Conference PaperPDF Available

An efficient algorithm to improve Hardware -in -the -loop therapy for ADHD, based on evoked neural coherences.

Authors:

Abstract

Various studies on ADHD patients come to the conclusion that in the spontaneous EEG the low frequency theta-band (4-7Hz) is increased, whereas the beta-(14-18 Hz), alpha-band and the sensorimotor rhythm (SMR) is decreased compared to healthy volunteers. [1;2;3]. Therefore the theta/beta-ratio is considered to be a good indicator for the diagnosis of ADHD also shown in standardized behavioral studies. [4]. It was therefore not too far fetched to utilize neurofeedback - training for ADHD treatments using the theta/beta-band ratio as a parameter at first. But the application of neurofeedback methods for the ADHD therapy showed ambivalent results: whereas some studies concluded the evidence for the effectiveness of the method for children not responding to medical therapy, [5; 6; 7], other studies state no improvements of hardware – in – the – loop – technique compared to pharmaceutical treatment [8; 9] Of a more serious concern regarding the neurofeedback method could possibly be that the method requires a degree of concentration and learning ability, which is actually lacking in this group.
An efficient algorithm to improve Hardware -in -the -loop therapy
for ADHD, based on evoked neural coherences
Matthias Schmidt, Dietmar Henrich & Tino Schmidt
University of Applied Sciences Zittau/Goerlitz, Institut of Applied Psycholoy, 02826 Goerlitz, Germany (corresponding author e-mail:
matthias.schmidt@hszg.de)
Technical University of Dresden, Institute of General Psychology, 01157 Dresden; Germany, e-mail: schmidtt@b-tu.de
Medical Engineering Department, Brandenburg Technical University 01968 Senftenberg, Germany and Technical University of Dresden,
NeuroImaging Center, 01157 Dresden, Germany (corresponding author e-mail: dietmar.henrich@b-tu.de).
I. INTRODUCTION
Various studies on ADHD patients come to the conclusion that in the spontaneous EEG the low frequency theta-
band (4-7Hz) is increased, whereas the beta-(14-18 Hz), alpha-band and the sensorimotor rhythm (SMR) is
decreased compared to healthy volunteers. [1;2;3]. Therefore the theta/beta-ratio is considered to be a good
indicator for the diagnosis of ADHD also shown in standardized behavioral studies. [4].
It was therefore not too far fetched to utilize neurofeedback - training for ADHD treatments using the theta/beta-
band ratio as a parameter at first.
But the application of neurofeedback methods for the ADHD therapy showed ambivalent results: whereas some
studies concluded the evidence for the effectiveness of the method for children not responding to medical
therapy, [5; 6; 7], other studies state no improvements of hardware in the loop technique compared to
pharmaceutical treatment [8; 9]
Of a more serious concern regarding the neurofeedback method could possibly be that the method requires a
degree of concentration and learning ability, which is actually lacking in this group.
II. EXTERNALIZING THE EFFECT OF NEUROFEEDBACK
A. Hardware in the loop - stimulation
To overcome this drawback of the neurofeedback method an external stimulus of to the neural system could be
applied by using a hardware in the loop - stimulation. The underlying thalamic to cortical mechanism is
considered proven. [10; 11; 12]. But the stimulation frequency is questionable. This might be the reason why this
method is not yet widely applied for the ADHD therapy, although some positive responses are reported
[13;14].
B. Patient adaption
The actual problem of hardware in the loop - methods is, that they are not tuned to the individual ADHD
patient, which is disadvantageous compared to neurofeedback.
At a glance the two methods neurofeedback versus hardware in the loop - stimulation might look very
similar: the first to try to transform a dysfunctional EEG into „normal“ EEG by active training of the patient and
the second utilizing an external stimulus in a more passive manner. The underlying neural mechanism for the
transformation through neurofeedback are not understood..
So far it seems clear that a more large scale transformation of the neural system is needed in order to achieve to
desired changes in the EEG, although from the outside it appears to be just an operant conditioning. [15].
Apparently with neurofeedback we change neuronal mechanism related to the ADHD symptomatics, but with
hardware in the loop - stimulation using passive amplification of the low amplitude frequency bands we
enhance processes not really related to ADHD. At the end the „dysfunctional“ EEG-pattern represent but the
summation potentials of different origins.
The disadvantage in the existing therapies lacking patient adaption explains why hardware in the loop -
stimulation methods show less efficiency compared to feedback method although they don’t rely on
concentration and learning abilities.
III. DISTURBANCE RELEVANT FREQUENCY COMPONENTS
A. Theoretical Background
The difficulty of every hardware in the loop - stimulation is to influence the relevant frequency component.
Since ADHD patients show an increased theta-band (4-7 Hz) and a decreased beta-band (14-18 Hz) EEG activity
it seems obvious to solely stimulate the beta band and to thus co-activate more power in this spectral bands.
Astonishingly this does not lead to an improvement in the ADHD symptoms. Moreover it has to be considered
that the misaligned theta over beta ratio is by itself just a symptom of deeper biological correlates in the brain.
Some studies suggest the misaligned theta over beta ratio is due to a lacking coherence in the phase
synchronization of different cognitive feedback loops [16;17]. In other words the connectivity of more distance
brain areas is disturbed, because the signal pathway of the intermitting layers is getting out of rhythm. Therefore
the patients lack the ability of action control and correction tuning [18; 19].
An effective hardware in the loop - stimulation method should primarily try to re-establish the
synchronization of the involved feedback loops and not only to recover the imbalances seen in the frequency
components purely in „mechanical“ fashion.
B. Evoked versus. Steady-state-coherences
The main problem is determining of the relevant disturbing frequencies causing a de-synchronization that may
result in an attention deficit.
Whereas frequency analysis requires time intervals in the order of several seconds to minutes and is therefore
suitable for steady-state coherences representing basic brain operations, the detection of the specific
incoherences a short term method is necessary in order to identify rapidly changing brain areas involved in the
stimulus processing
IV. EVOKED COHERENCES DETECTION METHOD
The EEG is continuously analyzed with time variant spectral decomposition showing both the amplitude and
frequency. This is in accordance tot he classical δ, ϑ, α andβ bands. Each frequency band is represented by a
sinusoidal oscillation.
The component related coherence values emerge from the cross-correlation of the corresponding oscillations of
various locations. Length and position oft he post-stimulus intervals are determined depending on the average
wavelength of the component under consideration, e.g. 0,5-2,5 average periods after the stimulus. To eliminate
random effects single stimulus evoked coherences are averaged in a series of equal stimuli.
Such a database can be obtained based on oddball paradigm, where patients should count sounds deviating from
a series of equal tones.
Based on of a time variant analysis a audiovisual stimulation should allow a real-time detection in the coherence
changes and to determine the changes in the cognitive system through hardware in the loop - stimulation.
In such a case one would apply a stimulation but unlike in the normal hardware in the loop - stimulation
method this time not with a fixed parameter set, but one adaptive through pre and post comparison thus
controlling how effective the stimulus is.. Moreover the time variant spectral analysis can be continued while
neurostimulating with dynamically adjusted parameters.
The procedure to detect significant changes in the coherence pattern of the EEG is described in the following
paragraph.
Assuming that at the beginning we are interested only at coherences at a single position p (p=1, .., P) we end up
with P-1 variables for each frequency band. An ICA component analysis would lead us to a reduction of M
principal components instead of P-1. Doing this for all possible positions p, we get P*M principal components
instead of P * (P-1)/2 primary variables for one electrode position. Due to the similarity of the electrical activity
in a close neighborhood, the principal components are therefore also very similar. A second order ICA is applied
where higher order components are determined such that their scores give different average values according to
different cognitive tasks, e.g. concentration versus relaxation. The principal components of second order 2Fk
(k=1,..,K) are derived from the first order components 1Fpm (m=1,..,M) via a multiplication of the corresponding
coefficients apm according to:
Based on this, it has to be clarified which electrode positions and which coherences gives the highest score
contributions. The interest is focused on the coherences instead of the principal components of the first degree.
Fpm in equation (1)
The principal components of the first order can be expressed in analogy to equation (1) as:
Where bpmq denotes the score factors from the first stage ICA and Cpq the coherence between electrode position p
and q (both being z-transformed)
Replacing (2) into (1) the result is:
The coefficients of the first order (b) and the second order (a) can be combined to a new coefficient g:
with
The principal components of the second order 2Fk results according to (4) with the partial contributions 2Fkp at the
electrode positions p:
The amplitude 2Fkpq of each single coherence between the electrode position p and q to 2Fkp is therefore:
(7) .
V. CONCLUSION
The detection of evoked coherences offers a technical feasible possibility assisting to alter the cognitive
processes activated autonomously during neurofeeedback.
The application of such an analytic method increases the efficiency of a neurofeedback training run in parallel.
Anyway in cases where neurofeedback is difficult due to lack in concentration and learning ability, like ADHD
or autism the analysis of evoked potentials for the control of hardware in the loop - stimulation could be an
decisive therapeutical advantage.
REFERENCES
[1] J. R. Chabot, F. di Michele, & L.J.E.R. Prichep, The clinical role of computerized EEG in the evaluation and treatment of learning and
attention disorders in children and adolescents,” in Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 2001, Vol. 13(2), p. 171f.
[2] A. R. Clarke, R. J. Barry, R. McCarthy & M. Selikowitz, “Age and sex effects in the EEG: differences in two subtypes of attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder,” in: Clinical Neurophysiology, 2001 Vol. 112, pp. 815-826.
[3] M. Holtmann, C. Stadler, U. Leins, U. Strehl & F. Poustka, Neurofeedback in der Behandlung der Aufmerksamkeitsdefizit-
Hyperaktivitätsstörung (ADHS),“ in: Zeitschrift für Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie und Psychotherapie, 2003, pp. 53-67.
[4] V.J. Monastra, J. F. Lubar, & M. Linden, The development of a quantitative electroencephalographic scanning process for attention
deficit- hyperactivity disorder: Reliability and validity studies,” in Neuropsychology, 2001, Vol 15 (1), pp. 136 144.
[5] S. Othmer, S. F. Othmer, & S. M. Clifford, “EEG biofeedback training for attention deficit disorder, specific learning disabilities, and
associated conduct problems.” in EEG Spectrum, 1991.
[6] D. A. Kaiser, & S. Othmer, “Effect of neurofeedback on variables of attentio n in a large multi-center trial.” in Journal of Neurotherapy,
2000, vol. 4, pp. 5 15.
[7] R. DeBeus, J. D. Ball, M. E. deBeus & R. Herrington, “ Attention training with ADHD children: Preliminary findings in a double-blind
placebo-controlled study.” Presented at the Annual conference of the international society for neuronal regulation, 2003, Houston.
[8] D. E.Cobb & J. R. Evans, “The use of biofeedback techniques with school -aged children exhibiting behavioural and/ or learning
problems,” in: Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 1981, vol. 9 (2), pp. 251 281.
[9] S. W. Lee, “Biofeedback as a treatment for childhood hyperactivity: A critical review of the literature,” in: Psychological Report, 1991,
vol. 68 (1), pp. 163 192.
[10] S. Bartley, “Relation of intensity and duration of brief retinal stimulation by light to the electrical response to the optic cortex of the
rabbit,” in: American Journal of Physiology, 1934, vol. 108, pp. 397-408.
[11] J. McClintic, “Physiology of the human body,” 1978, John Whiley & Sons, New York, NY.
[12] D. Siever, “Handbook of Neurofeedback: Dynamics and Clinical Applications”, Chapter: Audiovisual entrainment: history, p hysiology,
and clinical studies,” 2007 Binghamton, NY: The Haworth Medical Press, pp. 155-183.
[13] J. G. Patrick, “Improved Neuronal Regulation in ADHD: A Application of Fifteen Sessions of Photic Driven EEG,” in: J. Neurother,
1996, vol. 1, pp. 2736.
[14] M. Joyce & D.Siever, “Audio-visual entrainment program as a treatment for behavior disorders in a school setting,” Journal of
Neurotherapy, 2000, vol. 4(2): pp. 9-15.
[15] M. B. Sterman, “Physiological origins and functional correlates of EEG rhythmic activities: Implications for self-regulation” in:
Biofeedback and Self Regulation, 1996, vol. 21 (1), pp. 3 33.
[16] ,J.R. Chabot & G. Serfontein, “Quantitative Electroencephalographic profiles of children with attention deficit disorder. in: Biological
Psychiatry, 1996, vol. 40, pp. 951- 963.
[17] H. J. Volke, P. Dettmar, P. Richter, M. Rudolf, U. Buhss, “Evoced coherences of EEG in mental load: An investigation in chess
players,” in: Zeitschr. für Psychologie, 1999, vol. 207 (2), pp. 233-262.
[18] H. J. Volke, P. Dettmar, P. Richter, M. Rudolf, U. Buhss, “On-coupling and off-coupling of neocortical areas in chess experts and
novices as revealed by evoked EEG coherence measures and factor-based topological analysis—a pilot study” in: J. Psychophysiol., 2002,
vol. 16, pp. 2336.
[19] R. A. Barry, A.R. Clarke, R. McCarthy & M. Selikowitz, “EEG coherence in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a comparative
study of two DSM-IV types,” in: Clinical Neurophysiology, 2002, vol. 113, pp. 579-585
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
The efficacy of brain wave training and EEG biofeedback in the remediation of attentional deficits and specific learning disabilities is evaluated for a study population of fifteen school-age children in a clinical setting using psychological and academic testing. The Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children--Revised (WISC-R) is used in combination with the Wide Range Achievement Test (WRAT), Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT), the Tapping Subtest of the Harris Tests of Lateral Dominance, and the Benton Visual Retention Test (VRT). Behavioral changes are assessed by means of teacher and parental reporting. The training protocol is enhancement of EEG activity in the 15-18 Hz regime, with suppression of excessive activity in the 4-7 Hz and 22-30 Hz regions. Significant improvements in cognitive skills, academic performance, and behavior are found, and confirmed in follow-up. Average improvement in WISC-R full-scale IQ was 23 points. A preference for 15-18 Hz training versus 12-15 Hz training is indicated.
Article
Full-text available
Introduction. It has been suggested that the behavioral manifestations of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) are secondary to neurological abnormalities and are characterized as low brain wave disorders. ADHD children produce higher amounts of theta (5-7 Hz) and less beta (13-21 Hz) brain wave activity than normals. Many researchers are testing the therapeutic effectiveness of AudioVisual Entrainment (AVE) as a treatment for a variety of low arousal brain disorders. AVE is the repetitive and intermittent presentation of light and sound. AVE affects electroencephalographic (EEG) output in that brain wave output can be suppressed or enhanced at specific frequencies.Procedure. Thirty-four elementary students from two different schools were given AVE over the course of seven weeks. Participants were given the Test of Variables of Attention (TOVA) before and after participation. A second group of eight participants were in a special reading (SPALDING) class. All of the students in this class received the Standardized Test for the Assessment of Reading (STAR) and were compared with a control group, n = 12.Results. Overall inattention, impulsivity and variability as rated by the TOVA improved significantly. The eight students from the SPALD-ING reading program who received AVE improved their reading scores more than their classmates who served as controls. The results included normalization as recorded on the TOVA, substantial improvements in reading as recorded on the STAR, and improvements in general behavior as noted by teachers and parents.Discussion. The data suggests AVE was a useful experience for the participants. Parents and teachers reported the children were calmer and could focus better. The results met or exceeded our expectations.
Article
Full-text available
Background: Neurofeedback studies have been criticized for including small numbers of subjects. The effect of SMR-beta neuro-feedback training on the Test of Variables of Attention was evaluated in more than 1,000 subjects from thirty-two clinics.Methods: 1089 subjects (726 children, 324 females, 186 with ADHD or ADD diagnoses) underwent twenty or more sessions of SMR-beta neurofeedback training for attentional and behavioral complaints at thirty-two clinical settings affiliated with EEG Spectrum, Inc. Subjects were evaluated prior to training and at training completion. One hundred and fifty-seven subjects who elected extensive training (forty sessions or more) were tested after both twenty and forty training sessions.Results: Neurofeedback training produced significant improvement in attentiveness, impulse control, and response variability. Significant clinical improvement in one or more measures was seen in eighty-five percent of those subjects with moderate pre-training deficits.Conclusions: Neurofeedback training is effective in remediating atten-tional dysfunction. Nevertheless, large-scale studies with greater control (e.g., wait-list designs) are sorely needed.
Article
Full-text available
Postulated that when solving the same tasks chess experts and novices would show different evoked EEG coherence (EC). The following findings confirmed this supposition. Chess tasks of varying difficulty were presented to 25 male 18-34 yr old chess players of different performance levels. The EEG was recorded from 29 positions. EC were computed for post-stimulus intervals of 1.6 sec. A special averaging procedure (considering Fisher's Z transformation) was applied for summarizing the single EC of each type of task. These subject- and task-related EC-values were subjected to a MANOVA after a two-staged principal component analysis. Significant differences between experts and novices, compared with the resting condition, were found in the delta frequency range. The relevant cortical areas of experts, defined by a topological analysis of the differentiating principal components, were located rather posterior and more in the right hemisphere, compared with those of the novices. Furthermore, the essential EC of the experts showed higher values in the task situation (on-coupling), compared with the resting condition, whereas those of the novices were lower (off-coupling). The results are interpreted as a higher task-related functional integration of the cortical areas in experts. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
The present work investigates the validity of evoked coherences of the EEG as an indicator of mental load. Previous studies have shown that coherences of EEG, generally, react sensitively to subtle changes of a subject's mental date. Instead of the usually calculated ,,steady-state" values, the present study used evoked coherences, that, like evoked potentials, reflect neural information processing in the context of discrete events. Considering the well-known difficulties with the induction of mental load in laboratory conditions, especially in connection with EEG recording, chess playing was chosen as a model of mental activity. Chess diagrams of different complexity were presented for solution to 25 active members of chess clubs. For a differentiation between cognitive and perceptive load, a number of diagrams was provided with visual noise, which hampered recognition. Evoked coherences of EEG showed highly significant differences, nor only between perceptive and cognitive load, but also between different cognitive operations. In our experiment, perceptive load was reflected in the Theta frequency range, whereas cognitive load was reflected in Beta. A two-staged factor analysis procedure, in connection with a topological analysis, allowed the definition of cortical areas participating significantly in the discrimination of different conditions. The results were confirmed in a re-test study. Evoked coherences of EEG, thus, represent a sensitive tool for the investigation of mental load.
Article
This study tested a 15-session electroencephalograph (EEG) driven photic stimulation neural training procedure designed to enhance the regulation of brain wave activity and thus improve cognitive functioning in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) children. The subjects (N=25) were 8-14 year old children of intact families and were screened by a developmental pediatrician for other DSM-4 diagnoses and medical conditions. Some of the subjects were medicated and some were not. A quasi-experimental waiting control group design was used with repeated psychometric tests consisting of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children Third Edition (WISC-3), Raven Progressive Matrices (RPM), Wechsler Individual Achievement Test (WIAT), Achenbach Child Behavior Checklist and Profiles (CBCL-P), the computerized performance Test of Variables of Attention (T.O.V.A.), and two separate EEG measures. No significant changes were noted in any waiting period control group tests. Experimental results revealed highly significant (P< 05 two-tailed) EEG changes, improvements in the WISC-3 processing speed and freedom from distractibility scales, WIAT, CBCL-P, and T.O.V.A. fourth quarter commission error test scores. The results of this study are encouraging. The primary goal was accomplished and the hypotheses were supported by the data. Further study is indicated to explore the effects of longer treatment courses, different training goals, and better data procurement procedures using outcome measures of EEG variability coupled with successful psychometric performance.