ArticlePDF Available

Mother-offspring recognition in the domestic cat: Kittens recognize their own mother's call

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Acoustic communication can play an important part in mother-young recognition in many mammals. This, however, has still only been investigated in a small range mainly of herd- or colony-living species. Here we report on the behavioral response of kittens of the domestic cat, a typically solitary carnivore, to playbacks of "greeting chirps" and "meows" from their own versus alien mothers. We found significantly stronger responses to the chirps from kittens' own mother than to her meows or to the chirps or meows of alien mothers. Acoustic analysis revealed greater variation between vocalizations from different mothers than for vocalizations from the same mother. We conclude that chirps emitted by mother cats at the nest represent a specific form of vocal communication with their young, and that kittens learn and respond positively to these and distinguish them from chirps of other mothers and from other cat vocalizations while still in the nest. © 2016 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Dev Psychobiol 9999: 1-10, 2016.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Mother–Offspring Recognition
in the Domestic Cat: Kittens
Recognize Their Own Mother’s
Call
ABSTRACT: Acoustic communication can play an important part in mother-
young recognition in many mammals. This, however, has still only been
investigated in a small range mainly of herd- or colony-living species. Here we
report on the behavioral response of kittens of the domestic cat, a typically
solitary carnivore, to playbacks of “greeting chirps” and “meows” from their
own versus alien mothers. We found significantly stronger responses to the chirps
from kittens’ own mother than to her meows or to the chirps or meows of alien
mothers. Acoustic analysis revealed greater variation between vocalizations from
different mothers than for vocalizations from the same mother. We conclude that
chirps emitted by mother cats at the nest represent a specific form of vocal
communication with their young, and that kittens learn and respond positively
to these and distinguish them from chirps of other mothers and from other cat
vocalizations while still in the nest. ß 2016 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Dev
Psychobiol 9999: 1–10, 2016.
Keywords: vocalization; vocal recognition; mother-offspring communication;
maternal care; domestic cat; Felis silvestris catus; playback;
acoustic analysis
INTRODUCTION
In many mammals it is important for the early survival
of the young that they learn to recognize their own
mother and to distinguish her from other conspecifics.
This is clearly the case and has been best studied in
various herd-living ungulates (Nowak, Porter, L
evy,
Orgeur, & Schaal, 2000) and in colony-living species
such as various pinnipeds and bats (Insley, Phillips, &
Charrier, 2003; Jin et al., 2015, respectively). For the
newborn young of such species the attempt to suckle
from an alien mother, for example, may result not only
in rejection but also in serious injury or even death
(Harcourt, 1992; Le Boeuf, Whiting, & Gantt, 1972;
Trillmich, 1981; Wolski, Houpt, & Aronson, 1980).
Also for the young of more solitary species which are
hidden in nests or dens, it can be vital for their early
survival that they remain quiet at the approach of
predators or potentially infanticidal conspecifics
(Sieber, 1986; Torriani, Vannoni, & McElligott, 2006;
Va
nkov
a, Barto
s, & M
alek, 1997), and only respond
positively with approach to their mother.
Acoustic communication has been found to play an
important part in the recognition by newborn mammals
of their mother in a variety of mammals (Balcombe &
McCracken, 1992; Barfield, Tang-Martı
´
nez, & Trainer,
1994; Briefer & McElligott, 2011; Charrier, Mathevon,
& Jouventin, 2001; Jin et al., 2015; S
ebe, Nowak,
Poindron, & Aubin, 2007; Shillito Walser, 1986;
Trillmich, 1981). However, much of this work has been
done in herd- or colony-living species, with less
Manuscript Received: 11 June 2015
Manuscript Accepted: 14 February 2016
Correspondence to:P
eter Szenczi
Contract grant sponsor: Universidad Nacional Aut
onoma de
M
exico
Contract grant number: DGAPA- IN205513
Contract grant sponsor: Hungarian Academy of Sciences
Contract grant number: MTA 01 031
Contract grant sponsor: Postdoctoral Fellowship Program of the
Universidad Nacional Aut
onoma de M
exico
Article first published online in Wiley Online Library
(wileyonlinelibrary.com).
DOI 10.1002/dev.21402 ß 2016 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
Developmental Psychobiology
P
eter Szenczi
1
Ox
ana B
anszegi
2
Andrea Urrutia
2
Tam
as Farag
o
3
Robyn Hudson
2
1
Centro Tlaxcala de Biologı
´
adela
Conducta
Universidad Aut
onoma de Tlaxcala
Tlaxcala, Mexico
E-mail: peter.szenczi@gmail.com
2
Instituto de Investigaciones Biom
edicas
Universidad Nacional Aut
onoma de
M
exico
Distrito Federal, Mexico
3
MTA-ELTE Comparative Ethology
Research Group
Budapest, Hungary
information available on other taxonomic groups. This
may be because the need for individual recognition
between mothers and their offspring is less obvious for
solitary than for social species (but see below). It may
also be partly due to the difficulty of observing the
behavior of mothers and young of solitary, more
secretive species, and to the difficulty, exemplified by
carnivores, of experimentally manipulating young often
defended by well-armed mothers or other caretakers.
An exception to such difficulties is presented by the
domestic cat Felis silvestris catus. Cats can be easily
kept and experimented with under semi-natural free-
ranging conditions, and mothers readily permit the
handling and manipulation of newborn young by
familiar caretakers (Hudson, Raihani, Gonz
alez, Bau-
tista, & Distel, 2009 ; Raihani, Gonz
alez, Arteaga, &
Hudson, 2009). In addition, cats have a broad and
complex vocal repertoire, which they also employ in
communicating with their young (Brown, Buchwald,
Johnson, & Mikolich, 1978; Farley, Barlow, Netsell, &
Chmelka, 1992; Moelk, 1944; Scheumann et al., 2012;
Yeon et al., 2011). Although they do not construct nests
or dens, they are adept at hiding their kittens, already
well-furred at birth, in refuges (Feldman, 1993; Pontier
& Natoli, 1999). But as solitary-living obligate carni-
vores, cat mothers must spend considerable time away
from their young to hunt (Bradshaw, Goodwin,
Legrand-Defretin, & Nott, 1996; Martin, 1986), leaving
the young unprotected. During these periods, kittens
are most vulnerable to predators and potentially infanti-
cidal males (MacDonald, Apps, Carr, & Kerby, 1987;
Pontier & Natoli, 1999). Although their age is almost
impossible to determine, remains of cats, sometimes in
large quantities (Bateman & Fleming, 2012) have been
found in feces of several carnivores such as coyotes
(Morey, Gese, & Gehrt, 2007), foxes (Contesse, Heg-
glin, Gloor, Bontadina, & Deplazes, 2004; Kidawa &
Kowalczyk, 2011), and stone martens (T
oth, B
ar
any, &
Szenczi, 2011) in urban and natural environments alike.
The kittens typically remain still and silent when the
mother is absent and at the approach of animals other
than their mother (Haskins, 1977, own observations).
However, as we and others have observed, when
mothers approach the “nest” and while nursing, they
emit a particular soft call referred to in the literature as
a “chirp” and phonetically transcribed as
Iə
mhrn”
(Martin, 1986; Moelk, 1944). Chirps and purrs seem to
be the only vocalizations emitted by mothers at the nest
(own observations).
Although kittens are altricial and are born with
closed eyes and external auditory canals, they rapidly
develop good acoustic abilities. Already by around
postnatal day 5, electrophysiological recordings from
auditory cortex show responses to acoustic stimuli, and
most strongly to those of a naturalistic, biologically
relevant nature (Olmstead & Villablanca, 1980). By
around postnatal week 4 the auditory canals are
completely open and kittens have developed the fine
auditory acuity of the adult (Ehret & Romand, 1981;
Villablanca & Olmstead, 1979). There have, however,
been few studies of mother-young vocal communica-
tion in the cat (Haskins, 1977; Scheumann et al., 2012),
and to our knowledge only one of kittens’ behavioral
responses to mothers’ vocalizations at the nest (Lusche-
kin & Shuleikina, 1989). In that study the authors
investigated the homing efficiency of kittens able to use
visual, olfactory and auditory cues, although there was
not always a clear experimental differentiation between
the kittens’ use of particular types of acoustic or other
sensory stimuli to orient to the nest, nor whether the
acoustic stimuli were from their own or alien mothers.
In the present study it was therefore our aim to
investigate if kittens distinguish between their own
mother’s and alien mothers’ vocalizations at around
postnatal week 4, the age at which they start to leave
the nest. We predicted: 1) that kittens would respond
more strongly with positive behaviors such as approach
to their own mother’s chirps than to a presumably
unknown stimulus, her meows; and 2) that they would
respond more strongly and positively to the chirps of
their own mother compared to the chirps of an alien
mother.
METHODS
Animals
We collected data from 29 kittens of seven litters (average
litter size 4.14 1.8 SD) from three mixed-breed multiparous
females (2, 2, and 3 litters per mother) belonging to an
established breeding colony of cats (four adult females and
two adult males plus other visiting stray males), between May
and December 2014. With the intention of studying animals
in semi-natural conditions, the cats were kept in a private
house with a garden, which they were free to leave at will.
They were fed daily with commercial canned cat food and
fresh meat, and received regular treatment against parasites.
Water, milk, dried cat food and litter trays were always
available. There was sufficient space in the house for each
animal to have its own resting place, and for mothers to raise
their kittens in separate rooms, apart from other mothers and
their young. The doors of the rooms were remodeled to 1.2 m
in height, so that all the adult cats were free to jump in or
out, but the kittens were not able to leave. Mothers showed
little interest in the litters of other females and communal
nursing did not occur.
Mothers always gave birth in the house. A day or so after
delivery, the litters with their mothers were moved to one of
the rooms in a quiet, undisturbed part of the house. Within
2 Szenczi et al. Developmental Psychobiology
the rooms nest sites were provided: a commercial foam cat
bed (oval, 68 57 cm) was placed inside a large cardboard
box (60 80 70 cm). The top of the box was open and a
small floor-level opening (22 27 cm) was cut for the mother.
The box was removed when litters were 28 days old and
began leaving the bed.
Kittens were weighed at birth and daily thereafter to check
for normal growth and to habituate them to human presence and
handling. Each kitten was fitted at birth with a colored neck
ribbon for individual identification. From the 4th week onward,
kittens were fed daily with commercial canned cat food and ad
libitum dry cat food. Water and a litter tray were also provided.
All kittens survived to weaning at approximately 8 weeks
of age, when they were given away as pets with the help of
local veterinarians. As in our previous studies of kitten
behavior (Hudson et al., 2009; Hudson, Rangassamy, Salda
~
na,
B
anszegi, & R
odel, 2015; Raihani et al., 2009; Raihani,
Rodrı
´
guez, Salda
~
na, Guarneros, & Hudson, 2014), animals
were kept and treated according to the guidelines for the care
and use of animals in research of the Instituto de Investiga-
ciones Biom
edicas, Universidad Nacional Aut
onoma de
M
exico, and the National Guide for the Production, Care and
Use of Laboratory Animals, Mexico (Norma Oficial Mex-
icana NOM-062-200-1999).
Procedures
Recording of Vocalizations. For each litter, four kinds of
playback stimuli were used: “chirps” and “meows”, and either
from kittens’ own mother or from alien mothers at the same
lactational phase (Fig. 1 for sample chirp and meow spectro-
grams). Recordings were made in WAV format, using a
unidirectional microphone (Sennheiser ME66, Wedemark,
Germany; frequency range: 40–20,000 Hz) on a stand, con-
nected to a recorder (Tascam DR-40, Montebello CA; 96 kHz
/ 24 bit). Editing was carried out in Audacity 1.2.6, and
stimuli were saved and used in the playbacks in lossless AIFF
format. Recordings were obtained as follows:
“Chirps”. Continuous recordings were made at the nest
when litters reached 3 weeks of age. Recording was carried
out overnight (approx. 23:00–06:00 hr) to minimize back-
ground household and street noise. The microphone was
placed inside the box, 30 cm above the nest and pointing
towards it.
“Meows”. Recordings were made from each mother when a
highly preferred food (raw ground beef) was held above her.
The microphone was held on a boom pole 30 cm away from
the mother.
Playback Stimuli. Individual chirps and meows, when clear
and free of background noise, were cut from the recordings.
Stimuli were prepared by editing together a train of seven
chirps or seven meows, separated from each other by 2 s of
silence and in each test session were repeated twice (
X SE
¼ 36.5 1.7 s, n ¼ 2 7 different vocalizations for each
vocalization type). All of the chirps or meows in one 7-call
stimulus train belonged to the same mother, and to avoid
pseudoreplication each call and train of calls was used for
any of the litters only once. Sound pressure of all stimuli was
normalized to 60 dB measured with a sound pressure meter
(General DSM402SD, New York, NY) at 1 m distance from
the same speaker used in the tests. To ensure that all the
kittens were in the nest and were engaged in some neutral
behavior unrelated to the study (play, grooming), at the
beginning of the playback we included 1 min of pre-playback
video recording. Additionally, we continued video recording
for 1 min after the playback ended.
FIGURE 1 Spectrograms of sample chirps and meows from a single mother. Frequency range
0–10 000 Hz, window length .01 s, dynamic range 50 dB.
Developmental Psychobiology Kittens Recognize Their Own Mother’s Call 3
Experimental Procedure. Each litter was tested twice with
each of the four kinds of stimuli. The tests took place from
10:00–12:00 and 15:00–17:00 hr on four consecutive days,
starting when the kittens were 32–33 days old. The testing
schedule was balanced such that test days were alternated
between chirps (first and third days) and meows (second and
fourth days); the order of the own mothers’ and alien mothers’
vocalizations were played in reversed order the second time.
A40 50 cm screen made of white corrugated plastic was
placed in the litters’ room four days before the start of testing
so that the kittens could habituate to its presence. The screen
was placed between the nest and the door, about 1.5 m from
the nest. Two hours before each experiment the mother was
removed from the room. Playback of stimuli was conducted
in lossless AIFF format using a wireless speaker (SoundLink
1
Mini, Bose Inc., Framingham MA, frequency response 20–
20,000 Hz) connected to a 5th generation iPod
1
(Apple Inc.
Cupertino CA) placed behind the screen. Experimenters
checked that the kittens were awake and in the nest and then
left the room.
Video Recording and Analysis. Experiments were video
recorded (Sony HDR-CX130 camera placed so as to overlook
the test area) in the absence of the experimenter for later
analysis. Using Solomon Coder software (P
eter, 2011), the
following behaviors were scored separately for each kitten
from the video recordings:
Latency and duration of alertness (s). Alertness was
defined as a kitten stopping what it was doing, lifting its
head, pricking up its ears, and/or orienting towards the
stimulus.
Latency to approach the speak er (s). Time taken by a
kitten to approach within a 15-cm radius of the speaker or the
screen.
Time spent near the speaker (s). Time a kitten spent
within a 15-cm radius of the speaker or the screen.
In addition, we calculated the percentage of the kittens
from each litter that approached the speaker: defined as a
kitten within 15 cm of the speaker or screen.
Acoustic Analysis of Chirps and Meows. To t est whether the
chirps and meows in our tests were sufficiently distinctive to
be potentially used by kittens for individual r ecognition of
thei r mothers, the acoustic properties of the chirps and
meows of the thr ee mothers contributing to the stimulus
material were analyzed. Sixty-one good quality recordings of
chirps from 11 bouts (3, 3, and 5 from each mother, at least
5 calls per bout, taken during the periods they were nursing
each of their first tested litters), and 89 meows from 11 bouts
(7, 8, and 4 from each mother, at least 3 calls per bout, taken
during the periods they were nursing each of their li tters)
free of background noise were selected and were analyzed
usin g a custom made PRAAT script (Boersma & Weenink,
2015). The quantified parameters are listed in Supplementary
Material 1.
Data Treatment and Statistical Analysis
All behavioral data were analyzed using Generalized Linear
Mixed Models (GLMM) with nested random effects design.
Unless stated otherwise we used litter averages for the
response variables with a Poisson distribution. We used litters
as the unit of analysis because individual measures from
members of the same litter cannot be considered independent
(Zorrilla, 1997). Call type (chirp/meow) and call identity
(own mother/alien mother) were included as fixed factors, and
litter and mother identities (litter identity nested in mother
identity) were included in the models as random effects.
Where significant factor effects were found we performed
multiple comparisons using Mann–Whitney U-tests with
Bonferroni adjustments as post-hoc tests to identify signifi-
cant differences between groups. Statistical analysis of the
behavioral data was performed using Statsoft STATISTICA
10.0 (2011).
For acou stic an alysi s of the vocalizations we applied
Prin cipal Component Analysis (PCA) with covariance
matrix and Varimax rotation to simplify the set of acoustic
variables. In order to do this we standardized the variables
by transforming them to Z scores and ran the PCA on these
values. For chirps we formed 8 and for meows 9 factor
scales (for detailed str ucture and Chronbach alpha values
see Supplementary Material 2.). We then performed conven-
tional and permuted Discriminant Function Analysis (DFA
and pDFA) to test the acoustic discriminability of the three
mothers’ vocalizations. During the conventional DFA we
applied a forward stepwise method based on Wilk’s lambda
changes to find the subset of factors which best discrimi-
nated between the three individuals. For validation we us ed
leave-one-out cross validation. Finally, as the calls were
obtained from call bouts and so cannot be consid ered as
independent data points, we performed pDFA following the
suggestion of Mundry and Sommer ( 2007) to validate ou r
findings. We used an R script (written and provided by
Roger Mundry) with nested design, where our test factor
was the individua ls, while the ID of bouts from which the
calls originated was used as a fixed factor. We fed the
factor scores best discriminating the individuals based on
the DFA results into the analysis. This pDFA pick ed 100
rand om sel ections from the bouts to balance the sample and
to determine the baseline level of correct classification, and
then it generated 1000 randomly permuted samples and
reran the classification for each. The low numb er of random
samp les (ratio less than .05) with better discriminability
than t he balanced sample showed good validity o f the
discrimination. Again, a cross-validation was performed
with the unused items in the balanced DFA.
RESULTS
Behavioral Response of Kittens to Mothers’
Vocalizations
In all of our models we included the mothers’ and
litters’ (nested in mothers) identities as random factors,
4 Szenczi et al. Developmental Psychobiology
but we found no significant effect of either. Kittens’
latencies to show signs of alertness depended on the
type of stimulus only (GLMM: Call type
Call identity
x
2
¼ 3.17, p ¼ .075; Call type x
2
¼ 7.76, p ¼ .005; Call
Identity x
2
¼ 2.85, p ¼ .091), as they became alert
faster in response to chirps than to meows. Further
types of behavioral responses, however, differed more
clearly depending on the kind of stimulus. During and
after the playbacks of own mother’s chirps, kittens
remained alert significantly longer (GLMM: Call type
-
Call identity x
2
¼ 5.62, p ¼ .018; Call type x
2
¼ 61.3,
p < .00001; Call Identity x
2
¼ 45.6, p < .00001;
Fig. 2A) than with other types of playback. Kittens
were also quicker to approach the speaker (GLMM:
Call type
Call identity x
2
¼ 15.5, p ¼ .00008; Call type
x
2
¼ 13.0, p ¼ .0003; Call Identity x
2
¼ 15.0, p ¼ .0001;
Fig. 2B) and stayed near it for longer than for any other
stimulus type (GLMM: Call type
Call identity
x
2
¼ 2.11, p ¼ .14; Call type x
2
¼ 28.1, p < .00001; Call
Identity x
2
¼ 31.2, p < .00001; Fig. 2C). We also
calculated the percentage of kittens from each litter
that approached the speaker. Figure 2D shows that a
higher proportion of kittens did so during and following
the playback of their own mother’s chirps than when
other types of stimuli were used (GLMM: Call type
-
Call identity x
2
¼ .23, p ¼ .63; Call type x
2
¼ 17.5,
p ¼ .00003; Call Identity x
2
¼ 20.9, p < .00001).
Since there was possibly a social effect, where some
kittens merely imitated their siblings’ responses, we
also compared the data across stimuli of the first kitten
of each litter to show alertness and to go to the speaker.
Latencies of the first kittens to become alert during the
playbacks did not differ for the different vocalizations
(GLMM: Call type
Call identity x
2
¼ 3.41, p ¼ .067;
Call type x
2
¼ .34, p ¼ .55; Call Identity x
2
¼ 3.25,
p ¼ .071), which suggests that they were able to hear
all of them. However, they approached the speaker
significantly sooner during their own mother’s chirps
than during other types of playback (GLMM: Call
type
Call identity x
2
¼ 16.7, p ¼ .00004; Call type
x
2
¼ 16.8, p ¼ .00004; Call Identity x
2
¼ 13.6,
p < .0002).
Acoustic Analysis of Mothers’ Vocalizations
Chirps. The conventional DFA showed that the chirps
of the three mothers were acoustically distinct. For the
best discrimination three factors were included (F2-
Pitch [-ppm, f0mean, f0end, f0max, f0min, f0st], F3-
Pitch change [f0maxpoz, f0mxpozr, f0minpoz,
-f0mnpozr, f0chng], F7-Pitch variability [f0sd, f0range,
ppj]; n ¼ 3 mothers, n ¼ 61 calls, Wilk’s l ¼ .085,
p < .001), and 93.4% of the cases were correctly
classified (cross validated: 90.2%; Fig. 3, left panel).
The pDFA including only the first two factors (due to
the restriction that allows a lower number of variables
than the lowest number of calls within one bout, in this
case 3) showed poorer (51.79%) discriminability but
significantly different from the permuted sample (n ¼ 3
mothers; n ¼ 61 calls, p < .001), supporting the validity
of the conventional DFA.
Meows. In the case of meows the conventional DFA
showed lower but still good discriminability (77.8%;
71.7% cross-validated, Fig. 3, right panel) by three
factor scores (F1-Pitch [f0mean , -ppm, f0min, f0max,
f0st]; F3-Range of Intensity [-cmom, intmin, intmax,
intend]; F6-Call length [call_lenght, ppp, intmaxpoz,
f0minpoz]; n ¼ 3 mothers, n ¼ 89 calls, Wilk’s
l ¼ .367, p < .001). The pDFA was run with the first
two factors again, resulting in lower (23.36%) but still
significant discriminability (n ¼ 3 mothers; n ¼ 89 calls;
p < .05).
DISCUSSION
Returning to the main aim of the study, the results
clearly show that kittens of the domestic cat recognize
and respond to the distinctive chirp vocalizations
emitted by their mother as she approaches the nest and
while she nurses and cleans them inside it. Further-
more, kittens distinguish the chirps of their own from
those of other mothers at an equivalent lactational
(maternal) phase, as well as from the meows of their
own or from alien mothers. This suggests that the
mother’s chirp call may have evolved to have a specific
communicatory function, on the one hand signaling to
the young kittens that her arrival at the nest is not to be
feared, and as they grow older and start to be weaned,
using it to call them to leave the nest and follow her
(Moelk, 1944).
Acoustic analysis suggests that that the chirps of
the individual mothers can be sufficiently distinctive
and sufficiently stable on a range of physical charac-
teristics to represent individu al vocal “signatures (see
also Supplementary Material 2) enabling kittens to
distinguish ch irps of thei r own mother from those of
other females. Nevert heless, we have to admit that the
small sample size m ay exaggerate th is acoustical
distinction and if analyzing a larger number of
mothers’ calls we might observe stronger overlap
among individuals. To more rigorously test this, and
to avoid pseudoreplication (Kroodsma, 1989), each
train of chirps used in the present stu dy compris ed
seven different vocalizations cut from seven different
chirp bouts recorded for each female, and the kittens
were tested with two such trains of independently
Developmental Psychobiology Kittens Recognize Their Own Mother’s Call 5
obtained and constructed material on two separate
occasions. Thus, the kittens indeed seem to have
perceived and to have used inter-individual and not
simply inter-vocalization differences when responding
to their own versus another mother’s chirp s (cf.
Sauv
e, Beauplet, Hammill, & Charrier, 2015). In
addition , the design of the experim ents r equired that
the kittens leave the nest to approach the speaker,
rather than to return to their nest as in the study by
Luschekin and Shuleikina (1989) . T his excluded them
using olfactory, thermal or s patial cues as even much
younger kit tens have been shown to effectively use to
return to their nest (Freeman & Rosenblatt, 1978a,b).
A question arising from the present findings is what
prompts mothers to emit these distinctive calls on
approaching or entering the nest? While we were
recording at nests to obtain material to construct the
playbacks, we consistently found, in agreement with
previous reports (Luschekin & Shuleikina, 1989), that
mothers emit only chirps and purrs at the nest, and not,
for example, meows. Somewhat unexpectedly (but
noting that our sample size of mothers and litters was
FIGURE 2 Kittens were alert for significantly longer during and after the playback of their
own mother’s chirps than after the playback of other vocalizations (A); they approached the
speaker significantly sooner during and after playback of their own mother’s chirps than for other
types of playback (B); they spent significantly longer near the speaker during and after the
playback of their own mother’s chirps than for other types of playback (C); and significantly
more kittens in each litter approached the speaker during and after the playback of their own
mother’s chirps than with other types of playback (D). The data are presented as medians (bold
horizontal lines), upper and lower quartiles (boxes), and minimum and maximum values (dotted
lines). Each circle represents the average value for a litter and these may sometimes overlap.
Letters indicate significant differences as reported by multiple comparisons using Mann-Whitney
U-tests with Bonferroni adjustments following application of Generalized Linear Mixed Models.
Post hoc tests were run only when interaction effects were significant. Details of statistical tests
are given in the text.
6 Szenczi et al. Developmental Psychobiology
rather small), we also found that mothers did not start to
emit chirps until the kittens were around two to three
weeks of age (see also Martin, 1986). At present it is not
clear what stimulated the mothers to start doing this:
signals from the developing kittens such as an increase
in motor activity or an increase in the squeaking
vocalizations we often recorded from them in the nest in
response to their mother’s arrival, or a change in the
mothers’ hormonal and motivational state across lacta-
tion, or both? Here cross-fostering studies might help
provide an answer, for example, by investigating
whether chirping is delayed in mothers given younger
foster litters, or advanced in mothers given older litters.
The present findings also raise questions as to when
and how the kittens learn to recognize and respond to
their own mother’s distinctive chirps. Apparently they
do not do so prenatally (cf. Moon & Fifer, 2001) as they
should then also have responded to their mother’s
apparently similarly complex, distinctive and frequently
emitted meows. Furthermore, they would not be able to
learn their mother’s chirps until around the second or
third postnatal week, when as mentioned above, mothers
first start to emit these at the nest. This is consistent with
reports that the auditory canals are almost completely
open and that kittens have good auditory function by
this age, and virtually mature function by the age of one
month when we started to test them (Ehret & Romand,
1981; Olmstead & Villablanca, 1980).
It is certainly not difficult to imagine how kittens
could learn to respond positively to these calls via
pairing with food, warmth and licking provided by the
mother. Nevertheless, the ability of kittens to learn such
fine distinctions so early demonstrates their consider-
able cognitive abilities. It also raises the question
whether they might be pre-adapted to learning chirps,
and/or whether these vocalizations have specific quali-
ties particularly co-adapted to the kittens’ developing
auditory system? This could be addressed by presenting
kittens at the time of the mother’s presence at the nest
with chirps acoustically modified in specific ways, with
other cat vocalizations (for example meows), calls of
other species, or even non-biological acoustic stimuli.
FIGURE 3 Vocal individuality in calls based on the first two canonical discriminant functions
from the conventional DFAs. The data points represent individual calls of the three mothers. In
the case of Chirps (left panel), Function 1 contained the Pitch factor with the highest loading
(F2: 0.948), and was also moderately affected by the Pitch variability factor (F7: 0.547), while
Function 2 was mainly affected by the Pitch change factor (F3: 0.855). The centroids (mean
discriminant score of group) of Mothers 1 and 2 were close to each other, suggesting that their
voices were more similar, but they were still distinguishable by the cross validation (87% of the
two mothers’ calls correctly assigned). In the case of Meows (right panel), Function 1 contained
the Pitch and the Intensity range factors with the highest loading (F1: 0.842; F3: 0.739), while
Function 2 was mainly affected by Call length (F6: 0,737). The centroids (mean discriminant
score of group) of Mothers 1 and 2 were close to each other, suggesting that their voices were
more similar, but that they were still distinguishable by the cross validation (72% of Mother 2’s
calls and 67% of Mother 1’s calls were correctly classified). Details of statistical analysis are
given in the text.
Developmental Psychobiology Kittens Recognize Their Own Mother’s Call 7
Such experiments could also help decide how early
kittens can learn such stimuli by presenting them before
mothers start to emit chirps, thereby providing a means
of investigating kittens’ early sensory and cognitive
abilities. From previous studies it is already known that
kittens are able to learn to identify their own nipple(s)
in the litter’s “teat order”, presumably using olfactory
cues, within a few hours of birth (Hudson et al., 2009;
Raihani et al., 2009).
Returning to the observation in the Introduction that
most studies of mammalian mother-young acoustic
communication have been conducted in herd- or colony-
living species, the present study provides convincing
evidence that the young of species as solitary in the wild
state as the cat Felis silvestris catus may also learn to
recognize a distinctive call of their mother and to
distinguish it from equivalent vocalizations of other
mothers. Whether this has a functional significance for
cats later in life, for example when kittens leave the nest
to follow their mother on hunting forays, remains to be
investigated. Also remaining to be investigated is
whether, as in some other mammals, acoustic communi-
cation between mother and young in the cat is bi-
directional, that is, if mothers also learn to distinguish
the vocalizations of their own from those of alien kittens
(e.g., separation calls; Hudson et al., 2015). The
directionality of mother-young vocal recognition is
shaped by the ecological and social environment of the
species. In cases where selection pressure is high for
both parties to recognize each other, as in colonial or
herd-living species, usually mutual recognition is found
(Espmark, 1971; Insley, 2000; McCulloch & Boness,
2000; M
uller & Manser, 2008; S
ebe et al., 2007).
Theory suggests that in species where the mother hides
her offspring and the young should reveal their exact
location only to her, while she uses other, spatial and
olfactory cues to locate them, only the young are
responsible for mother-offspring acoustic recognition
(Briefer & McElligott, 2011; Sieber, 1986; Torriani
et al., 2006; Va
nkov
a & Malek, 1997), and as is reflected
in the distinctive vocal signature of individual mothers.
NOTES
We thank Karl-Heinz Esser for valuable advice on the
recording and analysis of vocalizations and Carolina Rojas
for excellent technical and bibliographical assistance.
REFERENCES
Balcombe, J. P., & McCracken, G. F. (1992). Vocal recogni-
tion in Mexican free-tailed bats: Do pups recognize
mothers? Animal Behaviour, 43(1), 79–87. doi: 10.1016/
s0003-3472(05)80073-9
Barfield, C. H., Tang-Martı
´
nez, Z., & Trainer, J. M. (1994).
Domestic calves ( Bos taurus) recognize their own mothers
by auditory cues. Ethology, 97(4), 257–264. doi: 10.1111/
j.1439–0310.1994.tb01045.x
Bateman, P., & Fleming, P. (2012). Big city life: Carnivores
in urban environments. Journal of Zoology, 287(1), 1–23.
doi: 10.1111/j.1469–7998.2011.00887.x
Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2015). Praat: doing phonetics
by computer (Version 5.4.08). Retrieved from http://www.
praat.org/
Bradshaw, J. W. S., Goodwin, D., Legrand-Defretin, V., &
Nott, H. M. R. (1996). Food selection by the domestic
cat, an obligate carnivore. Comparative Biochemistry and
Physiology Part A: Physiology, 114(3), 205–209. doi:
10.1016/0300-9629(95)02133-7
Briefer, E., & McElligott, A. G. (2011). Mutual mother-
offspring vocal recognition in an ungulate hider species
(Capra hircus). Animal Cognition, 14(4), 585–598. doi:
10.1007/s10071-011-0396-3
Brown, K. A., Buchwald, J. S., Johnson, J. R., & Mikolich,
D. J. (1978). Vocalization in the cat and kitten. Develop-
mental Psychobiology, 11(6), 559–570. doi: 10.1002/
dev.420110605
Charrier, I., Mathevon, N., & Jouventin, P. (2001). Mother’s
voice recognition by seal pups. Nature, 412(6850),
873–873. doi: 10.1038/35091136
Contesse, P., Hegglin, D., Gloor, S., Bontadina, F., &
Deplazes, P. (2004). The diet of urban foxes (Vulpes
vulpes) and the availability of anthropogenic food in
the city of Zurich, Switzerland. Mammalian Biology-
Zeitschrift f
ur S
augetierkunde, 69(2), 81–95. doi: 10.1078/
1616-5047-00123
Ehret, G., & Romand, R. (1981). Postnatal development of
absolute auditory thresholds in kittens. Journal of Compar-
ative and Physiological Psychology, 95(2), 304–311.
doi: 10.1037/h0077770
Espmark, Y. (1971). Individual recognition by voice in
reindeer mother-young relationship. Field observations
and playback experiments. Behaviour, 40(3/4), 295–301.
doi: 10.1163/15685397100438
Farley, G. R., Barlow, S. M., Netsell, R., & Chmelka, J. V.
(1992). Vocalizations in the cat: Behavioral methodology
and spectrographic analysis. Experimental Brain Research,
89(2), 333–340. doi: 10.1007/bf00228249
Feldman, H. N. (1993). Maternal care and differences in the
use of nests in the domestic cat. Animal Behaviour, 45(1),
13–23. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1993.1002
Freeman, N. C. G., & Rosenblatt, J. S. (1978a). The
interrelationship between thermal and olfactory stimula-
tion in the development of home orientation in newborn
kittens. Developmental Psychobiology, 11(5), 437–457.
doi: 10.1002/dev.420110508
Freeman, N. C. G., & Rosenblatt, J. S. (1978b). Specificity of
litter odors in the control of home orientation among
kittens. Developmental Psychobiology, 11(5), 459–468.
doi: 10.1002/dev.420110509
8 Szenczi et al. Developmental Psychobiology
Harcourt, R. (1992). Factors affecting early mortality in the
South American fur seal (Arctocephalus australis) in Peru:
density-related effects and predation. Journal of Zoology,
226(2), 259–270. doi: 10.1111/j.1469–7998.1992.tb03838.x
Haskins, R. (1977). Effect of kitten vocalizations on maternal
behavior. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psy-
chology, 91(4), 830–838. doi: 10.1037/h0077362
Hudson, R., Raihani, G., Gonz
alez, D., Bautista, A., & Distel,
H. (2009). Nipple preference and contests in suckling
kittens of the domestic cat are unrelated to presumed
nipple quality. Developmental Psychobiology, 51(4),
322–332. doi: 10.1002/dev.20371
Hudson, R., Rangassamy, M., Salda
~
na, A., B
anszegi, O., &
R
odel, H. G. (2015). Stable individual differences in
separation calls during early development in cats and
mice. Frontiers in Zoology, 12(Suppl 1), S12. doi:
10.1186/1742-9994-12-s1-s12
Insley, S. J., Phillips, A. V., & Charrier, I. (2003). A review
of social recognition in pinnipeds. Aquatic Mammals,
29(2), 181–201.
Insley, S. J. (2000). Long-term vocal recognition in the
northern fur seal. Nature, 406(6794), 404–405. doi:
10.1038/35019064
Jin, L., Yang, S., Kimball, R. T., Xie, L., Yue, X., Luo, B., ...
Feng, J. (2015). Do pups recognize maternal calls in
pomona leaf-nosed bats, Hipposideros pomona? Animal
Behaviour, 100, 200–207. doi: 10.1016/j.anbe-
hav.2014.12.006
Kidawa, D., & Kowalczyk, R. (2011). The effects of sex, age,
season and habitat on diet of the red fox Vulpes vulpes in
northeastern Poland. Acta Theriologica, 56(3), 209–218.
doi: 10.1007/s13364-011-0031-3
Kroodsma, D. E. (1989). Inappropriate experimental designs
impede progress in bioacoustic research: a reply. Animal
Behaviour, 38(4), 717–719. doi: 10.1016/s0003-3472(89)
80020-x
Le Boeuf, B. J., Whiting, R. J., & Gantt, R. F. (1972).
Perinatal behavior of northern elephant seal females and
their young. Behaviour, 43(1/4), 121–156. doi: 10.1163/
15685397300508
Luschekin, V. S., & Shuleikina, K. V. (1989). Some sensory
determinants of home orientation in kittens. Developmen-
tal Psychobiology, 22(6), 601–616. doi: 10.1002/
dev.420220606
MacDonald, D. W., Apps, P. J., Carr, G. M., & Kerby, G.
(1987). Social dynamics, nursing coalitions and infanticide
among farm cats, Felis catus. Advances in Ethology
(supplement to Ethology), 28, 1–66.
Martin, P. (1986). An experimental study of weaning in the
domestic cat. Behaviour, 99(3), 221–249. doi: 10.1163/
156853986 00568
McCulloch, S., & Boness, D. J. (2000). Mother–pup vocal
recognition in the grey seal (Halichoerus grypus) of Sable
Island, Nova Scotia, Canada. Journal of Zoology, 251(4),
449–455. doi: 10.1111/j.1469–7998.2000.tb00800.x
Moelk, M. (1944). Vocalizing in the house-cat; a phonetic
and functional study. The American Journal of Psychol-
ogy, 57(2), 184–205. doi: 10.2307/1416947
Moon, C. M., & Fifer, W. P. (2001). Evidence of transnatal
auditory learning. Journal of Perinatology, 1(20),
S37–S44. doi: 10.1038/sj.jp.7200448
Morey, P. S., Gese, E. M., & Gehrt, S. (2007). Spatial and
temporal variation in the diet of coyotes in the Chicago
metropolitan area. The American Midland Naturalist,
158(1), 147–161. doi: 10.1674/0003-0031(2007)158[147:
satvit]2.0.co;2
Mundry, R., & Sommer, C. (2007). Discriminant function
analysis with nonindependent data: Consequences and an
alternative. Animal Behaviour, 74(4), 965–976. doi:
10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.12.028
M
uller, C. A., & Manser, M. B. (2008). Mutual recognition
of pups and providers in the cooperatively breeding
banded mongoose. Animal Behaviour, 75(5), 1683–1692.
doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.10.021
Nowak, R., Porter, R. H., L
evy, F., Orgeur, P., & Schaal, B.
(2000). Role of mother-young interactions in the survival
of offspring in domestic mammals. Reviews of Reproduc-
tion, 5(3), 153–163. doi: 10.1530/ror.0.0050153
Olmstead, C. E., & Villablanca, J. R. (1980). Development of
behavioral audition in the kitten. Physiology & Behavior,
24(4), 705–712. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(80)90401-1
P
eter, A. (2011). Solomon Coder: a simple solution for
behavior coding (Version beta 15.02.08). Retrieved from
http://solomoncoder.com/
Pontier, D., & Natoli, E. (1999). Infanticide in rural male cats
(Felis catus L.) as a reproductive mating tactic. Aggressive
Behavior, 25(6), 445–449. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2337
(1999)25:6<445::AID-AB5>3.0.CO;2-K
Raihani, G., Gonz
alez, D., Arteaga, L., & Hudson, R. (2009).
Olfactory guidance of nipple attachment and suckling in
kittens of the domestic cat: Inborn and learned responses.
Developmental Psychobiology, 51(8), 662–671. doi:
10.1002/dev.20401
Raihani, G., Rodrı
´
guez, A., Salda
~
na, A., Guarneros, M., &
Hudson, R. (2014). A proposal for assessing individual
differences in behaviour during early development in the
domestic cat. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 154,
48–56. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2014.01.013
S
ebe, F., Nowak, R., Poindron, P., & Aubin, T. (2007).
Establishment of vocal communication and discrimination
between ewes and their lamb in the first two days after
parturition. Developmental Psychobiology, 49(4),
375–386. doi: 10.1002/dev.20218
Sauv
e, C. C., Beauplet, G., Hammill, M. O., & Charrier, I.
(2015). Mother–pup vocal recognition in harbour seals:
Influence of maternal behaviour, pup voice and habitat
sound properties. Animal Behaviour, 105, 109–120. doi:
10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.04.011
Scheumann, M., Roser, A., Konerding, W., Bleich, E.,
Hedrich, H., & Zimmermann, E. (2012). Vocal correlates
of sender-identity and arousal in the isolation calls of
domestic kitten (Felis silvestris catus). Frontiers in
Zoology, 9(1), 1–14. doi: 10.1186/1742-9994-9-36
Shillito Walser, E. E. (1986). Recognition of the sow’s voice
by neonatal piglets. Behaviour, 99(3), 177–188. doi:
10.1163/156853986 00540
Developmental Psychobiology Kittens Recognize Their Own Mother’s Call 9
Sieber, O. J. (1986). Acoustic recognition between mother
and cubs in raccoons (Procyon lotor). Behaviour, 96(1),
130–162. doi: 10.1163/156853986 00252
StatSoft I. (2011). STATISTICA (data analysis software
system) (Version 10.0). Tulsa, Oklahoma: StatSoft Inc,
Retrieved from www.statsoft.com
T
oth, M., B
ar
any, A., & Szenczi, P. (2011). A nyest
Budapesten.
Allattani K
ozlem
enyek, 96(1–2), 39–59.
Torriani, M. V. G., Vannoni, E., & McElligott, A. G. (2006).
Mother-young recognition in an ungulate hider species: A
unidirectional process. The American Naturalist, 168(3),
412–420. doi: 10.1086/506971
Trillmich, F. (1981). Mutual mother-pup recognition in
Galapagos fur seals and sea lions: Cues used and
functional significance. Behaviour, 78(1), 21–42. doi:
10.1163/15685398100248.
Va
nkov
a, D., & Malek, J. (1997). Characteristics of the
vocalizations of red deer Cervus elaphus hinds and calves.
Bioacoustics, 7(4), 281–289. doi: 10.1080/09524622.
1997.9753340
Va
nkov
a, D., Barto
s, L., & M
alek, J. (1997). The role of
vocalization in the communication between red deer hinds
and calves. Ethology, 103(10), 795–808. doi: 10.1111/
j.1439–0310.1997.tb00121.x
Villablanca, J. R., & Olmstead, C. E. (1979). Neurological
development of kittens. Developmental Psychobiology,
12(2), 101–127. doi: 10.1002/dev.420120204
Wolski, T. R., Houpt, K. A., & Aronson, R. (1980). The role
of the senses in mare - foal recognition. Applied Animal
Ethology, 6(2), 121–138. doi: 10.1016/0304-3762(80)
90064-4
Yeon, S. C., Kim, Y. K., Park, S. J., Lee, S. S., Lee, S. Y.,
Suh, E. H., ... Yang, B. G. (2011). Differences between
vocalization evoked by social stimuli in feral cats and
house cats. Behavioural Processes, 87(2), 183–189. doi:
10.1016/j.beproc.2011.03.003
Zorr illa, E. P. (1 997). Multiparous species present problems
(and poss ibilities) to developmentalists. Developmental
Psychobiology, 30(2), 141–150. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-
2302(199703)30:2<141::AID-DEV5>3.0.CO;2-Q
SUPPORTING INFORMATION
Additional supporting information may be found in the
online version of this article at the publisher’s web-site.
10 Szenczi et al. Developmental Psychobiology
... Domestic house cats are capable of recognizing their names among other words (Saito et al., 2019). One months old kittens display stronger responses to chirps and meows of their own mothers compared with calls of other mother cats (Szenczi et al., 2016). Although for adult cats no experiments on recognizing the calls of the species' vocal repertoire have been conducted, it is reasonable to hypothesize that adult cats are also capable of recognizing the sexual and individual-related properties of meows and other call types. ...
... We found that the average value of correctly classifying of meows to 11 individuals with DFA was 79.2%, suggesting a high potential of the meows produced in the mating season to advertise individual identity of the callers. At the same time, cat meows produced in non-mating season in motheroffspring context seem to be not prominently individualistic: playback experiments indicated that kittens recognized their own mother by her chirp calls rather than by meow calls (Szenczi et al., 2016). For the three mother cats whose calls were analysed, the DFA accuracy rate of correct recognition was 93.4% for the chirps and only 77.8% for the meows (Szenczi et al., 2016). ...
... At the same time, cat meows produced in non-mating season in motheroffspring context seem to be not prominently individualistic: playback experiments indicated that kittens recognized their own mother by her chirp calls rather than by meow calls (Szenczi et al., 2016). For the three mother cats whose calls were analysed, the DFA accuracy rate of correct recognition was 93.4% for the chirps and only 77.8% for the meows (Szenczi et al., 2016). However, judging by the spectrograms of the chirps and meows used as playback stimuli to kittens in the study by Szenczi et al. (2016), the only difference of the chirp call type from the meow call type was the inclusion of the articulatory phenomenon 'wave', described previously in detail for the whine vocalisations of red fox Vulpes vulpes (Gogoleva et al., 2008). ...
Article
This study investigates the frequency, temporal and power parameters in 11 (5 males, 6 females) captive feral domestic cats Felis silvestris catus, vocalising in their individual outdoor enclosures during the mating season. Discriminant function analysis (DFA) classified the meows to correct callers with 79.2% accuracy, which exceeded the chance level of 22.9 ± 2.8%, calculated with permutation test. Male meows were lower-frequency, with the maximum fundamental frequency of 0.37 ± 0.05 kHz vs 0.61 ± 0.16 kHz in females. Sex differences in the maximum, beginning and end fundamental frequencies varied from 32 to 39%, depending on acoustic parameter. The DFA classified the meows to correct sex with accuracy of 88.0%, which exceeded the chance level of 58.2 ± 3.1%. We discuss that the meows encode information about individual identity and sex and that acoustic differences in frequency parameters of the meows exceed sexual dimorphism of body size in domestic cat.
... First, we already discussed calls by great ape mothers, but they also occur in other primates [123,124], as well as in many nonprimate species, where mothers call to their infants to retrieve them. Examples include domestic cats (Felis silvestris catus [125]), and ungulates such as domestic sheep (Ovis aries [126]), cattle (Bos taurus [127]), goitred gazelles (Gazella subgutturosa [128]), or saiga antelopes (Saiga tatarica tatarica [129]). Second, immature-directed calls may serve to aid recognition of the mother's voice, as in domestic cats [125], Mexican free-tailed bats (Tadarida brasiliensis mexicana [130]), fur seals (Arctocephalus tropicalis [131]), or domestic sheep [126]. ...
... Examples include domestic cats (Felis silvestris catus [125]), and ungulates such as domestic sheep (Ovis aries [126]), cattle (Bos taurus [127]), goitred gazelles (Gazella subgutturosa [128]), or saiga antelopes (Saiga tatarica tatarica [129]). Second, immature-directed calls may serve to aid recognition of the mother's voice, as in domestic cats [125], Mexican free-tailed bats (Tadarida brasiliensis mexicana [130]), fur seals (Arctocephalus tropicalis [131]), or domestic sheep [126]. These examples show that even if IDC exists in an animal species, it is unlikely that these cases are functionally equivalent to human CDC. ...
Article
Full-text available
Humans communicate with small children in unusual and highly conspicuous ways (child-directed communication (CDC)), which enhance social bonding and facilitate language acquisition. CDC-like inputs are also reported for some vocally learning animals, suggesting similar functions in facilitating communicative competence. However, adult great apes, our closest living relatives, rarely signal to their infants, implicating communication surrounding the infant as the main input for infant great apes and early humans. Given cross-cultural variation in the amount and structure of CDC, we suggest that child-surrounding communication (CSC) provides essential compensatory input when CDC is less prevalent—a paramount topic for future studies.
... Across many independent evolutionary origins, begging is an energetically costly motor performance that selects for honest signaling in offspring [5,6]. Due to costs of communication, begging is usually paired with parental recognition, where infants learn to recognize their caregivers or nest sites [7,8]. For example, human infants learn to recognize their mothers apart from other individuals within the first week of life [9,10]. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Altricial young of many species beg parents for the nutrients required for healthy development. From human crying to begging chicks, young expend precious energy reserves to communicate their hunger. Despite repeated independent evolutionary origins, the neural basis of parent-directed communication by infants is unknown. Here, we examined the sensory and neural basis of begging behavior in tadpoles of the monogamous and biparental Mimetic poison frog (Ranitomeya imitator). In this species, tadpoles beg parents for egg meals by dancing. We used this robust motor display to determine that tadpoles use multimodal cues for caregiver recognition, where olfactory cues are necessary for caregiver recognition while visual cues provide an orientation goal. We found that dopamine related brain regions have higher neural activity in begging tadpoles and that dopamine signaling had opposing modulatory effects through D1 and D2 family receptors, similar to swimming behavior in other tadpole species. We then identified caudal posterior tuberculum dopamine neurons as more active during begging behavior and sensitive to caregiver olfactory cues. Projections of these dopaminergic neurons to the spinal accessory motor nucleus are required for begging displays. These findings support the idea that dopamine regulates olfactory-guided parental recognition in young and opens many avenues for studying how new communication behaviors can evolve from ancestral motor circuits.
... For example, female macaques learn to recognize the vocalizations of their own offspring during the second postnatal week, and retain this ability for at least 6 months (Jovanovic et al., 2003;Shizawa et al., 2005). Similarly, kittens learn their mother's vocalizations, and Australian sea lions can recall their mother's voice up to 2 years after weaning (Pitcher et al., 2010;Szenczi et al., 2016). Furthermore, the meaning of vocal cues are often learned through long-term social experience. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Many animal species use vocalizations to communicate social information and previous experiments in rodents have identified a range of vocal types that may be used for this purpose. However, social vocalizations are typically acquired during brief interactions between animals with no prior social relationship, and under environmental conditions with limited ethological relevance. Here, we establish long-term acoustic recordings from Mongolian gerbil families, a core social group that uses an array of sonic and ultrasonic vocalizations which vary with social context. Three separate gerbil families (two parents and four pups) were transferred to an enlarged environment and continuous 20-day audio recordings were obtained. We leveraged deep-learning based unsupervised analysis of 583,237 vocalizations to show that gerbils exhibit a more complex vocal repertoire than has been previously reported. Furthermore, gerbils displayed family-specific vocal repertoires, including differences in vocal type usage and transitions. Since gerbils live naturally as extended families in complex underground burrows that are adjacent to other families, these results suggest the presence of a vocal dialect which could be exploited by animals to represent kinship. These findings offer insight into the naturalistic vocal tendencies of gerbil families and position the Mongolian gerbil as a compelling animal to study the neural basis of vocal communication.
... In captive non-domesticated felids aside cheetahs, vocal correlates of caller identity were found in the long-distance calls of tigers Panthera tigris (Ji et al., 2013) and male Eurasian lynxes Lynx lynx (Rutovskaya et al., 2009). In addition, individualistic isolation calls were also reported in domestic kittens Felis silvestris catus (Scheumann et al., 2012) and in meows of adult female domestic cats (Szenczi et al., 2016). Weak correlates of caller sex were found in calls of wild-living lions (Pfefferle et al., 2007) and in cats of genus Felis (Peters et al., 2009). ...
Article
Adult cheetahs (Acinonyx jubatus) use long‐distance chirps for calling toward coalition partners (males), mates (both sexes), and cubs (females). Previously, these vocalizations were only investigated in captivity. This study estimates individual and sex‐related acoustic variation of the long‐distance chirps of 20 mature cheetahs (eight males and 12 females older than 4 years) in their natural habitats in Kenya. Male chirps were longer in duration and lower in the peak frequency and all fundamental frequency variables than female chirps. The average value for assignment of the chirps to correct sex with Discriminant Function Analysis (DFA) of 93.8% was significantly higher than by‐chance level of 52.9%. The average value of correct assignment of the chirps to individual with DFA was 79.5%, which was significantly higher than the level by chance of 14.8%. For 10 cheetahs recorded twice with time space of one or 2 years, DFA showed high values of correct assignment of the chirps to individual for both years (91.4% in the first year and 83.9% in the second year of recording), but cross‐validation of the chirps recorded in the second year by discriminant functions created for the chirps of the first year showed a dramatic decrease of correct assignment to the level expected by chance (27.2%). We discuss that long‐distance chirps of wild mature cheetahs provide reliable cues to sex and may also encode caller individuality, although this is not stable over time. Mature cheetahs (Acinonyx jubatus) use long‐distance chirps for calling toward coalition partners (males), mates (both sexes), and cubs (females). Long‐distance chirps of wild mature cheetahs provide reliable cues to sex and may also encode caller individuality, although this is not stable over time. Male chirps were longer in duration and lower in the peak frequency and all fundamental frequency variables than female chirps.
... An exception is the domestic cat in which due to mothers readily permitting the handling and experimental manipulation of their newborn young by familiar caretakers, mutual olfactory recognition between mothers and young has also been found and that the young retain a memory of their mother's scent for more than a year after permanent separation (Bánszegi et al. 2017b;Jacinto et al. 2019;Szenczi et al. 2021). Mothers may also emit specific vocalizations to greet or call their young to follow and which the young rapidly learn to distinguish from similar calls from other mothers (Szenczi et al. 2016). ...
Article
The mammalian order Carnivora is generally defined as species that feed exclusively or to some degree by eating other animals. The Carnivora comprise around 280 species, divided into 16 families, 13 of which are terrestrial and 3 aquatic. Carnivores are spread across the entire planet, including the two polar regions and on land and sea. Consistent with such diverse ecologies, there is no typical pattern of parental care distinguishing carnivores from other mammals. Using examples from different taxonomic families, our aim is to illustrate the diversity of parental care in Carnivora. Major topics include parental care before and after birth of the young, paternal, and alloparental care and the process of weaning. Given the position of many carnivores at the apex of food chains, a greater understanding of their patterns of parental care as a vital part of reproductive biology is essential to conservation programs.
... They were from a free-ranging colony based in a private house in Mexico City. A detailed description of housing of the animals is given in our previous articles (Szenczi et al. 2016;Bánszegi et al. 2017b). Mothers directed little attention to the litters of other females and communal nursing did not occur. ...
Article
Longevity of odour memories, particularly those acquired during early development, has been documented in a wide range of taxa. Here, we report that kittens of the domestic cat retained a memory into adult life of their mother´s body odour experienced before weaning. Kittens from 15 litters were tested when permanently separated from their mother at weaning on postnatal week 8, and tested again when 4 and 6 months and over 1 year of age. When presented with a simultaneous three-way choice between body odour of their own mother, of an unknown female of similar reproductive condition and a blank stimulus, weaning-age kittens sniffed the cotton swab with the odour of an unknown female longer. This preference, however, changed when as adults the subjects sniffed the cotton swab with their own mother’s odour longer. We conclude that kittens form a long-lasting memory of the body odour of their mother, and by implication, that mothers retain an individual odour signature sufficiently stable across age and changes in their reproductive state to be distinguishable by their adult offspring. What this means in functional or cognitive terms is not yet clear. Does such “recognition” have a specific biological function and a specific cognitive representation? Or is it rather part of a more general phenomenon well known in (human) olfaction of odours that are familiar generally being judged more pleasant, and that might then influence olfactory-guided behaviour in a variety of contexts?
... Pup-directed vocalizations are either produced by a single parent (cats; Szenczi et al., 2016) or by both (parrots: Berg et al., 2011), depending on parental investment, whereby to our knowledge these are exclusively produced by females in bats (reviewed in Kunz and Hood, 2000). Like all bat pups studied to date, S. bilineata pups produce isolation calls (ICs; Figure 1C) to solicit maternal care . ...
Article
Full-text available
Social feedback plays an important role in human language development and in the vocal ontogeny of non-human animals. A special form of vocal feedback in humans, infant-directed speech – or motherese – facilitates language learning and is socially beneficial by increasing attention and arousal in the child. It is characterized by high pitch, expanded intonation contours and slower speech tempo. Furthermore, the vocal timbre (i.e., “color” of voice) of motherese differs from the timbre of adult-directed speech. In animals, pup-directed vocalizations are very common, especially in females. But so far there is hardly any research on whether there is a similar phenomenon as motherese in animal vocalizations. The greater sac-winged bat, Saccopteryx bilineata, is a vocal production learner with a large vocal repertoire that is acquired during ontogeny. We compared acoustic features between female pup-directed and adult-directed vocalizations and demonstrated that they differed in timbre and peak frequency. Furthermore, we described pup-directed vocalizations of adult males. During the ontogenetic period when pups’ isolation calls (ICs) (used to solicit maternal care) are converging toward each other to form a group signature, adult males also produce ICs. Pups’ ICs are acoustically more similar to those of males from the same social group than to other males. In conclusion, our novel findings indicate that parent-offspring communication in bats is more complex and multifaceted than previously thought, with female pup-directed vocalizations reminiscent of human motherese and male pup-directed vocalizations that may facilitate the transmission of a vocal signature across generations.
Article
Vocalization may transmit information from the emitting animal, including information about his or her emotional state. This study aimed to compare the vocal and behavioral responses of domestic cats during an aversive and a pleasant situation. A total of 74 cats (29 males and 45 females) in the city of Curitiba, Southern Brazil, participated in the study; 68 (26 males and 42 females) were divided into two treatments: an aversive situation (AS), which was a car transport event where the cat was in a crate, or a pleasant situation (PS), where the cat was were offered a snack. The other animals (three males and three females) participated in both situations. Behavioral signals and individual vocalizations were registered through video recordings and further evaluated in each scenario. Cats in the PS had a higher fundamental frequency of vocalizations (10.1%), a lower range of pitches (tessitura) (33.9%) and twice the rate of head movement rates as compared to AS. For call duration there was significant interaction between treatment and sex. Additionally, there were differences in vocal parameters and behavioral signals due to sex, age and coat color. Females and kittens have higher fundamental frequencies may be due to anatomical characteristics. Solid-colored coated cats presented higher fundamental frequency than other coat colors. Overall, vocal parameters and behavioral signals seem useful indicators for studying the emotions of cats in different situations. Further studies are warranted to understand the subtleties of cat vocalization across sex, age and coat color.
Article
Full-text available
To date, no studies have examined the ontogeny of susceptibility to visual illusions in nonhuman mammals. Our previous study on the perception of the Delboeuf illusion by adult cats suggested they perceive this illusion, and that the visual processing involved in size judgment differs in the presence or absence of a misleading surround. We therefore asked whether weanling kittens are susceptible to the Delboeuf visual illusion, as adult cats are. Like the adults, kittens were presented with a series of 2-way food choice tasks where same- or different-size food portions were presented on same- or different-size plates. In control trials, the kittens significantly discriminated between 2 different amounts of food on same-size plates and, like adults, they chose the larger amount; when the difference between the food amounts was greater, the kittens chose the larger amount more reliably. Olfactory control trials confirmed that kittens, like adults, used visual cues when comparing quantities in this setting. In contrast to adults, however, in the illusion trials with same-size food portions on different-size plates, the kittens did not choose either of the 2 different-size plates significantly above chance and so did not appear to perceive the illusion. This suggests heterochronicity in the development of the cat visual system in which the ability to discriminate sizes develops before susceptibility to an illusion using these stimuli. Remaining questions include at what age susceptibility to visual illusions emerges and whether this depends on continued maturation of the brain, on experience of the visual world, or both. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Full-text available
Background: The development of ethologically meaningful test paradigms in young animals is an essential step in the study of the ontogeny of animal personality. Here we explore the possibility to integrate offspring separation (distress) calls into the study of consistent individual differences in behaviour in two species of mammals, the domestic cat (Felis silvestris catus) and the mound-building mouse (Mus spicilegus). Such vocal responses in young mammals are a potentially useful test option as they represent an important element of mother-offspring communication with strong implications for offspring survival. In addition, the neural control of vocalisation is closely associated with emotional state. Results: We found marked similarities in the pattern of individual responses of the young of both species to separation from their mother and littermates. In the domestic cat as well as in the mound-building mouse, individual differences in the frequency of calls and to a lesser extent in locomotor activity were repeatable across age, indicating the existence of personality types. Such consistencies across age were also apparent when only considering relative individual differences among litter siblings. In both species, however, individual patterns of vocalisation and locomotor activity were unrelated. This suggests that these two forms of behavioural responses to isolation represent different domains of personality, presumably based on different underlying neurophysiological mechanisms. Conclusions: Brief separation experiments in young mammals, and particularly the measurement of separation calls, provide a promising approach to study the ontogeny of personality traits. Future long-term studies are needed to investigate the association of these traits with biologically meaningful and potentially repeatable elements of behaviour during later life.
Article
Full-text available
Although evolutionary theory predicts that parents should discriminate between their own and others' offspring when levels of parental care are large, in some colonial breeding mammals, strong selective pressure would be expected to drive the evolution of mutual recognition between parents and offspring. Although less well studied than parental discrimination, in both birds and mammals, offspring discrimination of parents has also been documented. Nocturnal bats depend heavily on acoustic signals for communication, although little is known about whether bat pups can recognize their mothers based solely on maternal calls. We first investigated whether maternal calls of pomona leaf-nosed bats differ statistically among individuals. Then, we tested whether pups recognize their mothers' calls using a playback experiment. Echolocation pulses and directive calls of mother bats contain enough individual characteristics to allow for individual discrimination. Playback experiments showed that the pups responded with more vocalizations to playback of their own mother's versus another mother's directive calls, but not to playback of their own mother's versus another mother's echolocation pulses, suggesting that offspring can distinguish directive calls, but not echolocation pulses, of their own mother from those of other mothers. To our knowledge, this is the first study in which directive calls from bat mothers have been used to elicit vocalizations from pups and the first study to indicate that pups can recognize their mothers based solely on directive calls. These results advance our understanding of the function of acoustic signals in mothere-offspring recognition and the adaptive benefit of such communication, particularly the conditions under which a pup's recognition of its mother is critical.
Article
Mother–pup vocal recognition abilities in pinnipeds reflect maternal reproductive strategies. In otariids, mother–pup pairs are frequently separated during lactation, pups are highly mobile at an early stage, and the high densities of colonies increase the risks of confusion between individuals. Accordingly, vocal recognition between mothers and pups is well developed in this group. In contrast, among phocids, the young are less mobile and mothers normally stay with their pups throughout lactation. Hence, the risks of confusion between individuals are relatively low and mother–young vocal recognition abilities are less developed in this family. Harbour seals, Phoca vitulina, are unique among phocids as females forage during lactation and pups are highly mobile throughout the 21–42-day nursing period. We performed playback experiments on 18 breeding female harbour seals to assess their abilities to recognize the calls of their pup and to evaluate the effect of maternal protectiveness and pup vocal stereotypy on the recognition process. In addition, we performed propagation tests to assess pup call propagation efficiency in the environment. As early as 3 days after birth, females were more responsive to calls of their own pup than to calls of nonfilial pups. Female responses also varied depending on their protective behaviour displayed towards their pup. Vocal discrimination abilities were positively correlated with the level of individuality conveyed in pup calls. Considerable differences in the propagation efficiency of pup calls were measured among sites used for mother–pup reunions. This study provides experimental evidence that wild harbour seal females recognize the calls of their pup among others, and that they memorize previous versions of their pup's calls, taking into account age- and size-related effects on vocalizations. Propagation tests further suggest that some mother–pup reunion sites are better suited for vocal recognition than others.
Article
The first part of the paper describes the process of weaning in domestic cats. The subjects were seven families of cats living under laboratory conditions-each family consisting of a mother and her two kittens, living in their own large indoor pen. Observations were carried out at regular intervals from the third week after birth until the kittens were 10 weeks old. Weaning is viewed as the period during which the rate of milk transfer from mother to offspring drops most sharply. According to this definition, weaning commenced when the kittens were four weeks old and was largely completed by the time they were seven weeks old. During the weaning period mothers made suckling progressively more difficult for their kittens by increasingly adopting body postures that blocked access to their nipples. The amount of suckling declined sharply from four weeks after birth and seldom occurred after seven weeks. Kittens were first seen to eat solid food during the fifth week, and this was associated with a large increase in the variability of their daily weight gain. Prior to the start of weaning, mothers' food intake was approximately double that of non-lactating females. Male kittens grew more rapidly than their sisters and were significantly heavier. However, there was no evidence that males suckled more than females prior to the start of weaning. In general, weaning was characterised by a gradual reduction in the ease with which kittens could suckle, rather than by any overt rejection or aggression by the mother. The absence of any obvious weaning conflict is thought to be related to the favourable housing conditions (small litter size, ad libitum food, freedom from disturbances, etc.) used in this study. The second half of the paper describes the results of an experiment in which maternal lactation was interrupted during the first week of weaning. Seven Experimental (E) mothers were injected with the lactation-blocking drug bromocriptine on days 28, 30 and 33 post partum, each injection being sufficient to interrupt lactation for about 18-24 h. In the period immediately following the injections (days 29-46), E mothers and their kittens were more active than the controls, and E mothers washed their kittens more. Later on (days 47-70), E kittens suckled more than the controls-notably in the eighth week after birth, at a time when suckling would normally be rare. E mothers appeared to be more willing to let their kittens suckle during this period, as they adopted a fully accessible posture more often, and a blocking posture less often, than controls. The overall pattern of results is interpreted in terms of an initial withdrawal from the kittens in the period immediately after lactation was interrupted, followed by a later resumption of maternal care and a postponement of the end of weaning. Perhaps as a result of this continuation of suckling, the experimental treatment had no overall effect on the kittens' growth, although the Experimental kittens did grow more slowly in the week of the injections. One tentative hypothesis is that the apparent postponement of weaning represents a compensatory response to the earlier reduction in the rate of parental investment.
Article
Despite growing interest among biologists in animal personality, including in applied contexts, there have been few developmental studies of how and when differences in animal personality arise. And yet, efficient detection of personality differences early in development could be useful in selecting individuals for various management purposes. In a first step towards addressing this, we report results of a study of individual differences in general motor activity among littermates of the domestic cat, obtained using an observational method designed to overcome the difficulties of evaluating the behaviour of newborn altricial young. Three litters (14 kittens) were filmed in the absence of the mother at regular intervals across the 1st postnatal (pre-weaning) month. Six untrained observers independently viewed 10 videos for each litter and ranked the kittens in each video from the least to the most active. Significant differences were found between at least some kittens in all three litters (Friedman tests: Fr = 16.3, 25.8, 11.3; P < 0.0001, 0.0001, 0.0085, respectively), and there was significant agreement on kitten ranks among the six observers (Kendall coefficients of concordance: W = 0.84, 0.84, 0.55; P < 0.01 for the three litters, respectively). There was also significant agreement between the results of two observers using the ranking method and a quantitative method of behavioural assessment (Spearman rank order correlation: rs = 0.93, P = 0.001). We conclude that stable individual differences in general motor activity, possibly indicating differences in temperament, are present in kittens early in development, and that ranking the degree of such behaviour in a naturalistic setting provides a valid and efficient method of detecting such differences. It is now necessary to investigate if such early differences are predictive of later behavioural phenotypes.
Article
Piglets were tested for recognition of their sow's voice at 1, 2 & 6 days of age. Each piglet was tested individually by playing one minute of recorded grunts from its own sow and one minute of recordings from an alien sow, compared with a one minute period alone with no sound. The vocalizations of the piglets were recorded and analysed. Some piglets responded to their own sow's voice by making rapid 'quacking' grunts which differed in tone from the 'closed mouth grunts' made alone; some piglets responded by orientation and movements towards the sound. At one day old 29% piglets responded vocally exclusively to their own sow and 52 % responded when movement was included as a sign of recognition. 27 % piglets responded to both own and alien sow voices. At 2 days the response had become exclusive to their own sow's voice and 87 % piglets responded (57 % vocally). At 6 days 81 % piglets responded positively to their own sow's voice (62% vocally). The number of grunts increased in each minute of the tests with 1 and 2 day old piglets regardless of treatment, but at 6 days old, the piglets grunted more to their own sow's voice than to an alien.
Article
Two vocalizations, utilized by raccoon (Procyon lotor) mothers ("chitter") and their dependent cubs ("whistle") to establish and maintain contact among each other, are studied in captive groups containing a total of 6 mothers and their 23 cubs. The paper describes frequency of occurrence of the calls, contexts of the callers, individual variation in the call&apos;s structure and the responses of the callees both in uncontrolled situations and in playback experiments. Maternal chitters are most frequently used during the month following the first excursion of the cubs from the litter den by females approaching the cubs or leading them to the food. The frequency of whistling in cubs is high during the nestling period and decrease when they become mobile. The main context of whistling in the cub-mother relationship is answering maternal chitters. Mobile cubs follow their chittering mother silently. The chitters of 4 females differ individually, the main distinctive feature being frequency and pulse rate. The whistles of 12 cubs are individually distinctive, too; duration and frequency measures differ most between individuals. Hence, mothers and cubs have the potential for individual acoustic identification. Seven-week-old nestlings, tested in an experimental arena with successive playbacks, answered maternal chitters on average more often and faster than alien ones, warning grunts or a noise. Mobile cubs approached maternal chitters more often when tested in simultaneous playback experiments. Only 2 of 6 females, however, confronted with whistle-playbacks of their own vs alien cubs in their cages, responded more strongly to calls of their offspring. Results indicate that raccoons may be capable of recognizing family members by acoustic cues alone on a familiar vs strange basis. Advantages of using vocalizations in mother-young relationship and of the potential for acoustic identification and recognition in a solitary mammal are discussed.
Article
In Arctocephalus galapagoensis and Zalophus californianus wollebaeki, high calling activity and intensive interactions of mother and pup immediately after birth establish recognition within the first few hours (mother) or days (pup) of birth. Females of both species nurse exclusively their own young and reject strange ones, sometimes aggressively. The prompt reactions of pups to their mothers' Pup Attraction Calls (PACs) suggest that the mother too is individually recognized. Interindividual variability of calls provides a sufficient basis for individual recognition in both species. Pups (10 days to 2 yr old) can discriminate between their mothers' and strange females' PACs. Only by means of individual recognition can females in crowded otariid rookeries limit maternal investment to their own offspring. -from Author Arctocephalus galapagoensis Zalophus californianus wollebaeki