ArticlePDF Available

A neural link between affective understanding and interpersonal attraction

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Significance Humans interacting with other humans must be able to understand their interaction partner’s affect and motivations, often without words. We examined whether people are attracted to others whose affective behavior they can easily understand. For this, we asked participants to watch different persons experiencing different emotions. We found the better a participant thought they could understand another person’s emotion the more they felt attracted toward that person. Importantly, these individual changes in interpersonal attraction were predicted by activity in the participant’s reward circuit, which in turn signaled how well the participant’s “neural vocabulary” was suited to decode the other’s behavior. This research elucidates neurobiological processes that might play an important role in the formation and success of human social relations.
Confidence-related neural activity in the brain's reward system and individual changes in interpersonal attraction. (A) Brain regions where neural activity during the facial observation period covaried with self-reported confidence. (B) Scatter plot illustrating the correlation between neural activity in the right ventral striatum and self-reported confidence. (C) Scatter plot illustrating the partial correlation between confidence-related neural activity in the right ventral striatum and the observer's postobservation approach behavior (variance that can be explained by preobservation approach behavior is removed). (D) Scatter plot illustrating the partial correlation between confidence-related neural activity in the right ventral striatum and the observer's postobservation approach behavior (variance that can be explained by preobservation approach behavior and general target effects are removed). (E) Brain regions where neural activity during the emotion judgment period covaried with self-reported confidence. (F) Scatter plot illustrating the correlation between neural activity in the mOFC and self-reported confidence. (G) Scatter plot illustrating the partial correlation between confidence-related neural activity in the mOFC and the observer's postobservation approach behavior (variance that can be explained by preobservation approach behavior is removed). (H) Scatter plot illustrating the partial correlation between confidence-related neural activity in the mOFC and the observer's postobservation approach behavior (variance that can be explained by preobservation approach behavior and general target effects are removed). Note: SPMs (height threshold T[51] = 5.5, P = 0.05, FWE-corrected at voxel level in A; height threshold T[51] = 3.2, extent threshold k = 100 voxels, P = 0.001, FWEcorrected at cluster level in E) are superimposed onto a rendered surface and coronal/axial sections of a T1-weighted map of a standard brain (MNI space). For the scatter plots in B and F, trialwise data of each observer (i.e., 96 data points) were z-standardized and rank-ordered according to BOLD parameter estimates and then averaged across observers, separately for each rank. For the scatter plots in C, D, G, and H, targetwise data of each observer (i.e., 6 data points) were z-standardized and rank-ordered according to BOLD parameter estimates and then averaged across observers, separately for each rank. Error bars represent SEMs.
… 
Content may be subject to copyright.
A neural link between affective understanding and
interpersonal attraction
Silke Anders
a,1
, Roos de Jong
a
, Christian Beck
a
, John-Dylan Haynes
b,c,d
, and Thomas Ethofer
e,f
a
Social and Affective Neuroscience, Department of Neurology, Universität zu Lübeck, 23562 Luebeck, Germany;
b
Bernstein Center for Computational
Neuroscience, Charité Universitätsmedizin Berlin, 10115 Berlin, Germany;
c
Berlin Center for Advanced Neuroimaging, Charité Universitätsmedizin Berlin,
10117 Berlin, Germany;
d
Berlin School of Mind and Brain, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, 10099 Berlin, Germany;
e
Department of Biomedical Magnetic
Resonance, University of Tübingen, 72076 Tuebingen, Germany; and
f
Clinic for Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Tübingen, 72076 Tuebingen,
Germany
Edited by Susan T. Fiske, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, and approved February 11, 2016 (received for review August 20, 2015)
Being able to comprehend another persons intentions and emotions
is essential for successful social interaction. However, it is currently
unknown whether the human brain possesses a neural mechanism
that attracts people to others whose mental states they can easily
understand. Here we show that the degree to which a person feels
attracted to another person can change while they observe the
others affective behavior, and that these changes depend on the
observers confidence in having correctly understood the othersaf-
fective state. At the neural level, changes in interpersonal attraction
were predicted by activity in the reward system of the observers
brain. Importantly, these effects were specific to individual observer
target pairs and could not be explained by a targets general attrac-
tiveness or expressivity. Furthermore, using multivoxel pattern
analysis (MVPA), we found that neural activity in the reward system
of the observers brain varied as a function of how well the targets
affective behavior matched the observers neural representation of
the underlying affective state: The greater the match, the larger
the brains intrinsic reward signal. Taken together, these findings
provide evidence that reward-related neural activity during social
encounters signals how well an individualsneural vocabularyis
suited to infer another persons affective state, and that this intrin-
sic reward might be a source of changes in interpersonal attraction.
affective communication
|
confidence
|
intrinsic reward
|
multivoxel
pattern analysis
|
human social relations
Finding the rightcooperation partner is an important task for
individuals living in complex environments that require social
interaction and cooperation. To accomplish a common goal, in-
teraction partners must understand and continuously update in-
formation about their partners current intentions, motivation, and
affect, anticipate the others behavior, and adapt their own behavior
accordingly. From a sociobiological point of view, one thus might
expect that evolution has favored a neural mechanism that permits
individuals to select other individuals as their cooperation partners
whose behavior and communication signals they can easily decode.
However, the neural mechanisms that control human interpersonal
attraction and the selection of cooperation partners are not well-
understood.
Several influential theories in social psychology have stressed the
role of reward in interpersonal attraction (1, 2). The idea is that if a
social encounter with another person is rewarding, then the reward
will become associated with the other person, resulting in in-
terpersonal attraction (24). Until recently, neuroscientific research
into interpersonal attraction has focused mainly on determining the
neural mechanism underlying the evaluation of others based on the
physical attractiveness of their faces (e.g., 511). These studies con-
sistently show that neural activity in the ventral striatum and medial
orbitofrontal cortex (mOFC), core regions of the brainsreward
system that also respond to food and money (12, 13), increases in
response to faces that are perceived as attractive. Other studies show
that these brain regions also respond to another persons prosocial
behavior (1420). Although this research documents the role of the
brains reward system in interpersonal attraction, it does not explain
why social encounters often result in relational effects in interper-
sonal attraction (21) such that one individual is particularly attracted
to one person whereas another individual is more attracted to an-
other person. Here we focus on the role of nonverbal understanding
in interpersonal attraction. Specifically, we ask whether the human
brain possesses a neural mechanism that permits individuals to select
and approach other individuals as interaction partners whose affec-
tive behavior they can easily understand.
Recent work on perceptual learning provides a first hint that the
brains reward system might play an important role not only in
signaling facial attractiveness but also in the individual adjustment
of interpersonal attraction during social interaction. This work
suggests that whenever the brain evaluates sensory information, it
generates a neural signal in the ventral striatum that reflects the
amount of evidence available for stimulus evaluation (22) and that,
at the experiential level, is associated with subjective confidence
(23). Importantly, it has been proposed that such intrinsic confi-
dence signals can act as positive reinforcement signals (24). We
hypothesized that a similar confidence signal, reflecting the amount
of evidence available to decode another personsnonverbalbe-
havior, might serve as an intrinsic reward that becomes associated
with the interaction partner and thereby increases or decreases the
perceivers interpersonal attraction toward the interaction partner
during social encounters.
Considering further evidence from social neuroscience, we
reasoned that an individuals confidence in their understanding of
the others behavior might reflect how well the individualsneural
Significance
Humans interacting with other humans must be able to un-
derstand their interaction partners affect and motivations, often
without words. We examined whether people are attracted to
others whose affective behavior they can easily understand. For
this, we asked participants to watch different persons experi-
encing different emotions. We found the better a participant
thought they could understand another persons emotion the
more they felt attracted toward that person. Importantly, these
individual changes in interpersonal attraction were predicted by
activity in the participants reward circuit, which in turn signaled
how well the participantsneural vocabularywas suited to
decode the others behavior. This research elucidates neurobio-
logical processes that might play an important role in the for-
mation and success of human social relations.
Author contributions: S.A. and R.d.J. designed research; S.A. and R.d.J. performed re-
search; S.A. analyzed data; C.B. provided analysis tools; and S.A., J.-D.H., and T.E. wrote
the paper.
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
This article is a PNAS Direct Submission.
Freely available online through the PNAS open access option.
1
To whom correspondence should be addressed. Email: silke.anders@neuro.uni-luebeck.de.
This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.
1073/pnas.1516191113/-/DCSupplemental.
www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1516191113 PNAS Early Edition
|
1of10
PSYCHOLOGICAL AND
COGNITIVE SCIENCES
NEUROSCIENCE PNAS PLUS
vocabularyis suited to decode and interpret the targets behavior.
Neurobiological accounts of social cognition suggest that when
humans evaluate the inner state of another person, they implicitly
use neural representations of their own states as reference (e.g.,
2528), and empirical studies have provided evidence that is
consistent with this idea (e.g., 2939). Thus, we predicted that an
individuals intrinsic confidence that they correctly understood
another persons affective state would reflect how well the targets
behavior matches the observers neural representation of the
underlying state.
To examine the role of mutual understanding in interpersonal
attraction, we conducted two experiments, a behavioral experi-
ment (experiment I) and a combined behavioralfMRI (functional
magnetic resonance imaging) experiment (experiment II) with two
independent samples of volunteers. Participants in both experi-
ments were shown short video clips of six different female targets
who experienced and facially expressed two different emotions,
fear or sadness. After each video, participants were asked to judge
the targets affective state (fear or sadness) and to indicate how
confident they were that they had correctly understood the targets
affective state (Fig. 1). Before and after emotion observation,
we assessed the participantsinterpersonal attraction toward
each target at two levels. First, we asked participants to enlarge
a small picture of each target on a computer screen by re-
peatedly pressing a button until the picture had a size that
corresponded to a subjectively pleasant conversational dis-
tance.Thenumberofbuttonpressesexecutedtoenlargethe
picture of a given target was taken as measure of the partici-
pants approach behavior toward that target (modified after ref.
6). Next, participants were given three statements about each
target and asked to indicate how much they agreed with each
of these statements (Table 1). This way, we derived a moti-
vationalbehavioral measure (approach behavior) and a self-
report measure of each participants interpersonal attraction
toward each target before and after emotion observation. This
experimental design allowed us to link the participantsconfidence
that they correctly understood the targetsaffective state to
individual changes in interpersonal attraction. In experiment
II, we additionally measured the participantsbrain activity
during emotion observation. This enabled us to identify neural
activity in the brains reward system that predicted the par-
ticipantsself-reported confidence in their emotion judgments
and to link this neural activity to changes in interindividual
attraction. Finally, participants in experiment II completed an
emotion experience task immediately after the emotion ob-
servation task in which they were asked to experience and
express the two emotions (fear and sadness) themselves, using
instructions similar to those that were used when recording
the videos of the targets (37). This permitted us to compare
the patterns of neural activity elicited during emotion obser-
vation to those associated with the observers own emotional
experience. We refer to the level of correspondence between
these patterns as neural observationexperience matching (NOE
matching).
Experiment I tested whether observing another persons
affective behavior can lead to individual changes in interpersonal
attraction, and whether these changes are predicted by the ob-
servers subjective confidence that they correctly understood the
others affective state. Experiment II validated the findings of
experiment I and additionally investigated the neural mecha-
nisms that might mediate between affective understanding and
interpersonal attraction. To this end, we first examined whether
the participantssubjective confidence in their emotion judg-
ments was predicted by neural activity in the reward system of
their brains. Second, we used multivoxel pattern analysis (MVPA)
(40) to examine the relation between subjective confidence, neural
confidence signals, and NOE matching. Both analyses were per-
formed in a cross-validated hierarchical approach, using whole-
brain analyses to identify relevant brain regions, followed by
region-of-interest (ROI) analyses to examine the relation be-
tween neural signals within these regions and individual changes
in interpersonal attraction (Fig. S1).
Table 1. Statements used to assess interpersonal attraction
Original statement (German) Translation
Willingness to meet
Ich würde Sie gerne im echten Leben treffen I would like to meet her in real life
Expectation of intimate communication
Ich habe das Gefühl, dass sie mich verstehen würde I feel that she would understand me
Ich glaube, dass ich mit ihr über persönliche Probleme reden könnte I think I could discuss personal problems with her
Participants were asked to indicate how much they agreed with each statement on a Likert-type 7-point scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (definitely).
The first statement estimated the participants overt willingness to meet the target; the last two questions were averaged to estimate the participants
expectation that they could have an intimate communication with the target.
Fig. 1. Experimental design. To measure changes in interpersonal attraction during emotion observation, the participantsattraction toward each target was
assessed before (Left) and after (Right) emotion observation. (Middle) A sample emotion observation trial is shown (time intervals and screen shots are taken
from experiment II). A trial consisted of a facial observation period during which a short video clip of the target experiencing fear or sadness was shown,
followed by a fixation cross, an emotion judgment period, and a confidence rating period. Responses were given with a button box and fed back to the
observer (orange frame around the selected emotion and orange dot on the confidence scale).
2of10
|
www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1516191113 Anders et al.
Results
Experiment I.
Emotion judgments and self-reported confidence. In the behavioral
experiment, observers (21 women, 19 men) correctly labeled the
targets emotional state in the majority of trials (hit rate 74 ±3.6%
[mean ±SEM], T[39] =21, P<0.001), and the observersaverage
self-reported confidence for a given target closely reflected the
actual correctness of their emotion judgments for this target
(mean r=0.71 ±0.07 [back-transformed mean of Fisher-trans-
formed correlation coefficients], T[39] =9.8, P<0.001). This
indicates that observers had a valid internal model that allowed
them to infer the targetsaffective state and to accurately estimate
the correctness of their understanding. A two-way ANOVA with
between-subject factor observer (40 levels), within-subject factor
target (6 levels), and the observersself-reported confidence as
dependent variable (n=40 ×6×16 =3,840 trials) revealed, in
addition to a significant main effect of target (F[5,195] =95,
P<0.001, eta
2
=0.71), a significant observer-by-target interaction
(F[195,3600] =2.4, P<0.001, eta
2
=0.12). This indicates that an
observers subjective confidence that they correctly understood a
targets affective state did not only depend on the observers
general ability to recognize facial emotions, or the targetsgeneral
ability to express their emotion, but also on how well a particular
observer could tune into a particular targets affect.
Changes in interpersonal attraction. Overall, observers conducted
more button presses to approachthe observed targets after
than before emotion observation (T[39] =3.7, P<0.001; please
see Table S1 for results for self-reported interpersonal attrac-
tion). This is in line with previous research that shows that fa-
miliarity increases interpersonal attraction (e.g., 41). However,
the critical question in the current study was whether emotion
observation can lead to changes in interpersonal attraction that
differ between observers, such that one observer feels more
attracted to a particular target after emotion observation
whereas another observer feels less attracted to the same target,
even though both observers saw exactly the same behavior. In-
triguingly, this was the case. Between-subject variability of the
number of button presses observers conducted to approach a
given target (measured as the width of the 66% interval of button
presses for each target) was significantly larger after emotion
observation (mean width of the 66% interval for each target,
7.4 ±0.6 button presses) than before emotion observation
(mean width of the 66% interval for each target, 6.2 ±0.4
button presses) (T[5] =2.7, P=0.040; Fig. 2; please see Table S1
for results for self-reported interpersonal attraction).
Self-reported confidence and individual changes in interpersonal
attraction. Next, we asked whether the observed changes in in-
terpersonal attraction were predicted by the observersconfidence
that they correctly understood the targetsaffective state. To test
this, we computed partial correlations between each observers
confidence ratings and postobservation attraction scores for each
target. Importantly, to ensure that this correlation was not driven
by the observers initial attraction toward the targets, any variance
that could be explained by preobservation attraction (Table S2)
was removed from confidence ratings and postobservation attrac-
tion scores. This revealed significant positive partial correlations
between the observers confidence and postobservation attraction
both at the behavioralmotivational level (approach behavior)
and at the level of self-report (Table 2). To further control for
potential differences between targets in physical attractiveness
and facial expressivity, we removed average confidence ratings
and average attraction scores for each target (general target
effects,ref. 21) from each individual dataset and performed the
same partial correlation analyses as above. As predicted, partial
correlations between self-reported confidence and self-reported
interpersonal attraction remained significant after general target
effects had been removed (Table 2). This indicates that the link
between confidence and changes in interpersonal attraction
cannot be fully explained by a targets general attractiveness or
expressivity.
Experiment II.
Behavioral data: Emotion judgments, self-reported confidence, and individual
changes in interpersonal attraction. Behavioral data of experiment II
largely replicated those of experiment I. Observers (28 women, 24
men) correctly labeled the targets emotional state in the majority of
trials (mean hit rate 75 ±2.5%, T[51] =30, P<0.001), and their
self-reported confidence closely reflected the actual correctness of
their emotion judgments for a given target (mean back-transformed
r=0.42 ±0.01, T[51] =4.1, P<0.001). Furthermore, there was a
significant observer-by-target interaction in self-reported confidence
similar to that observed in experiment I (two-way ANOVA with
between-subject factor observer [52 levels], within-subject factor tar-
get [6 levels], and the observersconfidence ratings as dependent
variable [n=52 ×6×16 =4,992 trials]; main effect target, F[5,255] =
41, P<0.001, eta
2
=45; observer-by-target interaction, F[255,4624] =
2.9, P<0.001, eta
2
=0.14). As in experiment I, observers
conducted more button presses to approach the observed tar-
gets after than before emotion observation (T[51] =2.7,
P=0.005), and between-subject variability of the number of button
presses observers conducted to approach a given target increased
significantly from preobservation (mean width of the 66% interval
for each target 7.8 ±0.5 button presses) to postobservation (mean
width of the 66% interval for each target 9.1 ±0.5 button presses)
Fig. 2. Interindividual variability in the observersapproach behavior toward the targets before and after emotion observation. Data were first centered,
separately for each target and pre- and postobservation runs (i.e., the mean number of button presses executed for each target in each run was set to 0), and
then averaged across all targets, separately for pre- and postobservation runs.
Anders et al. PNAS Early Edition
|
3of10
PSYCHOLOGICAL AND
COGNITIVE SCIENCES
NEUROSCIENCE PNAS PLUS
(T[5] =4.2, P<0.001; Fig. 2; please see Table S1 for results for self-
reported interpersonal attraction). Finally, the pattern of correla-
tions between self-reported confidence and interpersonal attraction
closely reflected that of experiment I, with partial correlations be-
tween self-reported confidence and self-reported interpersonal at-
traction remaining significant after general target effects had been
removed from each individual dataset (Table 2).
Neural confidence signals in the brains reward system. In the first step
of the fMRI data analysis, we examined whether the observers
subjective confidence that they correctly understood the targets
affective state was associated with neural activity in reward-related
brain regions. For this, we used whole-brain univariate correlation
analyses. First, we computed the correlation between each ob-
servers trial-by-trial confidence ratings and trial-by-trial neural
activity during the facial observation period. This revealed a sig-
nificant positive correlation between self-reported confidence and
neural activity in the right ventral striatum (x=18, y=6, z=15;
T[51] =5.7, P=0.022, familywise error [FWE]-corrected at voxel
level; Fig. 3 Aand B). Second, we computed the correlation be-
tween each observers trial-by-trial confidence ratings and trial-by-
trial neural activity during the emotion judgment period. This
revealed a significant positive correlation between self-reported
confidence and neural activity in the mOFC (x=3, y=39, z=
18; T[51] =4.9, k=503, P<0.001, FWE-corrected at cluster
level; Fig. 3 Eand F) (please see Table S3 for brain regions
outside the reward system where neural activity increased signifi-
cantly with increasing subjective confidence).
ROI analysis: Neural confidence signals in the brains reward system and
individual changes in interpersonal attraction. Having shown that the
observers self-reported confidence reflected neural activity in
the brains reward system, we next asked whether these confi-
dence-related neural signals would predict changes in interper-
sonal attraction. For this, we averaged the neural activity within
the two clusters in the ventral striatum and mOFC, separately for
each observer and target, and performed a partial correlation
analysis between this neural activity and the observers post-
observation attraction scores for each target (with variance
explained by preobservation attraction removed from both var-
iables). This revealed significant positive partial correlations
between confidence-related neural activity and the observers
approach behavior in both clusters (Fig. 3 Cand Gand Table 2).
Again, these partial correlations remained significant when
general target effects (i.e., average levels of confidence-related
neural activity and average attraction scores for each target
across all observers) were removed from each individual dataset
(Fig. 3 Dand Hand Table 2). The only brain region outside the
reward system that showed a similar pattern of partial correla-
tions was the lingual gyrus (Table S4).
To ensure that the observed partial correlations between confi-
dence-related neural activity and postobservation attraction were
not due to the fact that we used the observers confidence ratings
(which we already knew predicted changes in interpersonal attrac-
tion) to identify clusters in the reward system that showed confide nce-
related neural activity, we performed a split-half cross-validation
analysis (42, 43) (see SI Materials and Methods for details). This
analysis replicated the significant partial correlations between con-
fidence-related neural activity and postobservation attraction in the
ventral striatum and mOFC (ventral striatum, mean back-trans-
formed r=0.17, T[51] =1.9, P=0.033; mOFC, mean back-
transformed r=0.23, T[50] =2.9, P=0.003) and the significant
partial correlation between confidence-related neural activity and
postobservation attraction after general target effects had been re-
moved in the mOFC (mean back-transformed r=0.11, T[50] =1.7,
P=0.047). This indicates that the observed correlations between
neural confidence signals in the reward system and interpersonal
attraction cannot be explained by nonindependencies.
NOE matching. In the next step of our fMRI data analysis, we asked
whether the observers self-reported confidence and neural
confidence signals are linked to the degree to which the patterns
of neural activity elicited during emotion observation matched
those associated with the observers own emotional experience.
For this, we used searchlight-based MVPA (44, 45), a technique
that allows estimation of the level of correspondence between
local patterns of neural activity within spherical neighborhoods
(the searchlights) across the entire brain volume (we refer to
this level of correspondence as neural observationexperience
matching; please see Materials and Methods for details). NOE-
matching maps were computed for each trial and observer.
These maps were then subjected to whole-brain correlation
analyses with (i) the observers trial-by-trial confidence ratings
Table 2. Partial correlations between self-reported confidence, neural confidence signals in the brains reward system, neural
observationexperience matching in the anterior insula, and interpersonal attraction after emotion observation
Postobservation attraction
Partial correlations
Residual partial correlations
(general target effects removed)
Approach
behavior
Willingness
to meet
Expected
intimacy of
communication
Approach
behavior
Willingness
to meet
Expected
intimacy of
communication
Measure r T r T r T r TrTrT
Experiment I (n=40)
Self-reported confidence 0.45* (4.5) 0.58* (5.4) 0.61* (7.5) 0.03 (0.3) 0.23* (1.7) 0.24* (1.7)
Experiment II (n=52)
Self-reported confidence 0.44* (4.5) 0.45* (5.2) 0.59* (6.9) 0.17 (1.6) 0.19* (1.7) 0.30* (2.8)
Confidence signals in VS 0.20*
,
(2.2) 0.06 (0.1) 0.12 (1.3) 0.21* (2.0)
Confidence signals in mOFC 0.26*
,
(3.1) 0.03 (0.3) 0.01 (0.1) 0.22*
,
(2.8)
NOE matching (cluster 1) 0.10 (1.2) 0.02 (0.2) 0.04 (0.4)
NOE matching (cluster 2) 0.11 (1.2) 0.00 (0.0) 0.02 (0.2)
Variance that can be explained by the observers initial interpersonal attraction toward the targets is removed from both variables in all analyses. Residual
correlations (i.e., correlations after general target effects are removed from both variables) are only reported for significant main correlations. r, back-
transformed average partial correlation coefficients; T,tvalues at random-effects group level; VS, ventral striatum. Please see Fig. 3 for the location of the
two clusters in the anterior insula.
*Significant correlations (P<0.05, one-tailed).
Correlations that remain significant in the split-half analysis (P<0.05, one-tailed) (see SI Materials and Methods for details).
4of10
|
www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1516191113 Anders et al.
and (ii) the observers trial-by-trial confidence-related neural
activity in the reward system (ventral striatum and mOFC, re-
spectively). This revealed (i) a significant positive correlation
between the observers self-reported confidence and NOE
matching in a cluster in the anterior insula (x=33, y=21, z=
9; T[51] =3.7, P=0.001, FWE-corrected at cluster level) and
(ii) a significant positive correlation between neural confidence
signals in the mOFC and NOE matching in a second, adjacent,
cluster in the anterior insula (x=27, y=30, z=12; T[51] =
4.2, P=0.001, FWE-corrected at cluster level) (Fig. 4; please
note that the actual overlap of the two clusters in the anterior
insula is much larger than shown in Fig. 4 because each voxel
represents a 9-mm spherical searchlight). No significant corre-
lation was observed between confidence-related activity in the
Fig. 3. Confidence-related neural activity in the brains reward system and individual changes in interpersonal attraction. (A) Brain regions where neural activity
during the facial observation period covaried with self-reported confidence. (B) Scatter plot illustrating the correlation between neural activity in the right ventral
striatum and self-reported confidence. (C) Scatter plot illustrating the partial correlation between confidence-related neural activity in the right ventral striatum and
the observers postobservation approach behavior (variance that can be explained by preobservation approach behavior is removed). (D) Scatter plot illustrating the
partial correlation between confidence-related neural activity in the right ventral striatum and the observers postobservation approach behavior (variance that can be
explained by preobservation approach behavior and general target effects are removed). (E) Brain regions where neural activity during the emotion judgment period
covaried with self-reported confidence. (F) Scatter plot illustrating the correlation between neural activity in the mOFC and self-reported confidence. (G) Scatter plot
illustrating the partial correlation between confidence-related neural activity in the mOFC and the observers postobservation approach behavior (variance that can be
explained by preobservation approach behavior is removed). (H) Scatter plot illustrating the partial correlation between confidence-related neural activity in the mOFC
and the observers postobservation approach behavior (variance that can be explained by preobservation approach behavior and general target effects are removed).
Note: SPMs (height threshold T[51] =5.5, P=0.05, FWE-corrected at voxel level in A; height threshold T[51] =3.2, extent threshold k=100 voxels, P=0.001, FWE-
corrected at cluster level in E) are superimposed onto a rendered surface and coronal/axial sections of a T1-weighted map of a standard brain (MNI space). For the
scatter plots in Band F, trialwise data of each observer (i.e., 96 data points) were z-standardized and rank-ordered according to BOLD parameter estimates and then
averaged across observers, separately for each rank. For the scatter plots in C,D,G,andH, targetwise data of each observer (i.e., 6 data points) were z-standardized
and rank-ordered according to BOLD parameter estimates and then averaged across observers, separately for each rank. Error bars represent SEMs.
Anders et al. PNAS Early Edition
|
5of10
PSYCHOLOGICAL AND
COGNITIVE SCIENCES
NEUROSCIENCE PNAS PLUS
ventral striatum and NOE matching (Table S5). As above, a
split-half cross-validation analysis, performed to ensure that the
correlation between NOE matching and confidence-related
neural activity in the mOFC was not due to nonindependencies,
replicated this effect (x=27, y=30, z=12; T[51] =3.5, P=
0.040, FWE-corrected at cluster level).
ROI analysis: NOE matching and individual changes in interpersonal
attraction. Finally, we tested whether NOE matching in the an-
terior insula also predicted changes in interpersonal attraction
directly. Interestingly, this was not the case (Table 2). This is in
line with our hypothesis that NOE matching is not directly as-
sociated with changes in interpersonal attraction but that these
changes are mediated by neural confidence signals in the brains
reward system.
Discussion
The goal of this study was to examine whether the human brain
possesses a neural mechanism that attracts individuals to other in-
dividuals whose nonverbal signals they can easily understand. To
pursue this goal, we conducted two experiments. In line with our
first prediction, data of experiment I and behavioral data of ex-
periment II show that an individuals interpersonal attraction to-
ward another person can change after a few minutes of emotion
observation, depending on the individuals subjective confidence
that they correctly understood the others affective state. fMRI data
from experiment II provide an initial understanding of the neural
processes that mediate between subjective understanding and in-
terpersonal attraction. First, we found that individual changes in
interpersonal attraction were predicted by confidence-related neu-
ral signals in the ventral striatum and the mOFC, core regions of the
brains reward system (12, 13). Second, we found that both the
observers subjective confidence and neural confidence signals in
the mOFC covaried with the degree of similarity between patterns
of neural activity elicited during emotion observation and those
associated with the observers own emotional experience (NOE
matching). This suggests that an individuals confidence in their
interpersonal judgments of affect, and ensuing changes in in-
terpersonal attraction, is partly determined by how well the other
persons affective behavior matches the observers neural rep-
resentation of the underlying state.
Confidence Signals in the Brains Reward System. The first important
finding of the current study is that confidence-related neural activity
in the brains reward system can act as an intrinsic reward signal,
predicting individual changes in interpersonal attraction. Previous
studies have shown that neural activity in the ventral striatum signals
internal confidence when subjects make perceptual judgments
about physical stimuli such as circles, lines, and moving dots (23,
24). The current study shows that neural activity in the ventral
striatum/mOFC covaries with subjective confidence when partici-
pants try to infer another persons current affective state from their
facial expression. This underlines a modality-independent role of
the ventral striatum/mOFC in signaling confidence. Furthermore,
confidence-related neural activity in the ventral striatum/mOFC
predicted changes in the observers interpersonal attraction toward
the target, providing behavioral evidence that confidence-related
activity in the brains reward system can act as a positive
reinforcement signal (24).
Three details of these findings are worth further discussion. First,
we observed a temporal dissociation of neural confidence signals in
the ventral striatum and in the mOFC: Confidence-related neural
activity in the ventral striatum occurred during the facial observa-
tion period of each trial, whereas confidence-related neural activity
in the mOFC occurred later, during the emotion judgment period.
This is in line with previous work that has shown a similar temporal
dissociation of neural activity in the ventral striatum/mOFC during
face evaluation, which has led to the suggestion that the mOFC has
a particular role in holding the outcome of stimulus evaluations
online for further processing (9).
Second, neural confidence signals in both the ventral striatum
and the mOFC were more closely associated with subsequent
changes in the observers approach behavior toward the target
Fig. 4. Neural observationexperience matching (NOE matching), self-reported confidence, and neural confidence signals in the mOFC. (A) Brain regions
where NOE matching covaried with self-reported confidence (orange, cluster 1) and neural confidence signals in the mOFC (red, cluster 2), respectively. (Band
C) Scatter plots illustrating the correlation between NOE matching in each cluster and self-reported confidence (orange)/neural confidence signalsinthe
mOFC (red). Note: SPMs (height threshold T[51] =3.2, extent threshold k=10 voxels, P=0.05, FWE-corrected at cluster level) are superimposed onto a
rendered surface and coronal/axial sections of a T1-weighted map of a standard brain (MNI space). For the scatter plots in Band C, trialwise data of each
observer (i.e., 96 data points) were z-standardized and rank-ordered according to NOE matching and then averaged across observers, separately for each
rank. Error bars represent SEMs.
6of10
|
www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1516191113 Anders et al.
than with changes in self-reported interpersonal attraction,
whereas the observers subjective confidence that they correctly
understood the targets affective state was more closely associ-
ated with changes in self-reported interpersonal attraction. This
is in line with previous studies that have stressed the role of the
ventral striatum in motivated behavior (6) and suggests a partial
dissociation between neural processes underlying motivational
behavioral and cognitive components of interpersonal attraction.
Third, confidence-related activity in the mOFC, but not in the
ventral striatum, reflected the degree of similarity between pat-
terns of neural activity elicited during emotion observation and
those associated with the observers own emotional states (NOE
matching). Again, this supports a particular role of the mOFC in
holding the outcome of stimulus evaluations online for further
processing (9).
Common Codingand Success of Affective Communication. The
second important finding of the current study is that both the ob-
servers subjective confidence and neural confidence signals in the
mOFC reflected the level of correspondence between patterns of
neural activity elicited in the anterior insula during emotion ob-
servation and those associated with the observers own emotional
experience (NOE matching). This extends previous studies that
observed overlapping activity in the anterior insula when partici-
pants experienced and observed pain (46, 47), disgust (30), or joy
(32) and more recent studies that used MVPA to examine whether
ones own pain and emotional experience and another personspain
and emotional experience are encoded in similar patterns of neural
activity (37, 38).
Importantly, the results of the current study not only provide
evidence that confidence can signal correspondence, they also
indicate that confidence and correspondence covary across in-
dividual observertarget pairs. This provides empirical evidence
for theoretical models of social interaction and communication
that propose that the more similar the observers and the targets
internal model of a given behavior, the easier it should be for the
observer to understand the targets inner state and to react ac-
cordingly (25, 48).
A similar link between correspondence of neural activity and
success of communication has been observed in the medial
prefrontal cortex (mPFC). A study using pseudohyperscanning
(a technique where a senderand a perceiverare scanned
one after the other in the same scanner but are connected by
audio or video recordings such that their brain activity can be
temporally aligned after scanning) showed that neural activity in
the mPFC is time-locked between speakers and listeners in-
volved in verbal communication. Strikingly, the listeners se-
mantic understanding of the story told by the speaker varied as a
function of the degree to which neural activity in the mPFC of
the listeners brain at time point t
1
predicted the speakers neural
activity in that region at time point t
2
(49). However, in that
study, all stories were told by a single speaker, so it remains
unclear whether there was a specific listener-by-speaker in-
teraction, similar to the observer-by-target interaction observed
in the current study.
Success of Communication and Interpersonal Attraction. Until re-
cently, neuroscientific research into interpersonal attraction has
been guided by the view that an individuals primary goal when
evaluating other individuals must be to identify potential mating
partners who possess high genetic fitness and fertility (e.g., 11, 50).
This research builds on a large literature that links physical at-
tractiveness to genetic fitness (for a review, see, e.g., ref. 51).
However, for species that live in complex environments that re-
quire social interaction and cooperation to maximize reproductive
success, being able to identify the right cooperation partners might
be equally important. The current study provides evidence that
potential cooperation partners qualify as right not only by their
willingness and competence to cooperate (1417) (for a theoret-
ical account see, e.g., ref. 52) but also by the degree to which their
communication signals can be reliably decoded by the other in-
dividual. Importantly, unlike interpersonal attraction due to a
targets fitness-signaling physical features, which seems to be fairly
consistent across perceivers (53, 54), the confidence-dependent
adjustment of interpersonal attraction found in the current study
seems to be specific for specific interaction partners. Indeed, ob-
servers showed more disagreement about which target they felt
attracted to after than before emotion observation. In social psy-
chology it has long been recognized (21) that attraction between
individuals is not only determined by general target effects (e.g., a
targets physical attractiveness) but also by specific perceiver-by-
target effects (relational effects) such as the match between a
targets affective behavior and a perceivers neural vocabulary
we describe here. Interestingly, it has been suggested that such
interaction partner-specific effects could underlie the forming
of social cliques within larger groups (1, 55). The current study
provides evidence that the brains reward system, signaling
how well ones neural vocabulary is suited to decode another
persons behavior, might play an important role in these social
processes.
Conclusion. In sum, we have shown that subjective understanding
during social interaction can modulate interpersonal attraction. In-
terestingly, the findings of the current study suggest that the neural
mechanisms underlying individual adjustments of interpersonal at-
traction during social encounters might act through internal reward
signals that are partly independent of external feedback, which
makes them perhaps less prone to cheating by potential cooperation
partners. To investigate the interaction between intrinsic confidence
signals and otherhonest or manipulativesignals sent back and
forth between communication partners and to examine the neural
determinants of the dynamics of human social relations in larger
groups (social connectomes) remain challenging tasks for future
studies. The current study suggests that mutual understanding is an
important factor in interpersonal attraction, and that further re-
search into the role of a common neural vocabulary in interpersonal
attraction will lead to a better understanding of the neurobiological
factors that define human social relations.
Materials and Methods
Participants. Forty volunteers (21 women, 19 men, all Caucasian, mean age
22.3 y, range 1830 y) completed experiment I, and 54 volunteers completed
experiment II. In experiment II, data of two participants were discarded
because of estimated head movements >3 mm within a functional imaging
run. The final sample in experiment II comprised 52 participants (28 women,
24 men, all right-handed and Caucasian, mean age 25.3 y, range 1835 y).
Participants reported no history of neurological or psychiatric disorders and
had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. All participants gave written
consent before participation and both studies were approved by the local
ethics committee (Universität zu Lübeck).
Stimuli. Videos of women experiencing fear or sadness were recorded in a
previous fMRI study in which participants were asked to imagine and sub-
merge themselves into a cued emotional situation and to facially express their
feeling to their romantic partner (36). Using prerecorded videos of women
who experienced and facially expressed fear and sadness toward their ro-
mantic partner ensured that all participants saw exactly the same behavior,
and allowed us to exclude the possibility that individual changes in the
participantsinterpersonal attraction toward the targets were due to dif-
ferences in the targets behavior toward different participants. Women
were chosen as targets because women have been shown to express their
emotions more accurately than men (56, 57). For the current study, videos of
fear and sadness of six different women (all Caucasian, age 2025 y) were
selected. Videos were cut into short clips, each covering the first 8 s of a 20-s
emotional period. The final set consisted of 48 different video clips (4 videos
for each target and emotion). Each of the 48 emotion video clips was shown
twice, resulting in a total of 96 emotion observation trials per participant.
For preexperiment familiarization with each target and the assessment of
Anders et al. PNAS Early Edition
|
7of10
PSYCHOLOGICAL AND
COGNITIVE SCIENCES
NEUROSCIENCE PNAS PLUS
interpersonal attraction (see below), a still picture of each target was cut
from the original recordings, showing the targets face during a 20-s
rest period.
Cover Story and Preexperiment Familiarization. Experimental procedures were
similar in both studies (Fig. 1). Upon arrival in the laboratory, participants
were told that the aim of the study was to investigate the relation between
response times and the neural processing of faces and emotional expres-
sions. Participants were then seated in front of a computer screen using
head phones and a chin rest to avoid distraction and to ensure that they
viewed all facial stimuli at the same distance. To support the cover story and
to familiarize participants with the targets, assessment of interpersonal at-
traction was preceded by a motor task in which participants were required
to press one of two response buttons in response to a visual cue (an arrow
pointing to the left or to the right, or a negative or positive word) as quickly
as possible. Response time trials were intermixed with a total of 96 short
presentations (200 ms) of still pictures of each target (16 presentations per
target) and targets were fully balanced over arrows and words, so that after
completion of the motor task, participants were well-familiarized with each
target. Participants in experiment I completed all parts of the study on the
same computer screen, and participants in experiment II completed all parts
except emotion observation and emotion experience (which were per-
formed during fMRI) on the same computer screen.
Assessment of Interpersonal Attraction. First, to assess the participantsin-
terpersonal attraction toward each target at the motivationalbehavioral
level, participants were asked to imagine that they would approach targets,
one after the other, for a casual conversation. At the beginning of each trial,
a small picture of the target appeared on the computer screen (about 40%
of the original picture size) and participants were asked to increase the size
of the picture by repeatedly pressing a button (increase about 4% per
button press, no decrease button) until a pleasant conversational distance
was reached. This task is a modified version of a task originally introduced by
Aharon and colleagues (6) to measure the reward value of faces, except that
the task used in the current study additionally mimics approach behavior by
increasing the size of the target with each button press.
Second, to assess the participantsself-reported attraction toward each
target, they were shown a still picture of each target (1 s) followed by three
statements about their interpersonal attraction toward the target. The
statements were adapted from the social attractionitems in McCroskey
and McCain (58) and related to the participants subjective motivation to
meet the target in real life (willingness to meet) and the participantsex-
pectation that they could have an intimate communication with the target
(expectation of intimate communication) (Table 1). Participants were asked
to indicate how much they agreed with each statement on a Likert-type
7-point visual scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (definitely) by pressing
the corresponding key on a keyboard. In both tasks, targets were presented
in different random orders before and after emotion observation.
Emotion Observation. In experiment I, emotion observation was divided into
four runs. During each run, 24 video clips, balanced across the six targets and
two emotions, were presented in randomized order. Each video clip was
followed by an emotion judgment question [Hat sie Furcht oder Trauer
empfunden?(Did she feel fearful or sad?)]. After the participant had
entered their emotion judgment by pressing the corresponding button on
the keyboard, a confidence question [Wie sicher bist Du, dass sie Furcht/
Trauer empfunden hat?(How confident are you that she felt fearful/sad?)]
and a 5-point visual scale ranging from 1 (I am guessing) to 5 (I am absolutely
sure) appeared on the screen. Responses were entered by pressing the cor-
responding number on the keyboard. The orientation of the scale (increasing
confidence values from left to right or right to left) was balanced across
participants. Each trial terminated with an intertrial interval of 1 s, during
which a fixation cross was shown.
In experiment II, emotion observation was divided into eight runs. During
each run, 12 video clips, balanced across the six targets and two emotions,
were presented in randomized order. Each video clip was followed by a
fixation cross (1 s), an emotion judgment screen (2 s), and a confidence screen
(3 s). The emotion judgment screen showed the words Trauer(sadness) and
Furcht(fear) side by side at the center of the screen, indicating that the
participant should convey their emotion judgment by the response button in
their left or right hand, respectively. The order of emotion words (left or
right) was balanced across targets and emotions within participants. After
the participant had entered their response, an orange frame appeared
around the chosen emotion word, providing the participant with feedback
about their response. The confidence screen showed a five-dot visual scale
ranging from 1 (I am guessing) to 5 (I am absolutely sure). An orange dot at
the central position of the scale indicated the starting position of the cursor.
Participants were asked to move the orange dot to the left or to the right by
pressing the button in their left or right hand, respectively, to indicate their
confidence about their emotion judgment. The orientation of the scale
(increasing confidence from left to right or right to left) was balanced across
participants. Each trial terminated with an intertrial interval of 8 or 10 s,
during which a fixation cross was shown (Fig. 1).
Stimulus presentation and response logging were controlled with Pre-
sentation software (Neurobehavioral Systems).
Emotion Experience. After completion of the emotion observation part,
participants in experiment II participated in four additional fMRI runs during
which they were asked to experience and express fear and sadness them-
selves. Participants were informed that the experimental setup during this
part of the experiment would be very similar to that for the women they had
just observed, except that their facial expression would not be recorded, and
that they would be asked to submerge themselves into frightening or sad
situations and to feel and express their feelings as soon as they saw the
corresponding word [Furcht (fear) or Trauer (sadness)] on the screen (please
see SI Materials and Methods for details). Each run (two runs per emotion)
comprised four emotional periods (20 s) and five interspersed periods (20 s),
during which participants were asked to relax. The order of emotions was
balanced across participants.
MRI Data Acquisition. MRI data were acquired on a 3-T scanner (Siemens
MAGNETOM Trio). A T1-weighted magnetization-prepared rapid gradient-
echo (MPRAGE) image [MPRAGE, 176 sagittal slices, resolution 1 ×1×1mm
3
,
field of view (FOV) 256 ×256 mm
2
, flip angle 8°, inversion time 1,100 ms],
used for spatial normalization of individual data, and a T2-gradient echo
image [39 axial slices per volume, slice thickness 3 mm +1-mm gap, in-
terleaved order, in-plane resolution 3 ×3mm
2
, FOV 192 ×192 mm
2
, echo
time (TE) 1 5.19 ms, TE2 7.65 ms, repetition time (TR) 425 ms], used to
compute individual field maps for correction of image distortions, were
obtained from each participant before functional imaging. One hundred
forty-five T2*weighted echoplanar images (EPIs) covering the whole brain
were acquired during each emotion observation run, and 96 EPIs were ac-
quired during each emotion experience run (35 axial slices per volume, slice
thickness 4 mm +0.4-mm gap, interleaved order, in-plane resolution 3 ×
3mm
2
,FOV192×192 mm
2
, TE 30 ms, TR 2,000 ms, generalized autocalibrating
partially parallel acquisition, factor 2). Functional runs were preceded by five
functional images not included in the analysis to allow for T1 saturation.
Data Analysis. MRI data were preprocessed with SPM8 (Wellcome Department
of Imaging Neuroscience, University College London; www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/
spm/software/spm8/). Preprocessing followed standard procedures and in-
cluded concurrent spatial realignment and correction of image distortions and
normalization into standard Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space (59)
at a spatial resolution of 3 ×3×3mm
3
using DARTEL (60). An additional
receiver coil sensitivity bias correction [using the New Segment tool of SPM8
with very light regularization (0.0001) and 60-mm smoothness] was conducted
after realignment and unwarping that corrected for differences in the scan-
ners bias correction between functional runs that occurred due to a technical
problem.
For the analysis of confidence-related neural activity, individual maps of
parameter estimates were computed for each participant based on a stan-
dard generalized linear model (GLM) that accounted for first-order auto-
correlations and low-frequency drifts (high-pass cutoff period 128 s). BOLD
(blood oxygen level-dependent) activity was modeled separately for each
emotion observation trial (n=96) using box car functions (three per trial),
convolved with a standard hemodynamic response function (hrf) that mod-
eled (i) video onset and duration (8 s), (ii) emotion judgment onset and du-
ration (3 s), and (iii) confidence rating onset and duration (3 s) (the latter were
included as regressors of no interest). For the analysis of neural observation
experience matching (see below), a second set of maps of parameter esti-
mates (n=16, 20-s box car functions convolved with the hrf) was obtained for
the emotion experience runs of each participant.
For the whole-brain analysis of confidence-related neural activity, trial-by-
trial correlation maps (BOLD parameter estimatesself-reported confidence)
were computed for each participant, Fisher-transformed, spatially smoothed
(8-mm isotropic Gaussian kernel), and tested at random-effects group level
(using Tstatistics).
For the analysis of NOE matching, we used a linear support vector machine
(SVM) as implemented in LIBSVM (https://www.csie.ntu.edu.tw/cjlin/libsvm)
with a linear kernel and a hard margin. The searchlight radius was set to
8of10
|
www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1516191113 Anders et al.
9 mm (123 voxels), and the searchlight was moved in steps of one voxel
through the entire brain volume. The classifier was trained on patterns of
neural activity associated with the participants own emotional experience
(n=8 samples per class) and tested on patterns of neural activity elicited
during video observation (n=96). To ensure that classification was based on
multivoxel patterns of neural activity and not on the average level of activity
within a sphere, the spatial mean of each local pattern was set to zero.
Because we reasoned that the classifiers decision confidence (the distance
between test sample and decision border) would provide a more accurate
estimate of the level of correspondence between a test pattern and the
reference patterns than the classifiers decision accuracy alone (which is bi-
nary variable), we computed a measure that reflected both the classifiers
decision accuracy and the classifiers confidence for this decision, the
weighted decision confidence. Mathematically, the weighted decision con-
fidence is the product of the classifiers decision accuracy [which was set to
{1} for correct decisions (classifiers decision matched the participants
judgment) and to {1} for incorrect decisions (classifiers decision did not
match the participants judgment)] and the classifiers decision confidence
(which is defined for a linear SVM as the distance between the test sample
and the hyperplane that separates the two classes).
For the whole-brain analyses of neural observationexperience matching,
three trial-by-trial correlation maps (NOE-matchingself-reported confidence,
NOE-matchingneural confidence signals in the ventral striatum, NOE-matching
neural confidence signals in the mOFC) were computed for each participant. As
above, these maps were Fisher-transformed, spatially smoothed (4-mm isotropic
Gaussian kernel; the small kernel size accounted for the fact that a searchlight-
based SVM already introduces some smoothness into the data), and tested at
random-effects group level (using Tstatistics) separately for each comparison.
Statistical significance of all random-effects statistical parametric maps
(SPMs) was assessed allowing for a probability of false positives of P=0.05,
corrected for multiple tests (familywise errors) across the whole volume
according to random-field theory (61). FWE correction was performed at
voxel level for the ventral striatum and at cluster level (using a height
threshold of T[51] =3.2 corresponding to P=0.001) for all other regions.
This accounted for the fact that clusters of activity were expected to be more
distributed in cortical regions than in the ventral striatum.
For the ROI analyses (i.e., neural confidence signals in the ventral striatum
interpersonal attraction, neural confidence signals in the mOFCinterpersonal
attraction, NOE matching in the anterior insulainterpersonal attraction), tri-
alwise BOLD parameter estimates/trialwise weighted decision confidences
were extracted from the corresponding cluster (identified in the whole-brain
analyses) and averaged separately for each target and participant. For the
cluster in the mOFC, BOLD parameter estimates were extracted from a 3-mm
sphere centered at the peak voxel ([33918]) because the mOFC cluster was
a large cluster that extended into the dorsomedial OFC.
All ROI-based correlation analyses were checked for outliers, defined as
values that deviated more than three times the interquartile range from the
first or third quartile. No outliers were detected in the main analysis, and two
outliers were detected in the split-half cross-validation (indicated by degrees
of freedom less than n1). For all ROI-based analyses, a probability of false
positives of P=0.05 (one-tailed) was accepted unless indicated otherwise,
and exact Pvalues are reported for P<0.200 and P>0.001.
Observer-by-target interactions in self-reported confidence were com-
puted with SPSS (version 22.0.0.1; IBM).
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS. The authors thank N. Dewies, E. Charyasz, and M. Erb
for help with data acquisition, and N. Weiskopf for technical expertise. This
work was funded by Bundesministerium für Bildung und Forschung (German
Federal Ministry of Education and Research) Grant 01GQ1105 (to S.A.).
1. Newcomb TM (1956) The prediction of interpersonal attraction. Am Psychol 11(11):
575586.
2. Byrne D (1997) An overview (and underview) of research and theory within the at-
traction paradigm. J Soc Pers Relat 14(3):417431.
3. Byrne D, Rhamey R (1965) Magnitude of positive and negative reinforcements as a
determinant of attraction. J Pers Soc Psychol 2(6):884889.
4. Gouaux C, Summers K (1973) Interpersonal attraction as a function of affective state
and affective change. J Res Pers 7(3):254260.
5. Nakamura K, et al. (1999) Activation of the right inferior frontal cortex during as-
sessment of facial emotion. J Neurophysiol 82(3):16101614.
6. Aharon I, et al. (2001) Beautiful faces have variable reward value: fMRI and behavioral
evidence. Neuron 32(3):537551.
7. ODoherty J, et al. (2003) Beauty in a smile: The role of medial orbitofrontal cortex in
facial attractiveness. Neuropsychologia 41(2):147155.
8. Kampe KKW, Frith CD, Dolan RJ, Frith U (2001) Reward value of attractiveness and
gaze. Nature 413(6856):589.
9. Kim H, Adolphs R, ODoh erty JP, Shimojo S (2007) Temp oral isolation of neural
processes underl ying face preference deci sions. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 10 4(46):
1825318258.
10. Cloutier J, Heatherton TF, Whalen PJ, Kelley WM (2008) Are attractive people re-
warding? Sex differences in the neural substrates of facial attractiveness. J Cogn
Neurosci 20(6):941951.
11. Bzdok D, et al. (2011) ALE meta-analysis on facial judgments of trustworthiness and
attractiveness. Brain Struct Funct 215(3-4):209223.
12. Haber SN, Knutson B (2010) The reward circuit: Linking primate anatomy and human
imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology 35(1):426.
13. Bartra O, McGuire JT, Kable JW (2013) The valuation system: A coordinate-based
meta-analysis of BOLD fMRI experiments examining neural correlates of subjective
value. Neuroimage 76:412427.
14. Rilling J, et al. (2002) A neural basis for social cooperation. Neuron 35(2):395405.
15. Rilling JK, Sanfey AG, Aronson JA, Nystrom LE, Cohen JD (2004) Opposing BOLD re-
sponses to reciprocated and unreciprocated altruism in putative reward pathways.
Neuroreport 15(16):25392543.
16. Tabibnia G, Satpute AB, Lieberman MD (2008) The sunny side of fairness: Preference
for fairness activates reward circuitry (and disregarding unfairness activates self-
control circuitry). Psychol Sci 19(4):339347.
17. Phan KL, Sripada CS, Angstadt M, McCabe K (2010) Reputation for reciprocity en-
gages the brain reward center. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 107(29):1309913104.
18. Jones RM, et al. (2011) Behavioral and neural properties of social reinforcement
learning. J Neurosci 31(37):1303913045.
19. Korn CW, Prehn K, Park SQ, Walter H, Heekeren HR (2012) Positively biased pro-
cessing of self-relevant social feedback. J Neurosci 32(47):1683216844.
20. Bhanji JP, Delgado MR (2014) The social brain and reward: Social information pro-
cessing in the human striatum. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci 5(1):6173.
21. Kenny DA, West TV, Malloy TE, Albright L (2006) Componential analysis of in-
terpersonal perception data. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 10(4):282294.
22. Kepecs A, Uchida N, Zariwala HA, Mainen ZF (2008) Neural correlates, computation
and behavioural impact of decision confidence. Nature 455(7210):227231.
23. Hebart MN, Schriever Y, Donner TH, Haynes J-D (2016) The relationship between
perceptual decision variables and confidence in the human brain. Cereb Cortex 26(1):
118130.
24. Daniel R, Pollmann S (2012) Striatal activations signal prediction errors on confidence
in the absence of external feedback. Neuroimage 59(4):34573467.
25. Gallese V (2003) The manifold nature of interpersonal relations: The quest for a
common mechanism. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 358(1431):517528.
26. Decety J, Jackson PL (2004) The functional architecture of human empathy. Behav
Cogn Neurosci Rev 3(2):71100.
27. Bastiaansen JACJ, Thioux M, Keysers C (2009) Evidence for mirror systems in emotions.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 364(1528):23912404.
28. Iacoboni M (2009) Imitation, empathy, and mirror neurons. Annu Rev Psychol 60:
653670.
29. Adolphs R, Damasio H, Tranel D, Cooper G, Damasio AR (2000) A role for somato-
sensory cortices in the visual recognition of emotion as revealed by three-dimensional
lesion mapping. J Neurosci 20(7):26832690.
30. Carr L, Iacoboni M, Dubeau M-C, Mazziotta JC, Lenzi GL (2003) Neural mechanisms of
empathy in humans: A relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic areas. Proc
Natl Acad Sci USA 100(9):54975502.
31. Wicker B, et al. (2003) Both of us disgusted in My insula: The common neural basis of
seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron 40(3):655664.
32. Pourtois G, et al. (2004) Dissociable roles of the human somatosensory and superior
temporal cortices for processing social face signals. Eur J Neurosci 20(12):35073515.
33. Hennenlotter A, et al. (2005) A common neural basis for receptive and expressive
communication of pleasant facial affect. Neuroimage 26(2):581591.
34. Mitchell JP, Banaji MR, Macrae CN (2005) The link between social cognition and self-
referential thought in the medial prefrontal cortex. J Cogn Neurosci 17(8):13061315.
35. Mitchell JP, Macrae CN, Banaji MR (2006) Dissociable medial prefrontal contributions
to judgments of similar and dissimilar others. Neuron 50(4):655663.
36. Buchanan TW, Bibas D, Adolphs R (2010) Associations between feeling and judging
the emotions of happiness and fear: Findings from a large-scale field experiment.
PLoS One 5(5):e10640.
37. Anders S, Heinzle J, Weiskopf N, Ethofer T, Haynes J-D (2011) Flow of affective in-
formation between communicating brains. Neuroimage 54(1):439446.
38. Corradi-DellAcqua C, Hofstetter C, Vuilleumier P (2011) Felt and seen pain evoke the
same local patterns of cortical activity in insular and cingulate cortex. J Neurosci
31(49):1799618006.
39. Lorey B, et al. (2012) Confidence in emotion perception in point-light displays varies
with the ability to perceive own emotions. PLoS One 7(8):e42169.
40. Haxby JV, et al. (2001) Distributed and overlapping representations of faces and
objects in ventral temporal cortex. Science 293(5539):24252430.
41. Moreland RL, Zajonc RB (1982) Exposure effects in person perception: Familiarity,
similarity, and attraction. J Exp Soc Psychol 18(5):395415.
42. Vul E, Harris C, Winkielman P, Pashler H (2009) Reply to comments on Puzzlingly
high correlations in fMRI studies of emotion, personality, and social cognition.
Perspect Psychol Sci 4(3):319324.
43. Poldrack RA, Mumford JA (2009) Independence in ROI analysis: Where is the voodoo?
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 4(2):208213.
Anders et al. PNAS Early Edition
|
9of10
PSYCHOLOGICAL AND
COGNITIVE SCIENCES
NEUROSCIENCE PNAS PLUS
44. Haynes J-D, Rees G (2006) Decoding mental states from brain activity in humans. Nat
Rev Neurosci 7(7):523534.
45. Kriegeskorte N, Goebel R, Bandettini P (2006) Information-based functional brain
mapping. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103(10):38633868.
46. Singer T, et al. (2004) Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory com-
ponents of pain. Science 303(5661):11571162.
47. Ochsner KN, et al. (2008) Your pain or mine? Common and distinct neural systems
supporting the perception of pain in self and other. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 3(2):
144160.
48. Wolpert DM, Doya K, Kawato M (2003) A unifying computational framework for
motor control and social interaction. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 358(1431):
593602.
49. Stephens GJ, Silbert LJ, Hasson U (2010) Speaker-listener neural coupling underlies
successful communication. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 107(32):1442514430.
50. Funayama R, et al. (2012) Neural bases of human mate choice: Multiple value di-
mensions, sex difference, and self-assessment system. Soc Neurosci 7(1):5973.
51. Senior C (2003) Beauty in the brain of the beholder. Neuron 38(4):525528.
52. Fiske ST (2012) Journey to the edges: Social structures and neural maps of inter-group
processes. Br J Soc Psychol 51(1):112.
53. Said CP, Haxby JV, Todorov A (2011) Brain systems for assessing the affective value of
faces. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 366(1571):16601670.
54. Freeman JB, Stolier RM, Ingbretsen ZA, Hehman EA (2014) Amygdala responsivity to
high-level social information from unseen faces. J Neurosci 34(32):1057310581.
55. Hogan R, Mankin D (1970) Determinants of interpersonal attraction. A clarification.
Psychol Rep 26(1):235238.
56. Buck RW, Savin VJ, Miller RE, Caul WF (1972) Communication of affect through facial
expressions in humans. J Pers Soc Psychol 23(3):362371.
57. Sabatelli RM, Buck R, Dreyer A (1980) Communication via facial cues in intimate
dyads. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 6(2):242247.
58. McCroskey JC, McCain TA (1974) The measurement of interpersonal attraction.
Speech Monogr 41(3):261266.
59. Worsley KJ, et al. (1996) A unified statistical approach for determining significant
signals in images of cerebral activation. Hum Brain Mapp 4(1):5873.
60. Ashburner J (2007) A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. Neuroimage
38(1):95113.
61. Collins DL, Neelin P, Peters TM, Evans AC (1994) Automatic 3D intersubject registra-
tion of MR volumetric data in standardized Talairach space. J Comput Assist Tomogr
18(2):192205.
62. Tzourio-Mazoyer N, et al. (2002) Automated anatomical labeling of activations in
SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain.
Neuroimage 15(1):273289.
10 of 10
|
www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1516191113 Anders et al.
... The more similar the activation, the more shared feelings are reported (Anders et al., 2020b). C) Strangers viewing facial expressions of a sender show an increase in ventral striatum and medial orbitofrontal cortex activity that is correlated with the perceiver's confidence in having correctly understood the sender's emotion and predicts changes in interpersonal attraction (Anders et al., 2016). D) When people were asked to look at images of another person exhibiting publicly inappropriate behavior (a situation associated with self-reported feelings of Fremdscham or vicarious embarrassment caused by another's inappropriate behavior), greater activations in the left insula and anterior cingulate cortex occurred, and decreased activation in the ventral striatum was observed. ...
... A similar study (Anders et al., 2016) revealed that emotional communication was associated with accompanying feelings of confidence in the perceiver if the communication was successful, irrespective of the emotion that was being communicated. Short videos clips of six different senders experiencing sadness and fear from the pseudo-hyperscanning study described above were shown to > 90 new participants. ...
... The participants' task was to decide, after each video clip, which emotion the sender had been experiencing, and to report how confident they felt about their judgement. Self-reported confidence covaried with (i) the re-activation of local networks in the anterior insula that were also activated when the perceivers experienced sadness and fear, respectively, themselves and (ii) neural activity in the ventral striatum and medial orbitofrontal cortex (mOFC) (two brain regions that play an important role in affiliation, see section 2. Furthermore, neural activity in the ventral striatum/mOFC and feelings of confidence during emotional communication were associated with increased feelings of attraction towards the sender after communication (Anders et al., 2016) (Fig. 11C). These findings are consistent with the hypothesis that seamless communication of emotional information can lead to affiliative feelings. ...
Article
Full-text available
Social feelings have conceptual and empirical connections with affect and emotion. In this review, we discuss how they relate to cognition, emotion, behavior and well-being. We examne the functional neuroanatomy and neurobiology of social feelings and their role in adaptive social functioning. Existing neuroscience literature is reviewed to identify concepts, methods and challenges that might be addressed by social feelings research. Specific topic areas highlight the influence and modulation of social feelings on interpersonal affiliation, parent-child attachments, moral sentiments, interpersonal stressors, and emotional communication. Brain regions involved in social feelings were confirmed by meta-analysis using the Neurosynth platform for large-scale, automated synthesis of functional magnetic resonance imaging data. Words that relate specifically to social feelings were identfied as potential research variables. Topical inquiries into social media behaviors, loneliness, trauma, and social sensitivity, especially with recent physical distancing for guarding public and personal health, underscored the increasing importance of social feelings for affective and second person neuroscience research with implications for brain development, physical and mental health, and lifelong adaptive functioning.
... In contrast to the participants' emotion recognition accuracy and confidence their performance awareness did not decline when the amount of available facial information was reduced by a face mask. In social contexts, performance awareness (knowing if one does or does not understand another person's signals) permits individuals to navigate smoothly in their social environment and to build functional social networks (Anders et al., 2016). At the neural level, performance awareness seems to be mediated by neurons in the ventral striatum that signal whether sufficient information is available to accurately decode a signal (Anders et al., 2016;Daniel & Pollmann, 2012;Hebart et al., 2016). ...
... In social contexts, performance awareness (knowing if one does or does not understand another person's signals) permits individuals to navigate smoothly in their social environment and to build functional social networks (Anders et al., 2016). At the neural level, performance awareness seems to be mediated by neurons in the ventral striatum that signal whether sufficient information is available to accurately decode a signal (Anders et al., 2016;Daniel & Pollmann, 2012;Hebart et al., 2016). The current study suggests that the reliability of this system decreases with age. ...
Article
Full-text available
Surgical face masks reduce the spread of airborne pathogens but also disturb the flow of information between individuals. The risk of getting seriously ill after infection with SARS-COV-2 during the present COVID-19 pandemic amplifies with age, suggesting that face masks should be worn especially during face-to-face contact with and between older people. However, the ability to accurately perceive and understand communication signals decreases with age, and it is currently unknown whether face masks impair facial communication more severely in older people. We compared the impact of surgical face masks on dynamic facial emotion recognition in younger (18–30 years) and older (65–85 years) adults (N = 96) in an online study. Participants watched short video clips of young women who facially expressed anger, fear, contempt or sadness. Faces of half of the women were covered by a digitally added surgical face mask. As expected, emotion recognition accuracy declined with age, and face masks reduced emotion recognition accuracy in both younger and older participants. Unexpectedly, the effect of face masks did not differ between age groups. Further analyses showed that masks also reduced the participants’ overall confidence in their emotion judgements, but not their performance awareness (the difference between their confidence ratings for correct and incorrect responses). Again, there were no mask-by-age interactions. Finally, data obtained with a newly developed questionnaire ( attitudes towards face masks, atom ) suggest that younger and older people do not differ in how much they feel impaired in their understanding of other people’s emotions by face masks or how useful they find face masks in confining the COVID-19 pandemic. In sum, these findings do not provide evidence that the impact of face masks on the decoding of facial signals is disproportionally larger in older people.
... Specifically, autistic targets tended to be rated negatively. Given that we generally like people whose behaviour we can interpret, more so than those whose behaviour we struggle to interpret (Anders et al, 2016), it is reasonable to enquire whether there is an association between being difficult to interpret for neurotypical people and being rated unfavourably by this group. Alkhaldi et al (2019) presented the target videos used by Sheppard et al to a new group of neurotypical perceivers who rated each target on an adaptation of Sasson et al's social favourability scales. ...
Article
Full-text available
This article proposes a link between autistic people being misperceived by the neurotypical majority and their being at risk of poor mental health and wellbeing. We present a transactional account of development in which the misperceptions (and consequent behaviour) of the neurotypical majority influences the perceptions and behaviour of autistic people such that they become increasingly separate and indeed isolated from mainstream society. This jeopardises their mental health and prevents autistic people from developing to full potential. The situation is not only problematical for the development of autistic people but is also to the detriment of wider society, in so far as autistic people are effectively prevented from contributing fully. This account assumes that some (not necessarily all) autistic people yearn to be included, to be productive and to be useful. It thus directly opposes accounts that view autism as an extreme case of diminished social motivation.
... Put differently, when observers believed to have experienced a similar feeling as the sender although they had not correctly identified the communicated emotion, observing the sender's facial expression might have led to an re-enactment of patterns of neural activity in the observer's brain that were dissimilar to the sender's neural activity but nevertheless elicited an affective experience in the observer that felt as if he was sharing the sender's current emotional experience. Results of a previous study suggest that an observer's confidence about how accurately they perceive a target's emotion depends on the similarity between fine-scale patterns of neural activity in the anterior insula elicited when the observer sees the target's facial expression and those elicited when the observer experiences the corresponding emotion themselves (Anders et al., 2016). It will be an important task for future studies to examine if and how the human brain detects that it has been "de-coupled" from a communication partner's brain (leading, e.g., to delusive experiences of shared affect), and how it "re-couples" to restore successful communication. ...
Article
Full-text available
Current theories in cognitive neuroscience assume that internal simulation, i.e., the reproduction of brain activity underlying another person’s inner state and behaviour in the perceiver’s brain, plays an important role in understanding others. Here we test the prediction that common neural activity during facial communication of affect leads to interpersonal understanding. Six female senders and 30 male observers (six of which were the senders romantic partners and 24 unknown others) underwent pseudo-hyperscanning fMRI (functional magnetic resonance imaging). Senders were asked to submerge themselves into emotional situations and facially express their emotional feelings as they arose to the observer. Observers were uninformed about the sender’s task and were asked to watch and feel with the sender. Using between-brain spatial correlation analysis we found that mere emotion recognition was not closely related to the degree to which an observer reproduced the sender’s spatial pattern of neural activity in his own brain. However, in runs in which the observer had correctly identified the communicated emotion between-brain similarity of spatial patterns of neural activity predicted the degree to which the observer experienced a similar emotional feeling as the sender. This effect remained significant when differences between romantic partners and unknown others and sender effects were removed. These findings are in line with previous studies that suggest that facial emotion recognition, at least at a coarse level, might be supported by neural processes that do not rely on internal simulation. Shared affective experiences, on the other hand, might arise from common neural activity between the sender’s and the observer’s brain, leading to a “shared space of affect” which might be critical for the flow of more subtle affective information between brains.
... Clearly, more research is needed to better understand the neural processes underlying interindividual effects in social responses towards unfamiliar others and how they translate to related concepts such as social proximity and distance (e.g. 31 ), interindividual attraction (e.g. 32 ), feelings of affection (e.g. 33,34 ) and social bonds (e.g. ...
Article
Full-text available
How empathically people respond to a stranger’s pain or pleasure does not only depend on the situational context, individual traits and intentions, but also on interindividual factors. Here we ask whether empathic responses towards unknown others are modulated by behavioural similarity as a potential marker of genetic relatedness. Participants watched two supposed human players who were modelled as having a strong (player LP) or weak (player NLP) tendency to lead in social situations executing penalty shots in a virtual reality robot soccer game. As predicted, empathic response were modulated by shared behavioural traits: participants whose tendency to lead was more similar to player LP’s tendency to lead experienced more reward, and showed stronger neural activity in reward-related brain regions, when they saw player LP score a goal, and participants whose tendency to lead was more similar to player NLP’s tendency to lead showed stronger empathic responses when they saw player NLP score a goal. These findings highlight the potentially evolutionary grounded role of phenotypic similarity for neural processes underlying human social perception.
... During this process, the sensory receptors convert vibrational energy from the partner's face and body to electrical impulses that the brain then uses to acquire social and emotional information 27,28 . A recent fMRI study showed that the human brain possesses a neural mechanism, which attracts individuals to partners whose affective nonverbal behavior they can easily understand 29 . From this point of view, emotional expressions that people display do not only communicate emotions -they embody human feelings, build social bonds and promote attraction. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Humans are social animals whose mental wellbeing is shaped by the ability to attract and connect with each other. In a dating world, in which success can be determined by brief interactions, apart from physical features, there is a whole choreography of movements, physical reactions and subtle expressions that drive humans’ sexual attraction. To determine what drives attraction, we measured nonverbal dynamics between people during real-life interactions outside the laboratory, where dating is most relevant. Participants wore eye-tracking glasses with embedded cameras, and devices to measure physiological signals including heart rate and skin conductance. Crucially, visible signals that can be controlled, such as facial expressions or gaze, did not predict attraction. Instead, attraction was predicted by synchrony in heart rate and skin conductance between partners, which are unconscious and difficult to regulate. Our findings suggest that shared emotionality is vital for mutual attraction. Moreover, physiological synchrony may provide a medium for translating visible expressions into embodied emotions, which can turn into intentions via somatosensory simulation. Significance Statement In our modern world where millions of people meet online without interacting face-to-face, the question “what defines attraction” has become very relevant. In this study, we used modern technologies to lay down a foundation for the processes that drive human attraction during real-life interactions. Contrary to common belief, we found that attraction is not predicted by a frequency of expression or eye fixation duration, nor is linearly related to the participant’s autonomic nervous system activity. Importantly, we found that the more a participant synchronized their heart rate and skin conductance with their partner, the more they felt attracted toward that person. This study reveals a fundamental physiological process that plays a role in the formation of romantic relations.
Article
The most important issues in developing an emotion-aware dialog system include how to recognize users’ emotions and how machine responses can be generated accordingly. However, studies on emotional dialogs have primarily focused on whether the system can produce utterances with suitable emotions; consequently, the content relevance and quality of the system’s responses have been ignored. Different from previous studies, we present a deep learning framework to explore the influence of emotions in dialogs. Our framework includes two modules (C-LSTM for emotion recognition and biLSTM-C for response generation), and it integrates both emotional and rational information to produce emotionally and semantically correct responses. This structured design has several advantages: the modules of emotion recognition and response generation can be constructed by any effective methods, the personalized dialog can be achieved by adjusting the emotion-response coupling mechanism to adapt to users’ conversational styles, and the results can be transparent and interpretable to users. Following the presented approach, we first assess the performance of model training for emotion recognition and response generation. With the learned models, we configure a series of experiments to investigate the effect of using emotion as a driving force to generate machine responses in length by designing evaluation strategies from different perspectives. Experiments and results highlight the importance and influence of emotions in dialogs.
Preprint
Expertise refers to outstanding skill or ability in a particular domain. In the domain of emotion, expertise refers to the observation that some people are better at a range of competencies related to understanding, experiencing and managing emotions, and these competencies may help them lead healthier lives. Individual differences in emotional expertise are represented by a wide variety of psychological constructs, including emotional awareness, emotional clarity, emotional complexity, emotional granularity, and emotional intelligence. These constructs derive from different theoretical perspectives, highlight different competencies, and are operationalized and measured in different ways. The full set of relationships between these constructs has not yet been considered, hindering scientific progress and the translation of these findings to aid mental and physical well-being. In this paper, we use a scoping review procedure to integrate these constructs within a shared conceptual space. Using domain-general accounts of expertise as a guide, we build a unifying framework for emotional expertise, and apply this to constructs that describe how people understand and experience their own emotions. Our approach offers opportunities to identify potential underlying mechanisms of individual differences in emotion, thereby encouraging future research on those mechanisms as well as on educational or clinical interventions.
Article
Employee-customer interactions have been shown to influence perceptions of service quality, but what encourages customers to approach an employee has yet to be explored. As technology is increasingly integrated into service and retail environments and employee presence is decreasing, much of the responsibility for initiating service encounters has shifted from employees to customers. However, many customers are reluctant to approach employees. This manuscript examines the process through which a customer decides whether to approach an employee. Apparel is among the most noticeable cues customers' use to evaluate employees, thus, signaling theory is used to examine employee apparel's effect on consumers' approach intentions. Additionally, embarrassment is shown to moderate the approach decision process. Three studies, including a 360° video experiment and a behavioral lab study, are used to assess the relationship between apparel and approach decisions. These findings provide evidence that expertise-signaling apparel may be more effective at increasing approach intentions.
Article
First impressions follow a complex process that is greatly influenced by the facial attributes and the sex of the displayer. This study aimed to evaluate whether the initial impression formed in friendship decisions is processed differently between the same-sex and opposite-sex targets in women. Twenty-four healthy female volunteers participated in a functional magnetic resonance imaging experiment, in which they appraised a given face and decided whether they wanted to befriend the target. Then, a post-scan subjective rating was performed for facial components such as cheerfulness, good-looks, and uniqueness. The data were used to perform univariate whole-brain analysis to identify the neural substrates of sex bias in impression formation, and exploratory parametric modulation analysis to examine the parametric effects of facial components. Results showed that a composite of diverse areas including the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, and posterior superior temporal sulcus were more engaged when viewing male faces than female faces. Parametric analysis revealed that bilateral lingual gyrus activities showed a negative parametric effect with cheerfulness for male faces, whereas left inferior parietal lobule activity showed a positive parametric effect with good-looks for female faces. During the formation of first impressions, diffuse areas related to emotion processing and conflict-monitoring were utilized when a person of the opposite sex was encountered compared to encounters with a person of the same sex. The perceived cheerfulness of a male face also showed a negative relationship with the identification of facial emotion in the male face; the more a female face was considered good-looking, a greater feeling of uneasiness appeared to be elicited. The findings from this study provide further evidence of sex bias in women during friendship encounters.
Article
Full-text available
Perceptual confidence refers to the degree to which we believe in the accuracy of our percepts. Signal detection theory suggests that perceptual confidence is computed from an internal "decision variable," which reflects the amount of available information in favor of one or another perceptual interpretation of the sensory input. The neural processes underlying these computations have, however, remained elusive. Here, we used fMRI and multivariate decoding techniques to identify regions of the human brain that encode this decision variable and confidence during a visual motion discrimination task. We used observers' binary perceptual choices and confidence ratings to reconstruct the internal decision variable that governed the subjects' behavior. A number of areas in prefrontal and posterior parietal association cortex encoded this decision variable, and activity in the ventral striatum reflected the degree of perceptual confidence. Using a multivariate connectivity analysis, we demonstrate that patterns of brain activity in the right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex reflecting the decision variable were linked to brain signals in the ventral striatum reflecting confidence. Our results suggest that the representation of perceptual confidence in the ventral striatum is derived from a transformation of the continuous decision variable encoded in the cerebral cortex.
Article
Full-text available
In the highly social life of humans, rewards that are sought and experienced are intertwined with social relationships and interactions between people. Just as we value non-social rewards such as food or money, we also value social outcomes (e.g., praise from a superior). We use social information to evaluate and form expectations of others and to make decisions involving others. Here we review research demonstrating how the neural circuitry of reward, particularly the striatum, is also involved in processing social information and making decisions in social situations. This research provides an understanding of the neural basis for social behavior from the perspective of how we evaluate social experiences and how our social interactions and decisions are motivated. We review research addressing the common neural systems underlying evaluation of social and non-social rewards. The human striatum, known to play a key role in reward processing, displays signals related to a broad spectrum of social functioning, including evaluating social rewards, making decisions influenced by social factors, learning about social others, cooperating, competing, and following social norms.
Article
In Press version: Perspectives in Psychological Science (May, 2009). This manuscript may differ from the final published version Reply to comments on “Puzzlingly high correlations in fMRI studies of emotion, personality, and social cognition”* EDWARD VUL, CHRISTINE HARRIS, PIOTR WINKIELMAN, AND HAROLD PASHLER Massachussetts Institute of Technology and University of California, San Diego * The paper formerly titled: Voodoo Correlations in Social Neuroscience We are grateful to the commentators for providing many stimulating and valuable observations. The main point of our article was to call attention to the overestimation of individual differences correlations in a subset of neuroimaging papers. To structure our discussion of these comments, we list the main points from our paper, note where commentators have agreed or disagreed with each, and provide our own reactions to their comments. MAIN POINTS 1. We claimed that approximately half of a sample of studies reporting individual difference correlations between brain activity and behavioral measures of personality, emotion, and social cognition computed the correlation by (a) computing a separate correlation across people for each voxel in a normalized brain, (b) identifying highly correlated voxels, and (c) calculating (and reporting) the mean (or peak) correlation from just these correlation hot spots. Address correspondence to Harold Pashler, Department of Psychology 0109, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093; e-mail: hpashler@ucsd.edu. There does not appear to be any disagreement over this point. We have invited any author who believes that we wrongly described how they computed correlation values to write us and elaborate on their procedure; so far, this has revealed no misclassifications. Thus, our claims about how the correlations were calculated have not been disputed. Nonetheless, a casual reader of Lieberman et al. (2009, this issue) might assume that there is some dispute here, as Lieberman et al. say we incorrectly described the “inferential procedure” of these studies. However, they are merely arguing that the correlation values were presented and interpreted differently than we say. Specifically, they contend that inferences in articles we criticized were based exclusively on the significance of the whole-brain analysis and that the reported correlation values were always understood to lack any meaningful interpretation and thus were never ascribed one. We will return to this issue below (see Point 3). 2. We argued that a correlation computed as described in Point 1 is statistically guaranteed to provide an inflated estimate of the true underlying (population) correlation— that is, it is guaranteed to provide a number whose
Article
Numerous experiments have recently sought to identify neural signals associated with the subjective value (SV) of choice alternatives. Theoretically, SV assessment is an intermediate computational step during decision making, in which alternatives are placed on a common scale to facilitate value-maximizing choice. Here we present a quantitative, coordinate-based meta-analysis of 206 published fMRI studies investigating neural correlates of SV. Our results identify two general patterns of SV-correlated brain responses. In one set of regions, both positive and negative effects of SV on BOLD are reported at above-chance rates across the literature. Areas exhibiting this pattern include anterior insula, dorsomedialprefrontal cortex, dorsal and posterior striatum, and thalamus. The mixture of positive and negative effects potentially reflects an underlying U-shaped function, indicative of signal related to arousal or salience. In a second set of areas, including ventromedial prefrontal cortex and anterior ventral striatum, positive effects predominate. Positive effects in the latter regions are seen both when a decision is confronted and when an outcome is delivered, as well as for both monetary and primary rewards. These regions appear to constitute a “valuation system,” carrying a domain-general SV signal and potentially contributing to value-based decision making.
Article
New brain imaging technology has emerged that might make it possible to read a person's thoughts directly from their brain activity. This novel approach is referred to as "brain reading" or the "decoding of mental states." This article provides a general outline of the field and discusses its limitations, potential applications, and also certain ethical issues that brain reading raises. The measurement of brain activity and brain structure has made considerable progress in recent decades. The mapping from brain activity patterns to thoughts is learned for each specific subject using brute force statistical pattern recognition techniques. It discusses the degree to which polygraphy and brain-based lie detection can be manipulated by trained subjects. A future application of brain reading technology, "neuromarketing," has received tremendous interest and there are repeated attempts to optimize marketing campaigns by adding brain-based sources of information.
Article
We present a unified statistical theory for assessing the significance of apparent signal observed in noisy difference images. The results are usable in a wide range of applications, including fMRI, but are discussed with particular reference to PET images which represent changes in cerebral blood flow elicited by a specific cognitive or sensorimotor task. Our main result is an estimate of the P-value for local maxima of Gaussian, t, χ2 and F fields over search regions of any shape or size in any number of dimensions. This unifies the P-values for large search areas in 2-D (Friston et al. [1991]: J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 11:690–699) large search regions in 3-D (Worsley et al. [1992]: J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 12:900–918) and the usual uncorrected P-value at a single pixel or voxel.
Article
The study reports a factor analytic investigation of the interpersonal attraction construct. Two‐hundred and fifteen subjects completed 30 Likert‐type, seven‐step scales concerning an acquaintance. Factor analysis indicated three dimensions of the interpersonal attraction construct which were labeled “task” “social” and “physical.” The results of this study and four replications suggest that the resulting 15‐item instrument can be expected to measure reliably three dimensions of interpersonal attraction.
Article
Previous research shows that the amygdala automatically responds to a face's trustworthiness when a face is clearly visible. However, it is unclear whether the amygdala could evaluate such high-level facial information without a face being consciously perceived. Using a backward masking paradigm, we demonstrate in two functional neuroimaging experiments that the human amygdala is sensitive to subliminal variation in facial trustworthiness. Regions in the amygdala tracked how untrustworthy a face appeared (i.e., negative-linear responses) as well as the overall strength of a face's trustworthiness signal (i.e., nonlinear responses), despite faces not being subjectively seen. This tracking was robust across blocked and event-related designs and both real and computer-generated faces. The findings demonstrate that the amygdala can be influenced by even high-level facial information before that information is consciously perceived, suggesting that the amygdala's processing of social cues in the absence of awareness may be more extensive than previously described.
Article
The communication of emotion via spontaneous nonverbal expression was studied as it functions in the social penetration process. Two measures of nonverbal sensitivity to facial cues, sensitivity to general or unknown others and sensitivity to a dating partner, were obtained with seventeen exclusively dating, but noncohabiting couples. It was predicted from social penetration theory that both the duration of the relationship and the degree of involvement in the relationship would be reflected in the dating partner's sensitivity to one another. Neither prediction was supported.