ArticlePDF Available

The effects of flattening fundamental frequency contours on sentence intelligibility in speakers with dysarthria

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

This study explored the relationship between F0 variability and intelligibility in persons with motor speech disorders. The literature suggests that monopitch may have detrimental effects on the intelligibility of individuals with dysarthria (and other speech disorders), but few studies have examined the independent effect of a Rat F0 contour on intelligibility in speakers with articulatory difficulties. An LPC resynthesis technique was used to reduce the speakers' sentence F0 range by 25%, 50% and 100% (flattened F0). Two dysarthric speaker groups were evaluated, one with prosodic inadequacy and imprecise articulation( hypokinetic) and one with imprecise articulation only (UUMN). Speech intelligibility was assessed using a word transcription task and an interval-scaling task. Results argue strongly for the perceptual importance of sentence-level F0 variations on speech intelligibility even when the F0 range is severely restricted. Results also suggest that the contribution of F0 to intelligibility may vary with type of dysarthria.
Content may be subject to copyright.
clinical lingui stics & phonetics, 2001, vol. 15, no. 3,181± 193
The ects of ¯ attening fundamental
frequency contours on sentence intelligibility
in speakers with dysarthria
K AT E B U N T O N ² , R AY D . K E N T ² ,
J A N E F . K E N T² a n d JO S E P H R . DU F F Y ³
² Waisman Center, University of Wisconsin-Madison, USA
³ Mayo Clinic, Rochester, MN, USA
(Received 15 April 2000; accepted 7 August 2000 )
Abstract
This study explored the relationship between F0 variability and intelligibility in
persons with motor speech disorders. The literature suggests that monopitch may
have detrimental eŒects on the intelligibility of individuals with dysarthria (and
other speech disorders), but few studies have examined the independent eŒect of
a ¯ at F0 contour on intelligibility in speakers with articulatory di culties. An
LPC resynth esis technique was used to reduce the speakers’ sentence F0 range
by 25%, 50% and 100% (¯ attened F0) . Two dysarthric speaker groups were
evaluated, one with prosodic inadequacy and imprecise articulation (hypokinetic)
and one with imprecise articulation only ( U UMN ) . Speech intelligibility was
assessed using a word transcription task and an interval-scaling task. Results
argue strongly for the perceptu al importance of sentence-level F0 variations on
speech intelligibility even when the F0 range is severely restricted. Results also
suggest that the contribution of F0 to intelligibility may vary with type of
dysarthria.
Keywords: Dysarthria, prosody, intelligibility.
Introduction
The term `monopitch’ is frequently used to describe certain types of dysarthria
(Darley et al., 1969a, b). The lack of F0 variability across an utterance (the presumed
physical basis of this term) has been substantiated by several acoustic studies
(Yorkston and Buekelman, 1981; LeDorze et al., 1992; Bunton et al., 2000) .
However, relatively little attention has been focused on how much the perceptual
Address correspondence to: Ray Kent, University of Wisconsin-Madison, 435 Waisman
Center, 1500 Highland Avenue, Madison, WI 53705 , USA. e-mail: kent@waisman.wisc.edu
Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics
ISSN 0269-920 6 print/ISSN 1464-5076 online Ñ2001 Taylor & Francis Ltd
http://www.tandf.co.uk/journals
DOI: 10.1080/02699200010003378
K. Bunton et al.182
system utilizes information supplied by prosodic patterns and its relationship with
speech intelligibility. Kent and Rosenbek (1982 ) reported that ¯ attened F0 contours
blur the contrast between adjacent units in the speech signal and have a detrimental
ect on intelligibility. However, they also noted that intelligibility and prosodic
disturbance are not necessarily equally impaired in all subjects.
Variation of F0 has been reported to contribute to intelligibility of neurologically
normal speech. Prosody has been shown to be an important perceptual cue for
vowel identity (Traunmuller, 1981 ) , stop consonant voicing (Haggard et al., 1970 ),
syllable stress ( Lehiste, 1970), and marking lexical boundaries (Liss et al., 1998 ).
Bradlow et al. ( 1996 ) studied both global and ® ne-grained diŒerences among talkers
as these diŒerences correlated with inter-speaker diŒerences in intelligibility. They
concluded that a highly intelligible speaker was one who produces sentences with a
relatively wide range of F0, a relatively expande d vowel space that includes a
substantial F1 variation, precise articulation of the point vowels and a high precision
of inter-segmenta l timing. Two studies that examined the independent eŒects of F0
contour on intelligibility in utterances produced by persons with no history of speech
disorders showed signi® cant diŒerences in intelligibility between the original utter-
ances and those with ¯ attened F0 contours (Laures and Weismer, 1999; Laures
et al., 1999 ). This decrease in intelligibility was found for subjects whose articulatory
precision was intact. For speakers with neurologic disease, who experience articulat-
ory di culties, there is little published informatio n about the independent relation-
ship between reduced F0 variation and speech intelligibility (for a review see Ramig,
1992 ). In developing an acoustic-phonetic model of intelligibility in dysarthria ( Kent
et al., 1989), it is of interest to know how much sentence-level prosody contributes
to speech intelligibility de® cits, in the presence of segmental-leve l articulatory de® cits.
The present study was designed to determine how a lack of sentence-level F0
¯ uctuation contribute s to speech intelligibility de® cits in two groups of speakers
with dysarthria. Speakers with hypokinetic dysarthria as the result of Parkinson s
Disease typically exhibit decreased F0 ranges along with some segmental-level articu-
latory de® cits. Alternatively, speakers who experienced a cerebrovascular accident
resulting in unilateral upper motor neuron dysarthria typically have imprecise
consonants but do not exhibit di culties with prosody.
Methods
Speakers
Ten sentences randomly selected from the Fisher-Logemann Test of Articulation
Competence (1971), each 5± 7 words in length, collected from four speakers diagnosed
with Parkinsons Disease (PD: 2M, 2F ), four with left cerebrovascula r accident
(LCVA: 2M, 2F ) , and two with right CVA (1M, 1F ) were analysed and used as
perceptual stimuli in the present study. The set of ten sentences was diŒerent for
each speaker, and was collected as part of a larger database on speech intelligibility.
Speaker characteristic s can be found in table 1. The range of intelligibility scores,
calculated from the Kent et al. ( 1989 ) intelligibility test, ranged from 85% to 96%.
This range of intelligibility was selected based on previous work demonstratin g that
the habitual amount of F0 variability decreases as overall intelligibility scores
decrease ( Bunton et al., 2000). The type of dysarthria was identi® ed by an experi-
enced speech pathologist (ibid.). These two types of dysarthria ( hypokinetic and
Flattening f undamental frequency contours 183
Table 1. Speaker characteristics. The type of dysarthria is based on judgement s of the Wnal
author who is a certiWed speech pathologist. The duration column includes time between
diagnosis and recording as well as length of time their speech has been aŒected if
diŒerent from medical diagnosis
Intelligibility
Subject Gender Age Diagnosis Duration Dysarthria mean (SD)
CS M 69 R CVA 27 days UUMN 86.7 (5.23)
TD F 23 R CVA 11 days UUMN 93.4 (2.7 )
FL F 78 L CVA 12 days UUMN 87.5 (1.6 )
BL M 71 L CVA 9 days UUMN 92.9
MB F 66 L CVA 18 days UUMN 85.46 (2.6 )
BT M 56 L CVA 8 days UUMN 86.52
EB F 75 P D 4 y speech/8 y Dx hypokin etic 92.52
LW F 61 P D 10 y speech/15 y Dx hypokinetic 95.25
LA M 70 PD 16 y hypokinetic 92.63
LB M 68 PD 1 y hypokinetic 88.95
unilateral upper motor neuron) were selected based on the perceptual prominence
of suprasegmental characteristics associate d with one but not the other, while both
dysarthrias are characterized by articulatory imprecision. Ten healthy, age-matched
speakers (® ve male, ® ve female) were included as controls.
Acoustic analysis
To quantify the habitual use of frequency variation for each speaker, the F0 minimum
and maximum were recorded for each utterance using Cspeech ( Milenkovic, 1997 ).
In addition, peak frequencies associated with each vocalic segment in the utterance
were measured, to ensure that the syllable structur e of the utterance was unchanged
during resynthesis of the utterances (see below).
Resynthesis
A linear predictive coding ( LPC) based algorithm that allowed for sentence-level
F0 modi® cations and did not aŒect the temporal or formant frequency characteristic s
of the utterances was used for resynthesis in the present study ( Milenkovic, 1999 ).
Each utterance was resynthesized prior to modifying the F0 contour. These unmodi-
® ed, resynthesized utterance s were used as control items in the listening experiments
to ensure that the technique itself did not have any eŒect on the intelligibility of the
sentences. The F0 contour of the utterance was then modi® ed in three ways: (1 ) each
voiced segment was ¯ attened by setting all pitch periods equal to the mean value
calculated over the entire utterance (hereafter referred to as ¯ at F0); (2) the range
of F0 variability was reduced by 25% while maintaining the syllable structur e of the
utterance; and (3) the range was reduced by 50%. For conditions 2 and 3, peak F0
for each vocalic segment was subtracted from the mean. This value was multiplied
by the percentage decrease (25% or 50%) and the resultant value was added to
original peak value, thus bringing the peak 25% (or 50%) closer to the mean value.
For example, if the F0 mean for an utterance was 150 Hz and the individual peak
was 200 Hz and a reduction of the F0 range by 25% was desired, the following
equation applies [( 150Õ200)Ö0.251200 5187.5 Hz] . This procedure was used for
K. Bunton et al.184
each individual peak within the utterance thereby preserving the F0 contour of the
speaker’ s original production. An example of a spectrogram and F0 contour for an
original utterance (panel a) an unmodi® ed, resynthesized utteranc e (panel b ), one
with an F0 range reduced by 25% ( panel c), and one with a ¯ at F0 ( panel d ) are
shown in ® gure 1 to illustrate that the resynthesis technique did not modify the
spectral and tempora l properties of the utterance. The utterance shown is `They all
know what I said’ produced by a NG female speaker. The 800 utterances ( ten
utterances per speakerÖfour resynthesis conditionsÖ20 speakers) were placed into
a single list to be used during the perceptual portion of the experiment.
Listeners
Ten graduate students ( 5M, 5F ) served as listeners. All students had taken a graduate-
level dysarthria course and had no self-reported hearing loss. Listeners with limited
exposure to dysarthric speech were selected to avoid ceiling eŒects related to either
the novelty of the speech or highly tuned listening skills. Listeners were seated in a
sound booth and stimuli were presented over a loudspeaker at a comfortable listening
level. The order of the utterances from the 800-utterance pool was generated randomly
by the computer for each listener to control for order eŒects. Speakers heard all 800
utterances for each of the listening tasks as well as 40 repeat utterances ( ten from
each resynthesis condition) to obtain a measure of intra-judge reliability. The listening
task was divided into four sections of 420 utterances each and listeners were given a
20-minute break between sections. The ® rst time the listeners heard the utterances
(sets 1 and 2), they were asked to write down what they heard as accurately as
possible. The second time the listeners heard the utterances (sets 3 and 4) they were
asked to assign a scale value of intelligibility using a 7-point equal-interval scale. The
scale was de® ned for listeners as `A scaling of 1 is equal to 0% intelligible; a scaling
of 7 is equal to 100% intelligible. For this study, intelligibility is de® ned as the ease
with which you understand the words spoken’.
Intelligibility measures
The purpose in intelligibility testing in the present study was twofold. The ® rst
measure of intelligibility, transcription, was selected to get a measure of the degree
to which the listener recovers the discriminations intended by the speaker, that is,
to determine the accuracy with which the speakers intended message was recovered
by the listener. The second measure, scaling, provided a quick, overall index of a
speaker’ s intelligibility based on the listeners’ perceived eŒort in recovering the
message. Segmental level errors produced by the speakers were not modi® ed during
resynthesis of the F0 contours , therefore, an explanatory approach to intelligibility
testing designed to identify the phonetic loci of the intelligibility de® cit such as
presented by Kent et al. ( 1989, 1990, 1992) was not appropriate for the present study.
Data analysis
Data analysis for sets 1 and 2 were achieved by calculating the number of words
correctly transcribed for each utterance and speaker, and averaging across listeners.
Scaled values (sets 3 and 4) were also averaged across listeners. Non-parametric
statistics were used to separatel y analyse the results of the transcriptio n and scal-
ing tasks.
Flattening f undamental frequency contours 185
Figure 1. Examples of spectrograms and F0 contours the token `They all know what I said’.
Panel a is the original utt erance, followed by a resynthesized, unmodiWed condition
(panel b), 25% reduced F0 range (c), and Xat F0 (d) produced by a NG female.
Reliability
Forty utterances were repeated from the set of 800 original tokens for each listener
to obtain measures of intra-judge reliability. The overall correlation coe cients
K. Bunton et al.186
between the ® rst and second presentation of each utteranc e ranged between 0.83
and 0.97 for the ten listeners on the transcriptio n task. For measures of inter-judge
reliability, the correlation coe cients were between 0.77 and 0.98 for each speaker
across listeners. For the scaling task, reliability was considerably lower for both
inter- and intra-judge measures. The inter-judge coe cients ranged from 0.62 to
0.88 and the intra-judge values ranged from 0.54 to 0.78. No systematic shifts ( i.e.,
improvements) in performance were noted for individual listeners across tokens.
Results
Acoustic characteristics of speakers
Table 2 shows the means and standard deviations for the mean F0, mean high and
low F0 and the range of F0 variability averaged across the ten utterance s for each
speaker. To calculate the range of F0 variation, the high and low frequency peaks
for each utterance were measured and values were then averaged across the utter-
ances. This method was used rather than subtracting the mean high and low F0
values to account for the variability in performance for individual speakers. The
range of F0 across utterances for all disordered speakers was smaller than for the
control speakers (see also Bunton et al., 2000 ) . The reduction in range varied from
22% to 68%. A compariso n of range of F0 variability with intelligibility scores failed
to show a relationship between F0 variability within an utterance and the overall
intelligibility score. In fact, the two PD speakers with the highest intelligibility scores
have the most restricted frequency ranges. This data can be seen in ® gure 2. In this
® gure the NG speakers are represented by diamonds, the PD speaker s by squares,
R CVA by triangles and L CVA by circles.
Table 2. Mean and standard deviations shown in parentheses for the mean, high, low, the range
of F0 values for each speaker
Speaker Gender Dx Mean F0 High F0 Low F0 F0 range
CS M R CVA 127.6 ( 4.3 ) 181.4 (14.8) 101.5 ( 5.9 ) 72.3 (13.3)
TD F R CVA 179.2 ( 22.3 ) 213.5 (15.4 ) 142.8 ( 8.9 ) 79.0 (16.4 )
FL F L CVA 186.5 ( 12.4 ) 242.8 ( 6.5 ) 164.7 (9.4 ) 70.5 ( 18.4 )
BL M L CVA 123.1 (16.6) 156.3 ( 8.4 ) 114.5 (5.4 ) 36.0 ( 17.2 )
MB F L CVA 158.8 (9.7) 186.9 ( 4.7 ) 135.8 (7.4 ) 55.3 ( 18.4 )
BT M L CVA 118.5 (9.8) 164.2 (13.5 ) 110.0 ( 10.6 ) 58.6 (13.2 )
EB F PD 215.4 ( 16.8 ) 240.5 ( 10.2 ) 192.5 ( 6.8 ) 57.2 (17.1)
LW F PD 199.7 ( 18.4 ) 215.5 (12.5 ) 181.4 ( 8.1 ) 45.0 (6.9 )
LA M PD 117.5 (13.1) 146.8 ( 11.4 ) 109.8 ( 10.0 ) 26.7 ( 16.1 )
LB M PD 146.5 ( 9.8) 168.2 (9.7) 135.3 (4.1) 59.2 (14.6 )
NG1 F 231.5 ( 16.1 ) 261.5 (13.2 ) 166.2 ( 16.8 ) 110.5 ( 16.4 )
NG2 F 202.8 ( 11.4 ) 260.8 (16.7 ) 154.8 ( 16.7 ) 125.7 ( 17.2 )
NG3 F 192.8 ( 21.5 ) 245.1 (12.5 ) 164.2 ( 21.7 ) 116.7 ( 13.7 )
NG4 F 215.7 ( 5.1 ) 269.4 (5.2 ) 141.1 (18.4 ) 132.5 (11.8)
NG5 F 216.7 ( 7.8 ) 277.6 (6.5 ) 175.2 (26.4 ) 109.5 (16.2)
NG6 M 126.8 (15.2) 188.2 ( 6.4 ) 90.5 (16.4 ) 105.2 (17.4)
NG7 M 158.7 (16.2) 190.9 ( 13.7 ) 110.0 (14.3) 89.4 (13.9 )
NG8 M 148.9 (9.8) 196.2 (13.4 ) 92.5 (12.6 ) 110.5 (17.2 )
NG9 M 151.0 (13.4) 199.8 ( 9.5 ) 99.9 (13.7 ) 93.8 (11.7 )
NG10 M 133.9 (21.1) 179.5 ( 13.6 ) 85.7 (6.9 ) 98.7 ( 13.4 )
Flattening f undamental frequency contours 187
Figure 2. Speech intelligibility scores plotted against natural F0 range for the three disorder
groups and mean values for the NG male and female speakers (diamonds5NG,
squares 5PD, triangles 5RCVA, circles 5LCVA).
Transcription data
Transcription scores were calculated by counting the number of words correctly
recorded out of a total of 70. Results of the perceptual transcription task showed
an expected decline in transcriptio n score corresponding to the systematic decrease
in the F0 range for the NG and CVA speakers (® gure 3), although for the NG-M
group the reduction in intelligibility was negligible. The PD speakers, on the other
hand, showed a decrease in accuracy for the habitual utterances and 25% and 50%
reduced F0 range utterances. The utterances with a ¯ attened F0 contour, however,
had higher transcription scores than those utterances resynthesized with reduced
ranges. The ¯ at F0 transcription scores were comparable to the speakers’ habitual
utterances. Plots of the frequency range and transcription scores are shown in
® gure 4 for individual speakers: the three panels of the ® gure each represent speakers
with a diŒerent neurologica l diagnosis. In these panels the female speakers are shown
as triangles and the male speakers as circles. The PD speakers (panel a) all showed
an increase in transcriptio n scores for ¯ at F0 utterances compared to the tokens
Figure 3. Means and standard deviations on the transcription task for each speaker group
across listeners. The total number of words possible was 70.
K. Bunton et al.188
Figure 4. Transcription scores plotted against F0 variation for individual speakers in the three
disorder groups.
with reduced F0 ranges. Results of non-parametri c statistica l analysis show signi® c-
ant diŒerences between habitual utterances and range-reduced utterances for the PD
speakers and between the habitual utterances and only ¯ at F0 utterances for the
other three speaker groups (table 3).
Scaling data
Results of the scaling task were similar to those found for the transcription task
gure 5) , the listeners rated utterance s with decreased or ¯ at F0 contours as less
intelligible or more di cult with scaling scores closer to 1. This is consistent with
the decline in transcriptio n scores, with the exception of the PD speakers. For the
two female PD speakers there was little change in their scaling scores across the
four resynthesis conditions, whereas for the male PD speakers there was a drastic
fall in the scaling scores when F0 contours were reduced. This occurred despite
Flattening f undamental frequency contours 189
Table 3. Results of non-parametric statistical analysis comparing the habitual utterances with
the modiWed ones. A letter indicates a signiWcant Wnding (T 5transcription, S 5scaling)
Speaker Group 25% reduction 50% r eduction Flat F0
NG M T
F T
PD M T S T
F T T
RCVA M T
F T
LCVA M T
F T
Figure 5. Mean scaling scores for each speaker group across listeners. A 7.0 Lickert scale was
used for this task.
improvement in transcriptio n scores when the F0 contour was ¯ attened (® gure 4).
Plots of the F0 range versus scaling scores for individual speakers are shown
in ® gure 6. Similar to the transcriptio n plots, female speakers are represented as
triangles and male speakers as circles.
Discussion
This study was designed to explore the relationship between F0 variability and
intelligibility in persons with hypokineti c and UUMN dysarthria associated with
PD and CVA, respectively. Results argue strongly for the perceptual importance of
sentence-level F0 variations on speech intelligibility in the presence of segmental-
level errors for both speaker groups. It appears that even though reduced F0 ranges
were characteristic of the habitual utterances produced by the dysarthric speakers
in the present study (table 2), the frequency information was an important perceptual
component of the signal. Further ¯ attening the F0 range using a resynthesis technique
resulted in signi® cant decreases in intelligibility for both groups as well as in the
control speakers. In the control group decreased intelligibility scores were also seen
as the F0 was compressed, however, the eŒect on overall sentence intelligibility was
roughly half that of the disorder groups (® gure 3). The mean decrease in transcriptio n
K. Bunton et al.190
Figure 6. Scaling scores plotted against F0 variation for the three disorder groups.
score for the NG-M and NG-F groups were 8.6 and 16.4 words, respectively,
whereas the decrease for the CVA groups were between 21.7 and 27.1 words.
Comparing the right and left CVA groups, lower transcription scores were found
for left versus right although the diŒerence was not statistically signi® cant.
One question guiding this experiment was whether or not there was a critical
amount of F0 variability below which speech intelligibility would decrease precipit-
ously. Looking at the ® ndings for the NG speakers in the present study, it does not
appear that there is such a value. A continuous and gradual decrease in transcription
scores was found as the frequency range was reduced. The same conclusion can be
drawn for the CVA speaker groups; a systematic decline in transcription scores,
which corresponded to the decreasing F0 range, was found even when the speakers
exhibited segmental-level compromise that may have increased the di culty of the
task for the listener. Results for the PD speakers, on the other hand, were peculiar.
These speakers showed a similar decrease in transcription scores compared to normal
and CVA speakers, but when F0 informatio n was removed entirely from the signal
Flattening f undamental frequency contours 191
their transcription scores improved to nearly what they were for their habitual
productions (® gure 4) . One possible explanation for this ® nding could be that the
arti® cially restricted frequency variability (seen in the 25% and 50% range reduction
resynthesis) provided con¯ icting or ambiguous information within the signal increas-
ing the listeners di culty in understanding the sentence. This seems consistent with
the lower scaling scores found during the second part of the experiment, especially
for the two male PD speakers (® gure 5). Another possible explanation is that because
PD is a degenerative disease these speakers have adopted alternative or compensatory
strategies to preserve speech intelligibility in the presence of other system limitations
(e.g., ability to modulate F0). In the present case, temporal cues seem to be the
most logical candidate and further investigation is warranted. In the utterances
which did not contain any frequency information, perhaps listeners were able to
successfully alter their perceptual strategies and use these `exaggerated duration
cues to aid in their understandin g of the utterances. For the utterances with arti® cially
limited F0 variability (resynthesized with 25% or 50% range reduction) there may
have been a mismatch between F0 and temporal information which resulted in lower
transcription scores. Further analysis of a second dysarthria type, also the result of
a progressive neurologic disease (e.g. ALS ), may shed light on whether and how
compensatory strategies are being used by these speakers to maintain speech
intelligibility.
Results of the scaling task were consistent with the transcription task: reduction
in the range of F0 variability corresponde d to lower scaling scores re¯ ective of
increased di culty or eŒort reported by the listeners. A regression analysis between
the transcription scores and the scaling scores, however, showed a fairly strong
relationship between the two tasks (® gure 7). However, low values of inter- and
intra-judge reliability raises questions about the ability of the listeners to perform
this task and thus limit conclusions that can be drawn from these results.
It was hypothesized in the present study that in dysarthric speech, where acoustic
representations of supraglottic articulation are likely to be de® cient, the eŒect of
¯ attening an F0 contour on speech intelligibility would be greater than for control
Figure 7. Regression analysis for the transcription score versus scaling score across speakers.
K. Bunton et al.192
speakers who show no articulatory di culties. Both the neurologically normal
speakers and those with neurologic disease showed a decrease in transcriptio n and
scaling scores as the F0 range was reduced, however, the eŒect was larger for the
speakers whose articulatory precision was compromised. Further, no critical amount
of F0 variability was found, and it appears that syllable contrast resulting from even
minor F0 variations is an important perceptual cue, thus contributing greatly to
speech intelligibility.
Acknowledgements
This work has been funded by NIH R01 DC00319 and T32 DC00042. Parts of this
work were included in a presentatio n made at the 10th Biennial Conference on
Motor Speech, 2000.
References
Bradlow, A., Torretta, G. and Piso ni, D., 1996, Intelligibility of normal speech I. Global
and ® ne grained acoustic-phone tic talker characteristics. Speech Communication,20,
255± 272.
Bunton, K., Ken t, R., Kent, J. and Ro senbek, J., 2000, Perceptuo-acoustic ass essment of
prosodic impairment in dysarthria. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics,14, 13± 24.
Darley, F., Aronson, A. and Brown, J., 1969a, DiŒere ntial diagnostic patterns of dysarthria.
Journal of Speech and Hearing Research,12, 246± 269.
Darley, F., Aronson, A. and Brown, J., 1969b, Clusters of deviant speech dimensions in
the dysarthrias. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research,12, 462± 496.
Fisher, H. B. and Logemann, J., 1971, Fisher-Logemann Test of Articulation Competence
(Boston: Houghton Mi‚ in).
Haggard , M., Ambler, S. and Callow, M., 1970, Pitch as a voicing cue. Journal of the
Acoustical Society of America,47, 613± 617.
Kent, J.F., Kent, R.D., Rosenbek, J., Wei smer, G., Martin, R., Sufit, R.and
Brooks, B. R., 1992, Quantitative description of the dysarthria in women with
amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research,35, 723± 733.
Kent, R.D., Kent, J.F., Weismer, G., Suf it, R., Rosenbek, J.C., Martin, R. E. and
Brooks, B. R., 1990, Impairment of speech intelligibility in men with amyotrophic
lateral sclerosis. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorder s,55, 721± 728.
Kent, R., Weismer, G., Ken t, J. and Ro senbek, J., 1989, Toward phonetic intelligibility
testing in dysarthria. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders,54, 482± 489.
Kent, R. and Rosen bek, J., 1982, Prosodic disturbance and neurologic lesion. Brain and
Language,15, 259± 291.
Laures, J. and Wei smer, G., 1999, The eŒect of a ¯ atte ned F0 on intelligibility at the sentence-
level. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research,42, 1148± 1156.
Laures, J., Bu nton, K. and Weismer, G., 1999, The eŒec t of Xattened F0 contours on sentence
intelligibility. Paper presented at the 1999 meeting of the American Speech, Language
and Hearing Association, San Francisco, CA.
LeDorze, G., Dio nne, L., Ryalls, J., Julien, M. and Ouellett, L., 1992, The eŒect of
speech and language therapy for a case of dysarthria associated with Parkinso n’s
disease. European Journal of Disorders of Communication,27, 313± 324.
Lehiste, I., 1970, Suprasegmentals (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).
Liss, J., Spitzer, S., Caviness, J., Adler, C. and Edwards, B., 1998, Syllabic strength and
lexical boundary decisions in the perception of hypokinetic dysarthric speech. Journal
of the Acoustical Society of America,104, 2457± 2466.
Milenkovic, P., 1997, Cspeech Version 4.0 (computer program: University of Wisconsin-
Madison).
Flattening f undamental frequency contours 193
Ramig, L., 1992, The role of phonation in speech intelligibility: a review and preliminary
data from patients with Parkinson’s Disease. In R. D. Kent (Ed.), Intelligibility in
speech disorders ( Amsterdam: John Benhamins), pp. 119± 155.
Traunmuller, H., 1981, Perceptual dimensions of openn ess in vowels. Journal of the
Acoustical Society of America,69, 1465± 1475.
Yorkston, K. and Bu ekelman, D., 1981, Ataxic Dysarthria: treatment sequences based on
intelligibility and prosodic considerations. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders,
46, 398± 404.
... In regard to sentence-level Chapter 1 18 research, features associated with F0 have been found to be linearly related to intelligibility. Decreased intelligibility has been observed for reduced F0 range for dysarthric (Bunton et al., 2001) and healthy speakers (e.g., Bradlow et al., 1996;Tjaden et al., 2014a;Watson & Schlauch, 2008). ...
... A broad consensus is that orthographic transcriptions yield reliable and valid measures (Bunton et al., 2001;Miller, 2013;Tjaden & Wilding, 2010) since they rely on the amount of information listeners accurately perceived. In contrast, scale ratings have been questioned since they rely on the listeners' Chapter 3 44 impression of intelligibility. ...
... This finding is consistent with the results of previous studies (Ganzeboom et al., 2016;Stipancic et al., 2016;Tjaden et al., 2014a), in which high interrater reliability values (above 0.90) were also reported for VAS. Reliability was also high for AcW, which is in line with the broad consensus that transcription yields good interrater reliability (Bunton et al., 2001;Miller, 2013;Tjaden et al., 2014a;Tjaden & Wilding, 2010). ...
... All the metrics presented in this literature review and their mathematical formulas are summarized in Table 1. Bunton et al., 2001;Daniele & Patel, 2013;Grabe & Low, 2002;Kim & Choi, 2017;Martinez-Sánchez et al., 2016;Tavi & Werner, 2020) Acoustic speech analyses can possibly identify markers of disease, its progression, and the severity of speech disorder (Bocklet et al., 2011;Kato et al., 2018;Khodabakhsh et al., 2015;Miller, 2017). ...
... Frota, S., Cruz, M., Cardoso, R., Guimarães, I., Ferreira, J., Pinto, S., & Vigário, M. (2021). Bunton et al., 2001;Daniele & Patel, 2013;Grabe & Low, 2000;Kim & Choi, 2017;Tavi & Werner, 2020;Tsao et al., 2006) Variable Definition Pitch ...
Article
Full-text available
The aim of this study was to assess prosodic features in Finnish speakers with (n=16) and without (n=20) Parkinson's disease (PD), as there are no published studies to date of prosodic features in Finnish speakers with PD. Chosen metrics were articulation rate (syllables/second), pitch (mean F0) and pitch variability (standard deviation F0), energy proportion below 1kHz (epb1kHz), normalized pairwise variability index (nPVI), and a novel syllabic prosody index (SPI). Four statistically significant results were found: 1) energy was distributed more to lower frequencies in speakers with PD compared to control speakers, 2) male PD speakers had higher pitch and 3) higher syllabic prosody index compared to control males, and 4) female PD speakers had narrower pitch variability than controls. In this study, PD was manifested as less emphatic and breathier voice. Interestingly, male PD speakers' dysprosody was manifested as an effortful speaking style, whereas female PD speakers exhibited dysprosody with a monotonous speaking style. A novel syllable-based prosody index was found to be a potential tool in analyzing prosody in disordered speech.
... A broad consensus is that orthographic transcriptions yield reliable and valid measures (Bunton et al., 2001;Miller, 2013;Tjaden & Wilding, 2010), since they rely on the amount of information listeners accurately perceived. In contrast, rating tasks have been questioned, since they rely on the listeners' impression of intelligibility. ...
... This finding is consistent with results of previous studies Stipancic et al., 2016;Tjaden et al., 2014), in which high interrater reliability values (above 0.90) were also reported for VAS. Reliability was also high for AcW, which is in line with the broad consensus that transcription yields good interrater reliability (Bunton et al., 2001;Miller, 2013;Tjaden et al., 2014;Tjaden & Wilding, 2010). Note that we did not measure intra-rater reliability due to several reasons. ...
Article
Full-text available
Speech intelligibility is an essential though complex construct in speech pathology. In this paper, we investigated the interrater reliability and validity of two types of intelligibility measures: a rating-based measure, through Visual Analogue Scales (VAS), and a transcription-based measure called Accuracy of Words (AcW), through two forms of orthographic transcriptions, one containing only existing words (EWTrans) and one allowing all sorts of words, including both existing words and pseudowords (AWTrans). Both VAS and AcW scores were collected from five expert raters. We selected speakers with various severity levels of dysarthria (SevL) and employed two types of speech materials, i.e. meaningful sentences and word lists. To measure reliability, we applied Generalizability Theory, which is relatively unknown in the field of pathological speech and language research but enables more comprehensive analyses than traditional methods, e.g. the intraclass correlation coefficient. The results convincingly indicate that five expert raters were sufficient to provide reliable rating-based (VAS) and transcription-based (AcW) measures, and that reliability increased as the number of raters or utterances increased. Generalizability Theory has proved effective in systematically dealing with reliability issues in our experimental design. We also investigated construct and concurrent validity. Construct validity was addressed by exploring the correlations between VAS and AcW within and across speech materials. Concurrent validity was addressed by exploring the correlations between our measures, i.e. VAS and AcW, and two external measures, i.e. phoneme intelligibility and SevL. The correlations corroborate the validity of VAS and AcW to assess speech intelligibility, both in sentences and word lists.
... In particular, intonation conveys emotional quality, with a relatively strong degree of vocal differentiation across specific emotional states (Scherer, 1986), and provides various linguistic cues. The contribution of F0 to speech intelligibility was examined in studies of normal and disordered speech (Binns & Culling, 2007;Bunton et al., 2001;Watson & Schlauch, 2008), which found that sentences or phrases characterized by relatively greater F0 variation, as indexed by measures such as F0 standard deviation 1 (F0SD), tended to be associated with relatively better speech intelligibility. Conflicting results were reported in studies on the effects of PD on F0. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective: Parkinson’s disease (PD) is associated with impairment in producing emotions conveyed by voice which could depend on motor limitations of the vocal apparatus and/or alterations in emotional processing. This study explores the relationship between the standard deviation of fundamental frequency (F0SD) of emotional speech and the volume of specific gray matter regions. Method: Fifteen PD patients and 15 healthy controls (HC) were asked to produce different emotions vocally elicited by reading short stories. For each vocal track, the F0SD was calculated as index of variability. All subjects underwent a structural magnetic resonance imaging and a voxel-based morphometry analysis. An ad hoc mask of brain regions implicated in emotional prosody was constructed to test the relationship between F0SD and the level of brain atrophy. Results: PD patients showed lower F0SD values than HC in the expression of anger. Neuroimaging results showed brain atrophy in PD patients in a widespread bilateral network, including frontal areas, left cingulate cortex, parietal areas as well as occipital cortices. In the PD group, a positive correlation was observed between F0SD values of anger and volumes of the bilateral supramarginal gyrus, left thalamus, right inferior frontal gyrus, and amygdala. Conclusions: The lower F0SD values observed in PD patients in anger production are consistent with their lower ability to express anger effectively through voice compared to HC. Our data demonstrated the involvement of right-lateralized areas, such as the inferior frontal gyrus and amygdala, which are typically involved in emotional prosody. Disturbances in emotion processing might contribute to speech production deficits in PD, probably in addition to the motor impairment of the articulatory system.
... Studies have shown that the maximum frequency range in patients with PD is often lower than normal population, which may be due to rigidity of the cricothyroid muscles in the larynx (Bunton et al., 2001;York, 2020). Two units of measurement can be used to measure the frequency range: hertz and semitone. ...
Article
Background: Although speech and voice disorders are common in Parkinson's disease (PD), there is insufficient evidence to support the effectiveness of behavioural speech therapies in these patients. Aims: This study aimed to examine the effects of a new tele-rehabilitation program, a combining of conventional speech therapy and singing intervention, on voice deficits in patients with PD. Methods & procedures: This study was a three-armed, assessor-masked, randomised controlled trial. Thirty-three people with PD were randomly assigned to the combination therapy, conventional speech therapy, or singing intervention group. This study followed the Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials guidelines for non-pharmacological treatment. Each patient participated in 12 tele-rehabilitation sessions over 4 weeks. The combination therapy group received speech and singing interventions simultaneously (respiratory, speech, voice, and singing exercises). Voice intensity as a primary outcome and the voice handicap index (VHI), maximum frequency range, jitter and shimmer as secondary outcomes were evaluated 1 week before the first intervention session, 1 week after the last intervention session and 3 months after the last evaluation. Outcomes & results: The results of repeated measures analysis of variance showed a significant main effect of time on all outcomes in all three groups after treatment (p < 0.001). There was a significant group effect for voice intensity (p < 0.001), VHI (p < 0.001), maximum frequency range (p = 0.014) and shimmer (p = 0.001). The combination therapy group demonstrated a significant outperformance in the VHI and shimmer than the speech therapy (p = 0.038) and singing intervention (p < 0.001) groups. The results of this study also indicated that combination therapy group compared to singing intervention group had a larger effect on voice intensity (p < 0.001), shimmer (p < 0.001) and maximum frequency range (p = 0.048). Conclusions & implication: The results demonstrated that combining speech therapy with a singing intervention delivered through tele-rehabilitation might be more effective in improving voice problems in patients with PD. What this paper adds: What is already known on the subject Parkinson's disease (PD) is a neurological disorder that frequently causes disturbances in speech and voice, which negatively affect patients' quality of life. Although speech difficulties occur in 90% of patients with PD, evidence-based treatment options for speech and language problems in these patients are limited. Therefore, further studies are required to develop and assess evidence-based treatment programs. What this study adds The findings of this study showed that a combination therapy program including conventional speech therapy approaches and individual singing intervention provided through tele-rehabilitation may have a greater effect on the improvement of voice problems in people with PD compared to speech therapy and singing intervention alone. What are the clinical implications of this work? Tele-rehabilitation combination therapy is an inexpensive and enjoyable behavioural treatment. The advantages of this method are that it is easy to access, appropriate for many stages of voice problems in PD, requires no prior singing training, encourages voice health and self-management and maximises treatment resources available to people with PD. We believe that the results of this study can provide a new clinical basis for treatment of voice disorders in people with PD.
... McGarr and Osberger [33] first suggested that impairment of intonation may cause poor intelligibility, and Kent and Rosenbek [34] further hypothesized that reduced F0 variation is the cause of poor intelligibility. This hypothesis was supported by studies using resynthesized speech that applied a flattened F0 contour of speech obtained from typical speakers [35][36][37][38] and those with dysarthria [39]. These findings suggest that the decrease in F0 ranges in Tones 2 and 4 impairs conversational intelligibility in the CT+ patients. ...
Article
Full-text available
In this cross-sectional study, we compared voice tone and activities relating to the laryngeal muscle between unilateral vocal fold paralysis (UVFP) patients with and without cricothyroid (CT) muscle dysfunction to define how CT dysfunction affects language tone. Eighty-eight female surgery-related UVFP patients were recruited and received acoustic voice analysis and laryngeal electromyography (LEMG) when the patient was producing the four Mandarin tones. The statistical analysis was compared between UVFP patients with (CT+ group, 17 patients) and without CT muscle (CT− group, 71 patients) involvement. When producing Mandarin Tone 2, the voice tone in the CT+ group had smaller rise range (p = 0.007), lower rise rate (p = 0.002), and lower fundamental frequency (F0) at the offset point of the voice (p = 0.023). When producing Mandarin Tone 4, the voice tone in the CT+ group had smaller drop range (p = 0.019), lower drop rate (p = 0.005), and lower F0 at voice onset (p = 0.025). The CT+ group had significantly lower CT muscle activity when producing the four Mandarin tones. In conclusion, CT dysfunction causes a limitation of high-rising tone in Tone 2 and high-falling tone in Tone 4, a property that dramatically limits the tonal characteristics in Mandarin, a tonal language. This limitation could further impair the patient’s communication ability.
... Reliability for the severity ratings, while similar to reliability of the other metrics reliant on listener judgment (i.e., transcription intelligibility and listener effort), was slightly lower than reliability for the metrics involving speaking rate (i.e., speaking rate and intelligible speaking rate). The reliability statistics reported here are consistent with those reported in previous studies (Bunton et al., 2001;Hustad, 2006aHustad, , 2006bKeintz et al., 2007;Stipancic et al., 2018;Xue et al., 2020;Yorkston & Beukelman, 1978, 1981. Taken together, our results suggest that clinicianbased adjectival ratings of dysarthria severity, as one of the most commonly used measures of speech severity (King et al., 2012), are a valid and reliable method for indexing known clinical measures of speech severity in persons with ALS, at least for the five-category approach we implemented. ...
Article
Purpose The main purpose of this study was to create an empirical classification system for speech severity in patients with dysarthria secondary to amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS) by exploring the reliability and validity of speech-language pathologists' (SLPs') ratings of dysarthric speech. Method Ten SLPs listened to speech samples from 52 speakers with ALS and 20 healthy control speakers. SLPs were asked to rate the speech severity of the speakers using five response options: normal, mild, moderate, severe, and profound. Four severity-surrogate measures were also calculated: SLPs transcribed the speech samples for the calculation of speech intelligibility and rated the effort it took to understand the speakers on a visual analog scale. In addition, speaking rate and intelligible speaking rate were calculated for each speaker. Intrarater and interrater reliability were calculated for each measure. We explored the validity of clinician-based severity ratings by comparing them to the severity-surrogate measures. Receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curves were conducted to create optimal cutoff points for defining dysarthria severity categories. Results Intrarater and interrater reliability for the clinician-based severity ratings were excellent and were comparable to reliability for the severity-surrogate measures explored. Clinician severity ratings were strongly associated with all severity-surrogate measures, suggesting strong construct validity. We also provided a range of values for each severity-surrogate measure within each severity category based on the cutoff points obtained from the ROC analyses. Conclusions Clinician severity ratings of dysarthric speech are reliable and valid. We discuss the underlying challenges that arise when selecting a stratification measure and offer recommendations for a classification scheme when stratifying patients and research participants into speech severity categories.
Article
Stroke frequently results in communication impairments that negatively impact quality of life and overall recovery, yet the biological mechanisms underlying these changes are not well understood. Ultrasonic vocalizations (USVs) in rodent models of disease and aging have been used to improve our understanding of the biological mechanisms that underlie vocal deficits and their response to interventions. Changes in USVs after middle cerebral artery occlusion (MCAO) in mice have been reported, yet rat models have significant anatomical and behavioral advantages over mice, including the ability to vocally train rats with an established paradigm. We sought to determine whether a unilateral MCAO rat stroke model provides a biologically and behaviorally relevant way to study post stroke vocalization deficits. We hypothesized that left MCAO would be associated with changes in USVs. Six weeks after MCAO or sham-control surgery, USVs were recorded in rats using an established mating paradigm. Stroke was associated with differences in USV acoustics including more frequent use of simple calls characterized by shorter durations and restricted bandwidths. These parameters were also found to correlate with post stroke lingual weakness. This is the first study to describe changes to rat USVs using a stroke model. These results suggest the unilateral MCAO rat stroke model is a biologically and behaviorally relevant model to understand how stroke affects vocal behaviors.
Article
Full-text available
Speech perception under adverse conditions is a multistage process involving a dynamic interplay among acoustic, cognitive, and linguistic factors. Nevertheless, prior research has primarily focused on factors within this complex system in isolation. The primary goal of the present study was to examine the interaction between processing depth and the acoustic challenge of noise and its effect on processing effort during speech perception in noise. Two tasks were used to represent different depths of processing. The speech recognition task involved repeating back a sentence after auditory presentation (higher-level processing), while the tiredness judgment task entailed a subjective judgment of whether the speaker sounded tired (lower-level processing). The secondary goal of the study was to investigate whether pupil response to alteration of dynamic pitch cues stems from difficult linguistic processing of speech content in noise or a perceptual novelty effect due to the unnatural pitch contours. Task-evoked peak pupil response from two groups of younger adult participants with typical hearing was measured in two experiments. Both tasks (speech recognition and tiredness judgment) were implemented in both experiments, and stimuli were presented with background noise in Experiment 1 and without noise in Experiment 2. Increased peak pupil dilation was associated with deeper processing (i.e., the speech recognition task), particularly in the presence of background noise. Importantly, there is a non-additive interaction between noise and task, as demonstrated by the heightened peak pupil dilation to noise in the speech recognition task as compared to in the tiredness judgment task. Additionally, peak pupil dilation data suggest dynamic pitch alteration induced an increased perceptual novelty effect rather than reflecting effortful linguistic processing of the speech content in noise. These findings extend current theories of speech perception under adverse conditions by demonstrating that the level of processing effort expended by a listener is influenced by the interaction between acoustic challenges and depth of linguistic processing. The study also provides a foundation for future work to investigate the effects of this complex interaction in clinical populations who experience both hearing and cognitive challenges.
Article
Purpose: This preliminary study was designed to assess the feasibility of Clear Speech Intervention Program (CSIP) on improving speech intelligibility of persons with idiopathic Parkinson’s disease (PD). Clear Speech is a behavioural approach which focuses on improving speech intelligibility of persons with PD within a relatively short frame of time. Method: A single group pretest-posttest design was used to assess the effectiveness of CSIP. Fifteen adults diagnosed with PD participated in 8 sessions of CSIP with each session lasting 45–50 minutes over a period of 4 weeks in South Korea. All the data were collected in a quiet room with an ambient noise level of equal to or less than 40 dB SPL. Data on acoustical/aerodynamic, auditory-perceptual, and self-perceptual measures were obtained pre- and post-intervention. The Phonatory Aerodynamic System (PAS) was used to analyse acoustical/aerodynamic data. Result: The acoustical/aerodynamic assessment revealed statistically significant (p < 0.01) improvements in habitual pitch, pitch range, speaking rate, and number of respirations. In the auditory-perceptual assessment, statistically significant (p < 0.01) improvements were observed in overall speech intelligibility, tone, intonation, intensity, and articulation. Medium to large effect sizes were observed for significant results. The Pearson correlation analysis showed significant (p < 0.01) and strong correlation between articulation and speech intelligibility. Conclusion: The preliminary data has provided promising results and warrants future large-scale studies with suitable controls. Additionally, future research must focus on investigating long term effects of the intervention.
Article
Full-text available
This study used a multi-talker database containing intelligibility scores for 2000 sentences (20 talkers, 100 sentences), to identify talker-related correlates of speech intelligibility. We first investigated "global" talker characteristics (e.g., gender, F0 and speaking rate). Findings showed female talkers to be more intelligible as a group than male talkers. Additionally, we found a tendency for F0 range to correlate positively with higher speech intelligibility scores. However, F0 mean and speaking rate did not correlate with intelligibility. We then examined several fine-grained acoustic-phonetic talker-characteristics as correlates of overall intelligibility. We found that talkers with larger vowel spaces were generally more intelligible than talkers with reduced spaces. In investigating two cases of consistent listener errors (segment deletion and syllable affiliation), we found that these perceptual errors could be traced directly to detailed timing characteristics in the speech signal. Results suggest that a substantial portion of variability in normal speech intelligibility is traceable to specific acoustic-phonetic characteristics of the talker. Knowledge about these factors may be valuable for improving speech synthesis and recognition strategies, and for special populations (e.g., the hearing-impaired and second-language learners) who are particularly sensitive to intelligibility differences among talkers.
Article
Full-text available
We report the results of a single-subject multiple baseline across behaviours experiment of a 74-year-old woman presenting with Parkinson's disease. Her speech was typical of a hypokinetic dysarthria. The main features of her dysarthric speech were: a restriction in the modulation of fundamental frequency, an inappropriate pitch level and a rate disturbance. Three measures of prosody were operationally defined as follows: (1) linguistic modulation of fundamental frequency, (2) mean fundamental frequency and (3) rate. Treatment focused on ameliorating these aspects employing a multiple baseline design. Measures during and post-therapy documented improvement for each of these three aspects of prosody. Independent judges were also more capable of understanding her speech and her speech prosody after therapy. Upon follow-up measures 10 weeks later, most of the improvement was maintained.
Article
Full-text available
Speech intelligibility and its phonetic and acoustic correlates were studied in a group of 10 women with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS). Intelligibility assessment with a word-identification test indicated that the most disrupted phonetic features pertained to velopharyngeal valving, lingual function for consonant contrasts of place and manner, and syllable shape. An acoustic signature analysis based on trajectories of the first and second formants in selected monosyllabic test words revealed that the mean slope of the second formant (F2) was reduced compared with that of a normal geriatric control group. This F2 slope reduction is interpreted to reflect loss of lingual motoneurons. Acoustic measures of phonatory function for sustained vowel prolongation demonstrated abnormalities in fundamental frequency, perturbations of frequency (jitter) and amplitude (shimmer), and signal-to-noise ratio. The data for women with ALS are compared with data for a normal geriatric control group of women and with data for a group of 25 men with ALS (Kent et al., 1990). Although the overall ranking of errors was similar for males and females with ALS, men were more likely to have impairments of voicing in syllable-initial position.
Article
Full-text available
The measurement of intelligibility in dysarthric individuals is a major concern in clinical assessment and management and in research on dysarthria. The measurement objective is complicated by the fact that intelligibility is not an absolute quantity but rather a relative quantity that depends on variables such as test material, personnel, training, test procedures, and state of the speaker. This paper reviews scaling procedures and item identification tests as they have been applied to dysarthric speech. Based in part on previous studies of speech of the hearing impaired, a profile has been designed to direct research on the acoustic or physiologic correlates of dysarthric intelligibility impairment. In addition, a word intelligibility test is proposed for use with dysarthric speakers. This test is designed to examine 19 acoustic-phonetic contrasts that are likely to (a) be sensitive to dysarthric impairment and (b) contribute significantly to speech intelligibility. Preliminary data from a sample of subjects with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis are presented to illustrate the use of this test in the phonetic interpretation of intelligibility impairment.
Article
Although associations between prosodic disturbances and neurologic lesion have been mentioned in the clinical literature, little progress has been made in the acoustic description of the abnormal prosodic patterns that typify neurologic dysfunctions. This report is a preliminary acoustic description of the prosodic disturbances associated with ataxic dysarthria, apraxia of speech, parkinsonian dysarthria, and right-hemisphere dysarthria. Several types of prosodic disturbance are identified and speculations are offered concerning their relation to site of lesion. In general, the patterns of prosodic disturbance described acoustically in this report confirm and elaborate G. H. Monrad-Krohn's (Problems of dynamic neurology. Jerusalem: Hebrew Univ. Press, 1963) observations.
Article
Dysprosody was studied in four groups of male subjects: subjects with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS) and mild intelligibility impairment, subjects with ALS and a more severe intelligibility loss, subjects with cerebellar disease, and neurologically normal controls. Dysprosody was assessed with perceptual ratings and acoustic measures pertaining to the regulation of duration, ƒ(0), and intensity within tone units of conversational samples. Intelligibility reduction and prosodic disturbance were not necessarily equally impaired in all subjects, and it is concluded that these are complementary indices of severity of dysarthria. Compared to the neurologically normal control group, the clinical groups tended to decrease the overall duration of tone units, produce fewer words in a tone unit, and use smaller variations in ƒ(0). Recommendations are offered for the assessement of.
Article
Speech intelligibility was studied in a group of 25 male patients with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS). The object of the study was to determine the phonetic impairments underlying the speech intelligibility deficits that frequently accompany ALS. Analyses with a word intelligibility test indicated that the most disrupted phonetic features involved phonatory (voicing contrast) function, velopharyngeal valving, place and manner of articulation for lingual consonants, and regulation of tongue height for vowels. The mean error proportion for the five most severely affected features correlated highly (0.97) with the intelligibility score (percent correct). The phonetic feature analyses are one index of bulbar muscle impairment in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis and also may help to direct the speech management in these individuals.
Article
The pitch change at the onset of voicing after a period of articulatory closure for a consonant reflects the state of the glottis during that closure. In initial position, a low rising pitch indicates a closed glottis and high falling pitch a glottis that is still partly open. An experiment is reported which shows that, for about 90% of the subjects, this pitch change in the vowel can cue the voiced/voiceless distinction for a preceding stop consonant in English. Control conditions suggest that this cue depends not upon low‐frequency energy content, but upon the pitch sensation.