality of
creativity. In P. Meusburger, J. Funke, & E.
Wunder (Eds.), Knowledge and Space (vol.
2, pp. 97-153). New York: Springer.
Miró, E., Cano-Lozano, M., & Buela-Casal,
G. (2005). Sueño y calidad de vida. Revista
Colombiana de Psicología, 14, 11-27.
Sibiu, Romania, June 2015
140
Montero, I., & Léon, O. (2007). A guide for
naming research studies in psychology.
International Journal of Clinical and Health
Psychology, 7(3), 847-862.
Ostendorf, F., & Angleitner, A. (1994). A
comparison of different instruments
proposed to measure the Big Five. European
Review of Applied Psychology, 44, 45-53.
Oudiette, D., Constantinescu, I., Leclair-
Visonneau, L., Vidailhet, M., Schwartz, S.,
& Arnulf, I. (2011). Evidence for the re-
enactment of a recently learned behavior
during sleepwalking. PLoS ONE, 6(3),
18056. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0018056
Pace-Schott, E. F., & Hobson, J. A. (2002).
The neurobiology of sleep: Genetics, cellular
physiology and subcortical networks. Nature
Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 591-605. doi:
10.1038/nrn895
*Randazzo, A., Muehlbach, M., Schweitzer,
P., & Walsh, J. (1998). Cognitive function
following acute sleep restriction in children
ages 10-14. Journal of Sleep Research,
21(8), 861-868.
Rechtschaffen, A., & Bergmann, B. M.
(2002). Sleep deprivation in the rat: An
update of the 1989 paper. Sleep, 25(1), 18-
24.
Ritter, M., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2014).
Creativity: The unconscious foundations of
the incubation period. Frontiers in Human
Neuroscience, 8, 215. doi:
10.3389/fnhum.2014.00215
Ritter, S., Strick, M., Bos, M., Van Baaren,
R., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2012). Good morning
creativity: Task reactivation during sleep
enhances beneficial effect of sleep on
creative performance. Journal of Sleep
Research, 21(6), 643-647. doi:
10.1111/j.1365-2869.2012.01006.x
Runco, A. (2004). Creativity. Annual
Review of Psychology, 55, 657-687. doi:
10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141502
Scott G., Leritz L. E., & Mumford M. D.
(2004). The effectiveness of creativity
training: A quantitative review. Creativity
Research Journal, 16, 361–388. doi:
10.1080/10400410409534549
Schredl, M. (1995). Creativity and dream
recall. Journal of Creative Behavior, 29(1),
16-24. doi: 10.1002/j.2162-
6057.1995.tb01420.x
*Schredl, M. (2004). Dream report length
and dream recall frequency: Are they
affected by the same factors?. Advances in
Psychology Research, 28, 15-25. doi:
10.2190/IC.28.4.e
Sio, U., & Ormerod, T. (2009). Does
incubation enhance problem solving? A
meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin,
135(1), 94-120. doi: 10.1037/a0014212
Starko, A. J. (2010). Creativity in the
classroom: Schools of curious delight. New
York: Routledge.
Torrance, E. P., Torrance, L. P., Williams, S.
J., & Horng, R. Y. (1978). Handbook for
training future problem solving. Athens, GA:
University of Georgia.
*Vartanian, O., Bouak, F., Caldwell, J. L.,
Cheung, B., Cupchik, G., Jobidon, M.,…
Smith, I. (2014). The effects of a single night
of sleep deprivation on fluency and
prefrontal cortex function during divergent
thinking. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience,
8, 214. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00214
Wagner, U., Gais, S., Haider, H., Verleeger,
R., & Born, J. (2004). Sleep inspires insight.
Nature, 22(427), 352-355. doi:
10.1038/nature02223
Wimmer, F., Hoffman, F., Bonato, A., &
Moffitt, R. (1992). The effects of sleep
deprivation on divergent thinking and
attention processes. Journal of Sleep
Research, 1(4), 223-230. doi:
10.1111/j.1365.2869.1992.tb00043.x
... If creativity is related to learning and technology, it will produce high-quality work. Recent research has shown that technology allows students to build a number of means that can help them create high-quality work in a creative context (Loveless, 2002). Therefore, creativity is one of the important competencies that need to be developed for students (Dawes & Wegerif, 2004). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study demonstrates that the KLEWS teaching technique is effective in developing students' creative abilities in STEM education with the subject of Optics. The KLEWS chart allows students to practice logical and argumentative thinking based on scientific information and evidence. Furthermore, teaching according to KLEWS chart stimulates students' curiosity, imagination, creativity, and desire to learn more about science. The mixed methods research was carried out on 161 eleventh grade students in Vietnam (including 80 students in the control group and 81 students in the experimental group). The research evaluates students' creative abilities by observing students' creative expression in normal STEM's topics without KLEWS chart in each group. Then, for the experimental group, organize teaching STEM topics using the KLEWS chart, and for the control group, organize teaching the topics of normal STEM. The results showed that the experimental group reached a high level of creativity (7.77/10 points), while the control group had a medium level of creativity (4.81/10 points). In addition, the research found that students were excited and active in STEM activities; applied the KLEWS chart to explore knowledge of STEM topics; confidently made a presentation and had the ability to use the keywords and scientific terms listed in the KLEWS chart and link them together. The findings of this study will help to clarify the effectiveness of developing students' creative capacity in STEM education using the KLEWS chart  .
... New technologies which are now widely used in creative industries, becoming a means to directly foster creativity, while contributing to the life and culture of society as a whole, as well as to find ways to overcome obstacles and solve specific industries' problems [9]. Primarily the distributed ledger technologies (DLTs), which had a strong influence on the industries lately, are here to rescue them. ...
Article
Full-text available
The paper presents an attempt to conceptualize the features of communication in the context of digital transformation mediated by distributed ledger technologies, blockchain, and non-fungible tokens in particular. The main hypothesis is that communication now takes place directly, without the mediation of third parties.
... As shown in Figure 4, students used media to find out the best method and design to construct their prototypes, water level alarm. Students use technology and media to help them produce high-quality work in a sense of creativity (Loveless, 2002). Creative thinking is novel thinking that produces ideas of values (Lubart & Sternberg, 1995) and became the outcome in this study. ...
Article
Full-text available
Education, research, and innovation have an impact on economic development in the world today. In a highly competitive economy, the government needs to concentrate on technologies to increase the value of production by concentrating on using innovative thinking to solve the problem. This study aimed to improve the creative thinking of students through extracurricular activities focused on the STEM Learning Based Project on the topic of density. The study used a one-group pre-test, post-test, and delayed post-test design. Quantitative results were used to see the effect of project-based learning as a model STEM lesson that was incorporated into extracurricular activities through 6E Teaching Learning Modeling as an intervention. There were 44 secondary school students in attendance. The tools used included photo analyzes and questionnaires on creative thinking with two subscales curiosity and flexibility, along with photo interpretation. The data was analyzed using MANOVA. The mean score for curiosity pre-test score was (M=2.65, SD=0.26), then increased to (M=3.42, SD= 0.59) for post-test and continued to increase to (M=4.25, SD=.27) for delayed post-test. In addition, the mean score of flexibility also indicates progress from pre-test (M= 2.60, SD=0.40), then increased to (M=3.48, SD=0.53) for post-test and continued to increase to (M= 4.13, SD=0.28) for delayed post-test. The study found that a STEM teaching-based learning project embedded in extracurricular activities through 6E Teaching Learning Modeling project-based learning that is incorporated into extracurricular activities could enhance students' creative thinking; curiosity, and flexible thinking.
... The role of sleep, relaxation and dreaming for creative performance has long been emphasized in the community (McKim 1972). Experimental evidence underpins the belief that sleep is indeed important for people to perform well on creative tasks (Wimmer et al. 1992;Marguilho et al. 2015). To allow for a contact-less and therefore convenient ways of sleep-tracking, the tool Sleepiz can be suggested. ...
Chapter
Neurodesign is a novel field of research, education and practice that emerges as a cross-disciplinary initiative. In 2019, the Hasso Plattner Institute (HPI) offered for the first time a neurodesign curriculum. The objective of neurodesign as we pursue it is to explore synergies at the intersection of (i) neuroscience, (ii) engineering and (iii) design thinking · creativity · collaboration · innovation. In this chapter, we share insights into the development of a curriculum that quickly became more comprehensive than we had anticipated for this initial implementation phase. Neurodesign evolves serendipitously driven by the passions of numerous protagonists who contribute their expertise, ideas and work results in a uniquely collaborative fashion. The chapter briefly summarizes input provided by neuroscientists and creative engineers from several countries and different continents, who contributed guest expert talks at the HPI to help build up a joint knowledge base. The major part of the chapter is a review of neurodesign projects that have emerged, often in collaboration with guest experts of the program. Overall, these projects indicate how intersections of neurodesign (i)–(ii)–(iii) open up cornucopias of opportunities. Especially the integration of engineering expertise has introduced many favourable dynamics. In terms of strategic reflections, this chapter shares “missions” we pursue in the development of neurodesign. These directions for further initiatives also commence a brief outlook on upcoming neurodesign developments.
... If the creativity relates to the learning and technology it will produce a high quality of work. In the recent study show that technology allows the students to construct several media that can help them to produce high quality of work in the creativity context (Loveless, 2002). STEM project-based learning has a chance to give a positive impact in creativity because students will develop their own idea to create the product. ...
Article
Full-text available
In some school, teacher-centered is commonly found in the learning process. The learning process itself is still in the form of direct transfer of knowledge from teacher to students. Actually, students will learn better if they are engaged in meaningful learning activity. STEM project-based learning is one of alternative teaching strategies that engaged students in meaningful learning. The aim of this study is to investigate the impact of STEM project-based learning on students' creativity in the topics of light and optics. The study used qualitative research with narrative design. Data collection technique that used is observation. The population is eight grade students in one of Junior Secondary School that is located in Bandung, Indonesia. The sample consist of 25 students that chosen based on purposive sampling technique. The data is obtained through Creativity Product Analysis Matrix (CPAM). There are three creativity dimension that used in this study which are resolution, elaboration and novelty dimension. Students’ creativity is obtained as much 76% which categorized as good. Based on the result, STEM project-based learning give the good impact on students' creativity. STEM project-based learning can be used as alternative teaching strategies in Junior Secondary School.
... In 2006, Passey reported a range of impacts on learning as a result of digital video experiences offered to hard-to-reach learners. Loveless (2002) reported some of the ways ICT was being used to support creativity in art. Webb (2005) analysed the affordances for meaning-making provided in the ICT-rich classroom environment. ...
... Digital technologies reveal features of interactivity, connectivity, capacity, range, speed and automatic functions which allow users to do things that could not be done, at least as effectively, without these tools (Loveless, 2002). The quick development of technology, especially social networks, has brought an improvement of technological tools which people are appropriating for use in their everyday lives. ...
Article
SAPO campus is an institutionally supported platform of web-based services aimed for use in educational contexts. This platform, resulting from a research partnership between the University of Aveiro Portugal and SAPO, an information and technology Portuguese company, aims to promote collaboration and sharing between its users and the development of students' personal learning environments primarily through the interaction with the broader learning community. Starting with a structural review of some theoretical constructs, our aim is to contribute for the reflection and the discussion about the importance of collaboration, motivation and knowledge sharing in promoting creativity and learning opportunities, specifically in the context of the SAPO campus platform.
... According to some studies students take great pride in their work when ICT is used in learning (Sime & Priestley, 2005) and believe that ICT motivates them in their schoolwork (Hayward et al., 2003). The results of a number of studies indicated that implementing ICT within the learning process led to positive motivational outcomes (Passey et al., 2004), independent work (Hayes, 2007) as well as to the enhancement of creativity (Loveless, 2002). Dimitrova et al. (2004) found that through the implementation of ICT, students were offered flexibility. ...
Article
Full-text available
This paper presents a European Community supported project which investigated whether the use of electronic and mobile technologies in the hands of young learners might engage them in learning activities and start to change their attitudes towards learning. The mLearning system was implemented in a Europe‐wide network of 14 schools with over 500 students and 46 teachers participating in the implementation. In the paper special focus is given to three different mLearning contexts/scenarios implemented within the setting of one of the Europe‐wide network of schools.mLearning in einem Europaweiten Netz von SchulenDieses Papier präsentiert ein von der Europäische Gemeinschaft unterstütztes Projekt, das erforschen sollte, ob der Gebrauch von elektronischen und mobilen Technologien in den Händen von jungen Anfängern diese im Lernen von Aktivitäten anregen und dazu führen könnte, ihre Einstellungen zum Lernen zu ändern. Das mLearning System wurde in einem Europaweiten Netz von 14 Schulen mit mehr als 500 Schülern und 46 Lehrern durchgeführt. In diesem Papier wurde der besondere Schwerpunkt auf drei unterschiedliche mLearning Zusammenhänge/Drehbücher gelegt, die innerhalb eines der europaweiten Netze von Schulen angelegt sind.Le mLearning dans un réseau d’écoles à l’échelle européenneCet article présente un projet soutenu par la Communauté Européenne qui a examiné si l’usage des technologies électroniques mobiles mises entre les mains de jeunes apprenants pourrait les amener à se lancer dans des activités d’apprentissage et commencer à changer leur attitude vis‐à‐vis de l’apprentissage. Ce système de m(obile)‐learning a été mis en place dans un réseau trans‐européen de 14 écoles avec une participation active de la part de plus de 500 élèves et de 46 professeurs. Dans l’article on met un accent particulier sur trois contextes/scénarios de m‐learning mis en route au sein d’un réseau transeuropéen d’écoles.El mLearning dentro de una red paneuropea de escuelasEste artículo presenta un proyecto apoyado por la Comunidad Europea en el cual se investigó si el uso de tecnologías electrónicas y móviles por parte de jóvenes alumnos pudiera conducirles a lanzarse a actividades de aprendizaje y empezar a cambiar su actitud con respecto al aprendizaje. Este sistema de m‐learning fue aplicado dentro de una red transeuropea que incluye 14 escuelas con más de de 500 alumnos y 46 profesores participando en la aplicación. El artículo hace hincapie en tres diferentes contextos/guiones de mLearning aplicados dentro de una de las redes transeuropeas de escuelas.
... If creativity is not inherent in human mental powers and is, in fact, social and situational, then technological developments may well be linked to advances in the creativity of individual users. The rhetoric constructed around (2002 ( : 2). Loveless (1999 explores some of the issues arising with regard to visual literacy and multimedia work for classroom teachers. ...
Article
Full-text available
Study Objectives Various aspects of human performance were assessed in children after sleep loss. Participants Sixteen children (7 males, 9 females) between the ages of 10 and 14 years Design and Interventions Children were randomly assigned to either a control (CTRL) group, with 11 hours in bed, or an experimental sleep restriction (SR) group, with 5 hours in bed, on a single night in the sleep laboratory. Measurements Both groups were evaluated the following day with a battery of performance and sleepiness measures. Psychomotor and cognitive performance tests were given during four 1-hour testing sessions at 2-hour intervals. Results A multiple sleep latency test (MSLT) documented shorter latencies for SR children than controls. Significant treatment differences were discovered in three of four variables of verbal creativity, including fluency, flexibility, and average indices. There were also group differences found on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), which may be indicative of difficulty learning new abstract concepts. Measures of rote performance and less-complex cognitive functions, including measures of memory and learning and figural creativity, did not show differences between groups, perhaps because motivation could overcome sleepiness-related impairment for these tasks. Conclusions Higher cognitive functions in children, such as verbal creativity and abstract thinking, are impaired after a single night of restricted sleep, even when routine performance is relatively maintained.
Chapter
Full-text available
Sleep has been a widely studied topic by psychology or by medicine, due to its impact on behavior, and homeostatic physical wellbeing. Somehow avoided by researchers, due to its complexity and difficulty in establishing strict criteria, creativity research has evolved significantly in recent years. The aim of this study was to perform a meta-analytic study relating sleep and creativity. No similar studies were found in the literature. This meta-analysis includes nine independent samples (representing 5826 participants) that met the inclusion criteria. The results showed the expected positive relationship between the amount of sleep and creativity (r = .35, 95%; CI = [.31; .40]). The homogeneity tests showed that there is no influence of moderators in this respect. Also indicated a weak relationship between sleep quality and creativity, but influenced by moderators. Although we have not conducted an analysis of moderators due to few number of studies, we conclude that the influence of sleep on creativity is more than a simple linear relationship.
Article
Full-text available
The dorsal and ventral aspects of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) are the two regions most consistently recruited in divergent thinking tasks. Given that frontal tasks have been shown to be vulnerable to sleep loss, we explored the impact of a single night of sleep deprivation on fluency (i.e., number of generated responses) and PFC function during divergent thinking. Participants underwent functional magnetic resonance imaging scanning twice while engaged in the Alternate Uses Task (AUT) – once following a single night of sleep deprivation and once following a night of normal sleep. They also wore wrist activity monitors, which enabled us to quantify daily sleep and model cognitive effectiveness. The intervention was effective, producing greater levels of fatigue and sleepiness. Modeled cognitive effectiveness and fluency were impaired following sleep deprivation, and sleep deprivation was associated with greater activation in the left inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) during AUT. The results suggest that an intervention known to temporarily compromise frontal function can impair fluency, and that this effect is instantiated in the form of an increased hemodynamic response in the left IFG.
Article
Full-text available
Creativity is one of the most important assets we have to navigate through the fast changing world of the 21st century. Anecdotal accounts of creative individuals suggest that oftentimes, creative discoveries result from a process whereby initial conscious thought is followed by a period during which one refrains from task-related conscious thought. For example, one may spend an embarrassing amount of time thinking about a problem when the solution suddenly pops into consciousness while taking a shower. Not only creative individuals but also traditional theories of creativity have put a lot of emphasis on this incubation stage in creative thinking. The aim of the present article is twofold. First, an overview of the domain of incubation and creativity is provided by reviewing and discussing studies on incubation, mind-wandering, and sleep. Second, the causes of incubation effects are discussed. Previously, little attention has been paid to the causes of incubation effects and most findings do not really speak to whether the effects should be explained by unconscious processes or merely by consequences of a period of distraction. In the latter case, there is no need to assume active unconscious processes. The findings discussed in the current article support the idea that it is not merely the absence of conscious thought that drives incubation effects, but that during an incubation period unconscious processes contribute to creative thinking. Finally, practical implications and directions for future research will be discussed.
Article
The subject of creativity has been neglected by psychologists. The immediate problem has two aspects. (1) How can we discover creative promise in our children and our youth, (2) How can we promote the development of creative personalities. Creative talent cannot be accounted for adequately in terms of I.Q. A new way of thinking about creativity and creative productivity is seen in the factorial conceptions of personality. By application of factor analysis a fruitful exploratory approach can be made. Carefully constructed hypotheses concerning primary abilities will lead to the use of novel types of tests. New factors will be discovered that will provide us with means to select individuals with creative personalities. The properties of primary abilities should be studied to improve educational methods and further their utilization. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Over the course of the last half century, numerous training programs intended to develop creativity capacities have been proposed. In this study, a quantitative meta-analysis of program evaluation efforts was conducted. Based on 70 prior studies, it was found that well-designed creativity training programs typically induce gaïns in performance with these effects generalizing across criteria, settings, and target populations. Moreover, these effects held when internal validity considerations were taken into account. An examination of the factors contributing to the relative effectiveness of these training programs indicated that more successful programs were likely to focus on development of cognitive skills and the heuristics involved in skill application, using realistic exercises appropriate to the domain at hand. The implications of these observations for the development of creativity through educational and training interventions are discussed along with directions for future research.
Book
Acknowledgements List of contributors 1. Introduction Robert J. Sternberg 2. Creativity research in English-speaking countries John Baer and James C. Kaufman 3. Creativity in Latin America: views from psychology, humanities and the arts David D. Preiss and Katherine Strasser 4. History of creativity in Spain Candido Genovard, Maria Dolores Prieto, Maria Rosario Bermejo and Carmen Ferrandiz 5. Past, present, and future perspectives on creativity in France and French-speaking Switzerland Christophe Mouchiroud and Todd I. Lubart 6. Creativity in Italy Alessandro Antonietti and Cesare Cornoldi 7. Creativity research in German-speaking countries Siegfried Preiser 8. Creativity under the Northern Lights: perspectives from Scandinavia Gudmund J. W. Smith and Ingegerd Carlsson 9. Creativity in Soviet-Russian psychology Olga Stepanossova and Elena L. Grigorenko 10. Creativity studies in Poland Edward Necka, Magdalena Grohman and Aleksandra Slabosz 11. Research on creativity in Israel: a chronicle of theoretical and empirical development Roberta M. Milgram and Nava L. Livne 12. Creativity in Turkey and Turkish-speaking countries Gunseli Oral 13. Development of creativity research in Chinese societies: a comparison of mainland China, Taiwan, Hong Kong and Singapore Weihua Niu 14. Creativity - a sudden rising star in Korea In-Soo Choe 15. Culture and facets of creativity: the Indian experience Girishwar Misra, Ashok K. Srivastava and Indiwar Misra 16. African perspectives on creativity Elias Mpofu, Kathleen Myambo, Andrew A. Mogaji, Teresa-Anne Mashego and Omar H. Khaleefa 17. Creativity around the world in 80 ways ... but with one destination Dean Keith Simonton Author index Subject index.
Article
Fantasy proneness refers to an extensive involvement in fantasy and daydreaming. Previous studies have shown that fantasy proneness overlaps with dissociative tendencies, as measured with the Dissociative Experiences Scale. We tested the hypothesis that deviant sleep experiences form the critical link between fantasy proneness and dissociation. Undergraduate students (N = 205) completed the Creative Experiences Questionnaire, the Dissociative Experiences Scale, and the Iowa Sleep Experiences Survey. Self-reports of sleep experiences, such as narcolepsy, vivid and unusual dreams, and deviant nocturnal experiences, but not lucid dreaming, were related to both fantasy proneness and dissociation. However, the relationship between fantasy proneness and dissociation was only partially accounted for by these sleep experiences. This suggests that deviant sleep experiences but also other, as yet unknown, factors contribute to the overlap between fantasy proneness and dissociation.