Available via license: CC BY 4.0
Content may be subject to copyright.
Open Journal of Social Sciences, 2016, 4, 20-24
Published Online February 2016 in SciRes. http://www.scirp.org/journal/jss
http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/jss.2016.42004
How to cite this paper: Joseph, E., Easvaradoss, V. and Solomon, N.J. (2016) Impact of Chess Training on Academic Perfor-
mance of Rural Indian School Children. Open Journal of Social Sciences, 4, 20-24. http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/jss.2016.42004
Impact of Chess Training on Academic
Performance of Rural Indian
School Children
Ebenezer Joseph
1
, Veena Easvaradoss
2
, N. Josiah Solomon
3
1
Government of India, Part-Time Research Scholar, Madras University, Chennai, India
2
Department of Psychology, Women’s Christian College, Chennai, India
3
Department of Statistics, Madras Christian College, Chennai, India
Received 19 January 2016; accepted 16 February 2016; published 23 February 2016
Abstract
The objective of this study was to examine the effect of chess training on academic performance of
middle school children in rural India. The impact of chess on various academic courses was ex-
amined. The sample consisted of 100 students of sixth grade with an intervention group under-
going chess training and a control group. Statistical tests were carried out to examine whether the
performance of students has improved after chess intervention. The results of the paired samples
t-test analysis showed significant improvement in academic performances of students in English,
social studies and science, after a year of training in chess skills. The study has important implica-
tions for education.
Keywords
Chess Training, Cognitive Development, Academic Performance, School Children
1. Introduction
Chess is a classic game of strategy, invented more than 1500 years ago in India. Legend has it that the ruler of
India asked his wise men to devise a way to teach the children of the royal family to become better thinkers and
better generals on the battlefield. Chess was the result. In the centuries since its invention, chess has spread to
every country in the world. Chess has long been considered a way for children to increase their mental prowess,
concentration, memory, and analytical skills.
Improved memory is just the tip of the iceberg. Reports from students, teachers, and parents noticed the academic
benefits of chess on math problem solving skills and reading comprehension, an increase in self-confidence, patience,
logic, critical thinking, observation, pattern recognition, analysis, creativity, concentration, persistence, self-control,
sportsmanship, responsibility, respect for others, self-esteem, coping with frustration, and many other influences
which are difficult to measure but can make a difference in student attitude, motivation, and achievement.
The beauty of chess as a teaching tool is that it stimulates children's minds and helps them to build these skills
E. Joseph et al.
21
while enjoying themselves. As a result, children become more critical thinkers, better problem solvers, and more
independent decision makers.
In the schools, chess often serves as a bridge, bringing together children of different ages, races and genders
in an activity they can all enjoy. Chess helps build individual friendships and also school spirit when children
compete together as teams against other schools. Chess also teaches children about sportsmanship how to win
graciously and not give up when encountering defeat. For children with adjustment issues, there are many ex-
amples where chess has led to increased motivation, improved behaviour, better self-image, and even improved
attendance. Chess provides a positive social outlet, a wholesome recreational activity that can be easily learned
and enjoyed at any age.
Chess has shown to improve the academic performance of children. The increase in academic performance
points to the fact that the children have been able to understand concepts better and have acquired better memo-
ry and problem-solving skills. Equipping the younger generation with these skills makes them empowered citi-
zens of the future, who might have far-reaching impact on the society at large.
Many schools all over the world encourage chess play to enhance academic performance. Studying chess sys-
tematically has also shown to raise students’ IQ and exam scores (Dullea 1982; Palm 1990; Ferguson 2000)
[1]-[3], as well as strengthen mathematical, language, and reading skills (Margulies 1992; Liptrap 1998; Fergu-
son 2000) [3]-[5].
In Marina, California, an experiment with chess indicated that after only 20 days of instruction, students’
academic performance improved dramatically. George L. Stephenson, chairman of the Marina JHS math de-
partment, reported that 55% of the students showed significant improvement in academic performance after this
brief smattering of chess instruction. Douglas Williams (2014) [6] and Hong and Bart (2007) [7] examined the
cognitive effects of chess instruction on students at risk of academic failure in Korea. They reported that chess
instruction produces higher chess skill ratings. This may lead to gains in levels of non-verbal intelligence among
students at risk of academic failure.
Although chess originated in India, research assessing its impact on the academic performance and cognitive
development of children is not available. The primary objective of this research is to evaluate and measure the
enhancement of academic performance with chess. This is an essential step towards proving the benefits of chess
for an Indian population. Such research can also help to seriously consider the use of chess within educational
curriculums and schools in India.
Keeping this objective in mind, the following research questions were posed: Does chess intervention cause
an increase in the academic performance of children in low socioeconomic backgrounds? Is there a significant
difference in the academic performance of children after a chess intervention of one year (approximately 30 ses-
sions a year)? Has chess intervention caused an increase in performance in specific subjects like English, ma-
thematics, science and social studies (assuming the other intervening variables to be constant)? How has chess
caused the following increases?
2. Research Design
The study used the experimental method research design. This study used a pre-test and post-test design with a
control group. The experimental group chosen purposively and a random control group. The independent variable
in the study was the chess intervention and the dependent variables were academic scores in English, mathematics,
science, social studies and Tamil (a local language). Chess training was given once a week for a duration of one
year.
3. Sample
The participants of the study were 100 sixth grade students studying in a semirural area in South Tamil Nadu,
India. The school caters primarily to children from the lower socio-economic status, with most of the parents
having only school education.
4. Measures
The students were assessed by their academic performance in English, mathematics, science, social studies and
Tamil before and after chess intervention for a period of one year.
E. Joseph et al.
22
5. Procedure
Students’ academic marks were collected from school. Their academic marks were again collected after chess
intervention of one year. Chess training consisted of standardized once in a week training sessions of one hour
duration over a period of one year. Clustering technique was used to form the training groups. Chess training was
done using the following methodology.
DVD learning (Winning Moves, Episodes 1 - 22)
Demonstration board
On-the-board training
Chess exercise through workbooks (Chess school 1A, Chess school 2, and tactics)
Chess playing on computer (Kasparov’s Chessmate, Fritz)
Tactical chess training using software (Maurice Ashley Teaches Chess, Advance Chess School, CT ART 3.0)
Mapping the prodigies games using chess base software and understating brain patterns of the child
Cognitive correction using softwares for problems such as perception for normal as well as special children
End game training using Theory and practice of End Games, ABC of Endgames and Winning Moves DVD
Ideas behind chess openings
Exposure to classical games
Chess playing sessions
Mock training tournaments
Participation in regular chess tournaments
Analysis of score sheets and thought patterns
From the Table 1 and Table 2, in the experimental group, we observe that there is no significant difference in
the Tamil average marks and there is significant difference in the English, maths, science and social studies in
sixth and seventh standard marks.
Table 1. Paired samples t-test of experimental group.
Mean
N
Standard. Deviation
Standard Error Mean
t
Pair 1
6th Std. Tamil average mark
52.00
48
15.684
2.264
−1.041
7th Std. Tamil average mark
53.60
48
15.994
2.309
Pair 2
6th Std. English average mark
55.10
48
14.565
2.102
−2.072
*
7th Std. English average mark
57.38
48
14.013
2.023
Pair 3
6th Std. Maths average mark
61.06
48
18.778
2.710
2.679
**
7th Std. Maths average mark
56.38
48
16.086
2.322
Pair 4
6th Std. Science average mark 53.81 48 18.571 2.680
−2.804
**
7th Std. Science average mark 57.71 48 15.545 2.244
Pair 5
6th Std. Social average mark 63.25 48 19.165 2.766
4.585
**
7th Std. Social average mark 55.15 48 16.151 2.331
*
p < 0.05,
**
p < 0.01.
Table 2. Paired samples t-test of control group paired samples statistics.
Mean N Standard. Deviation Standard Error Mean t
Pair 1
6th Std. Tamil average mark 55.36 52 17.147 2.378
−1.783
7th Std. Tamil average mark 57.60 52 16.722 2.319
Pair 2
6th Std. English average mark 59.43 52 15.833 2.196
−0.926
7th Std. English average mark 60.47 52 13.604 1.886
Pair 3
6th Std. Maths average mark 62.18 52 18.575 2.576
3.467
**
7th Std. Maths average mark 57.09 52 16.933 2.348
Pair 4
6th Std. Science average mark 55.72 52 19.455 2.698
−2.537
**
7th Std. Science average mark 59.99 52 15.665 2.172
Pair 5
6th Std. Social average mark 67.45 52 19.394 2.689
4.726
**
7th Std. Social average mark 58.60 52 16.414 2.276
*
p < 0.05,
**
p < 0.01.
E. Joseph et al.
23
6. Results
The results were analyzed using SPSS 20. There was a significant increase in the experimental group in all the
subjects except Tamil. Even though, the control group showed increase in academic performance in some of the
subjects, the gains of the experimental group were higher than those of the control group, and the earlier defi-
ciency in the academic performance was removed after chess intervention.
7. Discussion
It is interesting to note that the academic performance in almost all the courses (excluding Tamil) showed an in-
crease following chess training in this study. This is not totally unexpected as we observe that chess playing fo-
cuses on developing cognitive skills like focusing, visualizing, thinking ahead, weighing options, analyzing
concretely, thinking abstractly, planning, and juggling multiple considerations simultaneously, which would in-
variably have its effect on the academic performance of the students. Over time, chess helps develop patience
and thoughtfulness.
It was noticed in the study that the academic performance of the children (N = 48) who chose to play chess
was lower than that of the randomly chosen control group, prior to the intervention. It was also noticed in anoth-
er study conducted by the Emmanuel Chess Centre that the IQ scores of the children who chose to play chess
were higher than those of the randomly selected control group, that is the children who opt to play chess have
higher IQ but lower academic performance prior to the chess intervention. In this study, the impact of chess
training was analyzed to study whether the difference in the academic performance was bridged.
Trinchero (2013) [8] reported similar findings in 556 primary school children who were undergoing chess
training. One prominent result was that the experimental group that received chess training registered a modest
but statistically significant increase in scores on mathematics test items that required problem-solving skills on
complex tasks. This effect was greater among students who had more hours of chess instruction.
Scholz et al. (2008) [9] investigated the effects of chess training on mathematics learning among students
with learning disabilities, based on intelligence scores in the 70 - 85 IQ range. Classes from four elementary
schools in Germany were randomly assigned to two groups: (a) an experimental group that received chess in-
struction of one hour per week for one entire school year and (b) a comparison group that received supplemen-
tary mathematics instruction for one hour per week. The two groups did differ significantly, in their calculation
abilities for simple addition tasks and counting.
Smith and Cage (2000) [10] reported the effects of 120 hours of chess instruction on the mathematics achieve-
ment among rural, African-American secondary school students. They determined that the treatment group
scored significantly higher in mathematics achievement and non-verbal cognitive ability than the control group
after controlling for differences among pretest scores.
8. Implications
Chess is a game that can be used to develop cognitive skills in children. The simplicity of the game makes it a
versatile tool that can be used in schools, homes, and intervention centres. Children learn to play together and
win and lose; they learn valuable lessons that will generalize to academic functioning. It is evident from the
study that chess impacts cognitive development in children and there is a significant improvement in the aca-
demic performance of the children who underwent chess training.
References
[1] Dullea, G.J. (1982) Chess Makes Kids Smarter. Chess Life, 1.
[2] Palm, C. (1990) Chess Improves Academic Performance. New York City Schools Chess Program, 12.
[3] Ferguson, R. (2000) The Use and Impact of CHESS. Section B, USA Junior Chess Olympics Curriculum. 53.
[4] Margulies, S. (1992) The Effect of Chess on Reading Scores: District Nine Chess Program, Second Year Report. The
American Chess Foundation, New York.
[5] Liptrap, J.M. (1998) Chess and Standard Test Scores. Chess Life, 41-43.
[6] Williams, D. (2014) The Game of Chess: A Conduit to Increase Student Academic Achievement in Pinclas County; a
Policy Advocacy Document. 3.
[7] Hong, S. and Bart, W.M. (2007) Cognitive Effects of Chess Instruction on Students at Risk for Academic Failure. In-
E. Joseph et al.
24
ternational Journal of Special Education, 22, 89-96.
[8] Trinchero, R. (2013) Can Chess Training Improve Pisa Scores in Mathematics? An Experiment in Italian Primary
Schools. Kasparov Chess Foundation, Europe.
[9] Scholz, M., Niesch, H., Steffen, O., Ernst, B., Loeffler, M., Witruk, E. and Schwarz, H. (2008) Impact of Chess Train-
ing on Mathematical Performance and Concentration Ability of Children with Learning Disabilities. International
Journal of Special Education, 23, 138-148.
[10] Smith, J.P. and Cage, B.N. (2000) The Effects of Chess Instruction on the Mathematics Achievements of Southern,
Rural, Black Secondary Students. Research in the Schools, 7, 19-26.