Content uploaded by Eivind Hoff-Elimari
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Eivind Hoff-Elimari on Oct 20, 2016
Content may be subject to copyright.
European Journal of Government and Economics
Volume 3, Number 1 (June 2014)
ISSN: 2254-7088
24
Collective action problems: Disentangling
possible feedback loops between government
policies and the public’s value-change
Eivind Hoff-Elimari, Common Cause Network, Norway
Anat Bardi, Royal Holloway University of London, United Kingdom
Simon Matti, Luleå University of Technology, Sweden
Kristina Östman, KTH Royal Institute of Technology, Sweden
Abstract
Solving collective action problems, such as poverty reduction or climate change,
depends on interactions between governments' and voters' preferences regarding
pro-social actions. This paper examines whether the overall direction of change in
pro-social public policy precedes public value-change, rather than the other way
around. We examine change in the public’s pro-social values in six European
countries, as measured by the European Social Survey (ESS) during 2002-2012. In
these countries, we conducted an expert survey to rate governmental policy that
expresses these values over the same period, thereby examining value-change in
governmental policy. The chronological comparison of value-change of the public
with that of respective governments suggests that changes in pro-social
government policies may drive public value-change rather than vice versa. This
complements previous studies focused on the opinion-policy connection. Possible
political implications are discussed. The promising findings of this initial study point
to the importance of conducting larger-scale future studies.
JEL classification
Z18.
Keywords
Values; policy feedback; democracy.
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank WWF-UK, in particular Tom Crompton, for the financial
support and advice that enabled this research. We are also grateful to Ela Polek of
University College Dublin for valuable advice and encouragement. Last but not
least, we thank the anonymous political scientists who filled in the survey on
governments' political goals.
Hoff-Elimari, Bardi, Matti and Östman ● Collective action problems
25
Introduction
This research addresses the feedback loop between national political agendas and
value-priorities of the mass public in an attempt to further our understanding of how
political space for addressing large-scale collective action problems evolves.
Characterised by a temporal and spatial mismatch between costs and benefits, as
well as between problem causes and problem effects, a range of large-scale
collective action problems (e.g. Duit, 2011; Ostrom, 2000; Hodge & McNally, 2000)
or bigger-than-self problems (Crompton, 2010) exist as consistent challenges for
modern societies. For example, deficits of public pension regimes reward the
present beneficiaries at the expense of future tax-payers, and the impact of CO2
emissions today will be felt fully through climate change only in a few decades.
Addressing these problems present a significant challenge for contemporary
democratic governments. They cannot be addressed by appeals to individual (or
national) self-interest alone but rather require an appeal to, and an activation of,
the public’s moral-normative concerns to trigger both public policy support and
subsequent large-scale cooperative behaviour.
The interaction between public policy and public opinion has been a long standing
interest within social science research. As a key characteristic of democracy (e.g.,
Dahl, 1989), an abundance of previous research has focused on the effects of
opinion on policy, conceptualised as democratic responsiveness. Although the
magnitude and stability of these effects are still unknown, most scholars agree that
policy-makers, particularly with regard to high-salience issues, do not allow policy
decisions to flow too far away from public preferences (Page & Shapiro, 1983;
Stimson, MacKuen & Erikson, 1995; Glynn et al., 1999; Burstein, 2003; Wallner,
2008). However, when policy matches the opinions of the mass public, is this
match a sign of a responsive policy or of a responsive public? Addressing the
latter, researchers have demonstrated how political decision-makers regularly
attempt to impact public opinion by emphasising certain values (Feldman, 1988;
Caprara, Schwartz, Capanna, Vecchione & Barbaranelli, 2006); framing their
messages (Gilbert & Lindzey, 1998; Rein & Schön, 1993); and making “emotive
appeals” believed to increase legitimacy (Wallner, 2008) when presenting policy
proposals. Hence, although changes in mass-opinions give rise to new policy
suggestions by responsive decision-makers, public opinion may also be affected
by previous policy decisions. Indeed, an emerging literature on this interaction
argues that the public policy – public opinion interaction, rather than focusing on a
single direction of causality, should be thought of as a two-way system of feedback
loops, comprising both a dynamic policy representation and a dynamic public
responsiveness (e.g. the thermostatic model of responsiveness, cf. Wlezien, 1995,
1996, 2004; Soroka & Wlezien, 2004, 2010).
Drawing on Easton’s (e.g. 1953) traditional model of the political system, in which
public reactions to policy outcomes constitutes new inputs to the system, these
feedback loops can be both positive (self-reinforcing policy change) or negative
(leading to policy reversal) (Skocpol, 1993; Soss & Schram, 2007; Soroka &
Wlezien, 2010). For example, Pierson (1993) categorised these feedback effects
between public policies and the mass public as resource and incentive effects and
interpretive effects. The former have to do with the direct material impacts of public
policies, which may create vocal constituencies that will defend existing policies or
trigger opposition from those who lose out as a result of the policies. This is akin to
research on retrospective voting (e.g., Fiorina, 1981) suggesting that past
performances is a significant factor for evaluating political alternatives and
candidate support in upcoming elections. The interpretive effects, on the other
hand, change the public’s own cognitive processes. Soss (1999) has demonstrated
how one type of interpretive effect is political learning through individuals’ direct
experience with government programmes. Svallfors (2010) added that public
policies may also trigger a normative feedback mechanism, which may be
European Journal of Government and Economics 3(1)
26
independent of direct experience of public policies. Seen this way, public policy
may impact on publicly established views on how the world ought to be and
thereby contribute to creating or expanding political space for addressing also the
moral-normative issues and long time-horizons inherent in large-scale collective
action problems. In short, new policy may indeed be conducive to new politics
(e.g., Schattschneider, 1935). However, although the notion of a causal effect from
policy to opinion has received increased attention during the past decades,
research has primarily focused on how specific policies give rise to specific
changes in opinion on the particular issue addressed in policy (Soss & Schram,
2007; Soroka & Wlezien, 2010). In most of the empirical literature, therefore, the
broader political effects of policy – opinion interactions have been largely
overlooked.
Recognising the link between large-scale collective action problems and human
values and value change as documented by e.g. Schwartz (2006) or Schultz et al.
(2005), the primary aim of this article is to explore whether the value-priorities
expressed through national policy are associated with subsequent changes in the
value-priorities of the mass public, in the case of large-scale collective-action
problems. Thereby, this article contributes to further exploring one of the empirical
links between public policy and public opinion: public responsiveness. To fulfil this
aim, we use a combination of ESS data and expert surveys to compare how
national policy goals and mass public values have changed in six European
countries during 2002-2012. We do not seek to explain differences in the absolute
levels of value priorities across countries but focus on changes in value scores.
In the next section, we further theorise the proposed feedback mechanism between
national political agendas and mass public values, and outline a framework for
mapping and comparing the two on the same canvas. The following section
describes our method, survey and measurement strategies. The final two sections
present our results and discuss their implications.
Policy feedback and values
If there are strong correlations between public policies and voters’ preferences,
how can the directions of effects between the two be established? It is tempting to
consider policy and opinion as changing through “mutual adjustment, subject to a
never-ending endogenous feedback loop” (Svallfors, 2010, p. 121). Although this
may be true in many cases, in particular when viewed in a long-term perspective, it
does not help much in understanding the drivers of changes to public policy and
mass public opinion. This requires that each mechanism is studied independently.
Therefore, in this paper, we explore the suggestion that changes in the value
priorities reflected through governmental policy are associated with later changes
in the public’s value-priorities, implying that governments might have the ability to
consciously influence the values of the public.
What is the basis to expect that value emphases of national policy would be
associated with subsequent changes in the value-priorities of the mass public? As
mentioned above, recent developments in political science research on
responsiveness have demonstrated how the public indeed reacts to changes in
policy. Although these reactions, in part, can be due to the real life effects of a
policy (e.g. Pierson, 1993), public reactions to policy can also be a factor of the
political information surrounding implementation processes, allowing the public to
form opinions even without being familiar with the specifics of a policy change
(Jacobs & Shapiro, 2000; MacKuen, Erikson & Stimson, 1992; Zaller, 1992).
According to, for example, the thermostatic model of responsiveness (Wlezien
1995 & 1996; Soroka & Wlezien 2010), the public continuously gauge the
difference between their preferred level and direction of policy and the reality of
political decision-making, resulting both in short-term demands for policy
Hoff-Elimari, Bardi, Matti and Östman ● Collective action problems
27
adjustments as well as in long-term preference-modification in reaction to policy
change.
The most prominent psychological process of value change to date is the process
of adaptation (see detail and review in Bardi & Goodwin, 2011). That is, personal
values adapt to life circumstances such that for most values, those values that can
be readily pursued and fulfilled become stronger, and those values whose pursuit
is blocked become less important. This mechanism has been used in the past to
explain the value profiles of citizens of former communist countries in Europe as an
adaptation to life under communism (Schwartz & Bardi, 1997). Furthermore,
examining sub-populations, the value profiles of Polish migrants to the United
Kingdom have become more similar to the values of United Kingdom residents the
longer these migrants have remained in the country, probably as part of a process
of adaptation (Bardi, Buchanan, Goodwin, Slabu, & Robinson, 2014). A similar
process of adaptation may lead values of the public to change and adapt to the
values that governments pursue in their policies. Hence, it is plausible that values,
as expressed implicitly in governments’ policies, actions, and discourse, would later
become more important in the public as part of adaptation to the current regime.
In order to explore and compare policy feedback loops, the primary task is to
translate public policies and public opinion into something comparable across
countries and over time. Basic human values offer such an object as both mass
opinion and governmental policy can be conceptualised as expressing a coherent
set of value-priorities. Public policy, defined broadly as the output of the political
system (e.g. Dror, 1973; Premfors, 1989; Cochran & Malone, 1995; Easton, 1953;
Hjern, 1987), is to its essence constructed by a system of values specifying its
ultimate goals and guiding the choice of strategies for reaching them (cf. Amara,
1972; Jenkins-Smith & Sabatier, 1993; Kingdon, 1995). Values are thereby to be
considered the “backstop” (Tetlock et al., 1996) of public policy, and the way in
which value-conflicts and value trade-offs are handled throughout the political
system thereby determining its content (Stewart, 2009; Thatcher & Rein, 2004).
Similarly, we know from both social psychology and public opinion research that
people's opinions constitute a function of their general values (cf. Rokeach, 1973;
Converse, 1964; Glynn et al., 1999) and that value-priorities serve as a shortcut for
the formation of preferences on a range of political issues (e.g. Zaller, 1992;
Jacoby, 2006; Altemeyer, 1998; Rasinski, 1987; Brewer & Gross, 2005; Feldman,
1988; Hurwitz & Peffley, 1987).
Schwartz (e.g., 2011) has shown that both individuals and cultures on all
continents share a common structure of values, understood as guiding principles in
life, arranging a set of ten motivational value-types. The proposed structure of
individuals’ values has been examined in 75 nations worldwide (Schwartz, 2011)
and is widely referred to as a theoretical starting point in research on a number of
socio-political issues (e.g., Davidov, Schmidt & Schwartz, 2008; Caprara et al.,
2006; Barnea & Schwartz, 1998; Rohan & Zanna, 1998 & 1996; Sagiv & Schwartz,
1995; Stern et al., 1995). According to Schwartz (1992), basic human values are
organised in ten value types whose reciprocal relations can be represented as a
circumplex (see Figure 1), where proximity on the circumference indicates that
values are mutually compatible and that strengthening a value will strengthen
neighbouring values, whereas diametrical opposition indicates the opposite:
strengthening a value on one side of the circle will weaken the value on the
opposite side.
This representation of basic values as a circle allows the identification of four
higher-order value types according to two oppositions: Openness to change versus
Conservation (OtC/C), focusing the conflict between willingness for change and the
motivation to keep things as they are, and Self-enhancement versus Self-
transcendence (SE/ST), focusing the conflict between prioritising selfish interests
and prioritising the interests of others or one’s group (Schwartz, 1992, p. 43).
European Journal of Government and Economics 3(1)
28
Figure 1: Schwartz value types
Source: Holmes, Blackmore, Hawkins & Wakeford (2011)
Priorities amongst the basic values vary from individual to individual, from society
to society and over time. The aggregate value priorities across societies form the
very heart of culture (e.g., Hofstede, 1980; Schwartz, 1994) and are correlated with
the public’s political preferences (e.g., Caprara et al., 2006). For instance, self-
transcendence values such as equality and unity with nature are closely related to
pro-social and cooperating behaviour (e.g. Schwartz, 2006; De Groot & Steg, 2008;
Stern & Dietz, 1994) as well as to environmental concern (Schultz et al., 2005).
Similarly, the values that prevail in society are used by political leaders to set
policies. For example, welfare laws are likely to prevail in countries where values of
self-transcendence, such as justice and equality, are emphasised (Bardi & Sagiv,
2003).
Although the OtC/C-dimension has been suggested as closely associated with
general political-ideological reasoning, thereby predicting the formation of support
for political parties and programmes primarily on matters concerning the state-
individual relationship, e.g. between conservatism and liberalism (cf. Schwartz,
1992; Barnea & Schwartz, 1998; Caprara et al., 2006), the focus of this article is on
how values of the SE/ST-dimension are materialised in public policy and through
public opinion. As this paper focuses on policies addressing large-scale collective
action or bigger-than-self problems, which require for their amendment that people
prioritise the good of others rather than personal utility maximisation, the ST/SE
focus on priorities concerning individual or collective benefits is the appropriate
choice. In the next section we shall therefore examine empirically how six
European countries moved in different directions on the SE/ST-dimension in the
early 2000s and how their governments’ corresponding policy goals evolved over
the same time period.
Hoff-Elimari, Bardi, Matti and Östman ● Collective action problems
29
Method and data
Method
Value-priorities expressed through public opinion
The longest time series for regular measurements of Schwartz values at national
level is the European Social Survey (ESS). It is conducted every second year since
2002 (2003 for some countries) across Europe (including Russia, Turkey and
Israel). Each national survey consists of representative samples of between 1200
and 2800 respondents. A core module of questions has been included in all
rounds. This module includes Schwartz’ shortened 21-item Portrait Values
Questionnaire (PVQ) to map values. The questions describe a characteristic of a
fictitious person and the respondent is asked to evaluate how much the described
person resembles him or her on a scale from 1 (very much like me) to 6 (not like
me at all). Each of the ten value-types is expressed by two or three items. For
example, one of the items for universalism is: “She/he strongly believes that people
should care for nature. Looking after the environment is important to her/him.”
For each respondent, a mean score of all PVQ items was first computed and
subtracted from 7 to reverse the scale in order to make its interpretation more
intuitive: A high score means a respondent generally felt much like the person
portrayed. For each of the value types, power, achievement, benevolence and
universalism, each respondent’s mean score across the two or three items of the
value type was calculated and the scale again reversed by subtracting it from 7.
The respondent’s overall mean score across all the PVQ items was subtracted
from each of these values and 5 were added to the figures for each respondent to
eliminate any negative values. Each respondent’s mean of power and achievement
represent her self-enhancement score and the mean of universalism and
benevolence represent her self-transcendence score.
We applied the demographic weights provided by ESS to compensate for over-
and under-representation of certain age groups and household types to calculate
national mean self-transcendence and self-enhancement scores. The difference
between the two is the compound indicator we used to compare public opinion
over time. We refer to this as the public opinion net self-transcendence score.
Value-priorities expressed through governmental policy
The first five rounds of ESS gives a time series from 2002-2003 to 2010-2012 of
changing value priorities of public opinion on the SE/ST-dimension. To study
feedback effects, we also need a mirror measure of changes to governments’ value
priorities over the same time period. One option would have been to study changes
to material effects of public policies of most obvious relevance to value priorities on
the self-transcendence vs. self-enhancement opposition, such as marginal income
tax, immigration or environmental policies. However, we found that official statistics
were deficient in many fields before 2004 for newer EU Member States (e.g., on
immigration or environmental policies) and/or they covered a very small part of
what basic values might impact on (such as in the case of marginal income tax).
Furthermore, there may be significant time lags – the material effects of public
policies may appear long after they have been debated and adopted. We did
searches of the frequency of keywords associated with the SE/ST-dimension on
websites of heads of government over time (using Google Advanced Search) in
order to infer governments' value priorities, but we found that results were
extremely sensitive to the choice of single words and that it was not suited to
compare governments. We decided instead to construct a compound indicator of
what we could infer as being the different governments’ value priorities on the
SE/ST-dimension, based on experts’ observations.
European Journal of Government and Economics 3(1)
30
As noted by Svallfors (2010), the normative feedback mechanisms of public
policies that we are concerned with may be independent of direct experience of
public policies: It matters not only what governments do (and don't), but also how
they motivate their decisions. In other words, what matters is the sum of what the
public perceives of government action and inaction. Due to the lack of consistent
opinion polls on these matters, we decided to approach academic political
scientists, whom we presumed would have followed national politics with interest
since the first ESS round in 2002 whilst having relatively non-partisan views. We
constructed a questionnaire (Political Scientists Survey – PSS) with 12 political
goals representing typical government priorities that would be relevant across
Europe over the last decade. This questionnaire was sent to political scientists in
the six countries with the most significant variations in the public opinion net self-
transcendence score in the period from 2002-2003 to 2010-2012. We used our
networks to identify active researchers (with at least a PhD) at universities with a
good reputation in political science. Respondents were asked to rate the extent to
which their countries’ governments from 2002 to 2011 (we defined a government
as the tenure of a head of government) prioritised each of the 12 goals (six
reflecting SE-values, six reflecting ST-values) using a 5-point scale. The complete
list and details of the methodology can be found in the Appendix. Mean scores for
each of the 12 items for each government were calculated and then combined into
a government net self-transcendence score for each government, defined as:
Finally, we combined for each of the selected countries the graphs of net self-
transcendence scores from ESS and from PSS to allow for a discussion of
directions of effects between value change in public opinion and in government.
Data
ESS analysis
Our aim was to verify if the public’s value change may be associated with similar
change in governments' value emphases, and to see chronologically in which
direction these changes have occurred. For this particular aim, we focused our
analysis on those EU countries with the most significant changes to the net self-
transcendence score, both between the ESS1 and ESS5 and between two
consecutive ESS rounds. The countries with the largest changes in these
measures can be seen in Table 1 and Table 2 respectively.
Hoff-Elimari, Bardi, Matti and Östman ● Collective action problems
31
Table 1: Largest changes in net self-transcendence between ESS1 and ESS5
Table 2: Largest changes in net self-transcendence between consecutive ESS
rounds
Positive Negative
Sweden 0.27 ESS5 Czech Republic -0.45 ESS2
Greece 0.26 ESS5 Portugal -0.35 ESS2
Portugal 0.26 ESS3 Ireland -0.33 ESS5
Greece -0.32 ESS4
As a result, the Czech Republic, Spain, Portugal, Sweden, Ireland, and Greece
were selected for further study. Figures 2a-f show the evolution of self-
transcendence and self-enhancement for these countries over time, mirroring each
other.
Figures 2a-f: Public opinion self-transcendence and self-enhancement
Positive Negative
Spain 0.55 Czech Republic -0.73
Sweden 0.34 Ireland -0.26
Norway 0.15 Ukraine -0.24
European Journal of Government and Economics 3(1)
32
In Figure 3, the unpredictable evolution of the public opinion net self-
transcendence score in all six countries is illustrated. For instance, Sweden has
increased its score from a level that was already at the top. Spain started from a
medium position and moved to the top of the list. Conversely, the Czech Republic
started at the top only to end up with the lowest score of all six at the time of ESS5.
Figure 3: Public opinion net self-transcendence score for selected countries
The political scientist survey (PSS)
The expert survey was carried out using the web service freeonlinesurveys.com
which allowed the respondents to fill it in anonymously at their own leisure. Emails
were sent to 8-16 political scientists in each country with a link to the survey and a
two week period to respond. After nine days they were all approached again with a
reminder by email. The average response rate was 41% between the six countries,
with an average of 4 respondents each and 22 respondents in total. With the
exception of the Czech Republic and Portugal, all respondents followed the
instructions and filled in the survey for all their respective governments.
The inter-judge reliability, or the degree of agreement between the different
respondents for each government, was calculated using Cronbach’s alpha. The
range of values calculated was 0.61-0.92, except for the lower cases of the
Persson government in Sweden (0.47), the Barroso and Santana Lopes
governments in Portugal (0.52 and 0.57, respectively) and all governments in
Hoff-Elimari, Bardi, Matti and Östman ● Collective action problems
33
Greece. It was found that the unreliability for the Greek case was caused mainly by
one respondent. Once this data set was removed, all governments except for
Karamanlis had acceptable Cronbach’s alpha values of more than 0.6. The
Karamanlis government, which remained at an astonishingly low 0.09, is therefore
treated with caution, as discussed under results.
Results and discussion
To investigate feedback loops between the value-priorities expressed through
governmental policies and those expressed in public opinion, graphs were plotted
showing net self-transcendence both for public opinion and governments. A high
net self-transcendence score shows a tendency towards self-transcendence values
whereas a low score shows a tendency towards self-enhancement values. The
public opinion’s net self-transcendence score is tracked on the primary vertical axis
to the left and the governments’ net self-transcendence score on the secondary
vertical axis to the right (Figures 4, 5, 7, 8, 9 and 10).
As our primary interest is to explore whether changes to the value priorities
reflected in a government’s policies are associated with later changes to values of
the public, we focus on the extent to which the graphs show that a change to the
governments’ scores precedes changes to that of the public’s values, and we
discuss each of the six countries based on the graphs with ESS and PSS data. To
further our analysis, we also complement our results with election outcomes and
opinion polls as well as data from the ESS on the political preferences of the
respondents.
Spain
Figure 4: Net self-transcendence for Spain
Spain has experienced the highest increase in the ESS net self-transcendence
score. It appeared gradually and continuously between ESS1 and ESS5. The three
Spanish governments in office 2002-2011 also show the clearest differences in
their value priorities: The conservative Popular Party (PP) governments under José
María Aznar (1996-2004) and Mariano Rajoy (2011-) both receive a net self-
transcendence score about three points lower than the socialist PSOE government
European Journal of Government and Economics 3(1)
34
led by José Luis Rodríguez Zapatero (2004-2011). There is large consensus
amongst PSS respondents in this assessment, with Cronbach’s alpha of around
0.9 for all three governments. As the Zapatero government entered office between
ESS1 and ESS2 and stayed in office until after ESS5, it means the interesting
juncture to study is the change of government from Aznar to Zapatero, which
resulted from the March 2004 general elections.
The run-up to the elections shows that Zapatero became prime minister by
accident. The PP consistently led the opinion polls ahead of the 14 March elections
(Libertad Digital, 2004). This changed with the 11 March 2004 Madrid train
bombings, which left 191 dead and 1,800 injured. The government quickly blamed
the Basque terrorist organisation ETA, despite evidence to the contrary. The
government was seen to have tried to manipulate facts and the PP was
consequently defeated (Gordon, 2004).
As mentioned above, the Zapatero government is seen to have been very clearly
focused on self-transcendent political goals. The ESS data suggest that this may
have triggered a policy feedback loop confirmed by his re-election in 2008.
Why, then, did the feedback loop stop at the general elections in 2011, after which
the PP returned to government with a majority in parliament? We believe this may
be explained by the economic crisis in which Spain has been engulfed since 2008.
With the recession, unemployment has soared. In none of the worst crisis-ridden
euro-zone countries – Greece, Ireland, Italy, Portugal and Spain – have
incumbents been re-elected after 2009. In other words, just like the exogenous
event of the Madrid bombings may have triggered a policy feedback loop that took
the Spanish public opinion’s net self-transcendence score on an upward trend,
another exogenous event – the economic crisis – may have put a halt to it.
Thus, the Spanish case suggests that, in the absence of large external shocks,
governments may influence the value-priorities of the mass public rather than the
other way round.
Ireland
Figure 5: Net self-transcendence for Ireland
Hoff-Elimari, Bardi, Matti and Östman ● Collective action problems
35
The Irish public opinion net self-transcendence score remains remarkably stable
over most of the time period with relatively small oscillations up and down during
the tenures of Bertie Ahern and Brian Cowen (both Fianna Fáil, centre-right). This
is matched by a stable score for the same two governments in office until 2011.
This picture changes dramatically with the last ESS round (2011-2012) and the
change of government after the 2011 elections won by Enda Kenny’s Fine Gael
party (to the right of Fianna Fáil). Irish public opinion as well as the government
turned massively towards self-enhancement. Judging from Figure 5, both causal
directions between public opinion and government value priorities are possible:
ESS4 was recorded 11 September 2009-12 March 2010, the elections took place
on 25 February 2011, and ESS5 was recorded 20 September 2011-31 January
2012. The drop in the public opinion net self-transcendence score between ESS4
and ESS5 could thus have occurred before or after the February 2011 elections. In
any case, the fact that public opinion in ESS5 mirrors the Kenny government value
priorities, indicate possible policy feedback.
To compare the two potential causal directions between changes to value priorities
of government and those of public opinion, we look at opinion polls on voting
intentions between ESS4 and ESS5 (Figure 6).
Figure 6: Irish opinion polls from ESS4 to 2011 election
Source: Quinn, C. (2008)
The polls show that Kenny’s Fine Gael for most of the time period, including at the
time ESS4 was recorded, had a score that was at a level similar to their election
result in February 2011. It also had a consistent political line. If there was any
feedback between value priorities of government and those of public opinion, it is
more likely that the value change in public opinion occurred after the elections,
which would be in line with our hypothesis. The victory of Fine Gael may be
explained by the general pattern since 2009 that incumbents lose elections in
crisis-ridden euro-zone countries.
European Journal of Government and Economics 3(1)
36
Greece
Figure 7: Net self-transcendence for Greece
In Greece, the public opinion net self-transcendence score experienced a dip
between ESS2 and ESS4 (Greece did not take part in ESS3) followed by an
astonishing increase between ESS4 and ESS5. The government scores are stable
until George Papandreou’s PASOK (socialist) government 2009-2011, when it
increases significantly and then drops even more with the care-taking government
of Lucas Papademos. Note that the Kostas Karamanlis score needs to be
interpreted with caution: The participants in the PSS differ widely in their
assessment of his government.
We focus our attention on the most abrupt and significant change to the public
opinion net self-transcendence score, between ESS4 and ESS5. ESS4 took place
15 July-20 November 2009. This was after PASOK had won the European
elections in June 2009, confirmed by the general election on 4 October 2009 after
which Karamanlis was replaced by Papandreou. Thus the public opinion net self-
transcendence score at the time of the 2009 elections was rather contrary to the
value priorities of Papandreou. Probably as a result of the deep economic crisis,
the incumbent lost.
The subsequent rise of the public opinion net self-transcendence score under
Papandreou provides some evidence favourable to our hypothesis that the
government may influence public opinion more than the other way round.
Hoff-Elimari, Bardi, Matti and Östman ● Collective action problems
37
Portugal
Figure 8: Net self-transcendence for Portugal
Portugal experienced a dramatic downward shift in public opinion’s net self-
transcendence score between ESS1 and ESS2. It then bounced back up by ESS3,
although it remained below the ESS1 level. The dip corresponds to the José
Manuel Barroso government 2002-2004 (Social Democratic Party, centre-right),
followed by Pedro Santana Lopes 2004-2005 (also Social Democratic Party) after
Barroso had been appointed president of the European Commission. Santana
Lopes was defeated in the general elections of 20 February 2005 and was
replaced by José Sócrates of the Socialist Party. The bounce between ESS2 and
ESS3 corresponds to the first two years of the Sócrates government.
Portugal was the first country in the euro-zone to introduce fiscal austerity
measures, under the Barroso government, after it had broken the EU Stability and
Growth Pact ceilings on public deficits. This policy was not reversed by Santana
Lopes. It is hardly surprising, then, that both governments appear largely self-
enhancement oriented in the PSS (although there was significant disagreement
amongst our respondents for both these governments, with Cronbach’s alpha
being 0.52 and 0.57 respectively). The Sócrates government from 2005 received a
far higher net self-transcendence score.
As in the case of Greece, the coincidence of elections and ESS rounds makes it
straightforward to test our hypothesis for the Santana Lopes/Sócrates shift. ESS2
was carried out from 15 October 2004 until 17 March 2005. In other words, about
four of the survey’s five months of interviews took place in the run-up to the
elections. The ESS2 results show that the Portuguese public opinion had turned
strongly towards self-enhancement during the Barroso and Santana Lopes
tenures. Sócrates’ victory seems more the result of a “sanction vote” against
Santana Lopes, who carried the stigma of being a substitute for Barroso, had not
held any national electoral office and presided over an economic slowdown and
increased unemployment.
The subsequent jump in the public opinion net self-transcendence score between
ESS2 and ESS3, during Sócrates’ first two years in office, is in line with our
hypothesis. It leaves unexplained the slight, but gradual, decrease in the public
opinion’s net self-transcendence score between ESS3 and ESS5, while Sócrates
European Journal of Government and Economics 3(1)
38
was still in office. This decrease was, however, far smaller than the changes
between the three first ESS rounds. Sócrates’ subsequent defeat at anticipated
2011 elections and replacement by Pedro Passos Coelho (whose government has
a strong self-enhancement orientation) could give credence to the opposite to our
hypothesis, in other words that a shift in public opinion’s value priorities helped
trigger the election of Passos Coelho. But this change of government could equally
be put down to the general pattern of losing incumbents we also see in other crisis-
ridden euro-zone countries.
Of the three changes of political parties in government in the period studied, the
two first (Antonió Guterres/Barroso and Santana Lopes/Sócrates) could be
interpreted in line with a causal effect of public policy on public opinion and the last
one in line with the opposite effect.
Czech Republic
Figure 9: Net self-transcendence for the Czech Republic
The Czech Republic has experienced a steady decline of the public opinion's net
self-transcendence score. This started with the single biggest change between the
scores of two subsequent ESS rounds: Between ESS1 (24 November 2002-9
March 2003) and ESS2 (1 October 2004-13 December 2004), it declined by almost
0.5 points. Between the two ESS rounds, no general elections were held but
Stanislav Gross succeeded Vladimír Špidla as prime minister in August 2004. They
were both from the same political party (ČSSD, social-democrats), but the Gross
government received a much lower inferred net self-transcendence score than the
Špidla government.
The trigger of the change of government was the European elections in June 2004
(Czech Statistical Office, 2004). The ČSSD lost votes both to the centre-right ODS
party and to the communist party (KSČM), and ended up in fifth place. It should be
noted that the rate of participation was very low (28.32%). Špidla stepped down
and was replaced by his deputy prime minister Gross, who was seen as a youthful,
charismatic figure to reinvigorate the ČSSD. However, his premiership was quickly
tainted by cronyism and corruption claims (The Economist, 2005), in contrast to the
dull but honest reputation of Špidla. Gross was forced to resign in April 2005.
Hoff-Elimari, Bardi, Matti and Östman ● Collective action problems
39
It is difficult to find indicators that would support our hypothesis, given the large
change of the public opinion’s score but short period of time between Gross’ entry
into office in August 2004 and the ESS2 interviews (October-December 2004). It is
easier to find indicators to test the opposite hypothesis: That shifts in value
priorities of public opinion drove the changes to value priorities of government. An
indicator would be if the rejection of the ČSSD were confirmed by a general move
in ESS2 to the political right, as the political right is associated with self-
enhancement in the Czech Republic. ESS data show that this was not the case
between ESS1 and ESS2.
It is thus possible that other factors, such as the controversies around Gross and
the cynicism about politics that he inspired, contributed to the change in the public
opinion value-priorities between ESS1 and ESS2. It is also clear that prior to Gross’
government, there was already a potential for cynicism building up, as the Špidla
government had proven to be a fractious coalition and the prime minister was
unable to carry out many of his pledges.
To sum up, there is no clear evidence for either causal relationship between shifts
in the value priorities of governments and those of the public opinion for this
country.
During the rest of the time period, there was a slower but steady decline in the
public opinion’s net self-transcendence score, accompanied by governments
whose score remained relatively low and stable until 2010, when Petr Nečas took
over as prime minister. Its net self-transcendence score is significantly lower than
the preceding ones. However, with no clear trend breaks, neither in the ESS data
nor in the government scores, we avoid comparing hypothetical causalities for the
developments after the Gross government.
Sweden
Figure 10: Net self-transcendence for Sweden
Sweden is one of the countries with the highest public opinion net self-
transcendence score throughout all ESS rounds. The score was stable until a
significant increase appeared between ESS4 and ESS5. The only change of prime
minister occurred in 2006, when the social-democrat government led by Göran
European Journal of Government and Economics 3(1)
40
Persson was defeated in the general elections by a centre-right coalition led by
Fredrik Reinfeldt of the conservative party (Moderaterna). The PSS in Sweden only
comprised two respondents. Cronbach’s alpha is satisfactory for Reinfeldt (0.73)
but not for Persson (0.47), which means its score should be interpreted with
caution. However, the large difference between the two governments’ score (more
than 2.5 points) instills confidence that the Reinfeldt government’s net self-
transcendence score is lower than that of its predecessor.
If there were any causal links between shifts in value priorities of public opinion and
those of the government, a shift in public opinion should have occurred either
between ESS2 and ESS3 (which was conducted from 21 September 2006 to 3
February 2007, immediately after the September 2006 elections) or between ESS3
and ESS4 (which was conducted after two years of the Reinfeldt government).
Neither of them occurred. Other factors have been at play to increase the public
opinion net self-transcendence score between ESS4 (2008-9) and ESS5 (2010-
2011). Thus, in the Swedish case we do not find any clear evidence for a public
policy-public opinion effect.
Conclusions
In three (Spain, Greece and Ireland) of the six EU Member States with the highest
changes to public opinion net self-transcendence scores, findings are in line with
the idea that governments’ value priorities as expressed through public policy may
predict – in the same direction – self-transcendence values of public opinion. The
impact of such possible influence on re-election is, however, secondary to
exogenous shocks, such as economic downturns or trust-breaking revelations of
government misdoings. The combination of these factors – exogenous shocks and
government’s influence on public opinion – can produce contrasted trends, as
exemplified by Greece and Ireland: An acute economic crisis left incumbents
defeated in elections in 2009 and 2011 respectively; the new Greek government
had more self-transcendent political goals compared to its predecessor, whereas
the new Irish government had more self-enhancement oriented political goals. In
the ESS round carried out after these elections, public opinion had moved in the
direction indicated by the respective government changes.
In two cases (Czech Republic and Portugal) there is support for feedback loops
between value priorities expressed in government policies and public opinion, but
the data do not enable us to disentangle the direction of effects in the feedback
loop (Czech Republic), or they point to effects in both directions (Portugal). The
Czech case is, however, interesting by the fact that it is the country with the highest
increase in the purchasing power standard per inhabitant and the only one with a
fall in the unemployment rate throughout the period of all the six countries studied,
yet with the biggest decrease in the public opinion net self-transcendence score. It
confirms the insight from the contrasting cases of Ireland and Greece that
economic trends do not determine the direction of change in public opinion value
priorities.
In the last case (Sweden), we have not found support for any influence between
government and public opinion’s value priorities.
This paper is intended merely as a preliminary attempt to investigate the existence
and possible directions of effects between value priorities of the government and
those of public opinion, the confirmation of which would require, inter alia:
Inclusion of the other ESS countries (in particular to compare with countries with
small variations to public opinion net self-transcendence scores – specifically,
Slovenia, Netherlands, Great Britain, Denmark, Switzerland and Belgium)
Inclusion of feedback loops between governments and public opinion on
the value dimension Openness to Change vs. Conservation
Hoff-Elimari, Bardi, Matti and Östman ● Collective action problems
41
Better validation of the PSS design and a higher number of respondents in
each country
More objective measures of government's value priorities
Quantitative analysis with control factors such as economic growth or
unemployment
Inclusion of ESS6 data (2012-2013)
Systematic consideration of socio-economic variables
With these caveats, we draw the cautious conclusion that there are indeed possible
feedback loops between the value priorities of governments and of public opinion,
and that the values expressed by governments may influence value priorities of the
public more than the other way round. This can have far-reaching implications for
political strategies to solve Bigger-Than-Self problems such as child poverty or
climate change. Appeals to economic growth as an end in itself may undermine
voters’ willingness to finance welfare services to fight, for instance, child poverty.
Arguing for investments in renewable energy primarily as a competitive necessity
to prevent trading partners from “catching” future jobs could reduce voters’ support
for strong climate change policies in general.
If our conclusions are confirmed by others, it will also give more confidence to
politicians who want to focus on Bigger-Than-Self problems. The politicians’
influence is not limited to the direct effect of policies adopted under their tenure;
their rhetoric and their policies can make citizens more supportive of action to
tackle such problems – even beyond the next election.
References
Altemeyer, Bob (1998) 'The other "authoritarian personality"' in Mark P. Zanna (ed.)
Advances in experimental social psychology. San Diego, CA: Academic, pp. 47-92.
Amara, Roy C. (1972) 'Toward a Framework for National Goals and Policy
Research', Policy Sciences 3(1): 59-69.
Bardi, Anat and Lilach Sagiv (2003) 'The EU and Israel: Comparison of cultures
and implications' in Klaus Boehnke (ed.) Israel and Europe: A complex relationship.
Wiesbaden: Deutscher Universitäts-Verlag, pp. 13-26.
Bardi, Anat and Robin Goodwin, (2011) 'The dual route to value change: Individual
processes and cultural moderators', Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 42(2):
271-287.
Bardi, Anat, Kathryn E. Buchanan, Robin Goodwin, Letitia Slabu and Mark
Robinson (2014) 'Value stability and change during self-chosen life transitions:
Self-selection versus socialization effects', Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology 106(1): 131-147.
Barnea, Marina F. and Schwartz, Shalom H. (1998) 'Values and Voting', Political
Psychology 19(1): 17-39.
Brewer, Paul R. and Kimberly Gross (2005) 'Values, Framing, and Citizens’
Thoughts about Policy Issues: Effects on Content and Quantity', Political
Psychology 26(6): 929-948.
Burstein, Paul (2003) 'The Impact of Public Opinion on Public Policy: A Review and
an Agenda', Political Studies Quarterly 56(1): 29-40.
Caprara, Gian Vittorio, Shalom Schwartz, Cristina Capanna, Michele Vecchione
and Claudio Barbaranelli (2006) 'Personality and Politics: Values, Traits and
Political Choice', Political Psychology 27(1): 1-28.
European Journal of Government and Economics 3(1)
42
Cochran, Charles and Eloise F. Malone (1995) Public Policy. New York: McGraw-
Hill.
Converse, Philip (1964). 'The Nature of Belief Systems in Mass Publics', in David
Apter (ed.) Ideology and Discontent. New York: Free Press, pp. 206-261.
Crompton, Tom (2010). Common Cause: The Case for Working with our Cultural
Values. WWF-UK. URL (consulted April 2014):
http://assets.wwf.org.uk/downloads/common_cause_report.pdf
Czech Statistical Office (2004). URL (consulted April 2014):
http://www.volby.cz/pls/ep2004/ep11?xjazyk=EN
Dahl, Robert A. (1989). Democracy and its Critics. New Haven, CT: Yale University
Press.
Davidov, Eldad, Peter Schmidt and Shalom H. Schwartz (2008) 'Bringing Values
Back In. The Adequacy of the European Social Survey to Measure Values in 20
Countries', Public Opinion Quarterly 72(3): 420-445.
De Groot, Judith I. M. and Linda Steg (2008) 'Value Orientations to Explain Beliefs
Related to Environmental Significant Behavior: How to Measure Egoistic, Altruistic,
and Biospheric Value Orientations', Environment and Behavior 40(3): 330-354.
Dror, Yehezkel (1973) Public Policy Reexamined. London: Leonard Hill Books.
Duit, Andreas (2011) 'Patterns of Environmental Collective Action: Some Cross-
National Findings', Political Studies 59 (4): 900-920.
Easton, David (1953) The Political System. New York: Knopf.
The Economist (2005, 3-9 March) Gross misdemeanours. URL (consulted April
2014): http://www.economist.com/node/3723535
European Social Survey Round 5 Data (2010) Data file edition 2.0. Norwegian
Social Science Data Services, Norway - Data Archive and distributor of ESS data.
European Social Survey Cumulative File, ESS 1-4 (2011) Data file edition 1.0.
Norwegian Social Science Data Services, Norway - Data Archive and distributor of
ESS data.
Feldman, Stanley (1988) 'Structure and Consistency in Public Opinion: the Role of
Core Beliefs and Values', American Journal of Political Science 32(2): 416-440.
Fiorina, Morris P. (1981). Retrospective Voting in American National Elections.
New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Gilbert, Daniel T., Susan T. Fiske and Gardner Lindzey (1998) The handbook of
social psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Glynn, Carroll J., Susan Herbst, Garrett J. O’Keefe and Robert Y. Shapiro (1999)
Public Opinion. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
Gordon, Philip H. (2004) Madrid Bombings and U.S. Policy. Testimony to the
Senate Committee on Foreign Relations. URL (consulted April 2014):
http://www.brookings.edu/research/testimony/2004/03/31europe-gordon
Hayek, Friedrich A. (1960) The Constitution of Liberty. Chicago, IL: University of
Chicago Press.
Hjern, Benny (1987) 'Policy Analysis: An Implementation Approach', Paper
presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Political Science Association, 3-6
September 1987, Chicago.
Hodge, Ian and Sandra McNally (2000) 'Wetland restoration, collective action and
the role of water management institutions', Ecological Economics 35(1): 107-118.
Hoff-Elimari, Bardi, Matti and Östman ● Collective action problems
43
Holmes, Tim, Elena Blackmore, Richard Hawkins and Tom Wakeford (2011) The
Common Cause Handbook. Public Interest Research Centre. URL (consulted April
2014): http://valuesandframes.org
Hofstede, Geert (1980) Culture’s consequences: International differences in work-
related values. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Hurwitz, Jon and Mark Peffley (1987) 'How are foreign policy attitudes structured?
A hierarchical model', American Political Science Review 81(4): 1099-1120.
Jacobs, Lawrence R. and Robert Y. Shapiro (2000) Politicians Don't Pander:
Political Manipulation and the Loss of Democratic Responsiveness. Chicago, IL:
University of Chicago Press.
Jacoby, William (2006) 'Value Choice and American Public Opinion', American
Journal of Political Science 50(3): 706-723.
Jenkins-Smith, Hank and Paul Sabatier (1993) 'Methodological Appendix:
Measuring Longitudinal Change in Elite Beliefs Using Content Analysis of Public
Documents', in Paul Sabatier and Hank Jenkins-Smith (eds) Policy Change and
Learning: An Advocacy Coalition Approach. Boulder, CO: Westview Press, pp.
237-276.
Kingdon, John W. (1995) Agendas, alternatives and public policies (2nd edition).
New York: HarperCollins.
Libertad Digital (2004) Encuestas y sondeos. URL (consulted April 2014):
http://www.libertaddigital.com/suplementos/elecciones2004/encuestas.html
MacKuen, Michael B., Robert S. Erikson and James A. Stimson (1992). 'Peasants
or Bankers? The American Electorate and the US Economy', American Political
Science Review 86(3): 597-611.
Ostrom, Elinor (2000) 'Crowding out citizenship', Scandinavian Political Studies
23(1): 3-16.
Page, Benjamin I. and Robert Y. Shapiro (1983) 'Effects of public opinion on
policy', American Political Science Review 77(1): 175-190.
Pierson, Paul (1993) 'When Effect Becomes Cause: Policy Feedback and Political
Change', World Politics 45(4): 595-628.
Premfors, Rune (1989) Policyanalys: kunskap, praktik och etik i offentlig
verksamhet. Lund: Studentlitteratur.
Quinn, Ciaran (2008) Na Pobalbhreitheanna is deireanaí. URL (consulted July
2013): http://www.guthanphobail.net/pobalbhreitheanna.htm
Rasinski, Kenneth R. (1987) 'What’s fair is fair—or is it? Public values and
conflicting views about justice', Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 53(1):
201–211.
Rein, Martin and Donald Schön (1993) 'Reframing Policy Discourse', in Frank
Fischer and John Forester (eds) The Argumentative Turn in Policy Analysis and
Planning. Durham, NC: Duke University Press, pp. 145-166.
Rohan, Meg J. and Mark P. Zanna (1996) 'Value Transmissions in Families', in
Clive Seligman, James M. Olson and Mark P. Zanna (eds) The Psychology of
Values: The Ontario Symposium, Vol. 8. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates Publishers, pp. 253-76.
Rohan, Meg J. and Mark P. Zanna (1998) 'The "products of socialization": A
discussion of self-regulatory strategies and value systems', in Joel Cooper and
John M. Darley (eds) Attribution and social interaction: The legacy of Edward E.
Jones. Washington DC: American Psychological Association, pp. 279-292.
European Journal of Government and Economics 3(1)
44
Rokeach, Milton (1973) The Nature of Human Values. New York, NY: The Free
Press.
Sabatier, Paul A. and Hank C. Jenkins-Smith (1999) 'The Advocacy Coalition
Framework: An Assessment', in Paul A. Sabatier (ed), Theories of the Policy
Process. Boulder, CO: Westview Press, pp. 117-166.
Sagiv, Lilach and Shalom H. Schwartz (1995) 'Value priorities and readiness for
out-group social contact', Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 69(3): 437-
448.
Schattschneider, Elmer E. (1935) Politics, Pressures and the Tariff: A Study of
Free Private Enterprise in Pressure Politics, as Shown in the 1929-1930 Revision
of the Tariff. New York: Prentice-Hall.
Schultz P. Wesley, Valdiney V. Gouveia, Linda D. Cameron, Geetika Tankha,
Peter Schmuck and Marek Franěk (2005) 'Values and their Relationship to
Environmental Concern and Conservation Behavior', Journal of Cross-Cultural
Psychology 36(4): 457-475.
Schwartz, Shalom H. (1992) 'Universals in the content and structure of values:
Theory and empirical tests in 20 countries', in Mark P. Zanna (ed) Advances in
experimental social psychology (Vol. 25). New York: Academic Press, pp. 1–65.
Schwartz, Shalom H. (1994) 'Beyond Individualism-Collectivism: New cultural
dimensions of values', in Uichol Kim, Harry C. Triandis, Cigdem Kagitcibasi, Sang-
Chin Choi and Gene Yoon (eds) Individualism and collectivism: Theory, method,
and applications. London: Sage, pp. 85-119.
Schwartz, Shalom H. (2006) Basic Human Values: An Overview. Jerusalem: The
Hebrew University of Jerusalem.
Schwartz, Shalom H. (2007) 'A Theory of Cultural Value Orientations: Explication
and Applications', in Yilmaz R. Esmer and Thorleif Petterson (eds) Measuring and
Mapping Cultures: 25 Years of Comparative Value Surveys. Leiden: Brill, pp. 33-
78.
Schwartz, Shalom H. (2011) 'Values: Individual and cultural', in Fons J. R. van de
Vijver, Athanasios Chasiotis and Seger M. Breugelmans (eds) Fundamental
questions in cross-cultural psychology. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University
Press, pp. 463-493.
Schwartz, Shalom H. and Anat Bardi (1997) 'Influences of adaptation to communist
rule on value priorities in Eastern Europe', Political Psychology 18(2): 385-410.
Skocpol, Theda (1992) Protecting Soldiers and Mothers: The Political Origins of
Social Policy in the United States. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Soroka, Stuart and Christopher Wlezien (2004) 'Opinion Representation and Policy
Feedback: Canada in Comparative Perspective', Canadian Journal of Political
Science 37(3): 531-559.
Soroka, Stuart and Christopher Wlezien (2010) Degrees of Democracy.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Soss, Joe (1999) 'Lessons of Welfare: Policy Design, Political Learning, and
Political Action', American Political Science Review 93(2): 363-380.
Soss, Joe and Sanford F. Schram (2007) 'A Public Transformed? Welfare Reform
as Policy Feedback', American Political Science Review 101(1): 111-127.
Stern, Paul C. and Thomas Dietz (1994) 'The value basis of environmental
concern', Journal of Social Issues 50(3): 65–84.
Stern, Paul C., Thomas Dietz, Linda Kalof and Gregory A. Guagnano (1995)
'Values, Beliefs, and Proenvironmental Action: Attitude Formation Toward
Hoff-Elimari, Bardi, Matti and Östman ● Collective action problems
45
Emergent Attitude Objects', Journal of Applied Social Psychology 25(18): 1611-
1636.
Stewart, Jenny (2009) Public Policy Values. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.
Stimson, James A., Michael B. Mackuen and Robert S. Eriksson (1995) 'Dynamic
Representation', American Political Science Review 89(3): 543-565.
Svallfors, Stefan (2010) 'Policy feedback, generational replacement, and attitudes
to state intervention: Eastern and Western Germany, 1990–2006', European
Political Science Review 2(1): 119-135.
Tetlock, Philip E., Randall S. Peterson and Jennifer S. Lerner (1996) 'Revising the
value pluralism model: Incorporating social content and context postulates', in Clive
Seligman, James M. Olson and Mark P. Zanna (eds) The Ontario Symposium of
Values, Vol. 8. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc., pp. 25-47.
Thacher, David and Martin Rein (2004) 'Managing Value Conflict in Public Policy',
Governance: An International Journal of Policy, Administration, and Institutions
17(4): 457-486.
Wallner, Jennifer (2008) 'Legitimacy and Public Policy: Seeing Beyond
Effectiveness, Efficiency, and Performance', The Policy Studies Journal 36(3): 421-
443.
Wlezien, Christopher (1995) 'The Public as Thermostat: Dynamics of Preferences
for Spending', American Journal of Political Science 39(4) 981-1000.
Wlezien, Christopher (1996) 'Dynamics of Representation: The Case of US
Spending on Defence', British Journal of Political Science 26(1): 81-103.
Wlezien, Christopher (2004) 'Patterns of Representation: Dynamics of Public
Preferences and Policy', Journal of Politics 66(1): 1-24.
Zaller, John R. (1992) The nature and origins of mass opinion. New York:
Cambridge University Press.
European Journal of Government and Economics 3(1)
46
Appendix
Items of the political scientists’ survey (PSS) and correspondence with Schwartz
value types
Government priority Corresponding PVQ items in ESS
Inferred
corresponding
Schwartz value
type
1 Economic growth It is important to her/him to be rich. She/he wants to have a
lot of money and expensive things. Achievement
2
Publicly funded services (e.g.
health, pensions, education and
transport funded through taxes
and compulsory social security
contributions)
She/he thinks it is important that every person in the world
should be treated equally. She/he believes everyone should
have equal opportunities in life. Universalism
3 Making domestic companies
more competitive against foreign
ones
Being very successful is important to her/him. She/he hopes
people will recognise her/his achievements. Achievement
4 Respect for authority It is important to her/him to get respect from others. She/he
wants people to do what she/he says. Power
5 Fight corruption It is important to her/him to be loyal to her/his friends.
She/he wants to devote herself/himself to people close to
her/him. Benevolence
6 Liberalisation of regulated
sectors Being very successful is important to her/him. She/he hopes
people will recognise her/his achievements. Achievement
7 Redistribution (from richer to
poorer households) It's very important to her/him to help the people around
her/him. She/he wants to care for their well-being. Benevolence
8 Tolerance (for different lifestyles,
appearances and beliefs)
It is important to her/him to listen to people who are different
from her/him. Even when she/he disagrees with them,
she/he still wants to understand them. Universalism
9 Reducing the top marginal
income tax rate It's important to her/him to show her/his abilities. She/he
wants people to admire what she/he does. Achievement
10 Helping other countries in need
and showing solidarity with them
She/he thinks it is important that every person in the world
should be treated equally. She/he believes everyone should
have equal opportunities in life. Universalism
11 Prestige of the country It is important to her/him to get respect from others. She/he
wants people to do what she/he says. Power
12 Protection of the environment She/he strongly believes that people should care for nature.
Looking after the environment is important to her/him. Universalism
Processing of responses
Responses were coded numerically as follows:
Option Value
Contrary to goals: -2
Indifferent: 0
Goal unless public opposition: 1
One of priority goals: 2
The most important goal: 3
More options with positive than negative values were given as we expected more
answers with positive values and hence it was reasonable to allow for more
nuances to express these. This was confirmed by the distribution of answers. This
is also why the only option with a negative value was set at -2 to reflect the fact that
the options with positive values ranged from 1 to 3.