ArticlePublisher preview available

When are Do-Gooders Treated Badly? Legitimate Power, Role Expectations, and Reactions to Moral Objection in Organizations

Journal of Applied Psychology
Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

Organization members who engage in "moral objection" by taking a principled stand against ethically questionable activities help to prevent such activities from persisting. Unfortunately, research suggests that they also may be perceived as less warm (i.e., pleasant, nice) than members who comply with ethically questionable procedures. In this article, we draw on role theory to explore how legitimate power influences observers' responses to moral objection. We argue that individuals expect those high in legitimate power to engage in moral objection, but expect those low in legitimate power to comply with ethically questionable practices. We further propose that these contrasting role expectations influence the extent to which moral objectors are perceived as warm and subjected to social sanctions (i.e., insults, pressure, unfriendly behavior). We test our predictions with 3 experiments. Study 1, which draws on participants' prior workplace experiences, supports the first section of our mediated moderation model in which the negative association between an actor's moral objection (vs. compliance) and observers' warmth perceptions is weaker when the actor is high rather than low in legitimate power and this effect is mediated by observers' met role expectations. Study 2, an online experiment featuring a biased hiring task, reveals that the warmth perceptions fostered by the Behavior × Legitimate Power interaction influence observers' social sanctioning intentions. Finally, Study 3, a laboratory experiment which exposes participants to unethical behavior in a virtual team task, replicates Study 2's findings and extends the results to actual as well as intended social sanctions. (PsycINFO Database Record
When are Do-Gooders Treated Badly? Legitimate Power, Role
Expectations, and Reactions to Moral Objection in Organizations
Ned Wellman
Arizona State University
David M. Mayer, Madeline Ong,
and D. Scott DeRue
University of Michigan
Organization members who engage in “moral objection” by taking a principled stand against ethically
questionable activities help to prevent such activities from persisting. Unfortunately, research suggests
that they also may be perceived as less warm (i.e., pleasant, nice) than members who comply with
ethically questionable procedures. In this article, we draw on role theory to explore how legitimate power
influences observers’ responses to moral objection. We argue that individuals expect those high in
legitimate power to engage in moral objection, but expect those low in legitimate power to comply with
ethically questionable practices. We further propose that these contrasting role expectations influence the
extent to which moral objectors are perceived as warm and subjected to social sanctions (i.e., insults,
pressure, unfriendly behavior). We test our predictions with 3 experiments. Study 1, which draws on
participants’ prior workplace experiences, supports the first section of our mediated moderation model
in which the negative association between an actor’s moral objection (vs. compliance) and observers’
warmth perceptions is weaker when the actor is high rather than low in legitimate power and this effect
is mediated by observers’ met role expectations. Study 2, an online experiment featuring a biased hiring
task, reveals that the warmth perceptions fostered by the Behavior Legitimate Power interaction
influence observers’ social sanctioning intentions. Finally, Study 3, a laboratory experiment which
exposes participants to unethical behavior in a virtual team task, replicates Study 2’s findings and extends
the results to actual as well as intended social sanctions.
Keywords: ethics, legitimate power, role theory, person perception, social sanctions
Scholars have long emphasized the importance of preventing
unethical activity in organizations (Ashforth, Gioia, Robinson, &
Treviño, 2008;Donaldson & Preston, 1995;Freeman, 1984;Mar-
golis & Walsh, 2003). Numerous studies have chronicled the
suffering that organizations and their members experience as a
result of unethical practices such as workplace incivility (Ander-
sson & Pearson, 1999;Cortina, Magley, Williams, & Langhout,
2001), abusive supervision (Aryee, Chen, Sun, & Debrah, 2007;
Mitchell & Ambrose, 2007;Zellars, Tepper, & Duffy, 2002),
sexual harassment (Fitzgerald, Drasgow, Hulin, Gelfand, &
Magley, 1997;Gutek & Koss, 1993), and employee fraud (Green-
lee, Fischer, Gordon, & Keating, 2007;Kelly & Hartley, 2010). At
the individual level, exposure to unethical behavior increases psy-
chological distress and job withdrawal, while reducing job satis-
faction, commitment, and productivity (Aryee et al., 2007;Cortina
et al., 2001;Tepper, 2000). At the organizational level, studies
have documented the strategic, legal, and financial benefits of
preventing ethical misconduct (Ashforth et al., 2008;Bamberger,
2006;Karpoff, Lee, & Martin, 2008;Schnatterly, 2003). In fact, a
recent survey estimated that internal fraud alone costs organiza-
tions a staggering 5% of their annual revenue, which translates to
an annual global fraud loss of nearly $3.7 trillion (Ratley, 2014).
Scholars also agree that the willingness of employees to speak
up when they witness a practice or behavior they believe to be
morally wrong is essential in preventing unethical activity from
spreading insidiously within organizations (Leavitt, Reynolds,
Barnes, Schilpzand, & Hannah, 2012;Mesmer-Magnus & Viswes-
varan, 2005;Miceli, Near, & Dworkin, 2008;Sumanth, Mayer, &
Kay, 2011;Treviño, 1992). Indeed, approximately 20% of all
internal fraud cases in 2014 were detected as the result of proactive
employee complaints (Ratley, 2014). In this article, we focus on
these proactive complaints, which we refer to as “moral objec-
tion.” We formally define moral objection as the act of speaking up
or taking action to oppose a morally questionable practice, or
refusing to participate in the practice. Moral objection is similar to
employee voice in that it is a proactive challenge to the status quo
(Van Dyne & LePine, 1998). However, unlike most forms of
voice, it involves an explicit appeal to ethical principles, is not
necessarily constructive, and is not always directed at one’s hier-
archical superiors (Maynes & Podsakoff, 2014). Moral objection
Editor’s Note. Michelle K. Duffy served as the action editor for this
article.—GC
This article was published Online First February 15, 2016.
Ned Wellman, Department of Management, Arizona State University;
David M. Mayer, Madeline Ong, and D. Scott DeRue, Department of
Management and Organizations, University of Michigan.
The authors thank Lillian Chen, Kathleen Grace, Sophia Park, and
Michael Payne for help with data collection, and Samir Nurmohamed and
Nathan Pettit for assistance with study design.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Ned
Wellman, Department of Management, Arizona State University, W.P.
Carey School of Business, 400 E. Lemon St., Tempe, AZ 85287. E-mail:
edward.wellman@asu.edu
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Journal of Applied Psychology © 2016 American Psychological Association
2016, Vol. 101, No. 6, 793–814 0021-9010/16/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/apl0000094
793
... Starting with legitimate power, Raven and French (1958), Lord (1977), and Wellman et al. (2016) explain that legitimate power is characterised by an individual's status in a formal corporate hierarchy. The legitimate power of the observer can influence whistleblowing intentions through their status in the organisation and the dependence of the business on that individual. ...
... At the same time, the company may be more dependent on those with a higher status, since they have a greater technical or executive value (Near & Miceli, 1995). Furthermore, Wellman et al. (2016) contend that subordinates expect individuals with high legitimate power to engage in moral objection. Since legitimate power comes with an essential source of role expectation, employees with legitimate power feel morally obligated to report wrongdoing as a duty to the company they work for (Wellman et al., 2016). ...
... Furthermore, Wellman et al. (2016) contend that subordinates expect individuals with high legitimate power to engage in moral objection. Since legitimate power comes with an essential source of role expectation, employees with legitimate power feel morally obligated to report wrongdoing as a duty to the company they work for (Wellman et al., 2016). Staff with legitimate authority are expected to be confident and display courage when facing adversity (Mbago et al., 2018). ...
Article
Full-text available
We explore how power and justice dynamics influence whistleblowing behaviour in family firms, focusing on the under-explored construct of connection power. Power and justice are two important, interrelated forces strongly affecting moral behaviour. We hypothesise a moderated moderation model and use a 2 × 2x2 between-subject experiment with 331 participants to test our conceptual model. We find that the family relation (i.e., connection power) of the wrongdoer changes what we know about the relationship between the legitimate power of the observer and whistleblowing likelihood, and that, in some instances, observers with high legitimate power are even less likely to blow the whistle than those with low legitimate power. Our exploration further reveals that, although the family relation of the wrongdoer discourages would-be whistleblowers, even those with legitimate power, organisational justice consistently increases the likelihood of whistleblowing in every case.
... Reporting makes the transgressor's actions known, supposedly allowing third parties, defined here as those who receive reports of abusive behavior, to take corrective action to prevent similar behavior in the future. As a result, organizational scholars have hailed the importance of reporting and have called on employees to serve as reporters of abusive behavior (Wellman et al. 2016). ...
... Third, our findings challenge research, mostly from the behavioral ethics literature, that reporting problematic behaviors is generally effective. In many papers, a baseline suggestion is that reporting will be effective because it makes issues known to the organization (Mayer et al. 2013, Wellman et al. 2016). However, our research suggests that reporting behaviors do not always facilitate corrective action, in part because of how the reporter is perceived by third parties. ...
Article
Full-text available
Organizations encourage employees to report abusive behavior as such reports are believed to facilitate corrective action against transgressors. However, there are competing perspectives on whether reports made by women (versus men) will facilitate corrective action. On the one hand, a dominant stream of research suggests that reports made by women are often ignored and disregarded because women are not seen as credible. On the other hand, an emerging stream of research suggests that third parties will see reports made by women as serious and important. To reconcile these perspectives, we draw on aversive discrimination theory, which hints that the degree of corroboration about abuse plays a key role. That is, under situations of low corroboration, third parties are unlikely to take corrective action when women (versus men) make reports, but under situations of high corroboration, third parties are equally or even more likely to take corrective action when women (versus men) make reports. We additionally theorize and find that corroboration is particularly influential when the reporter’s general credibility is not established. Our empirical package includes six complementary studies: an archival data set of U.S. Government employees and five preregistered experiments. Funding: For financial assistance, we thank the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill [Junior Faculty Development Award].
... We constructed a virtual team chat design based on prior research on confronting prejudice and ethical voice (Chen & Treviño, 2022;Czopp et al., 2006;Wellman et al., 2016). This interactive design allowed us to realistically embed racist comments and assess participants' actual confrontation behaviors during the team chat. ...
Article
Full-text available
Despite Americans’ recent heightened awareness of racial inequality, anti-Asian racism at work remains underrecognized and largely unaddressed. In this research, we aim to understand why White bystander coworkers may fail to confront anti-Asian racism. Integrating the moral exclusion perspective and research on racial positions, we propose that due to perceiving Asian Americans as more foreign than other non-White coworkers, White coworkers are less likely to feel anger and engage in confrontation when witnessing anti-Asian racism at work. We first conducted a survey study (Study 1), demonstrating the external validity of the phenomenon that White coworkers are less likely to confront racism when the victim is Asian American versus Black. We then conducted two experiments (Studies 2 and 3) with a realistic, interactive design and behavioral measures of confrontation, supporting our hypothesized mechanisms (i.e., perceived target foreignness and anger). Study 3 further generalized our theory by including Hispanic/Latinx American targets as an additional comparison group and showing that the relative perceived foreignness among Asian American, Hispanic/Latinx American, and Black targets reduced White coworkers’ anger and confrontation. We then conclude by discussing the theoretical and practical implications of our work.
... We used a critical incident experiment technique, which is valuable because it offers the opportunity to capture phenomena that are real to employees and in relation to their behavioral responses to these real events. This method is also particularly useful to uncover how observers respond to ethical situations (see Flanagan, 1954;Kundro & Nurmohamed, 2021;Wellman et al., 2016). Participants were asked to recall a time over the past 6 months corresponding to one of three critical incident conditions (randomly assigned): high leniency, low leniency, and control. ...
Article
Full-text available
Although punishment deters misconduct, protects employees from harm, and maintains cooperation in organizations, not all leaders punish—some are lenient. Employees keenly watch leaders’ responses to misconduct. Leniency is often judged as unfair because it violates moral principles of justice, motivating observers to withhold support to leaders. Our research shifts the conversation to explain how moral consideration of offenders factors into the sensemaking of leaders’ leniency that influences observer reactions. Perceptions of offender personal need (distress from the offender’s personal life that is outside their control) raise observers’ humanitarianism, which is reflected in compassion. Compassion elicited from offender personal need motivates observers to reduce the distress from the situation, lessening the unfairness of the leniency and punitive reactions to the leader. Three experiments demonstrated that leniency elicited unfairness that reduced support to leaders; observers’ perceptions of offender personal need moderated the effects of leniency, reducing its unfairness and punitive reactions to leaders. In Studies 2 and 3, we found that compassion mediated the moderating effects of offender personal need. Only distress from personal need that is inflicted onto offenders (i.e., other-inflicted personal need), compared to distress from work performance need (Study 2) and self-inflicted personal need (Study 3), elicited compassion that lessened the unfairness of leniency. Study 3 also showed that self-inflicted personal need elicited contempt for the offender, which mediated the moderating effect of self-inflicted personal need, bolstering the unfairness of leniency and lessening support to lenient leaders. Implications to theory and practice are presented.
Article
Full-text available
Given the overall positive influence ethical leaders have on their followers’ performance, the literature has largely assumed that ethical leadership also facilitates the performance of leaders themselves. We challenge this assumption by adopting a within-person perspective to reveal more nuanced relationships between distinct forms of daily ethical leadership and daily leader performance. Building on the affect theory of social exchange (Lawler, 2001), we develop a theoretical model that examines the diverging effects of daily promotion- and prevention-focused ethical leadership on daily leader performance through the reciprocal influence of followers’ affective reactions. Specifically, we predict that whereas daily promotion-focused ethical leadership will elicit follower displayed gratitude toward the leader, daily prevention-focused ethical leadership will elicit follower displayed anger toward the leader. Downstream, we predict that follower displayed gratitude and anger will subsequently influence leaders’ in-role and extra-role performance. We also explore how overall social exchange quality shapes the daily affective and behavioral dynamics between leaders and followers. Results from three studies using a multimethod approach provide convergent support for our model. Overall, this research offers both theoretical and practical insights about the potentially unexpected leader-centric consequences of ethical leadership.
Article
Relying on the work on creative prototype and role theory, we demonstrate that having a creative identity can lead to moral objection depending on the implication of the act for one’s identity as a creative individual. In a pilot study using a survey of working adults, we find that employees’ creative identities and their intention to object in moral situations are positively and significantly correlated. Utilizing measurement-of-mediation (Study 1) and experimental mediation (Studies 2–4) approaches, we test and find support for the mediating role of norm-breaking motives predicting moral objection. In Studies 2–4, we find that creative identities lead to moral objection when moral objection is counter-normative and not when it is an expected, normative behavior. Across five studies ( N = 1,327), utilizing both experimental and correlational methods, this paper shows that creative identities can prompt moral objection when such objection is counter-normative, aligning with the creative prototype and reinforcing a creative identity through norm-breaking motives. We extend prior theories on the consequences of creativity, the creative prototype, creative identities, and their link with moral acts.
Article
Purpose Recognising the value of sustaining virtuous values in family business across generations, this paper aims to provide a conceptual framework and propose a mediated-moderated mechanism through which family members’ traits, such as family size and parenting style, influence the extent to which family business’s virtuous values transfer across generations. Design/methodology/approach The paper is based on systematic literature that was conducted using specific keyword searches in the business source databases of Emerald, ProQuest, ScienceDirect, EBSCOhost and SpringerLink. Findings This paper leads to a conceptual framework proposing a mediating relationship between family members’ traits and the transfer of virtuous values to the next generation. Further, two parallel mediators are proposed, moderated by traits of family members’ offspring, such as the age gap and gender of offspring. Research limitations/implications This paper proposes a conceptual framework focusing on transferring virtuous values across generations in the family business. It investigates family members’ traits, such as the size of the family and parenting style, to comprehend the family members’ traits and the transfer of virtuous values relationship. Practical implications The proposed conceptual framework should form the basis of interventions adopted by family business members to enhance the transfer to virtuous values across generations by positively impacting their moral self-efficacy and affective commitment to virtuous values. Originality/value Prior research on family businesses has primarily explored transgenerational succession. However, sustaining virtuous values across generations is equally important to retain a business’s legacy. Very limited scholarly attention has focused on these virtuous values in family business.
Preprint
Full-text available
Artificial Intelligence (AI) is increasingly being integrated into scientific research, particularly in the social sciences, where understanding human behavior is critical. Large Language Models (LLMs) like GPT-4 have shown promise in replicating human-like responses in various psychological experiments. However, the extent to which LLMs can effectively replace human subjects across diverse experimental contexts remains unclear. Here, we conduct a large-scale study replicating 154 psychological experiments from top social science journals with 618 main effects and 138 interaction effects using GPT-4 as a simulated participant. We find that GPT-4 successfully replicates 76.0 percent of main effects and 47.0 percent of interaction effects observed in the original studies, closely mirroring human responses in both direction and significance. However, only 19.44 percent of GPT-4's replicated confidence intervals contain the original effect sizes, with the majority of replicated effect sizes exceeding the 95 percent confidence interval of the original studies. Additionally, there is a 71.6 percent rate of unexpected significant results where the original studies reported null findings, suggesting potential overestimation or false positives. Our results demonstrate the potential of LLMs as powerful tools in psychological research but also emphasize the need for caution in interpreting AI-driven findings. While LLMs can complement human studies, they cannot yet fully replace the nuanced insights provided by human subjects.
Article
Full-text available
The relationship between subordinates' perceptions of abusive supervision and supervisors' evaluations of subordinates' organizational citizenship behavior (OCB) was explored among a sample of 373 Air National Guard members and their military supervisors. As predicted, the relationship between abusive supervision and subordinates' OCB was stronger among subordinates who defined OCB as extra-role behavior (compared with those defining OCB as in-role behavior), and this effect was fully mediated by the interactive effect of procedural justice and OCB role definitions. The study's implications for theory and research are discussed, its limitations are identified, and directions for future research are suggested.
Article
Full-text available
The motivation, planning, production, comprehension, coordination, and evaluation of human social life may be based largely on combinations of 4 psychological models. In communal sharing, people treat all members of a category as equivalent. In authority ranking, people attend to their positions in a linear ordering. In equality matching, people keep track of the imbalances among them. In market pricing, people orient to ratio values. Cultures use different rules to implement the 4 models. In addition to an array of inductive evidence from many cultures and approaches, the theory has been supported by ethnographic field work and 19 experimental studies using 7 different methods testing 6 different cognitive predictions on a wide range of subjects from 5 cultures.
Article
Full-text available
A role congruity theory of prejudice toward female leaders proposes that perceived incongruity between the female gender role and leadership roles leads to 2 forms of prejudice: (a) perceiving women less favorably than men as potential occupants of leadership roles and (b) evaluating behavior that fulfills the prescriptions of a leader role less favorably when it is enacted by a woman. One consequence is that attitudes are less positive toward female than male leaders and potential leaders. Other consequences are that it is more difficult for women to become leaders and to achieve success in leadership roles. Evidence from varied research paradigms substantiates that these consequences occur, especially in situations that heighten perceptions of incongruity between the female gender role and leadership roles.
Article
Full-text available
Cross-sectional studies of attitude-behavior relationships are vulnerable to the inflation of correlations by common method variance (CMV). Here, a model is presented that allows partial correlation analysis to adjust the observed correlations for CMV contamination and determine if conclusions about the statistical and practical significance of a predictor have been influenced by the presence of CMV. This method also suggests procedures for designing questionnaires to increase the precision of this adjustment.
Article
Full-text available
Stereotype research emphasizes systematic processes over seemingly arbitrary contents, but content also may prove systematic. On the basis of stereotypes' intergroup functions, the stereotype content model hypothesizes that (a) 2 primary dimensions are competence and warmth, (b) frequent mixed clusters combine high warmth with low competence (paternalistic) or high competence with low warmth (envious), and (c) distinct emotions (pity, envy, admiration, contempt) differentiate the 4 competence-warmth combinations. Stereotypically, (d) status predicts high competence, and competition predicts low warmth. Nine varied samples rated gender, ethnicity, race, class, age, and disability out-groups. Contrary to antipathy models, 2 dimensions mattered, and many stereotypes were mixed, either pitying (low competence, high warmth subordinates) or envying (high competence, low warmth competitors). Stereotypically, status predicted competence, and competition predicted low warmth.
Article
Full-text available
Studies that combine moderation and mediation are prevalent in basic and applied psychology research. Typically, these studies are framed in terms of moderated mediation or mediated moderation, both of which involve similar analytical approaches. Unfortunately, these approaches have important shortcomings that conceal the nature of the moderated and the mediated effects under investigation. This article presents a general analytical framework for combining moderation and mediation that integrates moderated regression analysis and path analysis. This framework clarifies how moderator variables influence the paths that constitute the direct, indirect, and total effects of mediated models. The authors empirically illustrate this framework and give step-by-step instructions for estimation and interpretation. They summarize the advantages of their framework over current approaches, explain how it subsumes moderated mediation and mediated moderation, and describe how it can accommodate additional moderator and mediator variables, curvilinear relationships, and structural equation models with latent variables.
Chapter
The purpose of this chapter is to outline the development of the idea of "stakeholder management" as it has come to be applied in strategic management. We begin by developing a brief history of the concept. We then suggest that traditionally the stakeholder approach to strategic management has several related characteristics that serve as distinguishing features. We review recent work on stakeholder theory and suggest how stakeholder management has affected the practice of management. We end by suggesting further research questions.
Article
Research dealing with various aspects of the theory of planned behavior (Ajzen, 1985 and Ajzen, 1987) is reviewed, and some unresolved issues are discussed. In broad terms, the theory is found to be well supported by empirical evidence. Intentions to perform behaviors of different kinds can be predicted with high accuracy from attitudes toward the behavior, subjective norms, and perceived behavioral control; and these intentions, together with perceptions of behavioral control, account for considerable variance in actual behavior. Attitudes, subjective norms, and perceived behavioral control are shown to be related to appropriate sets of salient behavioral, normative, and control beliefs about the behavior, but the exact nature of these relations is still uncertain. Expectancy-value formulations are found to be only partly successful in dealing with these relations. Optimal rescaling of expectancy and value measures is offered as a means of dealing with measurement limitations. Finally, inclusion of past behavior in the prediction equation is shown to provide a means of testing the theory's sufficiency, another issue that remains unresolved. The limited available evidence concerning this question shows that the theory is predicting behavior quite well in comparison to the ceiling imposed by behavioral reliability.