ArticlePDF Available

Mindful Living in Older Age: a Pilot Study of a Brief, Community-Based, Positive Aging Intervention

Authors:

Abstract

Although mindfulness-based interventions have been successfully used with older adults, there have been few interventions that (a) are created specifically for older adults, (b) are delivered in the community, and (c) aim to promote successful aging (rather than treating dysfunction/disorder). To this end, the current study piloted a brief ‘positive aging’ intervention, comprising two 150-minute sessions, with six female older adults living in the community. Data were gathered through focus groups that were interwoven throughout the intervention. Using thematic analysis, four main themes were identified: (a) aging as a mixed blessing, (b) understanding mindfulness, (c) the challenges of mindfulness, and (d) the benefits of mindfulness. Overall, the intervention was successful in introducing participants to mindfulness and potentially forming the basis of a longer-term practice. However, the study also highlighted important points regarding the challenges of practising mindfulness, in relation to which the paper makes recommendations pertaining to the teaching of mindfulness with older adults.
1
ORIGINAL PAPER
Mindful Living in Older Age: A pilot study of a brief, community-based, ‘positive aging’
intervention
Tim Lomas Itai Ivtzan Chee Yik Yong
___________________________
T. Lomas
Department of Psychology, University of East London, Water Lane, London, E15 4LZ
e-mail: t.lomas@uel.ac.uk
Note: This article may not exactly replicate the final version published in the Mindfulness. It is not
the copy of record.
2
Mindful Living in Older Age: A pilot study of a brief, community-based, ‘positive aging’
intervention
Abstract
Although mindfulness-based interventions have been successfully used with older adults, there have
been few interventions that, (a) are created specifically for older adults, (b) are delivered in the
community, and (c) aim to promote ‘successful aging’ (rather than just treating
dysfunction/disorder). To this end, the current study piloted a brief ‘positive aging’ intervention,
comprising two 150 minute sessions, with six female older adults living in the community. Data were
gathered through focus groups that were interwoven throughout the intervention. Using thematic
analysis, four main themes were identified: (a) aging as a mixed blessing; (b) understanding
mindfulness; (c) the challenges of mindfulness; and (d) the benefits of mindfulness. Overall, the
intervention was successful in introducing participants to mindfulness and potentially forming the
basis of a longer term practice. However, the study also highlighted important points on the
challenges of practising mindfulness, in relation to which the paper makes recommendations
pertaining to the teaching of mindfulness with older adults.
Key words: mindfulness; intervention; aging; older adults; community; wellbeing
3
One of the great public health challenges faced by Western Societies in the years ahead is managing
the demands posed by an aging population. Due to demographic changes, such as marked increases
in life expectancy over recent decades, it is estimated that the proportion of people aged over 65 in
the UK will increase from 16% currently to around 25% in 2050 (Crackness, 2010). These challenges
are exacerbated by recent economic and political trends, which have seen austerity policies imposed
across the Western world, leading to reduced public spending; e.g., in the UK, it is calculated that
local council spending will have dropped by almost 30% by 2015 (Johnstone, 2014). As a result, Age
UK suggest that care is ‘in crisis’ (Abrahams et al., 2014): on the one hand, there is rising demand for
care, with the number of people aged over 85 increasing by 30% between 2005 and 2014; however,
despite such increases, funding for care of the elderly has been reduced, by as much as 10% in real
terms from 2010 to 2014. Consequently, there is great need for cost-effective ways of meeting the
needs of an aging population.
Among the priorities in this regard will be promoting health and wellbeing in older age.
Physical and mental health burdens associated with aging mean older adults are liable to a marked
reduction in quality of life, particularly those aged over 75 (Department of Health, 2014). The health
burdens of older age are manifold. There is increased morbidity, with greater susceptibility to illness
such as cancer (Turner et al., 1999). There are degenerative issues, from osteoporosis (Kanis et al.,
1994) to diseases such as Alzheimer's (Ferri et al., 2006). Furthermore, these issues intersect in
complex ways with mental health; though there is not a greater prevalence of mental disorder
among older adults per se (Kessler et al., 2005), disorders like depression are associated with aspects
of older age, such as social isolation (Alpass & Neville, 2003) and loss of mobility (Strawbridge et al.,
2002). Taken together, the issues above show that wellbeing in older age is multidimensional,
involving a complex lattice of interlocking aspects; it is not simply about physical decline, but
complex psychological and social challenges too. This idea of multidimensionality is reflected in the
WHO’s (1948, p.1) definition of health as ‘a state of complete physical, mental and social well-being
and not merely the absence of disease or infirmity.’
Addressing the multidimensional burdens of older age is not only of importance to older
adults themselves, and as such a moral issue for society as Gandhi purportedly said, ‘A nation’s
greatness is measured by how it treats its weakest members’ but also an economic one. For
example, with mobility, falls are the most common cause of death from injury in the UK in the over
65s, and cost the NHS over £2 billion each year (Fenton, 2014), with each hip fracture costing around
£12,000 (Department of Health, 2009). With cognitive decline, dementia costs the UK around £26
billion per year, and is the biggest single economic illness burden per sufferer (£32,250) (Prince et
al., 2014), and the King’s Fund estimates that by 2026, dementia care will constitute 73% of all
4
mental health service costs, up from 66% today (McCrone et al., 2008). Thus, any effects to mitigate
the burdens of aging should also be a priority for society even if purely considered from an economic
perspective.
Given the importance of promoting health and wellbeing in old age, and moreover doing so
in a multidimensional way, a key question is, how can we do so? Efforts have been made to develop
interventions specifically designed or tailored for older adults to cultivate or protect the various
wellbeing factors affected by age outlined above (Fernández-Ballesteros et al., 2007). These include
physical activity initiatives (see Falck et al., 2015, for a review), Tai-Chi-based programmes to
improve strength and balance (Gryffin et al., 2015); reminiscence therapies to encourage positive
memory recall (Elias, Neville, & Scott, 2015); education interventions to facilitate on-going learning
(Yamashita et al., 2015); and nutrition programmes to promote healthy eating (Silva-Smith, Fleury, &
Belyea, 2013).
In light of the discussion above, it is vital for interventions to be of good cost-benefit value
and multidimensional in their outcomes (affecting multiple aspects of wellbeing). One particularly
promising intervention in this regard is mindfulness. Although ostensibly ‘just’ an awareness-training
technique (Kabat-Zinn, 1982), studies suggest mindfulness can impact on multiple dimensions of
wellbeing in older adults, including: ameliorating age-related cognitive decline, thus lowering the risk
of developing Alzheimer’s disease (see Gard et al. (2014) for a review); helping pain management
(Morone et al., 2008) and mobility (Adler & Roberts, 2006); and reducing loneliness (Creswell et al.,
2012). As such, mindfulness may function like a ‘master key,’ affecting multiple components of
wellbeing; in theoretical terms, this is because mindfulness is thought to enhance, among other
capacities, executive cognitive function, which plays a pivotal role in diverse physical and mental
behaviours, from emotional management to goal planning and implementation (Heeren et al.,
2009).
However, most studies with older adults have simply used conventional mindfulness-based
interventions (MBIs) with this particular population, most prominently Kabat-Zinn’s (1982)
Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction (MBSR) (e.g., Creswell et al., 2012), as well as Segal et al.’s (
2002) Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy (e.g., Foulk et al., 2014). As such, there are very few
MBIs created specifically for older adults. An exception is Zellner Keller et al. (2014), but theirs is an
8-month on-going programme, rather than a structured intervention. Moreover, most MBIs with
older adults studies involve clinical populations, with either mental disorders like depression (Lynch
& Bronner, 2006) or physical issues like back pain (Morone et al., 2008). There are exceptions that
focus on community populations, but these generally still utilise interventions developed for clinical
groups (e.g., MBSR), and are focused on correcting deficits, such as reducing emotional distress
5
(Young & Baime, 2010) or blood pressure (Palta et al., 2012). Morone et al.’s (2009) mind-body
intervention does appear to be designed specifically for older community-dwelling adults, but again,
is still somewhat ‘deficit focused, with a homework book entitled ‘Keep your brain alive.
Thus, there is considerable scope for creating MBIs that: (a) are specifically designed to
address the needs of older adults (rather than generic MBIs); (b) are delivered in the community
(rather than in clinical settings); and (c) aim to promote ‘successful aging’ (rather than just treating
dysfunction/disorder). This latter point is in keeping with the ethos of positive psychology (Seligman
& Csikszentmihalyi, 2000), defined as the ‘science and practice of improving wellbeing’ (Lomas et al.,
2014, p.ix). In this relatively new field, efforts are made to develop asset-based therapeutic
interventions to enhance wellbeing. Rather than treating people who are physically/mentally ill (who
are of course taken care of by clinical fields), preventative ‘prophylactic’ efforts are made to enhance
wellbeing among those who are currently well but are at-risk of developing physical and mental
health issues. One such group is healthy older adults aged 65-75. Such adults are liable to the health
risks associated with aging above, but may not currently suffer from these. Any efforts to stall or
slow down the onset of such issues in this age group will therefore be of value. Moreover, beyond
just stalling decline, the new subfield of ‘positive aging’ (Tornstam, 2005) aims to understand and
promote ‘successful’ (i.e., ‘flourishing’) aging, with the aim adding ‘more life to years, not just more
years to life’ (Vaillant, 2004, p.561).
As such, a brief ‘positive aging’ MBI was created specifically for older adults, entitled Mindful
Living in Older Age (MLOA), and was piloted with a group of six older adults, as outlined below.
There were a four interlinked research questions: (a) does the MLOA enhance wellbeing in ‘well’
older adults in the community?; (b) does the MLOA affect multiple and/or specific aspects of
wellbeing, including pain, mobility, happiness, and relationships?; (c) what conclusions from the
findings can be drawn relating to challenges to wellbeing in older age?; and (d) what
strategies/recommendations can be extrapolated from the findings pertaining to the use of
mindfulness to promote positive aging (not limited specifically to the MLOA)?
Methods
Participants
Six older adults were recruited from an educational/social group for older adults in London, the
University of the Third Age (U3A). Inclusion criteria were that participants be aged 65-75 and able to
understand English, while the exclusion criteria were previous experiences of practising mindfulness
and being diagnosed with dementia or other mental incapacitation. The administrator of U3A sent
an email to all members containing a brief description of the research, advising them to contact the
researchers if interested. Ten people did so, and were emailed an invitation letter containing further
6
details. Six were able to attend the two sessions; these were all female, university educated, in good
health, and with professional backgrounds. The research was approved by the University ethics
committee, and protocols were in place to ensure the wellbeing of participants (e.g., offering to
spend time with participants after sessions in case of distress).
Procedure & Measures
The intervention and data collection will be outlined together, since these were interwoven, i.e.,
data collection occurred throughout the intervention sessions. The intervention involved two 150-
minute sessions, one week apart, at a residence in London. Data collection consisted of focus groups
(FGs), comprising the participants and the first and second authors (who were also the instructors
for the intervention). Rather than just capturing the ‘before’ and ‘after’ effects of the intervention
through pre and post focus groups (as per Hefferon et al., 2012), group discussions during the
intervention itself were also audio recorded. These discussions essentially served as a ‘peri-
intervention’ FG, meaning that participants’ responses to activities were recorded in real time. (‘Peri’
is a Greek term for around/about.) As such, the analysis was able to capture participants’ ‘journey’
through the intervention. However, the FG were not simply methodological devices, but integral
elements of the intervention itself. That is, like most MBIs, the MLOA featured discussion segments,
in which participants could share experiences, develop understandings, and ask questions of the
instructors; the difference here is that such discussions were also recorded and harnessed as data.
Gathering qualitative data in this way, i.e., during the MBI itself, is relatively novel, having been
recently pioneered by Shonin, van Gordon, and Griffiths (2014).
Session 1 unfolded as follows. The first 45 minutes technically served as the ‘pre-
intervention’ FG, in which participants engaged in discussion around topics such as wellbeing.
However, in effect, this pre-intervention FG can actually be seen as an important part of the
intervention itself, as it helped created a group bond, and introduced participants to mindfulness.
This featured four main questions: What do happiness or wellbeing mean to you?; are there
particular challenges or benefits associated with this time of life?; what does mindfulness mean to
you?; and why were you drawn to participate in the intervention?. The intervention proper then
involved three practices, each of which were immediately followed by a 15 minute FG discussing the
activity. The first was a 15 minute guided mindfulness of breathing, featuring four stages: counting
to 10 at the end of the out-breath; focusing on the nostrils; focusing on the diaphragm; and open
mindfulness. The second practice was a 15 minute guided body scan.
In addition to these standard mindfulness practices, the third was a 15 minute reminiscence
activity adapted from Goldstein’s (2007) ‘Sacred moments’ MBI – in which participants were
invited to recall a ‘precious’ memory, and to feel where it manifested in their body. Now, it is
7
recognised that ‘ruminative reminiscing’ can not only be potentially detrimental to wellbeing
(Brinker, 2013), but conflicts with the kind of receptive openness to the present that characterises
mindfulness. However, rather than using mindfulness to help older adults refrain from reminiscing, it
was felt there may be a benefit from enabling them to bring a degree of mindfulness to this activity.
The rationale here is that while ‘ruminative reminiscing’ may be problematic, reminiscence itself can
be a valued activity, e.g., as a way to experience and construct a sense of meaning in life (Bohlmeijer
et al., 2007). Thus, it was hypothesised that facilitating a mindful reminiscence might prevent the
activity from becoming ruminative, therefore enabling it to remain potentially beneficial. Moreover,
it was not necessarily the case that the activity conflicted with the ethos of mindfulness: although
the cognitive object (i.e., the memory), was ‘in the past,’ participants were encouraged to be mindful
of how they were experiencing this object in the present. The inspiration and precedent for the
practice was Goldstein’s (2007) MBI, which included a similar activity, in which participants were
encouraged to contemplate a ‘sacred object’ – which could include a memory and to attend
mindfully to it. In the present study, participants were likewise encouraged to bring to mind a
‘precious’ memory. The instructor advised that this should not be too ‘poignant or highly
emotionally charged,’ but simply a memory that was positive and that they were ‘fond of.’ Following
this guidance, as per more conventional mindfulness practices, the instructor invited participants to
explore the way this memory was manifesting in the body, i.e., attending with mindful curiosity to
any visceral sensations that were present in relation to the memory.
Finally, the closing FG of the first session thus ended by participants being invited to commit
to practising mindfulness for 10 minutes every day (choosing from any of the three practices they
had learned in the session).
The second session began with a 15 minute FG, in which participants reflected on their
experiences of practising mindfulness during the week. The session then involved four mindfulness
practices, each immediately followed by a 15 minute FG discussing the activity. The first two were
the mindfulness of breathing and the body scan, as per week 1 (in order to reinforce participants’
understanding of these two key practices). The third was a 15 minute ‘mindful eating’ exercise.
Instead of simply using a raisin as per the MBSR protocol, attempts were made to make the activity
veridical by approximating a small meal, thereby suggesting to participants that all meals could be
eaten mindfully (cf. Dalen et al., 2010). Thus, participants helped themselves from a buffet featuring
cheese, bread, grapes and cake, and were guided through eating some of each of the elements
mindfully (and noticing the differences between them). The fourth practice was a 15 minute guided
‘mindful walking’ exercise (cf. Mills & Allen, 2000), undertaken at varying speeds in the garden of the
residence. The session ended with a 40 minute ‘post-intervention’ FG, which again though, was also
8
an integral debriefing component of the intervention itself. This featured three main questions: what
did you find most useful about the sessions; what did you find less useful; and are you likely to
continue practising mindfulness (and if so, which practices)?
Data Analysis
Analysis was conducted using inductive thematic analysis (Braun & Clarke, 2006), a useful method
for qualitative analysis of FGs conducted in relation to wellbeing interventions (e.g., Hefferon et al.,
2012). More specifically, the analysis followed most of the key steps of grounded theory (GT),
including close reading of the transcripts, followed by open and then axial coding (Strauss & Corbin,
1998). However, other elements that would make it a ‘full’ GT analysis were missing, including a
larger sample of participants, a final stage of selective coding, and the end-point development of
new theory. As such, out of respect for the fact that we did not undertake a full GT analysis, but
rather a form of GT ‘lite,’ following Pidgeon and Henwood (1997) we are simply referring to our
analysis using the more generic label of thematic analysis. After all, as Braun and Clarke (2006)
argue, most forms of qualitative analysis are essentially thematic analysis, in that their main unit of
analysis is the theme. Labels such as GT thus just arguably serve to delineate particular and specific
forms of thematic analysis. That said, as Pidgeon and Henwood point out, many empirical studies
that ostensibly use GT are actually similarly ‘lite,’ so our analysis could likewise perhaps simply be
regarded as a version of GT.
In terms of the analysis itself, as indicated above, rather than a simple ‘before’ versus ‘after’
analysis, FG data from throughout both sessions were combined and analysed together. The analysis
began with a close reading of FG transcripts (across both sessions). These were examined for
emergent themes as per the GT stage of open coding a process which generated over 100. Then,
as per the GT stage of axial coding, themes were compared with one another in a process of
constant comparison, and aggregated into meta-themes based on conceptual similarity, generating
around 20 meta-themes. These meta-themes were then also compared with each other, and again
aggregated into categories. This created four categories, which are the four main subsections of the
results section below. Unlike ‘full’ GT, we let the analysis rest at this point without choosing one
core category through a process of selective coding, or generating a new substantive theory as we
did not feel that the data were sufficient to generate a new theory per se.
Results
Across the two sessions, one overarching theme emerged: the dialectical nature of wellbeing. The
data were suffused with the sense that everything mindfulness, wellbeing, aging, and life itself
was a complicated blend of positive and negative elements. Within this overarching theme, four key
themes were identified: (a) aging as a mixed blessing; (b) understanding mindfulness; (c) challenges
9
of mindfulness; and (d) benefits of mindfulness. These will be discussed in turn, illustrated with FG
excerpts; all names are pseudonyms.
Aging as a mixed blessing
Our first category pertains to the process of aging. All participants articulated its ‘dialectical’ nature,
portraying it as a complex mix of light and dark elements. There were five subthemes here: feeling
blessed; liberation; embracing opportunity; concern for society; and fear of the ‘horrors.’
Near the start of the first session, perhaps cognizant of the common stigmatisation of aging
as a process of terminal decline, participants were keen to stress the benefits of aging. This was
initially prompted by a question posed at the start of the session, which asked ‘what do happiness or
wellbeing mean to you?’ This prompted insightful reflections on the nature of wellbeing and
happiness, and on their relative merits as labels. Among the first to reflect was Ruth, who began by
saying, ‘I prefer the term wellbeing, because I don’t think the aim in life is to be happy. Wellbeing
comes about from some kind of acceptance of what is.’ This was followed by her depiction of the
aging process.
As I get older, the word that comes to mind is blessed. It’s actually quite nice getting older,
I’m quite enjoying it. I’m waiting for the horrid bits to really start, and they’re just beginning
to now. But I just feel very grateful for the time that I’ve lived through, and as you get older,
things that used to matter don’t matter so much… So I just feel grateful really about this
stage of my life, because it’s more comfortable than it has been sometimes in the past.
This excerpt encapsulates the idea of aging as a mixed blessing. On the darker side, there is fear of
the onset of horrid’ aspects of aging, of which more will be said below. And yet, there is a strong
and genuine sense of it being a valued time. At this comment, all participants assented, and chimed
in with examples of why they were enjoying this phase of life. (Indeed, the potential for participants
to spark contributions from each other whether agreements or disagreements is a key strength
of the FG paradigm.) The two dominant interlinked themes here were liberation, and embracing
opportunity. With the former, essentially, participants were freed from the burden of having to
work, while still enjoying a degree of financial security through their pension. As Heather exclaimed,
Not going to work, but the money comes in! It makes me feel very relaxed and pleased.’ Ruth
described this as liberating:
There’s freedom. You don’t have to turn up to work. You can wear what you like and do what
you want. It’s a very nice feeling.
10
This liberation was the platform for the pursuit of diverse activities often newly adopted (i.e., post-
retirement) that were conducive to wellbeing. For instance, two participants spoke about the
exhilaration, and social bonding, they got from being part of group that regularly went walking in the
mountains. As Heather put it, ‘It’s like flying an aeroplane without having to be in a control, you can
see everything… It’s a wonderful experience.’ Annabel spoke of her enthusiasm for playing the piano,
a hobby she had just taken up.
Nobody has to tell me how to practice, I just do! It’s my way of relaxing. It’s just wonderful to
have a new thing, at my age!
However, as positive as such narratives were, participants also touched upon the negatives of aging.
There was a concern with the state of society generally, and that the social safety net that
participants had previously benefited from was being withdrawn. For instance, Lillian worried about
increased use of foodbanks, saying that ‘You have an awful conscience about people who don’t have
enough.’ However, the weightiest negative was fear about onset of the aging process, as alluded to
in Ruth’s excerpt above. As these participants were relatively young older adults (65-75), these were
mainly worries about physical issues that were yet to come; however, participants were alert to
ominous signs that such issues were encroaching. For instance, Josie worried that she was
developing a hip.’ As such, participants were very interested in the potential for mindfulness to help
them address physical issues, as discussed in the fourth category below.
Understanding mindfulness
The second category concerns participants’ journey towards a greater understanding of mindfulness,
from initial pre-conceptions through to arriving at a understanding that worked for them. This
comprised five themes: (a) misconceptions; (b) intelligent questioning; (c) valuing science; (d)
discursive sensitivity; and (e) appreciating contextualisation.
First, participants came to the intervention with various perspectives on mindfulness which
could from both a conventional Buddhist and a scientific stance be viewed as misconceptions.
Participants were aware of mindfulness prior to receiving the invitation to participate, with two
referring to it as a ‘buzzword’ in society (an appraisal that led both to harbour an element of
cynicism towards it, e.g., as simply a trendy activity). Evidently, this had not deterred them from
joining the intervention. However, their views were problematic vis-à-vis their potential involvement
in the intervention, as it made mindfulness seem prohibitively difficult and inaccessible.
11
Something I read on mindfulness give me idea that it involved emptying the mind… That’s
quite hard to do! (Ruth)
That said, participants were very keen to learn more about mindfulness, and were likewise open to
changing their perspective. Thus, a continual thread throughout both sessions was a process of
intelligent questioning. Some of this was procedural, e.g., when, where and for how long they should
practice. Alongside this, participants asked the instructors thoughtful questions about the emotional
dynamics of mindfulness. For instance, Annabel queried what you would do with a feeling like
anger,’ worrying whether anger might not be useful in some circumstances (e.g., in motivating one
to change an iniquitous situation), and thus whether making the mind unduly calm through
mindfulness might be problematic. As part of this questioning, participants were particularly drawn
to, and convinced by, scientific evidence and discourse as a validation of mindfulness. As Rita said,
I think that therapists have discovered in their work with neuroscientists that… I don’t know if
I am using the right words, but there is a new kind of little neural pathway can be opened up,
so therapy [involving mindfulness] is much more efficacious than drugs.
In their journey towards understanding mindfulness, participants appeared very attuned to the
nuances of language, and alert to how this affected their appraisal and experience of mindfulness.
For instance, after the body scan, Sarah said she found the guidance unhelpful, and that the notion
of taking one’s breath ‘to’ particular parts of the body prohibited her from engaging: The logic of my
mind thought, but your brain doesn’t go down to the bottom of your spine, so how on earth am I
going to take my awareness down there.’ Conversely, Ruth reflected on how a phrase in the
mindfulness of breathing (about the breath as a river’) really opened up her experience of the
practice, after having initially struggled to engage.
I’m not very comfortable on this chair, so that was on my mind. But then when you used the
word river, that really opened things up, like a string between [my chest] and [my core].
Finally, it helped participants when they could relate mindfulness to activities or experiences they
were already familiar with. Indeed, participants were very able and willing to draw parallels in this
regard. For instance, Annabel related mindfulness to the ‘creative listening’ she had practised in her
work as a psychotherapist. In being absorbed in the moment, Heather felt mindfulness was ‘just like
going back to childhood.’ Ruth related it to her enjoyment of sitting in her ‘beautiful garden’ and
12
appreciating the flowers. Stella imaginatively compared observing the breath to going into the
countryside, and noticing that, despite initial appearances, it was actually full of life.
At first you think it’s all quiet, and then you become aware that there are a million and one
little noises.
The challenges of mindfulness
In participants journey of engaging with mindfulness through the two sessions (and the intervening
week), there were some difficulties along the way. Four main ones emerged: procedural issues;
‘anaesthesia’; discomfort; and ‘dark encounters.’
First, all participants struggled with the ‘how’ of practice. They were often concerned that
they had got the instructions wrong. As Ruth said after the body scan, ‘I was worried if I was doing it
right. Sometimes I couldn’t feel anything.’ At times, some practices felt over-complicated; for
instance, during the mindful movement, Josie found that trying to pay attention to walking and
breathing was tricky: ‘When you said ‘don’t forget your breathing,’ I thought ‘Oh dear,’ and I couldn’t
do both things at once.’ As such, there was an on-going balancing act for the intervention instructor
between giving sufficient guidance and being intrusively prescriptive. On one hand, it was common
for participants to be unsure about what they were supposed to be doing (Stella). As such, in the
mindfulness of breathing, it was universally recommended by participants that the counting phases
were far easier to engage with than the more open stages (Ruth: ‘It’s much easier to focus on the
breath with the number’). Conversely, participants were resistant to guidance being overly intrusive,
and appreciated having the quietude to engage with the exercise. Heather light-heartedly chastised
the instructors after the reminiscence exercise:
Don’t talk too much! I got annoyed I’m afraid that you kept on interrupting my memory… I
was thinking ‘just shut up’!
A second theme might be termed ‘anaesthesia.’ This refers to participants not feeling any particular
emotion or sensation during the practice. This was particularly the case during the body scan. While
participants did appreciate this practice, as elucidated below, it was not uncommon for them to fail
to notice sensations in certain areas. This could be problematic when participants felt they ought to
experience such sensations (as some of instructions appeared to imply). As Stella put it, ‘I didn’t feel
anything in my heart particularly, and I didn’t know what I was supposed to feel!’ A related issue was
that participants could fail to engage with a practice, and/or found it boring, leaving them generally
13
nonplussed and underwhelmed by the experience. As Rita put it after the first mindfulness of
breathing session, I don’t feel as if it’s been a hugely exciting experience!
Conversely, participants were also perturbed to encounter uncomfortable experiences
during mindfulness. Some of these were physical pains or annoyances. As Heather said, ‘The mind
was quiet, but the body was complaining.’ Explaining further, Heather found it ‘hard to sit still, and
not annoy other people. I develop itches, all over the body, and I want to scratch out each of them.’
Participants also complained about intrusive thoughts: describing her efforts to do the reminiscence
practice, Josie complained that ‘as soon as you start to try and think of a positive memory, all this
other stuff comes in and says ‘naff off’!’ More seriously, participants reported what might be called
‘dark encounters,’ i.e., troubling or discomforting qualia. These were not especially common; for
instance, discussing her experience of practising during the intervening week (approximately 10
minutes per day), Heather said that ‘90% of it was very good.’ However, the remaining 10% was very
disquieting to her:
Sometimes it’s very difficult to keep out some very frightening thoughts that really upset
you… As soon as you close your eyes you can start visualising things you don’t like… it can be
extremely horrid… You can get these the thoughts coming in, which if you’re busy you don’t
get, because you’re doing things…
Participants therefore harboured some concerns about mindfulness opening them up to these kinds
of experiences. However, such concerns did not deter participants from practising mindfulness, nor
intending to in the future. This was because: (a) the benefits (see below) outweighed the negatives;
(b) with the benefit of their wisdom and experience, participants reported feeling adequate and able
to deal with these negatives; and (c) mindfulness was perceived as a relatively ‘mild’ practice. On
this latter point, two participants had previously tried other forms of meditation, e.g.,
transcendental meditation, ‘many many years ago,’ and discussed how such practices could be much
more ‘intense.’ Annabel (who had been a psychotherapist) spoke about the dangers of intensive
Buddhist retreats producing a ‘psychotic breakdown’ since people ‘don’t know what they are
pursing’ [i.e., don’t appreciate how challenging notions such as ego-transcendence can be].
Compared to such intensities then, Josie felt that mindfulness was both relatively harmless and more
convenient and ‘user-friendly.
14
In term of 10 minutes [just] thinking about breathing, it seems the opposite of having
anxious, intense experiences, it’s just about calming down and relaxing… I wouldn’t say a
quick fix, but it’s much easier to fit into my daily life.
The benefits of mindfulness
As the previous excerpt indicated, despite presenting some challenges, participants generally
experienced mindfulness as a beneficial activity. There were four main themes here: (a) calming; (b)
coping; (c) exploring; and (d) savouring.
As per Josie’s statement above, participants concurred that mindfulness was generally
calming. At the end of session 1, participants were asked if they would commit to practising 10
minutes of mindfulness per day during the following week. At the second session, all said they had
kept to this arrangement (except for a day or two), generally preferring to do the mindfulness of
breathing (as opposed to the body scan or the reminiscence practice) in the morning. In their reports
of the week, apart from some challenging instances (as noted above), participants tended to find
that brief periods of mindfulness allowed them to tap into a restorative sense of calm (even if
thoughts could still be intrusive). As Stella said,
It’s relaxing, it’s a nice thing to do, to make time for, it’s calming. It sort of centers you,
despite the fact that, you know, all kind of thoughts keep happening.
Moreover, even though participants had only been practising for a week, they suggested that it
could be useful in coping with stress in their daily life. In session 1, participants were encouraged to
try to be mindful at select points during the day. Subsequently, two reported that they engaged
mindfully with their breathing while using public transport which they usually found stressful and
that this helped them feel calmer (Josie: It slows me down a bit, calms me down). Participants thus
appreciated the value of mindfulness as an in-situ coping strategy, and suggested they would use it
as such in future.
I would certainly be inclined to the use it at, you know, at a time of stress… let’s go shut down
and just breathe (Stella)
Mindfulness also enabled participants to explore a different relationship with their embodied
experience. Some exploration links to the theme of coping. As noted above, participants were
concerned about the onset of physical issues; some had thus been drawn to the intervention on the
15
recommendations of friends, who had reported that mindfulness was able to ameliorate pain.
During the week, some were then pleased to note that practice, particularly the body scan, seemed
able to do this. Josie, who worried about her hip, focused her attention on this during meditation,
and found it ‘quite useful,’ saying it was ‘cathartic’ for the joint to be ‘covered in attention’ in this
way. Ruth, who also had hip trouble, reported that ‘the day after our first session I walked along the
road and I didn’t feel it at all, which was lovely, and I thought, ‘Maybe it’s done the trick, I’ll come
back again!’’ Annabel had a nice image for why mindfulness could be helpful for pain.
I feel it’s a bit like a kind parent saying to a child who’s fallen over, something reassuring like,
‘Oh dear, you’ve grazed your knee, it hurts a bit but it will get better.’ The pain is obviously
still there but the child has been reassured.
Beyond simply coping with pain, some embodied explorations were more overtly positive in nature.
After trying the body scan, for instance, Stella reported that ‘when we got up to the top of the head, I
felt quite weightless. I could float up like a balloon.’ Similarly, in the reminiscence practice, Annabel
felt a sensation of ‘warmth’ around her heart which she described as an ‘experience of pure love.’
Such reflections intersect with the final theme, that of savouring. Participants described particularly
enjoyed the mindfulness of eating and movement sessions. For instance, with eating, Ruth reported
that ‘I do love the sensation of eating,’ and that in doing so mindfully she was ‘much more conscious
of the taste.’ With walking, Rita was intrigued byall the insects on the leaves, which I hadn’t noticed
before,’ while Annabel commented on ‘what lovely light there was in the garden.’ She continued by
saying that she could envisage trying to extend this mindful savouring to walking around more
generally.
If you go to a beautiful garden, you’re walking differently aren’t you? Taking it all in in a
different sort of way. I suppose the point is though, even if you’re just doing a trip to the
shops, you can do it in a certain way, you can still notice the gardens and things around you.
Discussion
The results here provide a unique window onto the potential benefits of mindfulness in older age. At
the same time though, the findings also shed a valuable light onto the challenges of introducing
mindfulness to such a population. In terms of the specific intervention being piloted here, the MLOA
appears to be a useful vehicle for helping older adults begin to engage with mindfulness; findings
suggested it offered a brief, focused programme that was sufficient for initiating basic practices,
16
such as the mindfulness of breathing, and for ‘planting a seed’ for their ongoing practice.
Furthermore, beyond the MLOA specifically, the richness of the qualitative data provides a valuable
insight into how mindfulness is received and experienced by older adults, which offers pertinent
lessons for all clinicians/researchers seeking to offer MBIs to such a population.
Before considering the impact of the intervention per se, let us contextualise the discussion
by exploring the findings on aging more generally. Although some cultures afford respect and even
reverence to old age (e.g., associating it with wisdom), Western societies tend to depict aging as a
period of decline and decay (Löckenhoff et al., 2009). However, the emergent ‘positive aging’
paradigm holds that perceptions of aging are often misplaced: for instance, while recognising that
aging is attended by manifold hardships, Tornstam (2005, p.2) suggests there are ‘severe
mismatches between the nuisances and miseries we tend to project on old age and what those who
have reached advanced age tell themselves.’ For instance, longitudinal studies appear to show that
subjective wellbeing is ‘U-shaped’ through the lifespan: analysing data from around 500,000
Americans and Europeans, Blanchflower and Oswald (2008) found that although wellbeing declined
from youth into middle age, it rose again as people entered older ages.
The data here support such findings, with many participants finding benefits to aging,
particularly increased freedom from burdens such as needing to work and social obligations around
appearance (see Simmons and Betschild (2001) for analysis of the complex benefits and problems
linked to retirement). However, this positive view of aging must be qualified with two important
caveats. First, participants here constituted a relatively ‘privileged’ group of older adults, fulfilling
many of the predictive criteria for ‘successful aging,’ such as a high level of education, active
engagement in social groups/programmes (such as U3A), and a decent level of health (Vaillant,
2004). Second, even for these relatively ‘successful’ agers, aging was portrayed as a mixed blessing,
with a tangible fear of the onset of aging-related issues such as musculoskeletal complaints, e.g.,
painful and/or weak hip joints, which are one of the most significant physical burdens of aging
(Department of Health, 2009).
In this context, participants were keen to harness any benefits that mindfulness might offer
in enabling them to deal with such aging-related issues. The MLOA was promising in this regard:
even after just two sessions and one week of practice, it was reported that mindfulness had
appeared to ameliorate subjective experiences of pain. Indeed, Kabat-Zinn’s (1982) MBSR, the
prototypical MBI, was created for this very purpose, and was able to successfully treat chronic pain.
At this point though, it is worth raising a critical point regarding the validity of qualitative data in
assessing the efficacy of MBIs like the MLOA. It has been suggested by critical health theorists that
positive narratives of undertaking MBIs do not necessarily mean that participants ‘genuinely’
17
experience psychophysical benefits, but could be constructing a positive portrayal for other reasons,
such as simply ‘buying into’ discourses of mindfulness, or wanting to convey a desirable impression
to researchers (Ridge et al., 2008). However, there is no need to adopt such a critical stance here, as
participants were by no means reluctant to critique mindfulness or to admit to negative experiences
in relation to it. Thus, we might avail ourselves of Ricoeur’s (1981) differentiation between a
hermeneutics of ‘faith’ and of ‘suspicion.’ The former involves an empathic-interpretative
perspective in which we trust respondents’ accounts as being relatively accurate and faithful to their
actual experiences. The latter recognizes the performative functions of discourse, and that people
construct narratives for various reasons, such as the creation of positive self-identities. Ideally,
Ricoeur recommends a ‘double hermeneutics,’ in which both perspectives are taken into account to
provide a ‘more complete understanding of the participant’s lived experience’ (Frost et al., 2010,
p.15). Thus, while we recognise an element of performativity to the data, there is no reason we must
‘disbelieve’ it; as such, participants’ reports about the effect of mindfulness practices should be
taken as credible and reliable.
This credibility status applies not only to the amelioration of pain, but to the other beneficial
outcomes. Principle among such findings were that mindfulness could: (a) induce a feeling of calm
(corroborating the use of MBIs to help deal with anxiety, e.g., Goodman et al., 2014); provide an in-
situ coping technique to help defuse feelings of anger or stress (cf. the use of MBIs as a tool to help
manage aggression, e.g., Singh et al., 2011); (c) provide participants with opportunities to engage
with pleasant embodied sensations (cf. Caldwell’s (2014) notion of ‘bodyfulness’); and (d) enhance
savouring of daily activities like eating and walking (reflecting the findings of Quoidbach et al., 2010).
Moreover, most studies reporting these positive effects tend to be extensive programmes (e.g., 8
weeks) in clinical settings; as such, it is notable to also find these here in a brief, community-based
intervention.
However, this credibility status also applies to the negative findings pertaining to
mindfulness uncovered in the research. Until recently, it was suggested that amidst the widespread
enthusiasm for mindfulness, relatively little attention had been paid to possible risks and downsides
(Lustyk et al., 2009), with the exception of isolated studies such as Shapiro (1992). However,
researchers have begun to shine a spotlight on experiential challenges associated with mindfulness
in both clinical (Dobkin et al., 2012) and community settings (Lomas et al., 2015). For instance,
Lomas et al. found that meditation was a difficult skill to learn and practise, and that participants
often encountered troubling thoughts and feelings which were hard to manage. Similar themes were
uncovered here, and even if, according to one participant, mindfulness was ‘90% positive,’ it is
important to remain cognizant of the risks in the remaining 10%. It should be noted that much of the
18
original Buddhist literature on mindfulness does emphasise its challenging nature (Engler, 2003).
However, there is a danger of these pitfalls being insufficiently addressed in contemporary
conceptualisations of mindfulness. In correcting this lacuna, this should not only mean being careful
with the use of meditation in clinical populations such as people with a history of psychosis (Lustyk
et al., 2009). It is also important to advise community practitioners of potential challenges they may
encounter, and putting appropriate precautions and protocols in place; indeed, drawing on Lomas et
al., the current study sought to do just this, with the ethics protocols including provisions such as
instructors setting aside time after the sessions to talk with participants who may have been
troubled by their experiences.
Nevertheless, participants were not unduly troubled by their negative experiences, and on
the whole were positive about the intervention. That said, they offered many insightful comments
and questions throughout, providing valuable data on how to enhance the teaching of mindfulness
for older adults. (In obtaining this data, the innovative ‘pre-peri-post’ design of the research really
came to the fore.) First, in terms of participants being able to understand what mindfulness is, and
how it might be used in real life, it helps to relate it conceptually to activities they are already
familiar with, such as sitting in a garden appreciating the flowers. This helps to de-mystify
mindfulness, and to clear up common misunderstandings (e.g., that it involves ‘emptying the mind’).
It also helps participants to incorporate mindfulness into their existing patterns of behaviour. While
it is true that participants seemed very open to trying out new behaviours countering the
stereotype that older adults are ingrained in their habits (cf. Ludwig, 1998) they appeared
disinclined to adopt any radical changes to their overall lifestyle. Thus, the fact that mindfulness
could be slotted into their existing routines, e.g., as a daily practice that did not need to last more
than 10 minutes, and could be practised in the comfort of their home, was certainly in its favour.
Participants also gave useful advice regarding the instruction of mindfulness. For a start,
participants were sensitive to the nuances of language, which influenced the extent to which they
were able to engage with the practices. For instance, they appreciated the skilful use of similes (e.g.,
breath being like a river), but resisted crude metaphors that contradicted their understanding of
how the mind works (e.g., the implied notion that they could literally take their awareness to
different parts of the body). While the pedagogical effectiveness of different forms of language has
been well-studied in educational settings (Cameron, 2003), the teaching of mindfulness appears not
to have been analysed with the same level of linguistic attention, which future research could ideally
remedy. Participants also had useful views on the extensiveness of the instruction, wanting specific
and detailed guidance (e.g., appreciating the structured nature of counting breaths), but resisting
intrusive on-going instruction that disrupted their experience. Finally, in terms of attempting to
19
ensure ‘compliance’ in homework activities – often an issue in mindfulness research (Vettese et al.,
2009) and increase the likelihood of participants adopting a sustainable practice post-intervention,
it would appear better to ‘under-promise and over-deliver.’ Participants felt that being asked to
commit to just 10 minutes per day was very realistic and ‘doable,’ whereas a longer session would
have been onerous and likely to deter engagement. In any case, it was often reported that these 10
minute sessions frequently stretched to 20 or even 30 minutes once participants were engaged in
the practice.
Taking the above points together, the following recommendations for encouraging
mindfulness in older adults can be offered to researchers and instructors. First, make efforts to
relate mindfulness conceptually to existing activities. Second, enable practice to easily fit into
existing patterns of behaviour. Third, pay attention to language, and use similes/metaphors with
care. Fourth, give precise and structured guidance. Fifth, the counting of breaths is a particularly
good introductory practice. Sixth, avoid intrusive instruction during the intervention itself. Finally,
seventh, avoid onerous demands; 10 minutes home practice daily is a good length of time to
recommend.
Further work is of course needed, both to further corroborate the value of the MLOA, and in
relation to using mindfulness with older adults more generally. As ever, the current study had its
limitations. First, as noted above, there are limits to the claims one can make on the basis of
qualitative data, as such data is to an extent a performative construction (Ricoeur, 1981). As such,
any follow-up trial of the efficacy of the MLOA will avail itself of an experimental design and
quantitative methodologies. Second, as also addressed previously, participants here could be
deemed relatively privileged, being people who benefited from factors that predisposed them to
successful aging (e.g., high levels of education) and indeed by most metrics were already aging
successfully (Vaillant, 2004). While mindfulness can certainly be of benefit to all people, there is
arguably a more urgent need to bring it to more vulnerable populations such as people of low
socio-economic status, as exemplified in the work of Kerrigan et al. (2011) which future work on
the MLOA will endeavour to do. Also noteworthy is the fact that no men expressed an interest in
participating, despite males making up around one quarter of the membership of U3A (from where
participants were recruited). Such patterns align with observations that men are less likely to engage
with health-promoting activities, partly due to masculinity norms which encourage toughness and
invulnerability (Courtenay, 2000). As such, future trials of the MLOA might endeavour to reach out
to men specifically, as these are a relatively vulnerable population health-wise (Mansfield et al.,
2003).
20
Nevertheless, this study has shown that the MLOA can potentially play a useful role in
facilitating ‘positive aging.’ The aim was to ‘plant a seed’ in participants regarding the possibility of
‘mindful living’ in older age, and it seemed successful in this regard. In follow-up correspondence
two months after the sessions, four of the six participants said they were still practising some
mindfulness nearly every day at home, while one other participant practised occasionally when she
saw opportunities to do so in a group (she valued the social dimensions of practice more than the
meditation per se). As such, the MLOA seemed successful in introducing participants to mindfulness
and encouraging its ongoing use, thus hopefully, in the words of Vaillant (2004, p.561), helping to
add ‘more life to years, not just more years to life. Moreover, the brief nature of the MLOA means it
may potentially be more cost-effective than lengthier MBIs, which is an important consideration
given widespread reductions in healthcare spending (Abrahams et al., 2014), although more
extensive systematic trials and analyses in future will be required to assess this possibility.
Compliance with ethical standards
This study was funded by an internal ‘Early Career Researcher’ small grant from the University of
XXXX [redacted for the purposes of blinded review]. The authors declare that they have no conflicts
of interest. All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance
with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964
Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Informed consent
was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.
21
References
Abrahams, C., Green, M., & Mortimer, J. (2014). Care in Crisis. London: Age UK.
Adler, P. A., & Roberts, B. L. (2006). The use of Tai Chi to improve health in older adults. Orthopaedic
Nursing, 25(2), 122-126.
Alpass, F. M., & Neville, S. (2003). Loneliness, health and depression in older males. Aging & Mental
Health, 7(3), 212-216.
Blanchflower, D. G., & Oswald, A. J. (2008). Is well-being U-shaped over the life cycle? Social Science
& Medicine, 66(8), 1733-1749.
Bohlmeijer, E., Roemer, M., Cuijpers, P., & Smit, F. (2007). The effects of reminiscence on
psychological well-being in older adults: A meta-analysis. Aging and Mental Health, 11(3),
291-300.
Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qualitative Research in
Psychology, 3(2), 77-101.
Brinker, J. K. (2013). Rumination and reminiscence in older adults: Implications for clinical practice.
European Journal of Ageing, 10(3), 223-227.
Caldwell, C. (2014). Mindfulness & bodyfulness: A new paradigm. The Journal of Contemplative
Inquiry, 1(1), 69-88.
Cameron, L. (2003). Metaphor in educational discourse. London: A&C Black.
Courtenay, W. H. (2000). Constructions of masculinity and their influence on men's well-being: A
theory of gender and health. Social Science & Medicine, 50(10), 1385-1401.
Crackness, R. (2010). The ageing population Key Issues for the New Parliament 2010. London: House
of Commons Library Research.
Creswell, J. D., Irwin, M. R., Burklund, L. J., Lieberman, M. D., Arevalo, J. M., Ma, J., . . . Cole, S. W.
(2012). Mindfulness-based stress reduction training reduces loneliness and pro-
inflammatory gene expression in older adults: A small randomized controlled trial. Brain,
Behavior, and Immunity, 26(7), 1095-1101.
Dalen, J., Smith, B. W., Shelley, B. M., Sloan, A. L., Leahigh, L., & Begay, D. (2010). Pilot study: Mindful
Eating and Living (MEAL): Weight, eating behavior, and psychological outcomes associated
with a mindfulness-based intervention for people with obesity. Complementary Therapies in
Medicine, 18(6), 260-264.
Department of Health (2009). Fracture prevention services: An economic evaluation. London:
Department of Health.
Department of Health (2014). Ageing Well. London: Department of Health.
22
Dobkin, P. L., Irving, J. A., & Amar, S. (2012). For whom may participation in a mindfulness-based
stress reduction program be contraindicated? . Mindfulness, 3(1), 44-50.
Elias, S. M. S., Neville, C., & Scott, T. (2015). The effectiveness of group reminiscence therapy for
loneliness, anxiety and depression in older adults in long-term care: A systematic review.
Geriatric Nursing, 36(5), 372-380.
Engler, J. (2003). Being somebody and being nobody: A reexamination of the understanding of self in
psychoanalysis and Buddhism. In J. D. Safran (Ed.), Psychoanalysis and Buddhism: An
Unfolding Dialogue (pp. 35-79). Somerville, MA: Wisdom Publications.
Falck, R. S., McDonald, S. M., Beets, M. W., Brazendale, K., & Liu-Ambrose, T. (2015). Measurement
of physical activity in older adult interventions: a systematic review. British Journal of Sports
Medicine. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2014-094413
Fenton, K. (2014). The human cost of falls Public Health Matters. London: Public Health England.
Fernández-Ballesteros, R., Kruse, A., Zamarrón, M. D., & Caprara, M. (2007). Quality of life, life
satisfaction, and positive aging. In R. Fernández-Ballesteros (Ed.), Geropsychology: European
perspectives for an aging world (pp. 197-223). Cambridge, MA: Hogrefe & Huber.
Ferri, C. P., Prince, M., Brayne, C., Brodaty, H., Fratiglioni, L., Ganguli, M., . . . Huang, Y. (2006). Global
prevalence of dementia: a Delphi consensus study. The Lancet, 366(9503), 2112-2117.
Foulk, M. A., Ingersoll-Dayton, B., Kavanagh, J., Robinson, E., & Kales, H. C. (2014). Mindfulness-
based cognitive therapy with older adults: An exploratory study. Journal of Gerontological
Social Work, 57(5), 498-520.
Frost, N., Nolas, S. M., Brooks-Gordon, B., Esin, C., Holt, A., Mehdizadeh, L., & Shinebourne, P.
(2010). Pluralism in qualitative research: The impact of different researchers and qualitative
approaches on the analysis of qualitative data. Qualitative Research, 10(4), 441-460.
Gard, T., Hölzel, B. K., & Lazar, S. W. (2014). The potential effects of meditation on agerelated
cognitive decline: A systematic review. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,
1307(1), 89-103.
Goldstein, E. D. (2007). Sacred moments: Implications on well-being and stress. Journal of Clinical
Psychology, 63(10), 1001-1019.
Goodman, J., Guarino, A., Chenausky, K., Klein, L., Prager, J., Petersen, R., . . . Freeman, M. (2014).
CALM Pregnancy: Results of a pilot study of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for
perinatal anxiety. Archives of Women's Mental Health, 17(5), 373-387.
Gryffin, P. A., Chen, W. C., Chaney, B. H., Dodd, V. J., & Roberts, B. (2015). Facilitators and Barriers to
Tai Chi in an Older Adult Community: A Theory-Driven Approach. American Journal of Health
Education, 46(2), 109-118.
23
Heeren, A., Van Broeck, N., & Philippot, P. (2009). The effects of mindfulness on executive processes
and autobiographical memory specificity. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47(5), 403-409.
Hefferon, K., Mallery, R., Gay, C., & Elliott, S. (2012). ‘Leave all the troubles of the outside world’: a
qualitative study on the binary benefits of ‘Boxercise’ for individuals with mental health
difficulties. Qualitative Research in Sport, Exercise and Health, 5(1), 80-102.
Johnstone, R. (2014). Council spending ‘cut by nearly one-third since 2010’: Chartered Institute of
Public Finance and Accountancy.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1982). An outpatient program in behavioral medicine for chronic pain patients based
on the practice of mindfulness meditation: Theoretical considerations and preliminary
results. General Hospital Psychiatry, 4(1), 33-47.
Kanis, J. A., Melton, L. J., Christiansen, C., Johnston, C. C., & Khaltaev, N. (1994). The diagnosis of
osteoporosis. Journal of Bone and Mineral Research, 9(8), 1137-1141.
Kerrigan, D., Johnson, K., Stewart, M., Magyari, T., Hutton, N., Ellen, J. M., & Sibinga, E. M. S. (2011).
Perceptions, experiences, and shifts in perspective occurring among urban youth
participating in a mindfulness-based stress reduction program. Complementary Therapies in
Clinical Practice, 17(2), 96-101.
Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Merikangas, K. R., & Walters, E. E. (2005). Lifetime
prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity
Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62(6), 593-602.
Löckenhoff, C. E., De Fruyt, F., Terracciano, A., McCrae, R. R., De Bolle, M., Costa Jr, P. T., . . . Alcalay,
L. (2009). Perceptions of aging across 26 cultures and their culture-level associates.
Psychology and Aging, 24(4), 941-954.
Lomas, T., Cartwright, T., Edginton, T., & Ridge, D. (2015). A qualitative summary of experiential
challenges associated with meditation practice. Mindfulness, 6(4), 848-860. doi:
10.1007/s12671-014-0329-8
Lomas, T., Hefferon, K., & Ivtzan, I. (2014). Applied Positive Psychology: Integrated Positive Practice.
London: Sage.
Ludwig, F. M. (1998). The unpackaging of routine in older women. American Journal of Occupational
Therapy, 52(3), 168-175.
Lustyk, M. K., Chawla, N., Nolan, R. S., & Marlatt, G. A. (2009). Mindfulness meditation research:
Issues of participant screening, safety procedures, and researcher training. Advances in
Mind-Body Medicine, 24(1), 20-30.
Lynch, T. R., & Bronner, L. L. (2006). Mindfulness and Dialectical Behavior Therapy (DBT): Application
with depressed older adults with personality disorders Mindfulness-based treatment
24
approaches: Clinician's guide to evidence base and applications (pp. 217-236). San Diego, CA,
US: Elsevier Academic Press.
Mansfield, A., Addis, M., & Mahalik, J. (2003). "Why won't he go to the doctor?": The psychology of
men's help seeking. International Journal of Men's Health, 2(2), 93-109.
McCrone, P., Dhanasiri, S., Patel, A., Knapp, M., & Lawton-Smith, S. (2008). Paying the price: The cost
of mental health care in England to 2026. London: King's Fund.
Mills, N., & Allen, J. (2000). Mindfulness of movement as a coping strategy in multiple sclerosis: A
pilot study. General Hospital Psychiatry, 22(6), 425-431.
Morone, N. E., Greco, C. M., & Weiner, D. K. (2008). Mindfulness meditation for the treatment of
chronic low back pain in older adults: A randomized controlled pilot study. Pain, 134(3), 310-
319.
Morone, N. E., Rollman, B. L., Moore, C. G., Qin, L., & Weiner, D. K. (2009). A mindbody program for
older adults with chronic low back pain: Results of a pilot study. Pain medicine, 10(8), 1395-
1407.
Palta, P., Page, G., Piferi, R., Gill, J., Hayat, M., Connolly, A., & Szanton, S. (2012). Evaluation of a
mindfulness-based intervention program to decrease blood pressure in low-income African-
American older adults. Journal of Urban Health, 89(2), 308-316.
Pidgeon, N., & Henwood, K. (1997). Using grounded theory in psychological research. In N. Hayes
(Ed.), Doing Qualitative Analysis in Psychology (pp. 245-273). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
Prince, M., Knapp, M., Guerchet, M., McCrone, P., Prina, M., Comas-Herrera, A., . . . Salimkumar, D.
(2014). Dementia UK (Second ed.). London: Alzheimer's Society.
Quoidbach, J., Berry, E. V., Hansenne, M., & Mikolajczak, M. (2010). Positive emotion regulation and
well-being: Comparing the impact of eight savoring and dampening strategies. Personality
and Individual Differences, 49(5), 368-373.
Ricoeur, P. (1981). Hermeneutics and the Human Sciences (J. B. Thompson, Trans.). Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Ridge, D., Williams, I., Anderson, J., & Elford, J. (2008). Like a prayer: The role of spirituality and
religion for people living with HIV in the UK. Sociology of Health & Illness, 30(3), 413-428.
Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., & Teasdale, J. D. (2002). Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy for
Depression: A New Approach to Preventing Relapse. New York: Guilford Press.
Seligman, M. E. P., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology: An introduction. American
Psychologist, 55(1), 5-14.
Shapiro, D. H. (1992). Adverse effects of meditation: A preliminary investigation of long-term
meditators. International Journal of Psychosomatics, 39(1-4), 62-67.
25
Shonin, E., Gordon, W., & Griffiths, M. D. (2014). Meditation Awareness Training (MAT) for Improved
Psychological Well-being: A Qualitative Examination of Participant Experiences. Journal of
Religion and Health, 53(3), 849-863.
Silva-Smith, A. L., Fleury, J., & Belyea, M. (2013). Effects of a physical activity and healthy eating
intervention to reduce stroke risk factors in older adults. Preventive Medicine, 57(5), 708-
711.
Simmons, B. A., & Betschild, M. J. (2001). Women's retirement, work and life paths: Changes,
disruptions and discontinuities. Journal of Women & Aging, 13(4), 53-70.
Singh, N. N., Lancioni, G. E., Manikam, R., Winton, A. S. W., Singh, A. N. A., Singh, J., & Singh, A. D. A.
(2011). A mindfulness-based strategy for self-management of aggressive behavior in
adolescents with autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5(3), 1153-1158.
Strauss, A., & Corbin, J. (1998). Basics of Qualitative Research: Techniques and Procedures for
Developing Grounded Theory (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA Sage.
Strawbridge, W. J., Deleger, S., Roberts, R. E., & Kaplan, G. A. (2002). Physical activity reduces the risk
of subsequent depression for older adults. American Journal of Epidemiology, 156(4), 328-
334.
Tornstam, L. (2005). Gerotranscendence: A Developmental Theory of Positive Aging. New York:
Springer Publishing Company.
Turner, N. J., Haward, R., Mulley, G., & Selby, P. (1999). Cancer in old ageis it inadequately
investigated and treated? BMJ, 319(7205), 309-312.
Vaillant, G. (2004). Positive aging. In P. A. Linley & S. Joseph (Eds.), Positive psychology in practice
(pp. 561-580). Hoboken, New Jersey: Wohn Wiley & Sons.
Vettese, L. C., Toneatto, T., Stea, J. N., Nguyen, L., & Wang, J. J. (2009). Do mindfulness meditation
participants do their homework? And does it make a difference? A review of the empirical
evidence. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 23(3), 198-225.
Worth Health Organization (1948). Preamble to the Constitution of the World Health Organization as
Adopted by the International Health Conference, New York, 19-22 June, 1946. Geneva:
World Health Organization.
Yamashita, T., López, E. B., Keene, J. R., & Kinney, J. M. (2015). Predictors of Adult Education
Program Satisfaction in Urban Community-Dwelling Older Adults. Educational Gerontology,
41(11), 825-838.
Young, L. A., & Baime, M. J. (2010). Mindfulness-based stress reduction: Effect on emotional distress
in older adults. Complementary Health Practice Review, 15(2), 59-64.
26
Zellner Keller, B., Singh, N., & Winton, A. W. (2014). Mindfulness-based cognitive approach for
seniors (MBCAS): Program development and implementation. Mindfulness, 5(4), 453-459.
... Western societies currently face significant public health challenges prompted by the demands associated with increased life expectancy and an aging demographic (Lomas et al., 2016). Specifically, older adults represent the fastest-growing cohort with the highest rates of health care needs (Hazlett-Stevens et al., 2019). ...
... Specifically, older adults represent the fastest-growing cohort with the highest rates of health care needs (Hazlett-Stevens et al., 2019). This rising demand of seniors seeking medical care to address chronic medical conditions necessitates the urgent need for cost-effective ways to promote the health and well-being of an aging population (Hazlett-Stevens et al., 2019;Lomas et al., 2016). ...
... Although a priority for the field, research on the processes through which mindfulness affects well-being remains in its infancy, especially research that targets older adults (Geiger et al., 2016). With the efficacy of MBIs supported across multiple treatment domains, there is considerable room for the creation of MBIs designed to address the needs of older adults that encourage "successful aging" rather than the treatment of "disfunction [or] disorder" (Lomas et al., 2016). Importantly, a study by Hazlett-Stevens et al. (2019) reminds us that the primary aim of MBIs is the "intentional practice of acceptance" rather than the targeting of "symptoms for change" (p. ...
Article
Full-text available
A growing body of research supports the efficacy of mindfulness-based interventions (MBIs) to address a wide range of health concerns. This narrative literature review recognizes the important role these interventions play in response to public health challenges prompted by an aging demographic. Moreover, this review identifies opportunities to deliver cost-effective, community-based interventions tailored to older adults that promote successful aging. Complementary to this diverse demographic, MBIs allow participants to access naturally developed abilities that support overall well-being and resiliency later in life. The intersecting and beneficial effects of these interventions offer a well-suited, proactive, and holistic avenue to enhance quality of life for older adults.
... Emotional distress and depression in old age are influenced by a complex combination of factors (Lomas et al., 2016). The decline in mobility, the increase in social isolation, the increase of physical and cognitive disease, and the loss of close relationships increase the risk for depression (Rodda et al., 2011). ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Seniors in nursing homes are at higher risk for depression and emotional distress. The COVID-19 crisis and isolation have even increased the risk for cognitive decline and suicidality. Since social media is often unfamiliar to older adults, their treatment options were diminished during quarantine. Recently, studies showed the potential for Mindfulness-Based Intervention in improving cognitive functioning and psychological well-being among healthy older adults. Standardized courses such as MBSR and MBCT are not suited to the majority of seniors for several reasons. First, the interventions are too long and demanding, physically and cognitively. Second, they require an instructed counselor for delivery, which makes it almost impossible in times of quarantine, and third, very expensive. Hence, the purpose of this study was to examine whether similar improvements in emotional distress and cognitive functioning can be achieved through a brief intervention, that can be delivered by workers in nursing homes. Methods A course of 8 half-hour sessions each (MBIS: Mindfulness-Based Intervention for Seniors) was employed in two versions: (1) An 8-week course with weekly meetings (2) A 4-week course with 2 sessions per week and compared to a control care-as-usual group. Depression and mood were measured, as well as cognitive abilities in the Simon task. In addition, the level of Mindfulness skills was measured before and after the interventions. Results We showed that brief interventions succeeded in improving mindfulness Non-reactivity and produced changes in the facets Acting with awareness and Non-judging. More importantly, the brief intervention, in both frequency versions, improved the level of depression and mood (BDI and PHQ-9). At the cognitive level, an adaptive sequential effect appeared after the intervention only in the 8-week MBIS group. Discussion These findings indicate the effectiveness of this pilot of a short, simple, mindfulness-based intervention, in improving depression and psychological distress, as well as improving cognitive control over time. This may enhance significant developments in the field of treatment solutions for seniors, with a ready-to-use protocol to administer in nursing homes.
... Second, we provided latitude in practice duration, after some participants in previous cohorts complained of physical discomfort. Given evidence that longer practices are often viewed as unreasonable by older adults (Lomas et al., 2016), and a lack of evidence supporting longer home practices (Lloyd et al., 2018), we recorded two versions of the formal meditations, one at the standard 40-min duration, and another at a condensed 20-min duration. Finally, we provided additional optional written materials such as poetry and summary information to reinforce and expand upon each week's learning theme. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objectives Older adulthood presents unique challenges to mental health. We explored how a mindfulness training intervention adapted for older adults addressed these challenges, contributing to a qualitative profile of mindfulness pedagogy for older adults. Methods Mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) was adapted for a geriatric community sample with elevated depressive symptoms. Intervention effects were examined through a combination of scale-based assessment and qualitative interviews. Scales measured depressive symptoms, cognitive function, quality of life, and mindful attitudes. Interviews assessed stressors, goals, perceived obstacles, outcomes, and practice habits. A grounded-theory approach was used to identify responses in each category. Multiple regression analyses were used to identify qualitative responses that best predicted symptom reduction. Results The program was well-tolerated, positively endorsed, and associated with reduced depressive symptoms. Qualitatively, participants who identified family and mood as their primary stressors benefitted the most from course participation. A focus on present moment awareness was associated with decreased depressive symptoms, whereas a focus on hedonics, i.e., “feeling good,” was associated with poorer treatment outcomes. Participants maintained strong future practice intentions and preferred guided meditations to self-directed practice. Conclusions The adapted MBSR program was well-tolerated and efficacious in a geriatric community sample. Efficacy may be improved by emphasizing skill development outcomes rather than achievement of positive feeling states, and by prioritizing program referrals for coping with socioemotional rather than environmental stressors. Future research could extend this mixed-methods approach to deepen our understanding of the relationship between participants’ lived experiences and clinical outcomes.
... First, mindfulness is a difficult skill for older adults to learn and practice (Lomas et al. 2016). We found that the mindfulness group enjoyed the training less than the control group, which we theorize could be related to the difficulty and effort of FAM, especially compared to the ease and familiarity of mind wandering. ...
Article
Full-text available
Link to article: https://rdcu.be/b1vjp OSF: https://osf.io/kw6yp/?view_only=e900dbd0acb945d5b645833a3c4a715a Objectives: Many cognitive changes occur in later life, including declines in attentional control and executive functioning. These cognitive domains appear to be enhanced with mindfulness training, particularly focused attention mindfulness (FAM) meditation, suggesting this practice might slow age-related change. We hypothesized that FAM training would increase self-reported and behaviorally measured mindfulness; improve executive functioning, attentional control, and emotion regulation; and reduce self-reported daily cognitive errors in older adults. To address the call for higher methodological rigor in the field, we used a RCT design with an active control group, measured credibility and expectancy, used objective measures, and attempted to isolate mechanisms of action of mindfulness. Methods Fifty older adults aged 65 to 90 (M = 75.7, SD = 5.7) completed pre-training testing followed by 6 weeks of online daily FAM or mind-wandering (control) training and then returned for post-training testing. Results Conditions were comparable with respect to credibility and expectancy. However, most hypotheses were not supported. Though aspects of mindfulness, Fs(1,45) ≥ 7.42, ps ≤ .009, ηp²s ≥ 0.13, CIs90% = [0.02, 0.37] and inhibitory control, F(1,43) = 4.95, p = .031, ηp² = 0.10, CI90% = [0.01, 0.25], increased, this was not specific to mindfulness training. There was modest evidence of an improvement in attentional control specific to the mindfulness group, F(1,43) = 4.59, p = .038, ηp² = 0.10, CI90% = [0.01, 0.24], but this was not consistent across our two measures. Conclusions Results are encouraging for the continued study of mindfulness for improving aspects of attentional control in older adults. However, the present research requires replication in a larger, more diverse sample. Implications for future mindfulness studies with older adults are discussed.
... Farokh Abazari and his co-authors research about the relationship between anxiety and maternal-fetal attachment. Data has been analyzed using anxiety questionnaire of Spielberger, Cranley's maternal-fetal attachment questionnaire and SPSS version 20. Results show that there is a direct relationship between anxiety and maternal-fetal attachment such that concern of fetal abnormality and abortion is followed by increase of attachment in addition to increasing amount of anxiety. ...
... Attempts to teach and facilitate mindfulness, such as the recent creation of MBIs, emphasize that mindfulness is not simply a sedentary activity, but is a receptive mental state with which one could imbue all one's activities. As such, many MBIs include activities such as mindful walking and mindful eating (Lomas et al. 2016), and there are even MBIs focusing specifically on movement, such as the mindfulness-based exercise program developed by Tacón and McComb (2009). Then, of course, there are kinship activities based around helping people bring qualities such as awareness, calmness, and poise to their movements, from Tai Chi (Yeh et al. 2004) to yoga (Rani and Rao 1994). ...
Article
Full-text available
Amidst the burgeoning enthusiasm for mindfulness in the West, there is a concern that the largely secular ‘de-contextualized’ way in which it is being harnessed is denuding it of its potential to improve health and wellbeing. As such, efforts are underway to ‘re-contextualize’ mindfulness, explicitly drawing on the wider framework of Buddhist ideas and practices in which it was initially developed. This paper aims to contribute to this, doing so by focusing on Zen Buddhism, and in particular on Zen aesthetic principles. It concentrates on the seven principles identified by Shin’ichi Hisamatsu (1971) in his classic text Zen and the Fine Arts: kanso (simplicity); fukinsei (asymmetry); koko (austere sublimity); shizen (naturalness); daisuzoku (freedom from routine); sei-jaku (tranquillity); and yūgen (profound grace). The presence of these principles in works of art is seen as reflecting and communicating insights that are central to Buddhism, such as non-attachment. Moreover, these principles do not only apply to the creation and appreciation of art, but have clear applications for treating health-related disorders, and improving quality of life more generally. This paper makes the case that embodying these principles in their lives can help people enhance their levels of psychosomatic wellbeing, and come to a truer understanding of the essence of mindful living.
... Taking into consideration the approach of Positive Action Theory among young people (Seligman and Csikszentmihalyi, 2000;Lomas et al., 2016;Pawelski, 2016), various intervention programs have emerged over the last few decades which start by considering sports or physical activity as a way to achieve the objective of improving the personal and social development of teenagers thought to be 'at risk' (Escartí et al., 2010a,b). Although the aims and effectiveness of the programs have been varied, most of them have centered on increasing participants' moral reasoning, attributions, self-concept, self-perception of efficiency, and the understanding of other people's worlds. ...
Article
Full-text available
The performance of school children has been studied by considering partial relationships between several personal variables such as the link between cognition and motivation. However, contextual variables, such as a child’s willingness to accept social responsibility, also influence students’ social and academic performance. Thus, students with greater responsibility have a better attitude toward their studies, resulting in higher academic achievement. This 2-year study aims to reveal to what extent an intervention program affects student performance and is based on the Theory of Positive Action among young people proposed by Don Hellison and the Theory of Reasoned Action by Fishbein and Ajzen. The program focuses on positive influences on social and personal responsibility, taking into consideration parental styles, gender, and academic performance. The program was a part of the educational curricula in participating schools and it targeted four main areas: (a) teaching units using academic texts about social responsibility, (b) student training in mediation processes, (c) teacher training, and (d) family training and involvement. A total of 271 students took part from first and second year of Secondary Education (12–14 years old). The experimental group was made up of 132 students while the remaining 139 formed the control group. All participants completed the Assessment Scale of Social Responsibility Attitudes in Secondary Education and the Parent–Adolescent Communication Scale. Results show that students in the experimental group performed significantly better than those in the control group. Additionally, the issue of social responsibility seems to be related to commitment, self-discipline and perseverance. Regarding gender, males appear to score higher in the factor for well-mannered, friendly and tidy. Finally, a positive relationship has been identified between social responsibility attitudes and parenting with an open communicational style. This paper discusses the results so that schools can include programs aimed at improving social and personal responsibility.
Article
Bu çalışmanın amacı yaşlanma sürecini tanımlarken öne çıkan kavramları, pozitif yaşlanma kavramını, pozitif yaşlanmayı teşvik eden müdahaleleri ve stratejileri açıklamaktır. Amaç doğrultusunda Web of Science, Ebsco, Scopus, Pubmed ve Google Akademik veri tabanları kullanılarak anahtar kelimeler çerçevesinde erişilen makaleler incelenmiştir. Yaşlı nüfusun ve yaşlı yetişkinlerin yaşam süresinin artmasıyla, yaşlı yetişkinlerin yalnızca daha uzun değil, aynı zamanda biyopsikososyal açıdan nasıl daha iyi ve mutlu yaşayabilecekleri, refah düzeylerinin nasıl artabileceği, yaşlanma süreçlerinin nasıl destekleneceği gibi konulara odaklanıldığı görülmektedir. Bu noktada yaşlılığa özgü konulara odaklanan pozitif psikoloji ve pozitif gerontoloji hareketinin bir uzantısı olarak pozitif yaşlanma kavramı karşımıza çıkmaktadır. Pozitif yaşlanma, optimal, sağlıklı, başarılı, üretken, aktif yaşlanma gibi iyi yaşlanmanın çeşitli özelliklerini sağlık, biliş, aktivite, duygulanım ve fiziksel uygunluk gibi birçok alanla birleştiren çok boyutlu bir kavramdır. Güncel literatüre bakıldığında pozitif yaşlanmanın başarılı, sağlıklı, aktif, üretken yaşlanma gibi kavramları da içerisinde barındıran daha kapsayıcı bir kavram olduğu ifade edilmektedir. Bununla birlikte pozitif yaşlanma bu kavramların yerini alacak bir terim olarak yorumlanmamalıdır. Yaşlı yetişkinlerin yaşlanma süreçlerini anlamasına ve pozitif yaşlanmalarını teşvik etmeye yönelik müdahalelerin uygulanmasına ihtiyaç olduğu düşünülmektedir. Pozitif psikoloji ilkelerinin yaşlanma sürecinin hangi boyutlarını, nasıl etkilediğini ortaya koyacak daha fazla pozitif yaşlanma araştırmasının gerçekleştirilmesi önerilmektedir.
Article
Healthy ageing is becoming a demand rather than a privilege from personal to societal perspective. One of the inevitable components of healthy ageing is wellbeing which is dependent on the quality of the consciousness. Mindfulness nurtures consciousness leading to behaviour regulation and well-being. Generativity is an attribute which connects individual to the society and predicts well-being significantly. The primary objective of the research was to find the relationship between mindfulness, generativity and subjective well-being among the older adults. Whether pursuing hobbies and social engagements affect the mindfulness, generativity and subjective well-being of olds was also investigated. With the help of standardized questionnaires the data was collected from 205 willing male and female older adults between 60 and 70 years from selected areas of Mumbai and Pune Metropolitan Region. Independent sample t tests and regression analysis were used to test the hypotheses. The results revealed significant effect of pursuing hobbies and social engagement in mindfulness, generativity and subjective well-being of older adults from their counterparts. Mindfulness was found to be a significant predictor of generativity and wellbeing of older adults. Mindfulness and generativity together significantly predicted the subjective wellbeing although generativity did not individually contribute significant variance in the subjective wellbeing. The implications of the research are discussed in the context of enhancing subjective well-being in the older adults by mindfulness training and promoting activities to pursue hobbies and social engagement which would help them for healthy ageing.
Article
Background and Objectives: Few studies have focused on the utility of mindfulness-meditation for well-being in older adults. The present study investigated the feasibility, acceptability, and preliminary efficacy of an app-based mindfulness-meditation program among older adults. Research Design and Methods: A community-based sample of 46 participants aged between 63 and 81 (M = 70.85, SD = 4.70) engaged with a 30-day app-based mindfulness-meditation program for 10-minutes daily on their smartphones. Each meditation session comprised focusing on the breath, mentally scanning the body, monitoring the mind’s activity, and cultivating a nonjudgmental orientation toward experiences. Participants completed questionnaires at baseline, day 10, and day 30. Results: On average, participants completed 25 sessions and almost 4 hours of application use across the 30-days. Results of linear mixed effects models showed significant improvements in positive affect, negative affect, and life satisfaction across the study interval, but no meaningful change in total mindfulness or perceived stress. Furthermore, relative to high levels of smartphone efficacy, low smartphone efficacy was associated with higher perceived stress and negative affect, and less life satisfaction at baseline; and steeper improvements on these outcomes across the study interval. On average, older adults rated the app-based mindfulness-meditation training as interesting, enjoyable, valuable, and useful. Discussion and Implications: The findings provide preliminary support for the feasibility and acceptability of an app-based mindfulness-meditation program with community-dwelling older adults and demonstrate potential benefits for well-being. Results suggest the value of further research investigating the efficacy of digital mindfulness-meditation interventions for older adults via larger randomized-controlled trials. ***Full text available at https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/gnaa093/5871388***
Article
Full-text available
Background: Interventions to promote physical activity (PA) among older adults can positively impact PA behaviour and other health outcomes. Measurement of PA must be valid and reliable; however, the degree to which studies employ valid and reliable measures of PA is unclear. The purpose of this systematic review was to evaluate the measurement tools used in interventions to increase PA among older adults (65+ years), including both self-report measures and objective measures. In addition, the implications of these different measurement tools on study results were evaluated and discussed. Methods: Four electronic research databases (MEDLINE, PsychINFO, Web of Science and EBSCO) were used to identify published intervention studies measuring the PA behaviour of adults over 65 years of age. Studies were eligible if: (1) PA was an outcome; (2) there was a comparison group and (3) the manuscript was published in English. Data describing measurement methods and properties were extracted and reviewed. Results: Of the 44 studies included in this systematic review, 32 used self-report measures, 9 used objective measures and 3 used both measures. 29% of studies used a PA measure that had neither established validity nor reliability, and only 63% of measures in the interventions had established both validity and reliability. Only 57% of measures had population-specific reliability and 66% had population-specific validity. Conclusions: A majority of intervention studies to help increase older adult PA used self-report measures, even though many have little evidence of validity and reliability. We recommend that future researchers utilise valid and reliable measures of PA with well-established evidence of psychometric properties such as hip-accelerometers and the Community Health Activities Model Program for Seniors (CHAMPS) Physical Activity Questionnaire for Older Adults.
Article
Full-text available
Positive psychology has tended to be defined in terms of a concern with ‘positive’ psychological qualities and states. However, critics of the field have highlighted various problems inherent in classifying phenomena as either ‘positive’ or ‘negative.’ For instance, ostensibly positive qualities (e.g., optimism) can sometimes be detrimental to wellbeing, whereas apparently negative processes (like anxiety) may at times be conducive to it. As such, over recent years, a more nuanced ‘second wave’ of positive psychology has been germinating, which explores the philosophical and conceptual complexities of the very idea of the ‘positive.’ The current paper introduces this emergent second wave by examining the ways in which the field is developing a more subtle understanding of the ‘dialectical’ nature of flourishing (i.e., involving a complex and dynamic interplay of positive and negative experiences). The paper does so by problematizing the notions of positive and negative through seven case studies, including five salient dichotomies (such as optimism versus pessimism) and two complex processes (posttraumatic growth and love). These case studies serve to highlight the type of critical, dialectical thinking that characterises this second wave, thereby outlining the contours of the evolving field.
Chapter
This chapter measures successful aging by assessing four facets of health-both physical health and psychosocial health, both subjective health and objective health. First, with the passage of time, progressively diminished physical reserves are an inevitable part of aging, but, of course, the rate at which these diminished reserves occur is variable. Second, physical health can involve experiencing the biological ravages of age without feeling sick. Third, age and social class are important. The mission of positive aging is to add more life to years, not just more years to life. Results of the Study of Adult Development show that much of a septuagenarian's positive aging or lack of it is determined by controllable factors established before the age of 50. Seven factors that predict positive aging are not being a smoker, adaptive coping style, absence of alcohol abuse, healthy weight, stable marriage, exercise, and more years of education.
Article
Lifelong learning is receiving greater attention due to population aging in modern societies. Lifelong learning benefits individuals by supporting their physical, psychological, social, and economic well-being. However, older adults generally have lower motivation for learning than younger adults, and facilitating long-term participation in learning activities is still challenging. Previous studies mainly identified negative factors such as barriers and obstacles to individuals’ initial participation in lifelong learning programs. As such, less is known about positive factors that promote long-term participation. To address this gap, data were collected from 330 older adults who participated in the Osher Lifelong Learning Institute program in an urban community in the United States. Results from proportional odds ordinal logistic regression analysis demonstrated that gender, number of household members, income, religious affiliation, self-rated health, and number of courses taken were associated with satisfaction with the program. In hopes to promote true lifelong learning, possible explanations about the findings are explored and several recommendations for existing lifelong learning programs are derived in this study.
Article
Loneliness, anxiety and depression are common problems for older adults in long-term care. Reminiscence therapy is a non-pharmacological intervention that may be of some benefit. In comparison to individual reminiscence therapy, group reminiscence therapy is a preferred option when dealing with the resource constraints of long-term care. The aim of this paper was to systematically review the literature in order to explore the effectiveness of group reminiscence therapy for older adults with loneliness, anxiety and depression in long-term care. Results indicated that group reminiscence therapy is an effective treatment for depression in older adults, however to date, there is limited research support for its effectiveness to treat loneliness and anxiety. Further research and an improvement in methodological quality, such as using qualitative and mixed methods approaches, is recommended to help establish an evidence base and provide better understanding of the effectiveness of group reminiscence therapy. Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.