ArticlePDF AvailableLiterature Review

Understanding and encouraging volunteerism and community involvement

Taylor & Francis
The Journal of Social Psychology
Authors:

Abstract

Volunteerism and community involvement have been demonstrated to offer benefits both to communities and to volunteers themselves. However, not every method to encourage these behaviors is equally effective in producing committed volunteers. Drawing on relevant theoretical and empirical literatures, we identify features of efforts that are likely to produce intrinsically motivated other-oriented volunteers and those that may produce extrinsically motivated self-oriented volunteers. In particular, we explore ways to socialize young people to help and ways to build a sense of community focused on particular issues. We also examine requirements for community service and other approaches that highlight self-oriented benefits that volunteers may obtain. Finally, we return to a focus on the importance of intrinsic motivation for promoting sustained involvement in volunteers, even as we acknowledge that volunteers who come with extrinsic or self-oriented reasons can still offer much to communities and can be satisfied when their activities match their motivations.
ENCOURAGING VOLUNTEERISM 1
Understanding and Encouraging Volunteerism and Community Involvement
Arthur A. Stukas Mark Snyder E. Gil Clary
La Trobe University University of Minnesota Kutztown University
Please address correspondence concerning this article to Art Stukas, Department of
Psychology and Counselling, La Trobe University, Bundoora, Victoria 3086, Australia. Tel.
61-3-9479-1515. Email: A.Stukas@latrobe.edu.au.
Stukas, A. A., Snyder, M., & Clary, E. G. (2016). Understanding and encouraging
volunteerism and community involvement. The Journal of Social Psychology, 156,
243-255.
9 February 2016
ENCOURAGING VOLUNTEERISM 2
Abstract
Volunteerism and community involvement have been demonstrated to offer benefits
both to communities and to volunteers themselves. However, not every method to encourage
these behaviors is equally effective in producing committed volunteers. Drawing on relevant
theoretical and empirical literatures, we identify features of efforts that are likely to produce
intrinsically motivated other-oriented volunteers and those that may produce extrinsically
motivated self-oriented volunteers. In particular, we explore ways to socialize young people
to help and ways to build a sense of community focused on particular issues. We also
examine requirements for community service and other approaches that highlight self-
oriented benefits that volunteers may obtain. Finally, we return to a focus on the importance
of intrinsic motivation for promoting sustained involvement in volunteers, even as we
acknowledge that volunteers who come with extrinsic or self-oriented reasons can still offer
much to communities and can be satisfied when their activities match their motivations.
(150 words)
Keywords: volunteerism; community involvement; motivation; intrinsic motivation;
extrinsic motivation.
ENCOURAGING VOLUNTEERISM 3
Understanding and Encouraging Volunteerism and Community Involvement
Every year, millions of people around the world contribute time and effort for
organizations that provide help to people and groups in need, with contributions taking the
form of social support, physical assistance, organizing and advising, ensuring that group
activities can function, or acting on behalf of causes or movements designed to improve
quality of life. In many domains, core activities of people’s lives would be disrupted if
volunteers were not present to provide much needed help; for example, schools, health
clinics, animal shelters and countless other organizations rely heavily on the labor of
volunteers.
There have now been several decades of research on volunteering and community
involvement from a psychological perspective (see Stukas, Snyder, & Clary, 2015, for a
review). The general consensus is that the prosocial actions of volunteers offer numerous
benefits not only to communities but also to volunteers themselves (Snyder, Omoto, &
Dwyer, in press). While we are not saying that volunteering has no costs or downsides or that
all such activities are worth promoting (e.g., Clary & Snyder, 2002), on balance, bringing
people together in ways that enhance and improve the lives of community members has been
shown to be a good thing. Thus, the next step is to find ways to promote community
involvement and volunteering and to determine the factors that influence when and why
volunteer engagement leads to positive rather than to nil or negative outcomes.
In our earlier writing on social marketing” efforts to attract volunteers (e.g., Stukas,
Snyder, & Clary, 2008), we divided potential targets of recruitment efforts into three groups:
those ready to volunteer, those open to good offers to volunteer, and those resistant to
volunteering. Although it is not entirely clear why a person might fall into one or another of
these categories (and it may depend on the particular type of activity on offer), research has
identified numerous personal antecedents of volunteer activity, such as dispositional traits,
ENCOURAGING VOLUNTEERISM 4
interpersonal skills, and demographic characteristics (e.g., Omoto, Snyder, & Hackett, 2010;
or Wilson, 2012, for a review). This research raises the question of whether stable personal
characteristics lead people to volunteer, and whether non-volunteers without these stable
characteristics might be encouraged to get involved. Our earlier focus on people “open to
good offers” suggested that recruitment messages that target the important goals of particular
potential volunteers can work. However, methods to encourage those resistant to volunteering
seemed to have very mixed results. Moreover, not every “good offer” will result in sustained
commitments to volunteering even if volunteers are attracted for the short term.
In this article, we provide a conceptual overview of the factors underlying efforts to
promote volunteerism and community involvement, focused primarily on the ways in which
these efforts influence sustained patterns of volunteering. As we do so, we integrate the seven
articles that appear in this special issue of The Journal of Social Psychology on
“Volunteerism and Community Involvement” with the existing research literature.
Throughout the special issue, the authors investigate factors that make volunteering more
likely, satisfying, or sustained, ranging from informal and formal efforts focused on young
people to the broader motivating influences that lead adults to get involved. In the literature
and in the special issue itself, there is a tension between approaches that rely principally on
the intrinsically motivating aspects of volunteerism and community involvement and those
that rely on extrinsic motivators to encourage service. We hope that our analytic
considerations in this article, as well as the contributions of the articles in this special issue,
will help move this debate toward a resolution.
Socialization
Efforts to develop an ongoing commitment to prosocial behavior might work best if
they begin early, by encouraging young people to engage in such actions. Modelling of
giving and volunteering by parents, including offering opportunities for children to get
ENCOURAGING VOLUNTEERISM 5
involved, can set the stage for later prosocial behaviors by children (e.g., Eisenberg, Eggum-
Wilkens, & Spinrad, 2015; Staub, 2005). For example, McGinley, Lipperman-Kreda, Byrnes,
and Carlo (2010) demonstrated an indirect effect of parents’ own volunteering (and
encouragement of their children) on Israeli teens’ volunteerism; this relationship was
mediated by teens’ feelings of sympathy and self-perceptions of helpfulness. A civic family
orientation (operationalized as the extent to which the family discusses and gets involved
with community and political events) may also contribute to the amount of volunteering that
adolescents do (e.g., Van Goethem, van Hoof, van Aken, de Castro, & Raaijmakers, 2014).
As Luengo Kanacri et al. (this issue) suggest, one pathway to wider civic and community
engagement may be through donations to charities of choice; parents might encourage young
people to use their spending money in this positive way to later good effect. Ensuring that
such actions are attributed internally to prosocial concern and not externally to extrinsic
rewards or pressures would seem to be important in this regard (e.g., Fabes, Fultz, Eisenberg,
May-Plumlee, & Christopher, 1989).
Parents can also encourage prosocial attitudes and behavior through nurturant actions
toward their own children, including displays of warmth, use of authoritative but not
authoritarian parenting styles, and conversations about moral issues (see Eisenberg et al.,
2015, for a review). Resulting feelings of empathy or sympathy for those in need and
internalized prosocial values are likely to boost rates of volunteering and community
involvement (see Stukas et al., 2015). In addition, children who develop secure attachment
styles as a result of their bonds with parents are also more likely to volunteer later in life
(Gillath, Shaver, Mikulincer, Nitzberg, Erez, & Van Ijzendoorn, 2005).
Through socialization, children may learn that helping can make them feel good or
reduce negative feelings (e.g., Cialdini, Baumann, & Kenrick, 1981). These hedonistic goals
for prosocial behavior may relate to the desires for self-enhancement and protection from
ENCOURAGING VOLUNTEERISM 6
negative self-perceptions that have been identified as important motivations for volunteering
(e.g., Clary, Snyder, Ridge, Copeland, Stukas, Haugen & Miene, 1998). Socialization can
also help children to make the transition from normative helping (responding to social
sanctions) to autonomous helping (acting on internalized values), categories identified by
Rosenhan (1970) that are a function of both relations with parents (warm vs. ambivalent) and
parental modeling of helping behaviors (explicit or hypocritical). Yet, even those people who
have not experienced an upbringing that lends itself to internalized and autonomous helping
may be encouraged through specific situational factors and interventions to get involved in
their communities and to persist in their social actions (e.g., Clary & Miller, 1986). So, there
is hope that those who are not dispositionally inclined toward helping or who do not benefit
from childhood environments that encourage such action can be subsequently influenced
toward community involvement.
Sense of Community
The socialization of young people may also include introducing them to existing
communities and helping them to feel a sense of belonging to these groups. Religious
organizations are prominent examples and indeed religious participation has long been
associated with volunteering and community engagement (e.g., Hustinx, van Essen, Haers, &
Mels, 2015; Maton & Domingo, 2006). However, geographical communities such as
neighborhoods, towns, and cities also engage residents, young and old, through events and
opportunities to get involved. As Omoto and Packard (this issue) report, a strong sense of
community is associated with increased volunteering. This sense of community may be
location-based, as in their Study 1 of retirement community dwellers, or it may be a
community focused on an important issue or avocation, as in their Study 2 of environmental
volunteers. Omoto and Snyder (2002, 2010) have outlined how community can be both the
ENCOURAGING VOLUNTEERISM 7
environment (physical or not) in which involvement takes place and also the continually
changing collective process that people build through their efforts and identification.
Building a new community or instilling a sense of community may not be as easy as
introducing new members to thriving existing communities. We already know that many
volunteers are recruited by people from their social networks (Musick & Wilson, 2008;
Omoto & Snyder, 2002); this suggests that levels of social capital are linked to getting
involved, although social capital may be both cause and consequence (e.g., Flanagan, Kim,
Collura, & Kopish, 2015). But how can a sense of community be created or promoted to get
the ball rolling? Omoto and Snyder (2010) have used small-group workshops to foster a
psychological sense of community in people who have an initial interest in, and a concern
for, those affected by HIV and AIDS. Compared to a no-workshop control, their intervention
boosted sense of community and more importantly, boosted intentions to engage in different
forms of social action, such as giving money and volunteering for relevant community
organizations. Similarly, Thomas, McGarty, and Mavor (2015) drew on Kurt Lewin’s (1947)
ground-breaking studies showing that group interactions increased women’s willingness to
serve offal to their families during wartime, and demonstrated how 30-minute discussions of
a United Nations program designed to provide safe water to developing countries could boost
action tendencies and social identification in students. Thus, bringing people together in small
groups and working with them to create a feeling of shared sense of community may boost
their willingness to get involved in prosocial action.
Collaborative tasks can even increase prosocial behavior across previously delineated
group lines by invoking identification with a larger superordinate ingroup (e.g., Dovidio et
al., 1997). Whether it is appropriate to call these new ingroups “communities” is an open
question, but promoting new ways of construing group memberships may be the first step
toward building a broader sense of community. As Stürmer, Rohmann, and van der Noll (this
ENCOURAGING VOLUNTEERISM 8
issue) demonstrated, even efforts to encourage people to identify with as large a group as all
humanity may help to promote volunteerism for global causes. McFarland, Webb, and Brown
(2012) also revealed that an individual difference measure of Identification with All
Humanity predicted willingness to donate money for international relief efforts. Such efforts
may represent a form of “collectivism”, or actions designed to improve the welfare of a
group, one of the primary motivations for community involvement identified by Batson,
Ahmad, and Tsang (2002).
Under ordinary circumstances, however, most people may tend first to their own more
circumscribed ingroups or communities to the extent that they feel or are encouraged to feel
that they identify with them. This preference for ingroup helping may be more common in
longstanding and stable residential communities (e.g., Lun, Oishi, & Tenney, 2012). Thus,
some helping can be a closed system, creating bonding social capital where the social ties are
strong and inward-looking within groups (Putnam, 2000) but not bridging capital where ties
are weaker but span between groups. However, despite its benefits for social inclusion, we
must acknowledge that the promotion of “outgroup helping” can be complicated. When status
relations between groups are unstable, high powered groups may prefer to provide help that
keeps the other group in a position of dependency, whereas low powered groups may prefer
to receive help that promotes autonomy and self-help and may even reject dependency-
oriented help; see Nadler (2015) for a review. Indeed, diversity within communities is often
negatively related to rates of volunteering, possibly because it relates to a lack of trust
between people in different groups (e.g., Rotolo & Wilson, 2014). The implications are that
communities may be easier to build when they are homogeneous and promote ingroup
helping. However a moral imperative to create and sustain communities of people who are
willing to offer autonomy-oriented help to outgroup members in need would yield a host of
benefits both to recipients and to society (see Snyder et al., in press), not least because this
ENCOURAGING VOLUNTEERISM 9
form of help promotes equality, self-sufficiency, and social inclusion, principles that are held
in high regard in democratic societies (e.g., Jiranek, Kals, Humm, Strubel, & Wehner, 2013;
Nadler, 2015).
Service-Learning and Other Requirements to Serve
Some communities may rely on explicit social norms, and even actual social pressure,
to encourage their members to engage; for example, religious communities invoke moral
prescriptions to act benevolently (e.g., Johnson, Memon, Alladin, Cohen, & Okun, 2015).
However, other communities may also decide to use requirements to encourage and even
force involvement and prosocial action. For example, in the United States, residents of public
housing are subject to a Community Service and Self-Sufficiency Requirement (e.g., Seattle
Housing Authority, 2016) that involves eight hours of service (for those not engaged in paid
employment for at least that amount of time) with the objective of improving resident well-
being and self-sufficiency.
The most common use of requirements to engage in community service is in the
educational arena, where service-learning programs can be promoted as offering both benefits
to knowledge and benefits to communities. Indeed, the outcomes of service-learning
programs are varied and there can be no doubt that participants benefit in many ways (see
Stukas, Clary, & Snyder, 1999, and Yorio & Ye, 2012, for reviews). For example, Flanagan
et al. (2015) found that participating in community service activities increased adolescents’
reports of both bonding and bridging social capital and more so than participating in other
extracurricular activities; this benefit did not change depending on whether the service was
mandated or freely chosen. Thus, one benefit of even required activities is to expose students
to new people and to allow them to develop trusting bonds, both within groups and across
groups – this could bolster the development of a new broader psychological sense of
community, a further spur to involvement (as discussed above).
ENCOURAGING VOLUNTEERISM 10
At the same time, other studies suggest that prosocial motivations or intentions to
engage in future community service or volunteering may be undermined by requirements, at
least for a subset of participants. Our own research on “mandatory volunteerism” (Stukas,
Snyder, & Clary, 1999) found that university students who felt that requirements were
controlling or who claimed to be “not ready” to volunteer had lowered future intentions
compared to students who did not find the requirements as intrusive. More recently, in a
longitudinal study of more than 16,000 secondary school students, Horn (2012) found that
students showed increases in prosocial value orientation after engaging in community service
for humanitarian organizations -- but not when they felt explicitly pressured (or even strongly
encouraged) to do so. Institutional requirements to engage in community service were not
effective in boosting the prosocial values of students who were initially egoistic in their value
orientation, but requirements did not undermine the values of those who were originally more
prosocial. However, freely chosen community service had much stronger effects on the
internalization of prosocial values than required service for all students, including those
originally more egoistic. These findings corroborate longitudinal studies showing that
students who were originally positive toward volunteering when required sustained their
service activities further into the future than students who were less positive from the start,
particularly if they had engaged in a lot of service, required or not, during high school (e.g.,
Hart, Donnelly, Youniss & Atkins, 2007; Planty, Bozick, & Regnier, 2006).
To prevent some students from experiencing reactance against efforts to encourage
their involvement, programs promoting community service by educational institutions need
to be implemented with care, as suggested by Dienhart et al. (this issue). Dienhart et al. found
that required programs that were not properly introduced in terms of their rationale and
benefits could be interpreted by students as an extra burden which consequently undermined
their motivation to serve in the future. Thus, more clearly explaining why required
ENCOURAGING VOLUNTEERISM 11
community service programs fit with students’ own goals may be one possible solution. The
functional approach to understanding volunteerism argues that volunteer experiences that
allow participants to satisfy their most important motivations will be the ones that lead to
greater satisfaction and potentially other benefits and those that do not fulfill motives will be
less likely to do so (see Stukas, Worth, Clary, & Snyder, 2009). Therefore the undermining
effects of requirements may be ameliorated if participants can choose their own opportunities
or, better yet, be placed in service activities that are expected to offer benefits that are
matched to students’ primary motivations (but see Houle, Sagarin, & Kaplan, 2005, for a
caveat). Indeed, in a recent study of university business students in a required service-
learning program, Nicholls and Schimmel (2012) demonstrated that the extent to which
students saw their motivations fulfilled in their activities (as assessed by the Total Match
Index; Stukas et al., 2009) was a strong predictor of their future intentions to volunteer,
mediated by both positive attitude change toward service and satisfaction.
Another possibility for avoiding reactance is to increase the amount of “autonomy
support” that is offered by the volunteer placement organization, where efforts are made to
encourage people’s sense that the behaviors to be undertaken are personally desirable and
freely chosen (Gagne, 2003). Perceptions of autonomy support were negatively associated
with volunteer turnover (but only marginally predictive of hours volunteered) in Gagne’s
study of animal welfare volunteers. A further study by Millette and Gagné (2008) examining
volunteer position characteristics demonstrated that a position’s perceived motivating
potential (including perceptions of the variety of activities included, the impact on the lives of
other people, the level of autonomy, and the amount of performance feedback available) was
positively correlated with both volunteers’ satisfaction and supervisors’ ratings of volunteer
performance. Given that perceptions of controllingness and a lack of autonomy by helpers
have been related to reduced intentions to volunteer in the future (e.g., Stukas et al., 1999),
ENCOURAGING VOLUNTEERISM 12
reduced quality of help and reduced well-being for volunteers (e.g., Weinstein & Ryan,
2010), it seems important to ensure that any requirements or even strong encouragements to
engage in community service are tempered by efforts to retain volunteers’ perceptions of
freedom and intrinsic motivation.
External or Extrinsic Motivations to Volunteer
Requirements to engage in community service are but one external factor governing
the initiation of involvement, and in fact, there are many incentives for volunteering (e.g.,
Snyder et al., in press). Benefits for career advancement are a key tangible outcome for many
younger volunteers (see Clary et al., 1998; Snyder et al., 2000). These and other self-oriented
motivations (e.g., esteem enhancement, self-protection) may offer real benefits to volunteers,
particularly if service activities allow volunteers to fulfil these motivations when important
(e.g., Stukas et al., 2009). However, more self-oriented motivations may be associated with
reduced intentions to continue volunteering in the future and with lower psychological and
physical well-being (Gebauer, Riketta, Broemer, & Maio, 2008; Konrath Fuhrel-Forbis, Lou,
& Brown, 2012; Stukas, Hoye, Nicholson, Brown & Aisbett, 2016).
As Güntert, Kals, Strubel, and Wehner (this issue) point out, in line with self-
determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 2000), the extent to which motivations are associated
with feelings of autonomy and self-determination or feelings of control may be one of the
keys for discovering when and why certain motivations produce positive effects and others
do not. Theoretically, any goal can be pursued because it is self-determined and driven by
intrinsic processes, or because of some degree of control exerted through rewards or
punishments, including experiences of guilt or shame. However, extrinsically motivated
activities are typically associated with reduced persistence, as compared to intrinsically
motivated activities (e.g., Deci & Ryan, 2000). Therefore, volunteerism to gain instrumental
ENCOURAGING VOLUNTEERISM 13
self-benefits or to avoid negative emotions might not be sustainable and might not improve
well-being.
One reason why helping that is required, pressured, or controlled does not increase
well-being may be because it has been associated with lower needs-satisfaction (for
autonomy, relatedness, and competence) than helping that is freely chosen and self-
determined (e.g., Weinstein & Ryan, 2010). Yet, as Okun and Kim (this issue) conclude,
there may yet be a positive effect of being pressured into volunteering if this results in high
rates of behavior, at least with regard to purpose in life. Similarly, other concrete benefits
may accrue regardless of volunteers’ motivations, as long as they are afforded by the service
activities involved (e.g., Flanagan et al., 2015). Moreover, external influences to get involved
might only undermine the experiences and benefits of those who perceive the activities and
the pressures used to encourage them negatively (e.g., Horn, 2012; Planty et al., 2006;
Stukas et al., 1999).
In our own studies (e.g., Clary et al., 1998; Stukas et al., 2009), we have focused on
the ways in which the volunteer environment can be tailored to provide motivation-matching
benefits to volunteers and on demonstrating how this can predict greater satisfaction with
volunteering and higher intentions to continue. Following from the functional theories of
attitudes (Katz, 1960; Smith, Bruner, & White, 1959), we have identified six primary
functional motivations to volunteer that we assess with the Volunteer Functions Inventory
(VFI; Clary et al., 1998): to express important values; to increase understanding of the world,
an issue, other people, or one’s own abilities; to experience self-enhancement and positive
feelings toward the self; to fulfil the social expectations of one’s friends, family, and
reference group; to gain a protective benefit against negative feelings and self-beliefs; and to
secure career opportunities and advancement. Moreover, we have demonstrated how an
understanding of the motivations of potential volunteers can assist with the design of
ENCOURAGING VOLUNTEERISM 14
recruitment messages to attract them (e.g., Clary et al., 1998; Clary, Snyder, Ridge, Miene, &
Haugen, 1994). However, we have never explicitly discriminated between self-determined
and controlled motivations, seeing this as an orthogonal and qualitative dimension that might
characterize the same goal differently for different volunteers under different conditions. In
contrast, Güntert et al. (this issue) argue that some of the motivations that we have studied,
particularly the values and understanding motives, are relatively more self-determined than
others.
Other researchers have contrasted self-oriented egoistic and other-oriented altruistic
motivations without invoking self-determination theory. Typically, values motivation is
declared to be the only purely altruistic motivation (e.g., Cornelis, Van Hiel, & De Cremer,
2013; but see Omoto et al., 2010, who include community concern, a seventh prominent
motivation, as altruistic) with all of the other motivations involving some egoistic self-
oriented benefits. However, looser operationalizations that classify social or even
understanding motivation as other-oriented also exist (e.g., Konrath et al., 2012; Stukas et al.,
2016). In contrast, Finkelstein (2009) categorized only career as an external motivation,
judging the other five motivations to be internal, in that they could be satisfied within the
context of the volunteer activity itself. Generally, researchers have found that egoistic, self-
oriented motivations are associated with smaller effects or poorer outcomes but some
exceptions exist. For example, Omoto and Snyder (1995) found that egoistic motivations
were a significant predictor of volunteer longevity whereas more altruistic motivations were
not, suggesting that people with purely altruistic motivations may burn out. Cornelis et al.
(2013) found that self-oriented motivations were important predictors of “in-role” volunteer
behaviors, those that involve living up to the expectations of the role, whereas other-oriented
motivations predicted “extra-role” behaviors that go beyond what is expected. Nevertheless,
ENCOURAGING VOLUNTEERISM 15
promoting volunteering and community involvement by advertising extrinsic rewards and
benefits may result in a mixed bag of effects.
Although we argue that any important motivation that is fulfilled in a volunteer’s
experience should be satisfying and should lead to sustained behavior (e.g., Snyder et al.,
2000), we recognize that there may be constraints on this principle. For example, some
motivations, such as those focused on career or understanding goals, could be satiated by
sustained activities, as when volunteers meet the contacts and develop the skills that enable
them to secure paid employment or when they learn all there is to learn about a particular
issue or experience. Even activities that are matched to and fulfil primary motivations may
not promote ongoing commitments if terminal goals are the sole focus of volunteers. Indeed,
in our studies, volunteers’ satisfaction is generally more strongly influenced than future
intentions to volunteer by the matching of motivations to service activities (e.g., Clary et al.,
1998; Stukas, Daly, & Cowling, 2005; Stukas et al., 2009). Moreover, effect sizes for both
outcomes are typically smaller for matching of self-oriented motivations, particularly career
and protective motivations. Fortunately, in our administrations of the VFI, we have often
found values motivation to be rated most important, and, because volunteer activities are
generally framed in terms of their humanitarian or prosocial goals, this motivation may also
be relatively easy for volunteers to feel that they have fulfilled. As such, volunteers who have
strong needs to express and to act on their personal values may be easiest to attract and to
sustain.
Intrinsically Motivated or Values-based Community Involvement
Thus, we return full-circle to our emphasis on socialization and sense of community,
suggesting to those seeking to promote volunteering and community involvement that interest
in particular issues and specific groups needing help is the main driver of sustained
community service. To the extent that young people can be encouraged to internalize
ENCOURAGING VOLUNTEERISM 16
prosocial values and attitudes and to become involved (or to create) and to identify with
communities focused on social issues and people in need, then we expect volunteerism and
service to be more likely to become a mainstay of their lives. We are mindful that the
assessment of values-related motivation in the VFI focuses specifically on humanitarian and
altruistic values, but the expression of other personal values and beliefs should also lead
people to volunteer (see Shye, 2010), perhaps particularly for organizations that are not
specifically humanitarian in focus. Research that has investigated this issue has generally
found that the self-transcendence values, universalism and benevolence in Schwartz’s
typology (see Schwartz, 2010, for a review), are most associated with volunteer behavior
(e.g., Plagnol & Huppert, 2009). These values focus on enhancing the welfare of a personal
network (benevolence) or the welfare of all people and of nature (universalism); similar
associations have been found with a different measure of values that contrasts non-
materialistic with materialistic values (Kang et al., 2011).
Such broadly applicable values could be expressed by a variety of prosocial
behaviors. So, making it clear to this subset of people (those who are ready to volunteer;
Stukas et al., 2008) that there are ways to get involved may be sufficient to attract them. As
Bekkers and de Witt (2014) have pointed out, awareness of the need for volunteers is often a
sufficient factor in motivating people to get involved, particularly in the case of disaster relief
and other high profile emergencies. However, building a sense of community around
important issues that are intrinsically motivating to people with prosocial values and relevant
attitudes and providing them with behavioral pathways for expressing them may be a more
viable way to promote community involvement. This is the focus of Omoto and Snyder’s
work on the community of people affected by HIV and AIDS (e.g., 1995, 2002, 2010).
A desire to uphold important moral and philosophical principles may also underlie
some decisions to volunteer. For example, Batson et al. (2002) identified “principlism” as a
ENCOURAGING VOLUNTEERISM 17
significant motivation for community involvement; that is, acting on a moral principle, such
as social justice or social responsibility. Jiranek et al.’s (2013) recent inclusion of a social
justice function into the VFI reflects this focus. Creating a community around a moral
principle or on behalf of a group that deserves assistance on moral grounds may be one route
toward attracting and reinforcing those with similar beliefs. Indeed, focusing on the most
disadvantaged within a community and on improving their future outcomes through the
delivery of autonomy-oriented help (e.g., Luengo Kanacri, et al., this issue; Nadler, 2015)
may be an appealing avenue for action for those with prosocial and social justice related
attitudes.
Ensuring the future stability and health of our communities and all members within
them may actually be a crucial motivating aspect of their work for volunteers. Omoto and
Snyder (2010) proposed that people with a strong sense of community act on the belief that
“the community itself is an entity and resource worth sustaining, nurturing, and growing” (p.
237). As such, Maki, Dwyer, and Snyder’s research (this issue) highlighting how taking
future time perspectives can promote continued service may be consistent with such a
motivation. Maki et al. demonstrated that future time orientations may be dispositional but
also that they may be experimentally induced, yielding hope that people may be led to orient
toward prosocial actions that will benefit future members of their community through simple
means. Similarly, recent work by Bain and colleagues (e.g., Bain, Hornsey, Bongiorno,
Kashima, & Crimston, 2013; Milfont, Bain, Souza, Gouveia, & Kashima, 2014) suggests that
people may be most motivated to hold attitudes and to perform actions in the present to the
extent that they believe these actions will promote benevolence traits (morality and warmth)
within their society in the future. Promoting volunteering and community involvement as
pathways to this collective future of greater trust and cooperation, or to other beneficial
futures, may increase willingness to engage in such activities.
ENCOURAGING VOLUNTEERISM 18
Although one might be tempted to think that a focus on the future would be a primary
motivator only for the young, research on the construct of “generativity, a life task focused
on offering mentoring and guidance to the next generation (e.g., Erikson, 1963), suggests that
volunteering can be motivated by this purposefor adults of all ages (e.g., Snyder & Clary,
2004). As Snyder and Clary (2004) point out, generativity could be considered an other-
oriented motivation, similar to value expression, with less of a focus on obtaining immediate
benefits; in fact, some generative actions might be focused proactively on preventing future
problems rather than on addressing those problems already manifested. Snyder and Clary
(2004) and de Espanés, Villar, Urrutia, and Serrat (2015) have found modest correlations
between the motivations to volunteer assessed by the VFI (Clary et al., 1998) and the Loyola
Generativity Scale (McAdams & de St. Aubin, 1992). However, generativity was not strongly
related to values motivation, suggesting instead that it might reflect separate concerns not
encompassed by the VFI. Generativity did not differ across age groups and was a strong
predictor of volunteer commitment in Argentinian volunteers (de Espanés et al., 2015). Thus,
highlighting how volunteers’ contributions may make a difference for future generations may
be another route to recruiting those who may already be inclined to volunteer.
Having the chance to act on one’s important values and principles or to make
contributions to the future of communities may be intrinsically motivating. However, most
definitions of intrinsic motivation focus on the sheer enjoyment of activities themselves;
therefore, simply put, people may choose to volunteer because they find it fun. For example,
Omoto and Snyder’s (1995) research demonstrated that satisfaction was a significant
predictor of volunteer longevity. Vecina et al. (2012) found that volunteer engagement,
assessed as a multifactorial construct comprised of vigor, absorption, and dedication,
predicted satisfaction in new volunteers but ongoing commitment in more established
volunteers. Moreover, people may treat volunteering as a form of “serious leisure” (see
ENCOURAGING VOLUNTEERISM 19
Stebbins, 2015), committing to their activities with the same enthusiasm that they might give
to a career.
Another approach has been to examine volunteers’ varying interests in different types
of activities. Maki and Snyder (in press) have developed a Volunteer Interest Typology (VIT)
that assesses preferences for different activities, including those that focus on animals or the
environment as well as preferences for delivering dependency-oriented or autonomy-oriented
help. Their research suggests that volunteers with different motivations may be attracted to
different ways of volunteering; similarly, Clary, Snyder, and Stukas (1996) and Stukas et al.
(2016) reported motivational differences for volunteers who served different types of
organizations (focused on health, education, sport, young people, etc.) doing different types
of tasks (administrative, mentoring/befriending, fundraising, etc.). Of course, identifying
preferences for certain activities is not equivalent to determining that volunteers find these
activities intrinsically motivating because other extrinsic benefits may be available and
driving such preferences, but the absence of strong interest or engagement in the activities at
hand may suggest a lack of intrinsic motivation. Making certain that activities match the
interests of volunteers seems an important way to keep them motivated (e.g., Maki & Snyder,
in press).
Concluding Comments
Based on our considerations of the relevant theoretical and empirical literatures on
volunteerism and community involvement, we are optimistic that it is possible to build an
engaged society and even to recruit and involve those who are currently resistant to
volunteering. However, we are sensitive to the possibility that methods to encourage
community involvement may potentially result in two different classes of volunteers: those
who are primarily other-oriented and intrinsically motivated and those who are primarily self-
oriented and extrinsically motivated. Although no real harm (and potentially a lot of good)
ENCOURAGING VOLUNTEERISM 20
may be achieved by volunteers who are self-oriented and extrinsically motivated, their
commitment to sustained service may be lower than that of volunteers who are more other-
oriented and intrinsically motivated. However, volunteers with more intrinsic motivation and
other-oriented goals may receive more personal health and well-being benefits as a result of
their service. Therefore, methods that encourage people to develop and to internalize a
compassionate motivation to help others in need of their help may result in the most benefits
for all.
As we have seen, certain types of socialization experiences and encouragements to
build and to identify with communities focused on shared prosocial values and beliefs may
make other-oriented volunteerism more likely. Conversely, terminal extrinsic goals and
explicit requirements may result in self-oriented volunteerism that is not sustained for the
long-term. A fortunate caveat is that some heavy-handed efforts to encourage community
service do not seem to negatively impact those who have internalized altruistic and
humanitarian values. Moreover, the extent to which volunteers see that their motivations and
interests can be (and actually are) satisfied in the activities available to them is likely to be an
important influence on their decision to get involved and to remain involved.
We close, then, on a positive note, after considering relevant exemplars from the
literature that point to productive ways in which volunteering and community involvement
can be promoted to increase benefits for all. We are mindful, however, that a number of the
issues covered here have not been definitively resolved. Therefore, we look forward to a
future of more research on these topics and to its systematic application to promote
community involvement and an engaged society.
ENCOURAGING VOLUNTEERISM 21
References
Bain, P. G., Hornsey, M. J., Bongiorno, R., Kashima, Y., & Crimston, D. (2013). Collective
futures: How projections about the future of society are related to actions and attitudes
supporting social change. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39, 523-539.
Batson, C. D., Ahmad, N., & Tsang, J. (2002). Four motives for community involvement.
Journal of Social Issues, 58, 429-445.
Bekkers, R. & de Witt, Arjen. (2014). Participation in volunteering: What helps and hinders.
A deliverable of the project: “Impact of the third sector as Social Innovation” (ITSSOIN),
European Commission – 7th Framework Programme, Brussels: European Commission,
DG Research.
Cialdini, R. B., Baumann, D. J., & Kenrick, D. T. (1981). Insights from sadness: A three-step
model of the development of altruism as hedonism. Developmental Review, 1, 207-223.
Clary, E. G., & Miller, J. (1986). Socialization and situational influences on sustained
altruism. Child Development, 57, 1358-1369.
Clary, E. G., & Snyder, M. (2002). Community involvement: Opportunities and challenges in
socializing adults to participate in society. Journal of Social Issues, 58, 581-591.
Clary, E. G., Snyder, M., Ridge, R. D., Copeland, J., Stukas, A. A., Haugen, J., & Miene, P.
(1998). Understanding and assessing the motivations of volunteers: A functional
approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1516-1530.
Cornelis, I., Van Hiel, A., & De Cremer, D. (2013). Volunteer work in youth organizations:
Predicting distinct aspects of volunteering behavior from self- and other-oriented
motives. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 43, 456-466.
de Espanés, G. M., Villar, F., Urrutia, A., & Serrat, R. (2015). Motivation and commitment to
volunteering in a sample of Argentinian adults: What is the role of generativity?
Educational Gerontology, 41, 149-161.
ENCOURAGING VOLUNTEERISM 22
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and the “why” of goal pursuits: Human needs
and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227–268.
Dienhart, C. R., Maruyama, G., Snyder, M., Furco, A., Hirt, L., McKay, M. S., & Huesman,
R. (this issue). The impacts of mandatory service on students in service-learning classes.
Dovidio, J. F., Gaertner, S. L., Validzic, A., Matoka, K., Johnson, B., & Frazier, S. (1997).
Extending the benefits of recategorization: Evaluations, self-disclosure, and helping.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 401-420.
Eisenberg, N., Eggum-Wilkens, N. D., & Spinrad, T. L. (2015). The development of
prosocial behavior. In In D. A. Schroeder & W. Graziano (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of
prosocial behavior (pp. 114-136). New York: Oxford University Press.
Erikson, E. H. (1963). Childhood and society. New York, NY: Norton.
Fabes, R. A., Fultz, J., Eisenberg, N., May-Plumlee, T., & Christopher, F. S. (1989). Effects
of rewards on children's prosocial motivation: A socialization study. Developmental
Psychology, 25, 509-515.
Finkelstein, M. A. (2009). Intrinsic vs. extrinsic motivational orientations and the volunteer
process. Personality and Individual Differences, 46, 653-658.
Flanagan, C. A., Kim, T., Collura, J., & Kopish, M. A. (2015). Community service and
adolescents’ social capital. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 25, 295-309.
Gagné, M. (2003). The role of autonomy support and autonomy orientation in prosocial
behavior engagement. Motivation and Emotion, 27, 199-223.
Gebauer, J. E., Riketta, M., Broemer, P., & Maio, G. R. (2008). Pleasure and pressure based
prosocial motivation: Divergent relations to subjective well-being. Journal of Research in
Personality, 42, 399-420.
ENCOURAGING VOLUNTEERISM 23
Gillath, O., Shaver, P. R., Mikulincer, M., Nitzberg, R. E., Erez, A., & Van Ijzendoorn, M. H.
(2005). Attachment, caregiving, and volunteering: Placing volunteerism in an attachment-
theoretical framework. Personal Relationships, 12, 425-446.
Güntert, S. T., Kals, E., Strubel, I. T., & Wehner, T. (this issue). The quality of volunteers’
motives: Integrating the functional approach and self-determination theory.
Hart, D., Donnelly, T. M., Youniss, J., & Atkins, R. (2007). High school community service
as a predictor of adult voting and volunteering. American Educational Research Journal,
44, 197-219.
Hastings, P. D., Utendale, W. T., & Sullivan, C. (2007). The socialization of prosocial
development. In J. E. Grusec & P. D. Hastings (Eds.), Handbook of socialization (pp.
638-664). New York: Guilford.
Horn, A. S. (2012). The cultivation of a prosocial value orientation through community
service: An examination of organizational context, social facilitation, and duration.
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 41, 948–968. doi:10.1007/s10964-011-9714-y
Houle, B. J., Sagarin, B. J., & Kaplan, M. F. (2005). A functional approach to volunteerism:
Do volunteer motives predict task preference? Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 27,
337-344.
Hustinx, L., von Essen, J., Haers, J., & Mels, S. (Eds.) (2015). Religion and volunteering:
Complex, contested, and ambiguous relationships. Cham, Switzerland: Springer
International.
Jiranek, P., Kals, E., Humm, J. S., Strubel, I. T., & Wehner, T. (2013). Volunteering as a
means to an equal end? The impact of a social justice function on intention to volunteer.
The Journal of Social Psychology, 153, 520-541.
ENCOURAGING VOLUNTEERISM 24
Johnson, K. A., Memon, R., Alladin, A., Cohen, A. B., & Okun, M. A. (2015). Who helps the
Samaritan? The influence of religious vs. secular primes on spontaneous helping of
members of religious outgroups. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 15, 217-231.
Kang, C., Handy, F., Hustinx, L., Cnaan, R., Brudney, J. L., Haski-Leventhal, D., Holmes,
K., Meijs, L., Pessi, A. B., Ranade, B., Smith, K., Yamauchi, N., & Zrinsczk, S. (2011).
What gives? Cross-national differences in students' giving behavior. The Social Science
Journal, 48, 283-294.
Katz, D. (1960). The functional approach to the study of attitudes. Public Opinion Quarterly,
24, 163-204.
Konrath, S., Fuhrel-Forbis, A., Lou, A., & Brown, S. (2012). Motives for volunteering are
associated with mortality risk in older adults. Health Psychology, 31, 87-96.
Lewin, K. (1947). Frontiers in group dynamics II. Channels of group life; social planning and
action research. Human Relations, 1, 143–153. doi:10.1177/001872674700100201
Luengo Kanacri, B. P., Gonzalez, R., Valdenegro, D., Jiménez-Moya, G., Saavedra, P., Mora,
E., Miranda, D., Didier, L., & Pastorelli, C. (this issue). Civic engagement and giving
behaviors: The role of empathy and beliefs towards poverty.
Lun, J., Oishi, S., & Tenney, E. R. (2012). Residential mobility moderates preferences for
loyal versus egalitarian helpers. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 291–297.
Maki, A., Dwyer, P. C., & Snyder, M. (this issue). Time perspective and volunteerism: The
importance of focusing on the future.
Maki, A., & Snyder, M. (in press). Investigating similarities and differences between
volunteer behaviors: Development of a Volunteer Interest Typology. Nonprofit and
Voluntary Sector Quarerly. doi: 10.1177/0899764015619703
ENCOURAGING VOLUNTEERISM 25
Maton, K. I., & Domingo, M. R. S. (2006). Mobilizing adults for positive youth
development: Lessons from religious congregations. In E. G. Clary & J. E. Rhodes (Eds.),
Mobilizing adults for positive youth development (pp. 159-175). Springer US.
McAdams, D. P., & de St Aubin, E. D. (1992). A theory of generativity and its assessment
through self-report, behavioral acts, and narrative themes in autobiography. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 1003-1015.
McFarland, S., Webb, M., & Brown, D. (2012). All humanity is my ingroup: A measure and
studies of identification with all humanity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
103, 830-853.
McGinley, M., Lipperman-Kreda, S., Byrnes, H. F., & Carlo, G. (2010). Parental, social and
dispositional pathways to Israeli adolescents' volunteering. Journal of Applied
Developmental Psychology, 31, 386-394.
Milfont, T. L., Bain, P. G., Souza, R. V., Gouveia, V. V., & Kashima, Y. (2014). Examining
how projections about the future of society are related to present-day climate change
action. Psico, 45, 359-368.
Millette, V., & Gagné, M. (2008). Designing volunteers’ tasks to maximize motivation,
satisfaction and performance: The impact of job characteristics on volunteer engagement.
Motivation and Emotion, 32, 11-22.
Musick, M. A., & Wilson, J. (2008). Volunteers: A social profile. Bloomington, IN: Indiana
University Press.
Nadler, A. (2015). The other side of helping: Seeking and receiving help. In D. A. Schroeder
& W. Graziano (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of prosocial behavior (pp. 307-328). New
York: Oxford University Press.
ENCOURAGING VOLUNTEERISM 26
Nicholls, J., & Schimmel, K. (2012). Match/mismatch of the college business student service-
learning experiences: Drivers of perceived attitude change, satisfaction, and future
volunteering intentions. Journal of Higher Education Theory and Practice, 12, 91-99.
Okun, M. A., & Kim, G. Y. (this issue). The interplay of frequency of volunteering and
prosocial motivation on purpose in life in emerging adults.
Omoto, A. M. & Packard, C. (this issue). The power of connections: Psychological sense of
community as a predictor of volunteerism.
Omoto, A. M., & Snyder, M. (1995). Sustained helping without obligation: Motivation,
longevity of service, and perceived attitude change among AIDS volunteers. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 671-686.
Omoto, A. M., & Snyder, M. (2002). Considerations of community: The context and process
of volunteerism. American Behavioral Scientist, 45, 846-867.
Omoto, A. M., & Snyder, M. (2010). Influences of psychological sense of community on
voluntary helping and prosocial action. In S. Stuermer & M. Snyder (Eds.), The
psychology of prosocial behavior: Group processes, intergroup relations, and helping
(pp. 223-243). Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.
Omoto, A. M., Snyder, M., & Hackett, J. D. (2010). Personality and motivational antecedents
of activism and civic engagement. Journal of Personality, 78, 1703-1734.
Plagnol, A. C., & Huppert, F. A. (2009). Happy to help? Exploring the factors associated
with variations in rates of volunteering across Europe. Social Indicators Research, 97,
157-176.
Planty, M., Bozick, R., & Regnier, M. (2006). Helping because you have to or helping
because you want to? Sustaining participation in service work from adolescence to young
adulthood. Youth & Society, 38, 177-202.
ENCOURAGING VOLUNTEERISM 27
Putnam, R. D. (2000). Bowling alone: The collapse and revival of American community. New
York: Simon & Schuster.
Rosenhan, D. (1970). The natural socialization of altruistic autonomy. In J. Macauley & L.
Berkowitz (Eds.), Altruism and helping behavior (pp. 251-268). New York: Academic
Press.
Rotolo, T., & Wilson, J. (2014). Social heterogeneity and volunteering in US cities.
Sociological Forum, 29, 429-452.
Schwartz, S. H. (2010). Basic values: How they motivate and inhibit prosocial behavior. In
M. Mikulincer & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Prosocial motives, emotions, and behavior: The
better angels of our nature (pp. 221-241). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Seattle Housing Authority (2016). Community service requirement. Retrieved from
https://www.seattlehousing.org/residents/information/public/service/
Shye, S. (2010). The motivation to volunteer: A systemic quality of life theory. Social
Indicators Research, 98, 183-200.
Smith, M. B., Bruner, J. S., & White, R. W. (1956). Opinions and personality. New York:
Wiley.
Snyder, M., & Clary, E. G. (2004). Volunteerism and the generative society. In E. St. Aubin,
D. P. McAdams, & Kim, T. (Eds.), The generative society: Caring for future generations
(pp. 221-237). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Snyder, M., Clary, E. G., & Stukas, A. A. (2000). The functional approach to volunteerism.
In G. R. Maio & J. M. Olson (Eds.), Why we evaluate: Functions of attitudes (pp. 365-
393). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
ENCOURAGING VOLUNTEERISM 28
Snyder, M., Omoto, A. M., & Dwyer, P. C. (in press). Volunteerism: Multiple perspectives
on benefits and costs. In A. G. Miller (Ed.), The social psychology of good and evil (2nd
edition, pp. xxx-xxx). New York: Guilford Publications.
Staub, E. (2005). The roots of goodness: The fulfillment of basic human needs and the
development of caring, helping and nonaggression, inclusive caring, moral courage,
active bystandership, and altruism born of suffering. In G. Carlo & C. P. Edwards (Eds.),
Moral motivation through the life span (pp. 33-72). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska
Press.
Stebbins, R. A. (2015). Leisure and the motive to volunteer: Theories of serious, casual, and
project-based leisure. New York, NY: Palgrave Macmillan.
Stürmer, S., Rohmann, A., & van der Noll, J. (this issue). Mobilizing the global community to
combat Ebola: Psychological effects of the Band Aid 30 campaign.
Stukas, A. A., Clary, E. G., & Snyder, M. (1999). Service learning: Who benefits and why.
Social Policy Report: Society for Research on Child Development, 13, 1-19.
Stukas, A. A., Daly, M., & Cowling, M. J. (2005). Volunteerism and social capital: A
functional approach. Australian Journal on Volunteering, 10(2), 35-44.
Stukas, A. A., Hoye, R., Nicholson, M., Brown, K., & Aisbett, L. (2016). Motivations to
volunteer and their associations with volunteers’ well-being. Nonprofit and Voluntary
Sector Quarterly, 45, 112-132.
Stukas, A. A., Snyder, M., & Clary, E. G. (1999). The effects of “mandatory volunteerism”
on intentions to volunteer. Psychological Science, 10, 59–64.
Stukas, A. A., Snyder, M., & Clary, E. G. (2008). The social marketing of volunteerism: A
functional approach. In C. P. Haugtvedt, P. Herr, & F. Kardes (Eds.), Handbook of
consumer psychology (pp. 959-979). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
ENCOURAGING VOLUNTEERISM 29
Stukas, A A., Snyder, M., & Clary, E. G. (2015). Volunteerism and community involvement:
Antecedents, experiences, and consequences for the person and the situation. In D. A.
Schroeder & W. Graziano (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of prosocial behavior (pp. 459-
493). New York: Oxford University Press.
doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195399813.013.012
Stukas, A. A., Worth, K. A., Clary, E. G., & Snyder, M. (2009). The matching of motivations
to affordances in the volunteer environment: An index for assessing the impact of
multiple matches on volunteer outcomes. Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Quarterly, 38,
5-28.
Thomas, E. F., McGarty, C., & Mavor, K. (in press). Group interaction as the crucible of
social identity formation: A glimpse at the foundations of social identification for
collective action. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations.
doi:10.1177/1368430215612217
Van Goethem, A. A., van Hoof, A., van Aken, M. A., de Castro, B. O., & Raaijmakers, Q. A.
(2014). Socialising adolescent volunteering: How important are parents and friends? Age
dependent effects of parents and friends on adolescents' volunteering behaviours. Journal
of Applied Developmental Psychology, 35(2), 94-101.
Weinstein, N., & Ryan, R. M. (2010). When helping helps: Autonomous motivation for
prosocial behavior and its influence on well-being for the helper and recipient. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 222-244.
Wilson, J. (2012). Volunteerism research: A review essay. Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector
Quarterly, 41, 176-212.
Yorio, P. L., & Ye, F. (2012). A meta-analysis on the effects of service-learning on the social,
personal, and cognitive outcomes of learning. Academy of Management Learning &
Education, 11, 9-27.
ENCOURAGING VOLUNTEERISM 30
... wolontariat indywidualny i grupowy lub pośredni i bezpośredni. Inna klasyfikacja wskazuje z kolei na wolontariat stały, akcyjny, zagraniczny i na zasadach stażu lub praktyk (Leszczyński, 2020) czy na wolontariat motywowany wewnętrznie lub zewnętrznie (Stukas i in., 2016). W czasie masowej migracji obywateli Ukrainy do Polski można było w zasadzie obserwować każdy z wymienionych rodzajów wolontariatu. ...
... Wolontariat w swej istocie jest działaniem oddolnym, którego motywacje odnoszą się do kwestii społecznie uznawanych za ważne (Stukas i in., 2014(Stukas i in., , 2016Włodarczyk, 2011). Odkąd w Polsce funkcjonują organizacje trzeciego sektora, wolontariat ulega wielopoziomowej formalizacji. ...
Book
Full-text available
The book deals with the involvement of Polish NGOs in providing support to refugees after Russia's aggression against Ukraine in 2022. At that time, they faced a massive challenge of adapting their activities to the situation and quickly established cooperation with the environment. The authors pointed out that the foundation of this activity was human potential, with volunteers playing a key role. Based on literature studies and empirical research, the authors determined the resource potential of the organizations and its relationship to the scale of assistance provided. The number of Ukrainian citizens supported by Polish NGOs showed a statistically significant relationship with the number of employees of the organization, the level of knowledge among volunteers of the principles of providing professional assistance to refugees, the number of hours devoted to helping refugees, and the number of ways to motivate volunteers. The study fills a gap in the scientific literature on the nonprofit sector, the issue of spontaneous volunteering, and the concept of the resource potential of organizations. The monograph is the first scholarly publication in Poland to quantitatively present the role of NGOs and volunteers in the assistance provided to Ukrainian refugees during the initial period of the war. The book's conclusions are mainly addressed to managers of nonprofit organizations and policymakers responsible for providing a support system for refugees in times of crisis.
... There is a self-fulfilling prophecy effect when a student is led to believe that they will not succeed, and they may unconsciously work to confirm that belief (Stukas & Snyder, 2016). Therefore, a positive and appreciative attitude from the educator towards the balance between multilingualism and the mother tongue is a powerful way to support multilingual children (Herzog-Punzenberger, Vorstman, & Siarova, 2017). ...
Article
This study explores language development in bilingual children aged 3-6 years, focusing on both their native and secondary languages. It highlights educators' understanding of bilingualism's phases and their strategies, such as targeted questioning and interactive activities, to foster language proficiency. Parental insights, gathered through interviews, reveal concerns about language preferences, consistent use, and supporting dual language development. The research emphasizes the importance of creating supportive environments and adapting educational materials to enhance bilingual children's verbal skills. It also advocates for fostering community engagement and peer collaboration to strengthen educator competencies. The findings suggest a need for comprehensive support systems that address both educators' and parents' challenges, facilitating the integration of bilingual children into educational and social contexts.
... Addressing these challenges has become a top priority. Stukas et al. (2016) noted that volunteering is beneficial to both the community and the volunteers themselves, and that the volunteers are more motivated when the work aligns with their own interests. Gibson et al. (2024) mentioned that engaging community members who are not typically involved in community activities but have an interest in gardening or research can help broaden the diversity of the volunteer team. ...
Article
Full-text available
In cities, where people are becoming increasingly disconnected from nature, community green spaces and nature education have been recognized as effective ways to help people reconnect with the natural environment. In this study, we introduce the Mini Botanical Garden (MBG), a model that provides easily accessible natural spaces for community residents. With the support from the government and civil institutions, four MBGs were established in Ningbo city. Each MBG features outdoor plant areas and indoor nature education spaces. Various courses designed for children are offered at the MBGs, covering topics, such as plant and animal identification, nature observation, handcrafts, and writing. To gauge residents' attitudes toward the MBGs, questionnaires were distributed to families within the community. The results showed that the MBGs have positively impacted community environments and the lives of local residents, with all families agreeing that the MBGs provide a place for community members, especially children, to connect with nature. We believe the MBG model offers a practical solution to the problem of nature alienation in cities. ● Practitioner Points ○ Community green spaces offer significant benefits for nature education. Urban planners and nature education organizations should create strategies to maximize their potential. ○ Establishing Mini Botanical Gardens (MBGs) is beneficial for the popularization of urban nature education. They provide a comfortable natural environment and rich natural experiences for local residents while also offering practical venues for various educational activities. ○ The maintenance and daily operation of MBGs require the collaborative efforts of management staff, botanical experts, educators, and community members. This ensures a shared platform for ongoing communication and engagement.
... Such initiatives would promote a sense of belonging and reduce the emotional and social challenges faced by care leavers when forming relationships. Additionally, encouraging community engagement and volunteer opportunities will help care leavers gain a sense of belonging and acceptance, which is also crucial for their social and emotional well-being (Stukas et al., 2016). For instance, partnerships between childcare institutions and nonprofit organizations can be established to provide care leavers with meaningful opportunities to contribute to their communities, enhancing their social integration and self-esteem. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study examines the family formation journeys of care lea-vers in China, focusing on how they plan, navigate, and face challenges during this process. Using convenience and snowball sampling, 19 participants (16 care leavers, 3 care stayers) were interviewed. Thematic analysis identified three family pathways: (i) acquaintance-matched, (ii) self-matched, and (iii) marriage "on the way". The findings reveal ongoing social exclusion and obstacles to family formation, particularly for male care leavers, due to financial pressures, discrimination, and trauma. The study highlights the need for targeted support and social change to improve outcomes for care leavers in China.
Article
Community science, or citizen science, engages trained volunteers with professional researchers to answer a range of environmental ­questions. When buttressed with strong protocols and robust training, community science can be particularly useful in studying migratory fish by expanding the spatial and temporal scale of research. The Hudson River Eel Project is a community science program in which volunteers collect data on juvenile American Eel Anguilla rostrata numbers as the fish enter estuarine tributaries from the ocean. The Eel Project has been designed to (1) assist fishery agency managers in collecting robust data on young-of-year glass eel ingress, and (2) engage diverse audiences in relevant field science that leads to greater ecological stewardship and education. In this article, we describe the initial findings of 14 years of eel monitoring at six sites along the Hudson River estuary, explore how community science efforts can be applied to the needs of fishery agencies, and demonstrate the impact of community science in career development through short testimonials.
Article
In recent years, there has been a growing interest in community‐based art education (CBAE) programmes targeting underprivileged groups. The sustainability of these programmes largely hinges on the level of commitment and engagement demonstrated by the volunteer teachers. Drawing on altruistic and egoistic motivations as the primary components of volunteer behaviour, this study explored the types of motivations and influencing factors driving the involvement of four volunteer teachers in a CBAE programme for rural‐to‐urban migrant children residing in a manufacturing city in China. Using a multiple‐case study design, data were collected over 6 months, from April to September 2022, through semi‐structured interviews, long‐term participant observations and document analysis. The study findings revealed that all participants shared an altruistic motivation to address educational equity and positively impact the lives of migrant children through their engagement in the CBAE programme. In addition to altruistic motivations, two participants exhibited intrinsic motivations, as they derived personal satisfaction from teaching art and sharing the local culture. Meanwhile, the remaining two participants demonstrated extrinsic motivations driven by external factors such as employment demands and career development. The data further indicated that the volunteer teachers' prior artistic experiences, and their current positions and responsibilities, significantly shaped their sustained motivations towards the CBAE programme. Identifying personal and contextual factors that influence motivations to volunteer and leveraging migrant students' Funds of Knowledge are vital for successful CBAE programmes.
Article
This study investigated the relationships among vulnerable narcissism, the use of social network sites (SNSs), self‐esteem, and sense of community among adolescents. Based on a sample of 381 high school students, we explored whether vulnerable narcissism, exacerbated by the use of SNSs and low self‐esteem, contributes to difficulties in establishing community relationships. Hypotheses were tested to examine the associations among narcissism, sense of community, and self‐esteem, with the use of SNSs as a moderating factor. The results indicated a negative relationship between narcissism and sense of community, with self‐esteem mediating this relationship. Furthermore, the low or high use of SNSs moderates the negative relationship between narcissism and sense of community and self‐esteem such that with high use of social media, the relationship becomes positive. These findings suggest that vulnerable‐narcissist adolescents use social sites as facilitators of community relationships and support of their self‐esteem. Conversely, adolescents with low usage are less inclined to support real‐life comparisons, perceiving the community as a dangerous testing ground to avoid. This study highlights the importance of addressing vulnerable narcissism and the use of SNSs in interventions aimed at promoting community involvement and well‐being among adolescents.
Article
This case study focuses on the connections between sea turtles, volunteers, tourists, and other stakeholders in Atlantic Beach, North Carolina in the United States. Background information about sustainable tourism, stakeholder theory, and volunteerism is presented. Then, the dilemma of how to have both sea turtle conservation and sea turtle tourism co-exist is presented, along with complex issues, including those connected with education, training, and volunteerism. The case presents the issues involved with how to develop and maintain a balance between the environmental, socio-cultural, and economic aspects of sustainable tourism in connection with sea turtles.
Article
Full-text available
Because volunteerism is a planned activity that unfolds over time, people who more frequently focus on the future might also be more likely to initiate volunteerism and sustain it over time. Using longitudinal (Study 1) and experimental (Study 2) paradigms, we investigated whether time perspective, and in particular a person's orientation toward the future, is related to volunteers' beliefs and behavior. In Study 1, a person's dispositional level of future time perspective was closely linked to volunteer beliefs and behavior. In Study 2, people who wrote about the future reported higher intentions to volunteer, and this was particularly true for infrequent volunteers and those with lower levels of dispositional future time perspective. Across two studies, we found evidence that future time perspective, whether a chronic disposition or a pattern of thought elicited by someone else, is linked to volunteer beliefs and behavior.
Book
Religion is considered a key predictor of volunteering: the more religious people are, the more likely they are to volunteer. This positive association enjoys significant support in current research; in fact, it could be considered the ‘default perspective’ on the relationship between both phenomena. In this book, the authors claim that, although the dominant approach is legitimate and essential, it nonetheless falls short in grasping the full complexity of the interaction between religion and volunteering. It needs to be recognized that there are tensions between religion and volunteering, and that these tensions are intensifying as a result of the changing meaning and role of religion in society. Therefore, the central aim and contribution of this book is to demonstrate that the relationship between religion and volunteering is not univocal but differentiated, ambiguous and sometimes provocative. By introducing the reader to a much wider landscape of perspectives, this volume offers a richer, more complex and variable understanding. Apart from the established positive causality, the authors examine tensions between religion and volunteering from the perspective of religious obligation, religious change, processes of secularization and notions of post-secularity. They further explore how actions that are considered altruistic, politically neutral and motivated by religious beliefs can be used for political reasons. This volume opens up the field to new perspectives on religious actors and on how religion and volunteering are enacted outside Western liberal and Christian societies. It emphasizes interdisciplinary perspectives, including theology, philosophy, sociology, political science, anthropology and architecture.
Article
This naturalistic study examined differences in students' motivations for elective versus required service-learning (SL) classes. Students in two successive academic years' cohorts were surveyed by the SL center at a large Midwestern university. Analyses compared classes differing in requirements for community-based service. Students required to participate in community service as part of a class within a program required for admission to a university were less likely to: want to be involved in future community work; enroll in another SL class; and recommend their class, compared to other groups of students, including others from classes in which SL was required as part of the program in which students were enrolled. These findings suggest that students' motivations to participate in community-engaged activities are not shaped simply by whether or not community engagement is required in SL classes, but also by other factors including how the engagement opportunity is contextualized.
Article
The current study explores different routes to civic involvement by identifying how a context-specific dimension of empathy and beliefs of autonomy and dependency might jointly predict different types of giving behaviors (i.e., monetary donations), which in turn should predict civic engagement. The sample consisted of 1,294 participants (656 females) between the ages of 18 to 64 (Mage = 38.44, SD = 14.71), randomly selected from seven different cities in Chile. Even after controlling for gender, age, and the socioeconomic status of participants, results mainly support the role of giving behaviors as drivers of actual engagement in civic life. Monetary donations, in turn, are predicted by higher levels of empathy toward poverty and autonomy-oriented beliefs. Implications of these findings are discussed in terms of agentic perspectives on civic participation.
Article
Volunteers' motives have been differentially linked to various aspects of successful volunteering. Using self-determination theory, we propose that volunteer functions are systematically related to the experience of self-determined versus controlled motivation. This "quality of motivation," in turn, explains why motives are differentially associated with satisfaction. We conducted two studies: Study 1 (N1 = 824) addressed motives, quality of motivation, and satisfaction; Study 2 (N2 = 323) additionally examined function-specific benefits and the extent to which they match volunteers' motives. Overall, our hypotheses were supported: values, understanding, and social justice motives were positively associated with relatively self-determined motivation (RSM), whereas career, social, protective, and enhancement motives showed negative correlations. The relationships between motives and satisfaction were partially mediated by RSM. Concerning benefits, Study 2 corroborated these findings for values, protective, enhancement, and social justice. This research introduces a new perspective on the quality of volunteers' motives-with theoretical and practical implications.
Article
Two studies explored psychological antecedents of volunteerism, including several dispositional constructs and psychological sense of community (PSOC). In Study 1, 140 retirees completed measures of empathy, self-esteem, generativity, and PSOC, as well as involvement in volunteer organizations and weekly volunteering hours at two points in time. PSOC predicted concurrent and future volunteerism even after controlling for the other predictors. In Study 2 (n = 427), PSOC and measures of environmental concern and connectedness were used to predict current environmental volunteerism and activism. PSOC was the only measure reliably and uniquely related to these behaviors. Across two different domains and operationalizations of PSOC, the findings support the validity and utility of PSOC for understanding general and issue-specific volunteerism. More generally, they highlight social relationships and psychological connections as potential pathways to volunteerism and social action.
Article
A 1 × 3 experiment (N = 99) investigated the effects of the Band Aid 30 music video on psychological processes underlying the willingness to volunteer to combat Ebola. As expected, exposure to the Band Aid music video moderated the relationships among identification with the national or human community and willingness to volunteer. Identification with the national community was a stronger predictor of willingness to volunteer among participants in the two comparison conditions (i.e., the conditions of no exposure to the music video) than among participants in the music video condition. Conversely, identification with the human community was a stronger predictor of willingness to volunteer among participants presented with the music video than among participants in the two comparison conditions. In addition, two individual orientations emerged as positive predictors, namely, motivation to control prejudice (in the comparison conditions), and empathic concern (in the music video condition). Theoretical and practical implications are discussed.
Article
One developmental task in emerging adulthood is finding meaning and purpose in life. Volunteering has been touted as one role that fosters purpose in life. We examined whether the association between frequency of volunteering and purpose in life varies with pleasure-based prosocial motivation and pressure-based prosocial motivation in a sample of 576 undergraduates, ages 18-22 years old. In a regression analysis predicting purpose in life, the frequency of volunteering by pleasure-based prosocial motivation by pressure-based prosocial motivation interaction effect was significant (p = .042). Simple slopes analyses revealed that frequency of volunteering was not significantly (p = .478) related to purpose in life among college students who were low in both pleasure-based and pressure-based prosocial motivation. The findings of the present study highlight the importance of prosocial motivation for understanding whether emerging adults' purpose in life will be enhanced by volunteering.