DataPDF Available
PLEASE CONTINUE WITH THE ITEMS ON THE NEXT PAGE
Abbreviated POMS (Revised Version)
Name: Date:
Below is a list of words that describe feelings people have. Please CIRCLE THE NUMBER
THAT BEST DESCRIBES HOW YOU FEEL RIGHT NOW.
Not At All A Little Moderately Quite a lot Extremely
Tense 0 1 2 3 4
Angry 0 1 2 3 4
Worn Out 0 1 2 3 4
Unhappy 0 1 2 3 4
Proud 0 1 2 3 4
Lively 0 1 2 3 4
Confused 0 1 2 3 4
Sad 0 1 2 3 4
Active 0 1 2 3 4
On-edge 0 1 2 3 4
Grouchy 0 1 2 3 4
Ashamed 0 1 2 3 4
Energetic 0 1 2 3 4
Hopeless 0 1 2 3 4
Uneasy 0 1 2 3 4
Restless 0 1 2 3 4
Unable to concentrate 0 1 2 3 4
Fatigued 0 1 2 3 4
Competent 0 1 2 3 4
Annoyed 0 1 2 3 4
Discouraged 0 1 2 3 4
Resentful 0 1 2 3 4
Nervous 0 1 2 3 4
Miserable 0 1 2 3 4
Copyright © 1993
J.R. Grove, PhD
The University of Western Australia
Not At All A Little Moderately Quite a lot Extremely
Confident 0 1 2 3 4
Bitter 0 1 2 3 4
Exhausted 0 1 2 3 4
Anxious 0 1 2 3 4
Helpless 0 1 2 3 4
Weary 0 1 2 3 4
Satisfied 0 1 2 3 4
Bewildered 0 1 2 3 4
Furious 0 1 2 3 4
Full of Pep 0 1 2 3 4
Worthless 0 1 2 3 4
Forgetful 0 1 2 3 4
Vigorous 0 1 2 3 4
Uncertain about things 0 1 2 3 4
Bushed 0 1 2 3 4
Embarrassed 0 1 2 3 4
THANK YOU FOR YOUR COOPERATION
PLEASE BE SURE YOU HAVE ANSWERED EVERY ITEM
Citation:
Grove, J.R., & Prapavessis, H. (1992). Preliminary evidence
for the reliability and validity of an abbreviated Profile of Mood
States. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 23, 93-109.
Abbreviated POMS (Revised Version)
*** SCORING KEY ***
Scores for the seven subscales in the abbreviated POMS are calculated by summing the
numerical ratings for items that contribute to each subscale. The correspondence between items
and subscales is shown below.
Item Scale
Not At All A Little Moderate Quite a lot Extremely
Tense TEN 0 1 2 3 4
Angry ANG 0 1 2 3 4
Worn Out FAT 0 1 2 3 4
Unhappy DEP 0 1 2 3 4
Proud ERA 0 1 2 3 4
Lively VIG 0 1 2 3 4
Confused CON 0 1 2 3 4
Sad DEP 0 1 2 3 4
Active VIG 0 1 2 3 4
On-edge TEN 0 1 2 3 4
Grouchy ANG 0 1 2 3 4
Ashamed ERA Reverse-score this item [0 = 4, 1 = 3, 2 = 2, 3 = 1, 4 = 0]
Energetic VIG 0 1 2 3 4
Hopeless DEP 0 1 2 3 4
Uneasy TEN 0 1 2 3 4
Restless TEN 0 1 2 3 4
Can’t concentrate CON 0 1 2 3 4
Fatigued FAT 0 1 2 3 4
Competent ERA 0 1 2 3 4
Annoyed ANG 0 1 2 3 4
Discouraged DEP 0 1 2 3 4
Resentful ANG 0 1 2 3 4
Nervous TEN 0 1 2 3 4
Miserable DEP 0 1 2 3 4
Copyright © 1993-1999, J.R. Grove, PhD
The University of Western Australia
Item Scale
Not At All A Little Moderate Quite a lot Extremely
Confident ERA 0 1 2 3 4
Bitter ANG 0 1 2 3 4
Exhausted FAT 0 1 2 3 4
Anxious TEN 0 1 2 3 4
Helpless DEP 0 1 2 3 4
Weary FAT 0 1 2 3 4
Satisfied ERA 0 1 2 3 4
Bewildered CON 0 1 2 3 4
Furious ANG 0 1 2 3 4
Full of Pep VIG 0 1 2 3 4
Worthless DEP 0 1 2 3 4
Forgetful CON 0 1 2 3 4
Vigorous VIG 0 1 2 3 4
Uncertain… CON 0 1 2 3 4
Bushed FAT 0 1 2 3 4
Embarrassed ERA Reverse-score this item [0 = 4, 1 = 3, 2 = 2, 3 = 1, 4 = 0]
TEN = Tension Note that 2 of the items on the Esteem-related
Affect (ERA) subscale are reverse-scored prior
to being combined with the other items.
Total Mood Disturbance (TMD) is calculated
by summing the totals for the negative
subscales and then subtracting the totals for the
positive subscales:
TMD = [TEN+DEP+ANG+FAT+CON] –
[VIG+ERA].
A constant (e.g., 100) can be added to the TMD
formula in order to eliminate negative scores.
ANG = Anger
FAT = Fatigue
DEP = Depression
ERA = Esteem-related Affect
VIG = Vigour
CON = Confusion
... Participants completed a printed version of the Abbreviated POMS questionn [54]. The POMS allows responses to be segmented into five negative affect sub-scores ( sion, depression, anger, fatigue, and confusion) and two positive affect sub-scores (vig and esteem-related affect [ERA]). ...
... Participants completed a printed version of the Abbreviated POMS questionnaire [54]. The POMS allows responses to be segmented into five negative affect sub-scores (tension, depression, anger, fatigue, and confusion) and two positive affect sub-scores (vigour and esteem-related affect [ERA]). ...
Article
Full-text available
Chronic stress and obesity are major public health concerns and represent significant risk factors for a plethora of non-communicable diseases. Physical exercise represents a valuable health intervention in both cases, providing benefits for mental and physical health, as well as appetite regulation. While the emerging field of ‘green exercise’ suggests that the presence of nature may amplify the benefits of exercise, the quality of evidence has been questioned. To address this, we recruited 22 healthy females to complete a crossover randomised trial comprising a 75 min walk in both a forest and urban area, separated by 2–7 days. Markers of mood (Profile of Mood States), stress (sympathetic-adreno-medullar [resting heart rate, blood pressure] and hypothalamic–pituitary axis activation [salivary cortisol]) and eating behaviour (energy intake and salivary ghrelin) were measured before and after each walk. While both walking interventions improved mood and reduced physiological stress, the nature intervention (but not the urban intervention) also led to further improvements in total mood disturbance, depression, confusion and esteem-related affect (F(1,21) ≥ 4.98, p ≤ 0.037). Salivary ghrelin (F(20) = 0.229, p = 0.637) and energy intake (t(20) = −0.54, p = 0.60) did not respond differently in the two environments. Overall, while walking improved mood and physiological stress in both environments, walking in a forested environment provided additional benefits for mood not seen following the urban walk.
... To evaluate mood state, an abbreviated version of Profile of Mood States (POMSs) 33 was used twice during each experimental day, immediately before and after the 4-hour intervention, respectively. The questionnaire contained 40 items. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: University students often exhibit high levels of sedentary behavior that is negatively associated with cognition and mood. On the other hand, light-intensity physical activity (LIPA) may improve cognitive performance and mood. Therefore, this study investigated the acute effect of LIPA breaks during prolonged sitting on attention, executive functioning, and mood. Methods: A randomized crossover design was used in this study. Twenty-one healthy adults (15 women, age=24 ± 3y, BMI=23 ± 2kg/m2 ) completed three prolonged sitting conditions: (1) without a demanding cognitive task (SIT), (2) with a demanding cognitive task (COGN), and (3) with every 25 minutes sitting interrupted by a 5-minute walk (INTERRUPT). Attention, executive function (response inhibition, task shifting, and working memory updating), and mood were assessed before and after each condition. Results: Linear mixed models analyses showed that prolonged sitting frequently interrupted by LIPA (INTERRUPT) or with cognitively demanding activities (COGN) significantly improved task shifting compared to SIT. However, INTERRUPT did not significantly improve task shifting compared to COGN. No significant acute effects on attention, response inhibition, working memory updating, or mood were found. Conclusions: Frequent LIPA breaks or cognitively demanding activities have a selective, acute positive impact on one aspect of cognitive performance compared to idle sitting. No evidence were found that LIPA breaks have an acute improvement on attention, executive function and mood compared to sitting with cognitive loading. To further investigate the effect of PA on cognitive performance, it is necessary to consider cognitive loading and control for the cognitive activity during sitting in the experimental design.
... The two parts were combined in the evening questionnaire. The POMS questionnaire contains 40 items where five negative subscales and two positive subscales including anger, fatigue, depression, confusion, esteem-related affect, and vigor (Grove and Prapavessis 2016). Total Mood Disturbance (TMD) is calculated by summing the totals for the negative subscales and then subtracting the totals for the positive subscales. ...
Article
The effects of two window states (closed or open) on the bedroom environment and on sleep quality were investigated. Twenty-seven subjects (14 males and 13 females, 20-33 years old) without sleep disorders and chronic diseases participated. The subjects slept for two consecutive nights with windows open and two consecutive nights with windows closed in four dormitory rooms adapted for the purpose of this study, one person at a time. The order of exposure was balanced among participants. Bed temperature, room temperature, relative humidity (RH), carbon dioxide (CO2), particles (PM2.5), and noise were monitored during sleep. Sleep quality was measured using subjective ratings, a wrist-worn sleep tracker, and (for one group of 14 subjects only) polysomnography (PSG) for home use; snoring in this sub-group and awakenings were also registered. Higher PM2.5 and noise levels were found with windows open, while higher room temperature, RH, and CO2 levels were measured with windows closed. There were no differences between conditions in terms of objectively measured sleep stages but the subjects with the PSG attached snored significantly less and woke up significantly less often when sleeping with windows open. Start sleep time, end sleep time, total sleep time (TST) and time in bed (TIB) measured with the sleep tracker were confirmed by the measurements made using PSG, light sleep (N1 + N2) and sleep latency were in moderate agreement but there was no significant agreement for REM and deep sleep (N3). When sleeping with windows open, the subjects rated the air as fresher but reported higher noise levels, feeling less rested, a worse mental state and well-being, and their replies on the Groningen sleep quality scale indicated poorer sleep quality. There was no clear association between the performance test score and sleep quality. These results suggest that sleeping with windows open can provide some benefits by increasing ventilation with outdoor air, reducing CO2 concentrations, improving air quality as indicated by the subjectively rated air freshness and some of the parameters defining sleep quality, but it may also result in some discomfort if there are episodic loud noise events outdoors. Further studies are required to clarify the role of open windows in achieving good sleep quality.
... To explore possible interactions between self-reported affect, tDCS, and performance improvements, participants completed the Profile of Mood States (POMS) prior to stimulation and at the conclusion of the experiment [57,58]. The POMS includes seven unique subscales, Tension, Fatigue, Vigor, Confusion, Esteem-Related Affect, Depression, and Anger. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: After two decades of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) research, it is still unclear which applications benefit most from which tDCS protocols. One prospect is the acceleration of learning, where previous work has demonstrated that anodal tDCS applied to the right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (rVLPFC) is capable of doubling the rate of learning in a visual camouflaged threat detection and category learning task. Goals: Questions remain as to the specific cognitive mechanisms underlying this learning enhancement, and whether it generalizes to other tasks. The goal of the current project was to expand previous findings by employing a novel category learning task. Methods: Participants learned to classify pictures of European streets within a discovery learning paradigm. In a double-blind design, 54 participants were randomly assigned to 30 min of tDCS using either 2.0 mA anodal (n = 18), cathodal (n = 18), or 0.1 mA sham (n = 18) tDCS over the rVLPFC. Results: A linear mixed-model revealed a significant effect of tDCS condition on classification accuracy across training (p = 0.001). Compared to a 4.2% increase in sham participants, anodal tDCS over F10 increased performance by 20.6% (d = 1.71) and cathodal tDCS by 14.4% (d = 1.16). Conclusions: These results provide further evidence for the capacity of tDCS applied to rVLPFC to enhance learning, showing a greater than quadrupling of test performance after training (491% of sham) in a difficult category learning task. Combined with our previous studies, these results suggest a generalized performance enhancement. Other tasks requiring sustained attention, insight and/or category learning may also benefit from this protocol.
... Internal and external factors can contribute to a context-dependent variability in the users' perception of the technology. In this study, participants' mood will be measured using the abbreviated version of the Profile of Mood States (POMS) [13]. In addition, the participants will experience three different challenging simulated driving conditions, including heavy traffic, rainy conditions, and clear, daytime conditions. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
While Fully Automated Vehicles (FAVs) have the potential to significantly expand older adults' access to mobility, limited research has focused on older adults' perceptions of such technology. The current driving simulation-based study will investigate factors that may govern older adults' perceptions of FAVs with respect to trust, acceptability, and safety. Participants (65+) will experience scenarios of manual and fully automated driving in a high-fidelity driving simulator. Their perceptions of the FAV will be measured before and after the driving experiences using questionnaires. Physiological and behavioral data will also be collected throughout the driving sessions to investigate whether positive or negative perceptions of technology are associated with behavioral or physiological responses. In addition, driving performance and driving styles of participants will be captured during manual driving to investigate whether an alignment between an individual's driving style and the FAV driving style will lead to a more positive perception towards FAVs.
Chapter
In the 21st century, with the development of society and the economy, people are paying more and more attention to the special needs of special groups of people, for example, pregnant women’s problem of transportation. As we could imagine, FAV (Fully Automated Vehicles) has the potential to become the measurement of this problem, by improving the safety and flexibility of traffic for pregnant women. However, FAV can only help when women can accept FAV while pregnancy. According to the existed research, Chinese women have less trust in smart driving technology than men. We need to question: why? This article will study on factors affecting pregnant women’s acceptance of fully automated vehicles, by showing the participants fully automated driving videos and playing semi-immersive video simulations. After our work, these theories can counteract the FAV design for pregnant women groups, so that pregnant women’s travel problems can be optimally solved, thereby improving the quality of their life and increasing their employment of FAV and the willingness to buy fully autonomous vehicles. The research in this article aims to explore the relationship between pregnant women’s acceptance of fully automated vehicles, individual internal factors, and weather conditions in the driving environment.
Article
Full-text available
Health indeed is Wealth. The Corona Virus Pandemic has impacted the best health-care systems world-wide; even as the world grapples with the massive loss of human life causing pain to millions. The Pandemic is no more just a medical health challenge; it has thrown up spiritual and emotional challenges as it is affecting our belief system. Prevention is Better than Cure is the mantra. Since, there is no allopathic medicine solution available; people are adopting various measures like social distancing and personal sanitization. There is an urgent need to assist the individuals to take all the preventive measures possible to boost their immunity, improve the respiratory system, and lessen the anxiety, stress, and depression. It can be difficult to not feel anxiety and sometimes panic over the coronavirus outbreak, with the onslaught of updates on news and social media. Yoga has emerged as the perfect tool that an individual can follow to establish physical, mental, and spiritual balance to develop robust health and combat physical and emotional challenges. Yoga offers another path, a fresh perspective with which to deal with events that are beyond our control. The different asanas of yoga can help us to prevent COVID-19 by boosting our immune system and managing the stress that one is going through in this time of uncertainty. This study tries to specifically evaluate the impact of the preventive measures undertaken through Yoga practice. 126 respondents from different states of India were requested to perform yoga daily for 30 days. Significant changes were observed and are mentioned in detail.
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Fully Automated Vehicles (FAVs) have the potential to improve older adults' quality-of-life by enhancing their mobility. Such benefits can only be realized if FAVs are acceptable and thus used by older adults. However, older adults' acceptance of FAVs is reported to be the lowest amongst all Levels of Automation (LoA). The current driving simulation-based study provides preliminary insights into the factors that may be associated with older adults' acceptance of FAVs. Such insights can, in turn, inform user-centered FAV designs that are acceptable for older adults and can thereby enable the benefits of using FAVs by this population. Specific associations that were considered were those between older adults' acceptance of FAVs and internal factors characterizing the individuals, external factors characterizing the driving environment, and FAV features.
Article
The current study explored the effect of sexual content in advertisements and the programme‐advertisement congruence on the memory of sexual and non‐sexual advertisements. Seventy participants (41 females, 29 males) were randomly allocated to one of four conditions. They viewed either ‘The Bachelorette’ (sexual programme) or ‘I’m a Celebrity … Get Me Out of Here’ (non‐sexual programme) with sexual or non‐sexual fragrance advertisements placed within the programme. The order of the advertisements was counterbalanced across participants to control for order effects. Free and cued recall of the advertisements were assessed. As predicted, there was a main effect of advertisement type, with the sexual advertisements better recalled than the non‐sexual advertisements. Contrary to our prediction, the sexual content of the programme did not have an effect on advertisement recall. There was an interaction between the advertisement type and programme type; sexual advertisements were remembered better when embedded within a sexual programme. Limitations of this study are considered.
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any references for this publication.