Content uploaded by Greg Orji Obiamalu
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Greg Orji Obiamalu on May 12, 2016
Content may be subject to copyright.
Open Journal of Modern Linguistics, 2016, 6, 97-104
Published Online April 2016 in SciRes. http://www.scirp.org/journal/ojml
http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/ojml.2016.62009
How to cite this paper: Nweze, I. M., & Obiamalu, G. O. (2016). Optional Agreement Patterns in Igbo. Open Journal of Mo-
dern Linguistics, 6, 97-104. http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/ojml.2016.62009
Optional Agreement Patterns in Igbo
Ifeoma M. Nweze1, Greg O. Obiamalu2
1Department of languages/Linguistics/Literary Studies/Visual Arts, Federal University Ndufu-Alike Ikwo, Ikwo,
Nigeria
2Department of Linguistics, Nnamdi Azikiwe University, Awka, Nigeria
Received 4 March 2016; accepted 12 April 2016; published 15 April 2016
Copyright © 2016 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.
This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution International License (CC BY).
http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
Abstract
Apart from dominance and precedence relationship, other relations exist in syntactic construc-
tions. One of such relations is the agreement relation. Scholars have discussed the clausal agree-
ment relations of the subject and object. Many of such scholars working on Igbo grammar hold the
opinion that Igbo verb forms do not inflect to mark agreement with their arguments. This study,
which is purely descriptive without any theoretical colouration, describes and illustrates the op-
tional agreement patterns in Igbo, with the intention to highlight the possibility of number agree-
ment between Igbo verbs and their external and internal arguments. The study shows that clitic
elements such as “cha/ga” and “nu” optionally attach to the verb in agreement with the plural
subject or object in a sentence. Igbo verbs also take some extensional suffixes to agree with their
arguments. We therefore gloss them as AGR morphemes.
Keywords
Igbo, Optional Agreement, Clitic
1. Introduction
When we speak or write, we make genuine efforts to select appropriate words. In languages, words agree with oth-
ers for intelligibility of sentences in discourse situations. In linguistics, meaning is attached to words and mean-
ing derived from words in syntagmatic relationship is basically dependent on the agreement existing among them.
In the Principles and Parameters Approach, agreement is assumed to be a universal element and different languages
have different ways of marking it. It is also important to note that there are many languages that do not show any
overt marker of agreement. Igbo seems to belong to that group. However, recent studies in Igbo have shown that
Igbo operates a default agreement pattern (See Dechaine, 1993; Obiamalu, 2010). The present study attempts to
investigate optional elements found on the verbs that signal agreement with the Igbo DP subjects.
I. M. Nweze, G. O. Obiamalu
98
The rest of the paper is divided as follows: Section 2 is the Literature Review which starts with the concept of
agreement as a category in syntax and ends with the discussion of Agreement in Igbo. Section 3 presents and
analyses the data. Section 4 is the summary and conclusion.
2. Literature Review
According to Trask (1993: p. 13), “agreement is a phenomenon by which appearance of one item in a sentence
in a particular form requires a second item which is grammatically linked with it to appear in a particular form”.
Crystal (1985) postulates that traditionally, it refers to a formal relationship between elements, whereby the form
of one requires a corresponding form of another to express person, gender, number etc. Asher (1994) says it is a
relationship between a semantic or formal property of one element and a formal property of another. The above
definitions imply that there is matching of features between two separate elements in a construction such that “B”
agrees with “A” in “C”. In other words, ‘B’ copies the number, person, gender, etc. features of the items in “A”.
Nzerem (2008: p. 1) exemplifies the matching features in the agreement relation as follows: if a sentence con-
tains proper noun “Hilda” and later a pronoun “she”, and they refer to the same person, we say that they agree in
number (for both are singular) and gender (for both are feminine. The illustration above shows that the pronoun
“she” (B) agrees with “A” (Hilda, the proper noun) in C (person and number). Tallerman (2005) observes that
agreement occurs when the form of a syntactic constituent changes in order to agree with the form of other
co-units within a linguistic domain such as a sentence, clause or phrase. In literature, agreement is synonymous
with concord. Eka (2001: p. 181) puts forward three subtypes of concord in English-subject-verb concord, pro-
noun/antecedent concord and time reference concord. The subject-verb concord according to Quirk and
Greenbaum (1973) is controlled by three principles. They are: Grammatical concord, Notional concord and the
Principle of proximity. Vande-Guma et al. (2013: p. 103) says that the grammatical features which trigger
grammatical agreement are reflected in linguistics as grammatical gender, person, number and case. Drawing
attention to the subject-complement and subject-object concord, she gave the following instances.
1) a. The child was an angel. b. The children were angels.
2) a. John hurt his foot. b. John and Beatrice hurt their feet.
Radford, Atkinson, Britain, Clahsen & Spencer (1999) identified lexical, functional and inflectional catego-
ries in syntactic constructions. The above authors observe that one of the three distinct categories is not an actual
word, yet it (Infl) causes a change on the other two such that the “s” suffix though not a word but added to the
lexical word “eat” marks tense and number agreement. According to Vande-Guma, Okebalama & Onukawa
(2013: p. 98), agreement and other inflectional categories such as tense, mood, aspect and case reflect important
syntactic functions in the formation of sentences and clauses. Pollock (1989) contend that INFL node which was
formally analysed in GB as carrying the features of tense and AGR be divided into two functional heads-Tense
and AGR phrases. One of the assumptions within the minimalist model hold that functional categories AGR and
Tense contain case and phi features (number, person and gender) that may check off against features of their NP.
Marantz (1995: p. 363) postulates that “the functional nodes AGR and T serve only to carry the morphological
features of Ns and Vs”.
The role played by verb morphology in showing agreement cannot be overlooked. Radford (1997) contends
that agreement results when an inflectional morphology shows a syntactic relationship between the constituents
of a sentence, clause or phrase. He observes that English, for instance, uses inflectional suffixes “-s”, “es”, “ren”
etc. in constructions as in:
3) a. A boy came, b. Two boys came.
4) a. She has a child b. She has two children.
The above morphemes are bound to the verbs. In Tiv the subject-verb agreement obtains though not in all
situations; the copula verbs “ngu (is) and mba (are)” inflect to mark number agreement with their respective
subjects (Vande-Guma, Okebalama & Onukawa, 2013: p. 103). In Igbo language, verbs do not inflect for num-
ber agreement but may optionally cliticize with another type of bound morpheme different from inflectional suf-
fixes which may check AGR features. According to Nweze (2009), the second category of bound morpheme is
the clitic. Clitics are bound morphemes that are neither affixes nor words though they include contracted words.
It is an item which appears intermediate between a word and an affix (Trask, 1993: p. 46). Clitics are named
with regards to the position of occurrence such as proclitics, enclitics, endoclitics and mesoclitics. They serve to
give additional meaning to sentences. When they function as mesoclitics and sometimes enclitics, they are part
I. M. Nweze, G. O. Obiamalu
99
of the verb since they indicate “aspect and tense”. They mark plurality and are used to reflect agreement in
number.
Agreement relation in Igbo has in recent times become a subject of controversy among scholars working on
Igbo grammar (Emenanjo, 1978; Manfredi, 1993; Mbah, 1999; Okeke, 2008; Uwalaka, 2003; Obiamalu, 2010;
and Ikegwuonu, 2008). Some argue (Emenanjo, 1978; Manfredi, 1993) that Igbo has no tense agreement marker,
aspect is the only functional category that is marked. Uwalaka (2003: p. 10) argues that Igbo does not permit the
co-occurrence of an aspectual morpheme with tense morpheme. Obiamalu providing further evidence on
Uwalaka’s contribution says that “V” moves to aspect and since the aspectual morpheme and tense do not
co-occur in Igbo, the V + Asp is blocked from movement to “T” so that the TP and AspP share the same head
and occur under the same node. Okeke (2008) remarks that there is no gender agreement in Igbo; reflexives and
their antecedent agree only in person and number. Mbah (1999, 2006) and Ikegwuonu (2008) observe that Igbo
verb forms do not inflect to mark agreement with their external arguments; the Agreement features manifests
only in Yes/No question involving the insertion of the pronominal copy of the NP immediately after the NP
through the movement of the subject to the SPEC position. She (Ikegwuonu, 2008: p. 48) strongly advocates
that the “notion of number agreement is only relevant to pronouns.” Anyanwu (2007: pp. 93-94) contends that
the complement clause verb covertly copies the tense properties of the matrix verb and notes as follows:
The verb of the complement clause necessarily refers to a sub-part or aspect of a single over-all event. For this
reason, the embedded verb acquires covertly the tense properties of the matrix verb. This means that tense, hav-
ing been overtly marked on the matrix verb becomes a feature of the embedded verb; hence, it is redundant to
mark it again.
Obiamalu (2010) following Dechaine (1993) holds that Igbo has overt morphological agreement marker.
They observe that E-prefix is a default AGR marker in negative perfective and past tense constructions. In the
Igbo language, the investigation with respect to AGR relation remains open-ended as more researches reveal
further findings on the subject matter. The present study investigates optional agreement patterns in Igbo con-
structions to show that some Igbo verbs can be formally marked to reflect the grammatical number of their ex-
ternal and internal arguments. In the present study, we investigate optional morphological markers of agreement
in Igbo constructions.
3. The Data
The optional agreement markers could be classified into two types: clitics and extensional suffixes.
3.1. The Clitic “Cha” as an Agreement Marker in Igbo
Nweze and Ikegwuonu (2012) observe that the singular/plural phenomenon exists in Igbo though not as obtained
in the Indo-European languages. One of the ways plurality is signalled is the use of the clitics. Consider the Igbo
data in (5) below.
5) a. Nwok à nà-èzu oh
man DEM DUR-steal stealing
“This man is a thief”
b. Nwok nà nwaànyị̀ à nà-èzu oh
Man and woman DEM DUR-steal stealing
“This man and woman are thieves”
c. Nwok nà nwaànyị̀ à nà-èzu-cha oh
Man and woman DEM DUR-steal-CL stealing
“This man and this woman are thieves”
Notice that 5b & c have the same meaning. The coordinated nouns in 5b & c make them plural subjects. The
presence of “chá” in 5c is therefore optional. Its absence does not rule out the grammaticality of the sentence as
shown in 5b. One may argue that in 5b the agreement morpheme is suppressed (null). We shall therefore refer to
the clitic “chá” in 5c as an agreement morpheme. It is ungrammatical to use “chá” with a singular subject as
shown in 6.
6) a. *Nwok à nà-èzu-cha oh
man DEM DUR-steal-CL stealing
An interesting question to ask is why we refer to “cha” as a clitic rather than a verbal suffix? This is simply
I. M. Nweze, G. O. Obiamalu
100
because “cha” is syntactically mobile. It can occur elsewhere other than after a verb. Consider 7 below (adapted
from Emenanjo, 2015: p. 261)
7) a. Ndi à chà bìà-rà òriri
PLU DEM CL come-rV feast
“All these came to the feasting”
b. Ndi à bìà-chà-rà òriri
PLU DEM come-CL-rV feast
“All these came to the feasting”
c. Ndi à bìà-rà òriri cha
PLU DEM come-rV feast CL
“All these came to the feasting”
It is interesting to note that “cha” can occur between the verb root and an inflectional suffix. This is evident by
its occurrence before the inflectional rV Tense/Aspect morpheme in 7b. The fact that it can occur elsewhere as in
7a & c leads us to assume that “cha” in 7b is a clitic rather than an extensional suffix. It seems “cha” has no inherent
tone. Its tone is determined by the tone of the peceding segment. The argument for clitichood is outside the scope
of the present paper. (For details of the argument, see Anagbogu, 2001, 2004; Onukawa, 2001; Emenanjo, 2015)
We present below more data from different forms of Igbo constructions to show that “cha” is an optional plu-
ral agreement marker. We shall henceforth gloss “cha” as AGR.
8) a. Àda bụ̀ onye katọlikì
Ada be person catholic
“Ada is a catholic”
b. Àda nà Chik bụ̀ (ndị) katọlikì
Ada and Chike be PLU catholic
“Ada and Chike are catholics”
c. Àda nà Chike bụ̀ -chà (ndị) katọlikì
Ada and Chike be-AGR (PLU) catholic
“Ada and Chike are catholics”
Observe that in 8b, the nominal “ndị” (an inherent plural lexical element) is the only indicator of plurarity. 8c
uses the clitic “cha” to express plurarity hence making “ndi” redundant and so can be deleted. Below are further
examples. We use parenthesis to show the optionality of the “cha” morpheme.
9) a. Ụlọ̀ m bụ̀ ụlọ̀ aj
house 1S be house sand
“My house is a mud house”
b. Ụlọ̀ m nà kè gị bụ̀ (chà) ụlọ̀ aj
house 1S and PART 3S be (AGR) house sand
“My house and your house are mud houses”
10) a.Osisi ogologo dị̀ n’ọhịa
stick long be P forest
“There is long stick in the forest”
b. Osisi ogologo nà osisi mkpụmkpụ dị̀ (chà) n’ọhịa
stick long and stick short be (AGR) P forest
“There are long and short sticks in the forest”
11) a. Nwaànyì à bụ̀ ams
woman DEM be witch
“This woman is a witch”
b. Ụmụ̀ nwaànyì à bụ̀ (chà) ams
PL woman DEM be (AGR) witch
“These woman are witches”
12) a. Ji à rèrè ùre
yam DEM rot rot
“This yam is rotten”
b. Ji ndi à rè-(chà)-rà ùre
yam PL DEM rot-(AGR)-rV rot
I. M. Nweze, G. O. Obiamalu
101
“These yams are rotten”
13) a. Àda lụ̀ -bù-rù di
Ada marry-PRIOR-FACT.PAST husband
“Ada was married/Ada was a married woman”
b. Àda nà kèchi lụ̀ -(chà)-bù-rù di
Ada and Nkecki marry-(AGR)-PRIOR-FACT.PAST husband
“Ada and Nkechi were once married/Ada and Nkechi were married women”
That “cha” in the examples above is an optional agreement clitic and not verbal extensional suffix, could be
demonstrated by its occurrence in othe positions as shown in 14.
14) a. Ụlọ̀ m nà k gị bụ̀ ụlọ̀ aj (cha)
house 1S and PART 3S be house sand (AGR)
“My house and your house are mud houses”
b. Ji ndi à (chà) rè-rè ùre
yam PL DEM (AGR) rot-rV rot
“These yams are rotten”
c. Àda nà kchi lụ̀ -bù-rù di (cha)
Ada and Nkecki marry-PRIOR-FACT.PAST husband (AGR)
“Ada and Nkechi were once married/Ada and Nkechi were married women”
“cha” is an enclitic coming after the verb in 8c, 9b, 10b, 11b, 12b and 13b. It comes after the object NP in 7c,
14a and 14c. It appears after the subject NP in 7a and 14b. In all cases, it functions as a optional plural indicator
in agreement with the plural subject of the sentence.
3.2. Other Agreement Clitics
Apart from “cha” there are some other clitics that function as optional agreement markers. They are “ga” and
“nu”. We shall present examples of their occurrence.
15) a. M/Ị/Okeke bụ̀ onye ìbèribè
1S/2S/Okeke be person foolishness
“I/You/Okeke am/are/is a fool”
b. *M/Ị/Okeke bụ̀ gà onye ìbèribè
1S/2S/Okeke be AGR person foolishness
c. Ànyi/Ụnụ̀ /Okeke nà Okafọ̀ bụ̀ (gà) ndi ìbèribè
1P/2P/Okeke and Okafor be (AGR) people foolishness
“We/You/Okeke and Okafor are fools”
d. Ànyi/Ụnụ̀ /Okeke nà Okafọ̀ bụ̀ ndi ìbèribè (gà)
1P/2P/Okeke and Okafor be people foolishness (AGR)
“We/You/Okeke and Okafor are fools”
15b is ungrammatical because the clitic “ga” does not go with singular subjects. Notice that because 15c has
plural subjects, “ga” can optionally go wiith them. This shows that “ga” is an optional agreement marker. 15d
shows that “ga” is a clitic because it can occur in another position, in this case, word final position.
We present below, instances of ‘nụ’ functioning as agreement marker.
16) a. Bìa ebe à
come place DEM
“Come here”
b. Èmeka bìa ebe à
Emeka come place DEM
“Emeka come here”
c. Èmeka nà Ibè bìa-(nụ̀ ) ebe à
Emeka and Ibe come-(AGR) place DEM
“Emeka and Ibe should come here”
d. Kpèe-nụ̀ ikpe ahụ̀ ọ̀ fụma
judge-AGR case DEM well
“Judge that case well”
I. M. Nweze, G. O. Obiamalu
102
The clitic nụ is a second person plural marker which goes with imperative sentences. Since most imperative
sentences do not have an overt subject as in 16a & d, the nụ clitic serves as the indicator as to whether the covert
subject is singular or plural. In this case it is not optional. Notice that it is optional in 16c where the subject is
overt.
3.3. Verbal Extensional Suffix “sị” as an Agreement Marker
Extensional suffixes are those that cast additional meaning to the roots to which they are affixed. In the words of
Emenanjo (1978: p. 97) “the term extensional’ is used in African linguistics for referring to elements, usually af-
fixes, which function principally as meaning modifiers, that is, extending the meaning of the word with which
they are used”. Uchechukwu (2011) observes that they refer to the suffix components of the verb plus suffix
structures with the suffixes expressing additional ideas. In otherwords, they are those elements that help to ex-
tend the meanings of the roots to which they are attached. Consider the Igbo examples adapted from Nweze
(2011: p. 91) below:
17) a. chụ + mì-chụmì
chase + in chase in
b. chụmì + ta-chụmìta
chase in + towards chase in towards
c. chụmìta + kpọọ-chụmìtakpọọ
chase in towards + complete chase in towards completely
d. chụmì + kọ + rita-chụmìkọrịta
chase in + complete + together together chase all far inside
The examples in 17 show how extensional suffixes can modify or add more meanings to the verb root. Some
of these extensional suffixes such as sị could be used in agreement with the subject or object NP. Consider the
following examples.
18) a. Chike nwè-rè ụgbọàlà
Chike own-rV car
“Chike owns a car”
b. Chike nwè-(sị̀ )-rị̀ ọ̀ tụtụ ụgbọàlà
Chike own-(AGR)-rV(past) many car
“Chike owns a lot of cars”
19) a. Àfọ̀ bụ̀ ahà ahịa ndị Ìgbò
Afo be name market people Igbo
“Afo is the name of an Igbo market day”
b. Àfọ̀ , Èke, kwo na Orìè bụ̀ -(sị̀ ) ahà ahịa ndị Ìgbò
Afo, Eke, Nkwo and Orie be-(AGR) name market people Igbo
“Afo, Eke, Nkwo and Orie are names of market days in Igbo”
20) a. Èmeka chì-tà-rà akwà
Emeka carry-towards-rVpast cloth
“Emeka brought a cloth/some clothes”
b. È̀meka chị̀ -tà-sị̀ -rị̀ akwà
Emeka carry-towards-AGR-rVpast cloth
“Emeka brought some clothes”
21) a. Nwok ahụ̀ mànyè-rè aka n’àkpà
Man DEM put-rVpast hand P pocket
“The man put his hand into his pocket”
b. Ụmụ̀ nwok ahụ̀ mànyè-sì-rì aka n’àkpà
PLU nwoke DEM put-AGR-rVpast hand P pocket
“The men put their hands in their pockets”
The interesting thing to note here is that the morpheme sị is the plural agreement morpheme which tells one
whether the subject or object is singular or plural. In the absence of a plural modifying noun such as ụmụ, ndị,
ọtutụ, it is si that tells us that either the subject or the object is a plural noun as in 20b. The point we are making
here is that while the extensional suffix “si” could be used alone to mark plural, it is still in a way optional since
I. M. Nweze, G. O. Obiamalu
103
the above sentences could be expressed without any overt plural agreement marker. This is illustrated in 22
below.
22) Ụmụ̀ nwok ahụ̀ mànyè-rè aka n’àkpà
PLU nwoke DEM put-rVpast hand P pocket
“The men put their hands in their pockets”
4. Summary and Conclusion
The study focuses on Igbo number agreement. The prompting of the study is the necessity to re-examine the
postulation that Igbo verbs do not inflect to mark agreement with their arguments. It investigates optional
agreement patterns in Igbo constructions to show that some Igbo verbs can be formally marked to reflect the
grammatical number of both their external and internal arguments.
The work using different types of Igbo constructions, explored optional agreement pattern in the language. It
discovers that the verb cliticizes with the morphemes “cha”, “ga” and “nu” to form alternate elements that mark
agreement with their external or internal arguments. Igbo verbs also take some extensional suffixessuch as “sị”
to agree with their arguments.
In conclusion, Igbo language is very rich in its ability to produce options in varying syntactic constructions
and relations. We therefore termed the constituents that mark number agreement between the subjects or the
objects and the verbs as AGR morphemes. The nature of these optional AGR morphemes needs to be explained
theoretically and we shall leave that for another study.
References
Anagbogu, P. N. (2001). A Study of the Igbo Clitic. Nsukka Journal of African Languages and Literatures, 2, 46-54.
Anagbogu, P. N. (2004). Syntax and Semantics of the Clitic: The Case in Igbo. In O.-M. Ndimele (Ed.), Language and Cul-
ture: A Festschrift fo Okon Essien. Aba: NINLAN in Collaboration with Emhai Press.
Anyanwu, O. N. (2007). The Syntax of Igbo Causatives: A Minimalist Account. Landmark Series 2. Port Harcourt: M & J
Grand Orbit Communications Ltd.
Asher, R. E. (1994). The Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics (Vol. 2, p. 840). London: Pergamon.
Crystal, D. (1985). A Dictionary of Linguistics and Phonetics (4th Ed.). Oxford: Blackwell.
Dechaine, R.-M. (1993). Negation in Igbo and Yoruba. In V. Manfredi (Ed.), Niger-Congo Syntax and Semantics, 135-150.
Eka, D. (2001). Elements of Grammar and Mechanics of the English Language. Uyo: Samuf Nig. Limited.
Emenanjo, E. N. (1978). Elements of Modern Igbo Grammar. Ibadan: Oxford University Press.
Emenanjo, E. N. (2015). A Grammar of Contemporary lgbo. Port Harcourt: M & J Grand Orbits.
Ikegwuonu, C. N. (2008). INFL Phrase in Igbo. An MA Dissertation, Department of Linguistics, Igbo and Other Nigerian
languages, Nsukka: University of Nigeria.
Manfredi, V. (1993). Inflection by Default. In V. Manfredi, & K. Reynolds (Eds.), Niger-Congo Syntax and Semantics (Vol.
6, pp. 91-112). Boston, MA: Boston University African Studies Center.
Marantz, A. (1995). The Minimalist Program. In G. Webelhuth (Ed.), Government and Binding Theory and the Minimalist
Program (pp. 4349-382). Cambridge: Blackwell.
Mbah, B. M. (1999). Studies in Syntax: Igbo Phrase Structure. Nsukka: Prize Publishers.
Mbah, B. M. (2006). GB Syntax: Theory and Application in Igbo. Enugu: St. John Afam Publications.
Nweze, I. M. (2009). Clitic Marking in Igbo: A Case Study of Nsukka Dialect Cluster. Nsukka Journal of African Languages
and Linguistics, 5-6, 55-69.
Nweze, I. M. (2011). Extensional Suffixes in Igbo: A Morpho-Syntactic and Semantic Analysis. In C. Uchechukwu (Ed.),
Igbo Language Study Series (Vol. 4, pp. 89-100). Onitsha: Varsity Publishing Company Ltd.
Nweze, I. M., & Ikegwonu, C. N. (2012). Expression of Plurality in Igbo. In O.-M. Ndimele (Ed.) Language, Literature &
Communication in a Dynamic World, a Festschrift for Chinyere Ohiri-Aniche (No. 9, pp. 651-664). Port Harcourt: M & J
Grand Orbit Communications Ltd.
Nzerem, J. K. (2008). Rules of Concord in English Language. Journal of Nigerian Languages and Culture, 10, 257-266.
Obiamalu, G. O. (2010). Agreement by Default and V-Movement in Igbo. In O. Ndimele (Ed.), Language, Policy, Planning
& Management in Nigeria: The Nigerian Festschrift Series (No. 8, pp. 917-924). Port Harcourt: M & J Grand Orbit
Communications Ltd.
I. M. Nweze, G. O. Obiamalu
104
Okeke, C. O. (2008). Reflexivization and Reciprocality in Igbo. Journal of Linguistic Association of Nigria (JOLAN), 11,
185-197.
Onukawa, M. C. (2001). Is the rV Applicative Suffix an Extensional Suffix? In C. Uchechukwu (Ed.), Extensional Suffixes
and Clitics in Igbo: Igbo Language Studies Series (Vol. 4, pp. 15-30). Awka.
Pollock, J.-Y. (1989). Verb Movement, Universal Grammar and the Structure of IP. Linguistic Inquiry, 20, 365-424.
Quirk, R., & Greenbaum, S. (1973). A University of Grammar of English. London: Longman.
Radford, A. (1997). Syntactic Theory and the Structure of English. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139166706
Radford, A., Atkinson, M., Britain, D., Clahsen, H., & Spencer, A. (1999). Linguistics: An Introduction. Cambridge: Cam-
bridge University Press.
Tallerman, M. (2005). Understanding Syntax (2nd ed.) London: Hodder Arnold.
Trask, R. L. (1993). A Dictionary of Grammatical Terms in Linguistics. London: Routledge.
Uchechukwu, C. (2011). Reclassification of Extensional Suffixes. In C. Uchechukwu (Ed.), Extensional Suffixes and Clitics
in Igbo: Igbo Language Studies Series (Vol. 4, pp. 31-62). Awka.
Uwalaka, M. A. (2003). Tense and V-Movement in Igbo. 4th World Congress of African Linguistics, Rutgers, 17-22 June
2003.
Vande-Guma, C., Okebalama, C. N., & Onukawa, M. C. (2013). Projection of the AGR-sP in Tiv Syntax. Nsukka Working
Papers in Language, Linguistics and Literature, 1, 97-107.
Abbreviations and Conventions Used
AGR, Agreement marker
CL, Clitic
DEM, Demonstrative
DUR, Durative Aspect marker
FACT, Factative Aspect marker
P, Preposition
PART, Particularizer
PAST, Past tense marker
PLU, Inherent plural nominal modifier
PRIOR, Priory Aspect marker
rV, verbal suffix where V = vowel of the verb root
1S, first person singular pronoun
2S, second person singular pronoun
3S, third person singular pronoun
1P, first person plural pronoun
2P, secondd person plural pronoun
unmarked vowel (u), High tone
marked with grave accent (ù), Low tone
marked with macron (), Downstep tone