ArticlePDF Available

Why Do People Practice Mindfulness? An Investigation into Reasons for Practicing Mindfulness Meditation

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Mindfulness meditation leads to a range of positive outcomes, yet little is known about the motivation behind choosing to practice meditation. This research investigated reasons for commencing and continuing mindfulness meditation. In both qualitative and quantitative analyses, the most frequently cited reason for commencing and continuing meditation practice was to alleviate emotional distress and enhance emotion regulation. A substantial proportion of participants also reported continuing meditation to enhance well-being, though very few commenced or continued meditation practice for spiritual or religious reasons. In brief, the overwhelming majority of participants in the present study reported practicing mindfulness to alleviate emotional distress. Further research is needed to examine reasons for meditation across more diverse samples, and whether reasons for meditation differentially predict outcome.
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
ORIGINAL PAPER
Why Do People Practice Mindfulness? An Investigation
into Reasons for Practicing Mindfulness Meditation
Christopher A. Pepping
1
&Benjamin Walters
2
&Penelope J. Davis
2
&
Analise ODonovan
2,3
#Springer Science+Business Media New York 2016
Abstract Mindfulness meditation leads to a range of positive
outcomes, yet little is known about the motivation behind
choosing to practice meditation. This research investigated
reasons for commencing and continuing mindfulness medita-
tion. In both qualitative and quantitative analyses, the most
frequently cited reason for commencing and continuing med-
itation practice was to alleviate emotional distress and enhance
emotion regulation. A substantial proportion of participants
also reported continuing meditation to enhance well-being,
though very few commenced or continued meditation practice
for spiritual or religious reasons. In brief, the overwhelming
majority of participants in the present study reported practic-
ing mindfulness to alleviate emotional distress. Further re-
search is needed to examine reasons for meditation across
more diverse samples, and whether reasons for meditation
differentially predict outcome.
Keywords Mindfulness .Meditation .Reasons for
meditation .Meditation practice
Introduction
Mindfulness is commonly defined as the process of Bpaying
attention in a particular way: on purpose, in the present mo-
ment, non-judgmentally^(Kabat-Zinn 1994,p.4).
Mindfulness meditation, therefore, involves the intentional
self-regulation of attention to present moment experience,
coupled with a non-judgmental and accepting stance toward
whatever may arise (Baer 2003; Kabat-Zinn 1990; Kabat-
Zinn 1994). Although there are other forms of meditation,
including concentrative meditation and guided meditation,
here we focus exclusively on mindfulness meditation.
Much evidence indicates that mindfulness meditation leads
to a range of positive outcomes (Keng et al. 2011), yet little is
known about the reasons individuals choose to practice mind-
fulness meditation. Most of the empirical literature has exam-
ined mindfulness meditation in clinical interventions (Keng et
al. 2011), whereas little research has examined mindfulness
for its original goal, namely personal growth and self-
liberation (Shapiro 1994). According to Shapiro (1992,
1994), there are three goals or reasons for meditation: self-
regulation, self-liberation, and self-exploration. Self-
regulation refers to the motivation to reduce stress and pain
and enhance well-being, whereas self-exploration refers to the
use of meditation to increase self-awareness and self-under-
standing. Finally, self-liberation refers to the practice of mind-
fulness meditation for spirituality reasons or personal growth
(Shapiro 1992; Shapiro 1994).
Shapiro (1992) examined goals and expectations for prac-
ticing meditation in a group of 27 meditators who had signed
up for an intensive Vipassana meditation retreat. Ten partici-
pants (37 %) reported self-regulation as their motivation, nine
(33.3 %) identified self-liberation, six (22.2 %) identified self-
exploration as their primary motivation, and two were classi-
fied as identifying otherreasons. Interestingly, reasons for
*Christopher A. Pepping
c.pepping@latrobe.edu.au
1
School of Psychology and Public Health, La Trobe University,
Melbourne, VIC, Australia
2
School of Applied Psychology, Griffith University,
Brisbane, Australia
3
Menzies Health Institute, Queensland, Australia
Mindfulness
DOI 10.1007/s12671-016-0490-3
practicing meditation shifted somewhat from self-regulation,
to self-exploration, to self-liberation as meditation experience
increased, with experienced meditators more likely to identify
self-liberation as their motivation.
Carmody et al. (2009)alsoexaminedreasonsforengaging
in mindfulness meditation practice in a sample of 309 partic-
ipants attending a mindfulness-based stress reduction program
for stress-related difficulties. Based on the research of Shapiro
(1992), participants were asked to rate the importance of two
reasons pertaining to self-regulation, two reasons pertaining to
self-exploration, and two reasons pertaining to self-liberation.
The maximum score for each type of intention was 10. Results
revealed high levels of intention for self-regulation (9.34),
self-exploration (8.35), and self-liberation (8.26).
Furthermore, intentions were largely unrelated to outcome
measures of psychological distress, a finding difficult to inter-
pret easily because of the low variability of the intention
ratings.
In brief, much evidence reveals mindfulness meditation
enhances a wide range of positive outcomes, yet little is
known about the reasons people choose to practice medita-
tion. There is good reason to predict there may be a range of
reasons individuals choose to practice meditation. However,
the only two empirical studies to investigate this have relied
on either very small samples of highly experienced meditators
in intensive retreats (Shapiro 1992) or samples characterized
by stress-related illness (Carmody et al. 2009). It is therefore
important to examine reasons for practicing meditation in larg-
er samples not characterized only by individuals experiencing
stress-related concerns.
The aim of the present research was to identify reasons
people choose to commence mindfulness meditation practice,
and reasons for continuing to practice, in two phases: (a) a
qualitative analysis of open-ended responses pertaining to rea-
sons for commencing and continuing meditation practice, and
(b) a quantitative analysis of reasons for commencing and
continuing meditation practice. The use of qualitative analysis
was appropriate as an initial step. Thematic analysis explores,
identifies, and analyzes themes from qualitative data, indepen-
dent of theory, or preconceived hypotheses (Braun and Clarke
2006). We therefore asked participants open-ended questions
relating to their reasons for engaging in mindfulness medita-
tion, followed by quantitative questions. No specific hypoth-
eses were developed as the goal was to identify themes relat-
ing to reasons for engaging in meditation.
Method
Participants
Participants were 190 adults attending a large urban university
(149 female, 41 male) ranging in age from 17 to 53
(M= 21.34, SD=5.76) who had practiced mindfulness medi-
tation. Of these, 71 participants (57 female, 14 male) ranging
in age from 17 to 41 (M= 21.45, SD = 4.94) reported having a
current mindfulness meditation practice. Most current medita-
tors (n =58) practiced at least weekly, with the remainder prac-
ticing less than once per week. Thirty-three participants had
practiced meditation for less than 1 year, 29 for between 1 and
5 years, and 9 for over 5 years. The remaining 119 (92 female,
27 male) participants, ranging in age from 17 to 53
(M= 21.27, SD = 6.22), reported having practiced mindfulness
meditation previously, but no longer practiced. There were no
significant differences in age or gender breakdown between
those with a current mindfulness practice and those with prior
meditation experience. Participants were undergraduate psy-
chology students participating for 1 h experimental credit.
Procedure
Participants were recruited through an online advertisement
on the university research participation website, where the
inclusion requirement of having experience in mindfulness
meditation was explained. Participants were informed the re-
search was designed to assess reasons for engaging in mind-
fulness meditation.
Measures
Participants completed an online questionnaire. After
providing demographic information, participants
responded to two open-ended questions that were subse-
quently coded for the qualitative analysis. The first
question was asked to all participants (n= 190): BWhy
did you first choose to start practicing mindfulness med-
itation? What were your reasons?^The second question
was asked only to those participants who reported hav-
ing a current mindfulness meditation practice: BWhat are
your reasons for continuing to practice mindfulness
meditation?^
Participants were next asked to respond to a series of ques-
tions about their reasons for meditation. All participants
responded to the following question: BWhy did you start prac-
ticing mindfulness meditation?^and were asked to rate how
important were each of the following reasons: relaxation; re-
duce anxiety; feel calmer; improve interpersonal relationships;
reduce/manage physical pain; regulate my emotions (manage
emotions more effectively); manage difficult thoughts; in-
crease concentration and attention; spiritual or religious rea-
sons; and I wanted to learn more about mindfulness/I was
curious about mindfulness meditation. Initially participants
were asked to respond to these on a 7-point scale, ranging
from not important to very important. We subsequently col-
lapsed responses into three discrete groups: Blittle to no
importance^,Bmoderate importance^,andBmajor
Mindfulness
importance^for ease of interpretation. Scores of 1 reflected no
importance, and scores of 23 reflected little importance. Very
few participants rated potential reasons at these levels, thus we
collapsed these three scores to reflect the category of Blittle to
no importance^.Scoresof45 were coded as Bmoderate
importance^, and scores of 67 were coded as Bmajor
importance.^
Participants with a current practice also responded to
the following question: BWhy do you continue to prac-
tice mindfulness meditation?^by rating the importance
of each of the following reasons: relaxation; reduce anx-
iety; feel calmer; improve interpersonal relationships; re-
duce / manage physical pain; regulate my emotions
(manage emotions more effectively); manage difficult
thoughts; increase concentration and attention; spiritual
or religious reasons; and I continue to learn about my-
self through mindfulness. These were scored using the
same method described above. Importantly, these ques-
tions were asked after the open-ended qualitative ques-
tions so as to not influence responses to the open-ended
questions.
Data Analyses
For the qualitative component, an inductive thematic
analysis was used to code and analyze qualitative re-
sponses (Braun and Clarke 2006;2013;Richardsand
Morse 2007). Responses were coded at multiple themes
when a single response tapped multiple concepts. For
the quantitative component, the frequency with which
participants endorsed particular reasons for commencing
and continuing meditation practicewereexamined,andt
tests were performed to examine differences between
groups based on meditation experience.
Results
Qualitative Analyses
A detailed description of this analysis can be obtained from
the first author. The major findings are presented here.
Analysis of the reasons for commencing mindfulness medita-
tion practice revealed four themes:
(i) Reduction of negative experiences. Most responses
(94.74 %) referred to beginning mindfulness meditation
to cope with or reduce negative experiences, especially
negative emotional experiences involving stress, anxiety,
panic, and depression.
(ii) Well-being. Many respondents (31.05 %) referred to the
use of mindfulness meditation as a tool for enhancing
aspects of their lives, such as increased happiness,
greater self-awareness, improved performance, and
greater alertness and concentration. Importantly, re-
sponses at this theme were associated with the use of
mindfulness to attain a desired, positive state or outcome
rather than to reduce a negative state or outcome.
(iii) Introduction by an external source. Beginning mindful-
ness meditation on the recommendation of another per-
son was given as an important reason by a substantial
minority of participants (28.42 %).
(iv) Religion/spirituality. A small number of participants
(6.32 %) mentioned beginning meditation for spiritual
or religious reasons. All those who named a religion
referred to Buddhism.
Analysis of reasons for continuing mindfulness meditation
practice also revealed four themes:
(i) Reduction of negative experiences. Most respondents
(95.77 %) commented that continuing mindfulness med-
itation practice was helpful in reducing negative experi-
ences and managing such things as anger, anxiety, stress,
and tension headaches.
(ii) Well-being. Many (74.65 %) reported that continued
practice was associated with greater happiness and im-
proved psychological health and life satisfaction.
(iii) Perception of effectiveness. Some (18.31 %) mentioned
that they continued meditating because it was useful or
beneficial, without specifying particular benefits (e.g.,
BI have found it very helpful^).
(iv) Religion/spirituality. A small number (4.23 %) reported
continued practice for religious and spiritual reasons.
Quantitative Results
Ratings of the importance of various reasons for beginning
and continuing mindfulness meditation are presented in
Figs. 1and 2. As can be seen, the four top reasons for both
beginning and continuing mindfulness meditation practice
were to feel calmer, relaxation, to reduce anxiety, and to reg-
ulate emotions more effectively. In contrast, very few partici-
pants reported commencing or continuing mindfulness medi-
tation for spiritual or religious reasons.
Next, we compared current meditators who have practiced
for less than 1 year (n= 33) with current meditators who have
practiced for more than 1 year (n=36) across each of the 10
reasons. A series of independent samples ttests were per-
formed with alpha set at p< 0.005 to account for multiple tests.
Results revealed less experienced meditators were more likely
to have commenced (t(67) = 3.54, p= 0.001) and continued (t
(67) = 3.21, p= 0.002) mindfulness meditation practice to re-
duce physical pain compared their more experienced counter-
parts. No other significant differences emerged.
Mindfulness
Discussion
The results of the present research revealed marked similarity
in the reasons for both beginning and continuing the practice
of mindfulness meditation, a similarity evident in both the
qualitative and quantitative data. The vast majority of partic-
ipants began practicing mindfulness meditation to reduce neg-
ative emotional experiences, to manage their emotions more
effectively, and to feel calmer, and these same factors were
responsible for the continued practice of mindfulness
meditation.
Another major reason for practicing mindfulness medita-
tion was to enhance well-being, including happiness, self-
awareness, alertness, and concentration. Interestingly, sub-
stantially more participants reported well-being as a key rea-
son for continuing mindfulness practice than for commencing
mindfulness practice, perhaps indicating that as individuals
gain greater experience, they become more aware of the
benefits of mindfulness to also increase health and well-being.
This is consistent with Shapiros(1992) research that as med-
itation experience increased, participants moved from engag-
ing in mindfulness for self-regulation purposes towards self-
exploration and self-liberation purposes (e.g., for personal
growth, self-understanding, liberation, or spirituality).
Similarly, in the quantitative analyses, meditators with less
experience (less than 12 months) were more likely to practice
mindfulness to reduce physical pain compared to those with
more meditation experience. This finding is certainly consis-
tent with the idea that as experience increases, meditators
move more towards self-exploration and self-liberation.
However, it is important to note that this was the only signif-
icant difference between these two groups, and future research
is needed to replicate this finding to ensure it is indeed a
reliable pattern.
Very few participants in the present sample reported prac-
ticing mindfulness for its original purpose; namely spiritual or
0
20
40
60
80
100
120
140
160
180
200
Major Importance
Moderate Importance
Little-No Importance
Fig. 1 Reasons for Commencing
Mindfulness Practice (N=190).
Reasons are presented in order of
importance (proportion indicating
major importance or moderate
importance)
0
10
20
30
40
50
60
70
80
Major Importance
Moderate Importance
Little-No Importance
Fig. 2 Reasons for Continuing to
Practice Mindfulness (N=71)
Reasons are presented in order of
importance (proportion indicating
major importance or moderate
importance)
Mindfulness
religious reasons. Given that this research was conducted in a
large university in a western country, perhaps the specific
population may influence results. Most of the Western litera-
ture on mindfulness has focused on the use of mindfulness in
the context of therapy, rather than for spiritual purposes.
Perhaps different reasons for practicing mindfulness medita-
tion would emerge in other cultures, and in samples with
greater mindfulness meditation experience.
As previously mentioned, there was marked similarity in
the findings from the qualitative and quantitative data.
Interestingly, however, in the quantitative analysis many re-
ported they continue to practice mindfulness because they
continue to learn about themselves, and because it has bene-
ficial effects on relationships. These were not identified as
themes within the qualitative analysis, which may indicate
that these two reasons were not particularly salient when
asked to specify reasons in general. Nonetheless, when asked
explicitly about these reasons, more than half of participants
endorsed learning about themselves and improved relation-
ships as reasons for continuing.
Because the aim of the qualitative component of was to
assess and analyze themes, consistent with thematic analysis
(Braun and Clarke 2013), we did not use Shapiros(1992)
work to inform our coding of responses. However, our results
are remarkably similar, and the key themes emerging from the
qualitative and quantitative analyses support Shapirosstruc-
ture of intentions for meditation practice described earlier.
Most engaged in mindfulness for self-regulation.
Specifically, themes relating to reducing negative experiences,
and enhancing well-being, are consistent with the self-
regulation intention defined by Shapiro (1992,1994). The
themes pertaining to increased self-awareness, clarity, and
learning about oneself, are consistent with Shapirosself-
exploration intention. Fewer participants engaged in mindful-
ness meditation for these reasons. Finally, the theme relating
to spiritual or religious reasons clearly corresponds to
Shapiros self-liberation intention. However, in the present
research, very few reported engaging in mindfulness for self-
liberation reasons. The present sample was a sample of young
adults likely to be relatively new to the practice of mindful-
ness. It seems possible that as they continue to practice mind-
fulness, their reasons for doing so may shift toward self-liber-
ation. However, this remains to be investigated empirically.
Limitations and Future Directions
There are several limitations to acknowledge. First, partici-
pants were asked to retrospectively recall reasons for com-
mencing meditation. The retrospective reports of reasons for
commencing mindfulness and current reports of reasons for
continuing mindfulness were generally very similar, which
may reflect the influence of current perceptions. However,
there were also some clear distinctions which are suggestive
of validity of responses. The only way in which reasons for
commencing mindfulness practice can be definitively deter-
mined is to assess these reasons prior to the commencement of
mindfulness practice, and future research should address this
question.
Reasons for meditation specified in the quantitative
component were not exhaustive, and some may choose
to engage in meditation for other reasons. However, the
inclusion of open-ended qualitative questions meant that
any additional information was captured in the qualita-
tive component. We asked the open-ended qualitative
questions prior to presenting the list of ten reasons for
engaging in meditation to prevent these researcher-
derived reasons from influencing open-ended responses.
Nonetheless, the possibility remains that participant re-
sponses to such open-ended questions may influence
responding to the list of questions as participants may
be motivated to remain consistent. Further, participants
were not asked whether their reasons for practicing
mindfulness meditation referred to formal mindfulness
retreats, accessing mindfulness teachers, or to private
meditation. Future research should consider reasons for
practicing meditation across a range of contexts.
The sample was a fairly homogenous group of young
adults in a Western country, and results cannot be assumed
to generalize to other groups of meditators. Similarly, females
were overrepresented, and results should be interpreted with
this in mind. Future research is needed to examine reasons for
meditation across broader, more representative samples.
Nonetheless, the present research provides important informa-
tion pertaining to reasons for meditation in meditators in a
Wester n co un try.
Future research is needed to examine the trajectory of en-
gagement in mindfulness meditation longitudinally to exam-
ine intentions more accurately, and to assess potential change
in intentions as meditation experience increases over time.
Perhaps the most critical question that remains to be ad-
dressed, however, is whether reasons for practicing mindful-
ness meditation influence the outcome of such practices. It
seems possible that mindfulness meditators who commence
meditation practice for self-regulation reasons might benefit
differently than those practicing for self-regulation or self-
liberation reasons. Shapiro and colleagues (2006) argue that
intentions are critically important as they remind an individual
of their reasons for practicing meditation, though to date there
is limited evidence to support this proposition (Shapiro 1992).
If reasons for practicing meditation do indeed influence out-
come, this would suggest that assessing reasons for meditation
at the beginning of mindfulness-based interventions may be
important. In brief, further research is needed to examine rea-
sons for mindfulness meditation longitudinally across repre-
sentative samples, and whether the intention behind practicing
meditation influences outcome.
Mindfulness
References
Baer, R. A. (2003). Mindfulness training as a clinical intervention: a
conceptual and empirical review. Clin Psychol Sci Pract, 10(2),
12543. doi:10.1093/clipsy.bpg015.
Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology.
Qual Res Psychol, 3,77101. doi:10.1191/1478088706qp063oa.
Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2013). Successful Qualitative Research: A
Practical Guide for Beginners. London: Sage.
Carmody, J., Baer, R. A., Lykins E., & Olendzki, N. (2009).An empirical
study of the mechanisms of mindfulness in a mindfulnessbased
stress reduction program. J Clin Psychol, 65,61326. doi:10.
1002/jclp.20579.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living: Using the wisdom of your
body and mind to face stress, pain and illness. New York: Delacorte.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1994). Wherever you go, there you are: Mindfulness
meditation in everyday life. New York: Hyperion.
Keng, S.-L., Smoski, M. J., & Robins, C. J. (2011). Effects of mindful-
ness on psychological health: a review of empirical studies. Clin
Psychol Rev, 31,104156. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2011.04.006.
Richards, L., & Morse, J. (2007). Readme First for a UsersGuideto
Qualitative Methods (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks Calif.: Sage
Publications.
Shapiro, D. H. (1992). A preliminary study of long-term meditators:
goals, effects, religious orientation, cognitions. J Transpers
Psychol, 24,2339.
Shapiro, D. H. (1994). Examining the content and context of meditation:
a challenge for psychology in the areas of stress management, psy-
chotherapy, and religion/values. J Humanist Psychol, 34,10135.
doi:10.1177/00221678940344008.
Mindfulness
... Through stress management, emotion regulation, cognitive function enhancement, and physical and mental health improvement, mindfulness meditation provides an effective way for people to self-regulate and improve their quality of life. However, scientific research is still in the process of deeper exploration, and more findings on positive thinking meditation will reveal to us its wider application areas and potentials [3][4][5][6]. ...
... This experiment needs to design the light pole plate by itself, and the structure of its design is shown in Fig. 1, which includes 8 light sources (red numbers 1-8, wavelengths of 720 nm and 870 nm, respectively), which are indicated by the 8 transmitting probes. 16 receiving probes (blue numbers [1][2][3][4][5][6][7][8][9][10][11][12][13][14][15][16]. Maintaining a fixed distance of 5 cm between the individual probes, this setup will result in the formation of detection channels between each transmitting probe and each receiving probe, forming a total of 28 channels (green straight lines). ...
Article
Full-text available
Positive thinking meditation, as an effective method that can reduce stress and enhance the sense of well-being in life, is becoming a hot research topic in related fields in various countries. In this paper, bio-signal data, such as EEG, of subjects were collected through meditation experiments, and combined with fNIRS signals to form a bio-signal dataset for research. The collected data is processed using independent component analysis to reduce the interference of noise in the data for the study. The stages of the subjects’ positive thinking meditation were classified and identified by deep learning models and long and short-term memory neural networks. To address the problem of misidentification caused by uneven data distribution, this paper uses data enhancement methods to expand the diversity of data and improve the classification accuracy of the model. To address the shortcomings of deep learning models and data enhancement methods in data labelling, this paper integrates the transfer learning method with the MobileNetV2 model, the RestNet50 network model and the Xception model through weighted averaging to form a network integration model for classifying the phases of mindfulness meditation. The model achieves up to 99.4% accuracy on the subject’s biosignal dataset, an improvement of 8.4% compared to the data-enhanced deep learning model. The deep learning machine optimization model can effectively classify the stages of positive thinking meditation, which can be a highly valuable reference for the rational arrangement of positive thinking meditation training strategies.
... No quantitative measures exist for feasibility. Some studies have examined why individuals initiate and continue mindfulness exercises (Pepping et al., 2016) and their perceived benefits and doubts (Sears et al., 2011). Developing scales that measure the feasibility of mindfulness will enable us to know the extent of feasibility of mindfulness practices quantitatively and compare feasibility among the methods and program, and contribute to selecting the appropriate approach to practice for individuals. ...
... Based on the above procedures and subsequent examination of the wording, the first and third authors developed 20 preliminary items for the MDRS (Table 1) and 29 for the MERS (Table 2). Items created were consistent with perceived benefits and doubts of mindfulness (Sears et al., 2011), reasons for initiating or continuing mindfulness training (Pepping et al., 2016), and daily experiences during meditation in a sample of novice practitioners (Osin & Turilina, 2022), which would involve adequate content validity. ...
Article
Full-text available
We explored body‐scan postures suitable for people with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) tendency by developing and validating the Mindfulness Encouraging/Discouraging Reactions Scales (MERS/MDRS), using university students. In Study 1, we conducted a survey to collect typical positive and negative reactions during mindfulness exercises from 21 participants and created the preliminary items. In Study 2, 192 participants completed existing state/trait mindfulness scales and the preliminary MERS and MDRS after mindful breathing. Based on an item response model, we developed and validated MERS and MDRS. In Study 3, 19 participants were categorized into one of four groups: (a) combined, (b) hyperactive/impulsive, (c) inattentive, and (d) without ADHD tendencies. They performed body‐scan meditations with each of the counterbalanced postures (upright, slumped, leaning‐back sitting, and supine), and completed the questionnaires. The analysis showed that those with hyperactivity/impulsivity tendency found the body‐scan meditation a challenge with the slumped posture and easier to perform in the supine posture; the upright posture provided high and low MERS to the hyperactivity/impulsivity tendency group and combined group, respectively; and sleepiness correlated with MERS in the supine posture ( r = .49) and the upright posture ( r = .51). We identified helpful body scan postures for people with ADHD tendency, using MERS and MDRS, but it was noted that these scales were created solely based on intuitive impressions for beginners, and it is not recommended that the items included in them be aimed for or avoided.
... Today, people's motivations for meditating are similar. The most often cited motivations for having tried meditation are to reduce stress and anxiety, alleviate depression, manage emotions, increase resilience, and enhance overall happiness and life satisfaction, among other health-related reasons (Anderson et al., 2019;Burke et al., 2017;Cramer et al., 2016;Lam et al., 2023aLam et al., , 2023bMacinko & Upchurch, 2019;Pepping et al., 2016). Additionally, numerous studies have directly demonstrated the health benefits of meditation in both clinical and non-clinical settings (Galante et al., 2021;Goldberg et al., 2018;Luberto et al., 2018). ...
Article
Full-text available
Objectives The objective of this research was to map the Sussex Mindfulness Meditation (SuMMed) model to US adults, thereby demonstrating its utility for future researchers. Additionally, the research explored engagement with meditation beyond what the model explicitly prescribes, including with apps. This served as a means to offer potential refinements to the model and deepen our understanding of meditation habit formation. Method This research used a representative survey of 2000 adults in the USA. The sample was rigorously recruited using the Ipsos KnowledgePanel. Results In mapping the SuMMed model, 69.7%, 19.1%, 2.3%, and 9.0% of US adults were in the Pre-Intention, Preparation, Action, and Maintenance Stages of meditation habit formation, respectively. In the Preparation stage, 34.5% planned to meditate within the next 7 days, while only 21.9% intended to start a regular meditation practice within the next month. Additionally, people with less mature meditation practices were more likely to intend to use meditation apps, compared to established meditators. Conclusions The SuMMed model offers a useful way to understand meditation among US adults and future researchers should leverage it confidently. However, people often tried meditation out of curiosity and appeared to distinguish between trying and experimenting with meditation, versus having a practice. Given this, we recommend a modification to the Preparation stage of the SuMMed model to integrate meditation trial and experimentation. Additionally, meditation apps may be more useful to support early meditation practices, compared to established practices. Preregistration This study was not preregistered.
... Given the benefits of meditation on mental wellness, many engage in meditation to alleviate feelings of anxiety. In 2016, a study found that 94.74% of undergraduate psychology students reported reduction of negative emotional experiences (such as stress, anxiety, panic, and depression) as their reason for beginning meditation, with 95.77% also continuing to meditate for the same reason [44]. Another study found that 37% of 27 young adults reported that their primary reasons for signing up to participate in an intensive Vipassana meditation retreat was to improve self-regulation (which was conceptualized as goals of "becoming more relaxed" and "learning to stop negative thoughts" in the study) [45]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Public health interventions implemented during the COVID-19 pandemic may exacerbate anxiety symptoms for many. We conducted this study to better understand the role of leisure activity in promoting mental wellness during times of social isolation and reduced access to recreation facilities and mental health support services. We analyzed nationally representative survey data collected by Statistics Canada as part of the Canadian Perspectives Survey Series (CPSS) during May 4–10 (CPSS 2) and July 20 to 26, 2020 (CPSS 4). Data related to leisure activity and anxiety symptoms as measured by a score of more than 10 on the General Anxiety Disorder scale were examined using descriptive and log-binomial regression analyses. Survey sampling weights were applied in all analyses, and regression results were adjusted for sociodemographic characteristics. Exercise and communication with friends and loved ones were the most frequently reported leisure activity. Prevalence of moderate to severe anxiety symptoms reported by participants was lower in CPSS 4 compared to CPSS 2. Results of adjusted log-binomial regression analyses revealed lower prevalence of moderate to severe anxiety symptoms in those who engaged in exercise and communication, while those who meditated exhibited higher prevalence. In conclusion, leisure activities, such as exercise and communication with loved ones, can promote mental wellness. Future research should clarify the role of meditation for mental wellness promotion during periods of social isolation.
Article
This study explores the introductory experiences and expectations of mobile-based meditation (MbM) among university students, a non-clinical population, to address gaps in mindfulness research regarding individual needs. Using interpretative phenomenological analysis, semi-structured online interviews were conducted with six students. The analysis identified three interconnected themes in initial MbM practice: calm (induced by clarity, escape, portability, autonomy, and reduced distractions); focus (affected by setting, novelty and structure of guided meditations, disentanglement of thoughts, and a ‘wakeful sleep’ experience); and self-awareness (focused on bodily sensations, memories, abilities, emotions, and life’s pleasures). While self-awareness was generally positive, perceived limitations in focus caused demotivation and discontinuation of MbM for some participants. Initial mindfulness experiences often heightened expectations regarding calmness, adherence, and continued use. The study acknowledges potential positivity bias and challenges in capturing the nuanced temporal dynamics of introductory MbM experiences. These findings underscore the importance of clear communication from MbM app developers and healthcare providers about the purpose and potential emotional variability of mindfulness practice, including the integration of focus-related metrics, before individuals begin using MbM. This research contributes novel insights into the psychological variability of MbM experiences, both between and within individuals, and offers practical implications for the design and implementation of MbM interventions.
Article
Full-text available
Bu çalışma, bireylerin özsaygı düzeyinin vergi uyumu davranışları üzerindeki etkisini incelemektedir. Araştırmada, gönüllü ve zorunlu vergi uyumu ile vergiden kaçınma ve vergi kaçırma davranışları, özsaygı perspektifinden değerlendirilmiştir. Nicel bir yöntem benimsenmiş ve 428 katılımcıdan elde edilen veriler analiz edilmiştir. Korelasyon ve regresyon analiz sonuçları, özsaygının gönüllü vergi uyumu üzerinde anlamlı bir etkisinin olduğunu, ancak diğer boyutlarla olan ilişkilerinin istatistiksel olarak anlamlı olmadığını göstermiştir. Bulgular, özsaygının bireylerin gönüllülük temelli vergi uyumu davranışlarında önemli bir rol oynadığını ortaya koymaktadır. Abstract This study examines the impact of individuals' self-esteem levels on tax compliance behaviors. The research evaluates voluntary and enforced tax compliance, as well as tax avoidance and tax evasion behaviors, from a self-esteem perspective. A quantitative method was adopted, and data collected from 428 participants were analyzed. The results of correlation and regression analyses revealed that self-esteem significantly affects voluntary tax compliance but has no statistically significant relationship with other dimensions. The findings highlight the critical role of self-esteem in shaping individuals' voluntary tax compliance behaviors.
Preprint
Mindfulness meditation is a form of mental training rooted in ancient wisdom traditions and is focused on cultivating a non-judgmental stance toward present-moment awareness. Here, we synthesize cognitive-behavioral effects in long-term meditators (LTMs) resulting from diverse and prolonged meditation practices. Preliminary evidence suggests that LTMs exhibit increased cognitive-sensory integration and decoupling of affective processes, as demonstrated in enhanced interoceptive awareness, reduced negative affective pain perception, and more rational decision-making. Additionally, LTMs may experience more emotional neutrality, self-boundary dissolution, and less normative self-awareness. Neuroimaging findings include increased bottom-up activation, particularly within the salience network (interoception, pain, affect), and reduced connectivity between the executive (dorsolateral prefrontal cortex) and salience (dorsal anterior cingulate cortex) networks (reduced pain). Research also displayed reduced amygdala activation to fear (reduced negative affect), increased temporoparietal junction activation (pre-reflective experiential processes, empathy), and altered midline default-mode network activation, which is associated with emotional neutrality and pre-reflective experiential processes, such as non-ordinary states of consciousness. Methodological limitations, specifically heterogeneous predictor variables, restrict the interpretation of trait effects, temporal dynamics in cognitive processing, and the unique influences of meditative activities. These limitations indicate the need for a unified research framework and a systematic neurophenomenological investigation of advanced meditation—through the study of unfolding states, stages, and endpoints in meditative development. In summary, LTMs display a distinct neurophenomenological gestalt of mindfulness, wherein meditative expertise is reflected in altered general brain processing, potentially enhanced cognitive integration, increased cognitive flexibility and self-regulation, and heightened non-dual awareness—signifying a potentially important form of embodied cognition.
Article
The main aim of this study was to investigate adolescents' eustress–distress and mindfulness by examining mental toughness (MT) using network and mediator analyses. The study included 414 adolescents. The results showed that MT was positively related to eustress and mindfulness, but distress was negatively related to MT. Based on the network analysis findings, we tested mindfulness' mediating role in the relationship between eustress–distress and MT. The findings showed that mindfulness played a significant mediating role. However, the mediating role of mindfulness was negative for the relationship between distress and toughness and positive for eustress. These findings advance eustress, distress, and mindfulness as mechanisms for understanding the effects of MT. Considering current knowledge of MT, eustress, distress, and mindfulness, the results are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Thematic analysis is a poorly demarcated, rarely acknowledged, yet widely used qualitative analytic method within psychology. In this paper, we argue that it offers an accessible and theoretically flexible approach to analysing qualitative data. We outline what thematic analysis is, locating it in relation to other qualitative analytic methods that search for themes or patterns, and in relation to different epistemological and ontological positions. We then provide clear guidelines to those wanting to start thematic analysis, or conduct it in a more deliberate and rigorous way, and consider potential pitfalls in conducting thematic analysis. Finally, we outline the disadvantages and advantages of thematic analysis. We conclude by advocating thematic analysis as a useful and flexible method for qualitative research in and beyond psychology.
Article
Full-text available
Interventions based on training in mindfulness skills are becoming increasingly popular. Mindfulness involves intentionally bringing one's attention to the internal and external experiences occurring in the present moment, and is often taught through a variety of meditation exercises. This review summarizes conceptual approaches to mind-fulness and empirical research on the utility of mindfulness-based interventions. Meta-analytic techniques were incorporated to facilitate quantification of findings and comparison across studies. Although the current empirical literature includes many methodological flaws, findings suggest that mindfulness-based interventions may be helpful in the treatment of several disorders. Methodologically sound investigations are recommended in order to clarify the utility of these interventions.
Article
Studies have primarily examined meditation's effects as a self regulation strategy for stress management. Fewer studies have examined its utility as a self exploration strategy for enhancing psychological health in psychotherapy and behavior change. And, few studies have examined meditation's effect regarding its original religious purpose as a self liberation strategy to enhance spiritual growth and wisdom, and cultivate compassionate service. This article examines the reasons underlying this differential proportion of studies on each of the above variables and details the merits and limitations of research that attempted to remove the religious and philosophical context of meditation in order to focus on its content. The article then examines why it has been necessary to reintroduce the context of meditation as a variable, whether that context be stress management, psychotherapy, or a religious perspective. Finally, based on the mentalist and cognitive revolution, this article asks: "Is God always a confounding variable in meditation research?" If the only research tool a person has is a hammer, then all questions begin to look like the head of an undriven nail. -Abraham Maslow When a pickpocket meets a saint, all he sees are pockets. Anonymous
Article
Tested 5 hypotheses related to the goals, religious orientation, and cognitions of long-term meditation with 27 students (mean age 35.6 yrs) of meditation who had signed up for a 2-wk or 3-mo meditation retreat. Ss completed the Motivation, Expectation, Adherence instrument (D. H. Shapiro, 1980) at baseline and 1 and 6 mo after the retreat. Results show that reasons for continuing meditation shifted overall along a continuum of self-regulation, self-exploration, and self-liberation as a function of time; that effects equaled or exceeded goals for the majority of Ss; and that the effects meditators received from the practice were significantly related to what they wanted. Length of practice was associated with religious orientation and with the nature of cognitions when the S did not practice. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Within the past few decades, there has been a surge of interest in the investigation of mindfulness as a psychological construct and as a form of clinical intervention. This article reviews the empirical literature on the effects of mindfulness on psychological health. We begin with a discussion of the construct of mindfulness, differences between Buddhist and Western psychological conceptualizations of mindfulness, and how mindfulness has been integrated into Western medicine and psychology, before reviewing three areas of empirical research: cross-sectional, correlational research on the associations between mindfulness and various indicators of psychological health; intervention research on the effects of mindfulness-oriented interventions on psychological health; and laboratory-based, experimental research on the immediate effects of mindfulness inductions on emotional and behavioral functioning. We conclude that mindfulness brings about various positive psychological effects, including increased subjective well-being, reduced psychological symptoms and emotional reactivity, and improved behavioral regulation. The review ends with a discussion on mechanisms of change of mindfulness interventions and suggested directions for future research.
Article
S. L. Shapiro and colleagues (2006) have described a testable theory of the mechanisms of mindfulness and how it affects positive change. They describe a model in which mindfulness training leads to a fundamental change in relationship to experience (reperceiving), which leads to changes in self-regulation, values clarification, cognitive and behavioral flexibility, and exposure. These four variables, in turn, result in salutogenic outcomes. Analyses of responses from participants in a mindfulness-based stress-reduction program did not support the mediating effect of changes in reperceiving on the relationship of mindfulness with those four variables. However, when mindfulness and reperceiving scores were combined, partial support was found for the mediating effect of the four variables on measures of psychological distress. Issues arising in attempts to test the proposed theory are discussed, including the description of the model variables and the challenges to their assessment.
Readme First for a User’s Guide to Qualitative Methods Thousand Oaks Calif
  • L Richards
  • J Morse
Wherever you go, there you are: Mindfulness meditation in everyday life
  • J Kabat-Zinn
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1994). Wherever you go, there you are: Mindfulness meditation in everyday life. New York: Hyperion.