Content uploaded by Stephen P. Becker
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Stephen P. Becker on Jan 12, 2019
Content may be subject to copyright.
CHAPTER15
Sluggish cognitivetempo
Stephen P. Becker and Russell A. Barkley
Introduction
ere has been longstanding interest in how best to categor-
ize the heterogeneity of children with ADHD (1, 2). In an eort
to understand the variability of inattention specically, there has
recently been a resurgence of interest in sluggish cognitive tempo
(SCT) (3– 6), a set of behavioural symptoms characterized by
daydreaming, mental fogginess/ confusion, and slowed behaviour/
thinking. is chapter provides a brief history of the SCT construct
and summarizes the rapidly accumulating research examining SCT
as distinct from ADHD and other psychopathologies and the extent
to which SCT is uniquely related to functioning in various domains
of major life activities. We conclude by commenting on issues of
terminology and oering key directions for future research.
History ofthe sluggish cognitive
tempo construct
e rst description of attentional problems characterized by
underarousal and daydreaming is believed to have appeared in
a textbook by Sir Alexander Crichton in 1798 (7). As Barkley
(5)observed, Crichton’s description of individuals with ‘low power’,
who are generally unsocial and insensible to external objects,
may align with current descriptions of SCT. Nevertheless, the
scientic study of SCT began in the 1980s when the third edition
of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM)
(8)created two subtypes of attention decit disorder (ADD):one
with hyperactivity (ADD+H) and one without hyperactivity
(ADD– H). In turn, researchers began to investigate dierences
between children with ADD+H or ADD– H. A thorough review
of this research is beyond the scope of this chapter (see Barkley
[5] and Becker etal. [4] for more thorough reviews). It was during
this time that researchers rst began to evaluate the extent to which
a set of symptoms characterized by sluggishness, apathy, lethargy,
and being in a world of one’s own were part of, or distinct from,
inattention as dened in the DSM (9– 11). However, when SCT
symptoms showed poor predictive power for identifying ADHD in
the DSM- IV eld trials (12), the study of SCT largely stalled. at is
until an inuential review by Milich etal. (2)appeared as well as an
empirical article by McBurnett and colleagues (13), both published
in 2001. ese papers emphasized the importance of further study
of the SCT construct. In turn, a slow but steady increase in the
number of SCT studies began to enter the published literature
(4). Specically, since these two seminal papers were published in
2001, over 50 papers related to SCT have been published, most in
just the past 5years (14). As the ndings from these studies have
accumulated (and are reviewed next), the study of SCT has largely
shied away from eorts to identify dierences between subtypes
of ADHD to examine SCT in its own right, independent from and
in contrast toADHD.
Sluggish cognitive tempo is distinct
fromADHD
It is now clear that SCT symptoms are empirically distinct from
symptoms of ADHD. Becker etal. (14) recently conducted a meta-
analysis that included factor analytical studies with over 19,000 chil-
dren and adults, and found strong support for 13 SCT symptoms
that loaded consistently on an SCT factor as opposed to an ADHD
factor. ese 13 symptoms are listed in Box 15.1. Although add-
itional measurement work needs to be conducted now that these
13 items have been identied, it is important to note that parent,
teacher, adult self- report, and child self- report ratings scales (15–
20), as well as a semistructured clinical interview (21), have been
developed that include at least most of these 13 items. Studies using
these measures demonstrate that SCT can be reliability assessed,
with excellent internal consistency and test– retest reliability, and
moderate interrater reliability (14). Importantly, the internal
validity of SCT has been identied across a range of sample types
(clinical, community, epidemiological), age ranges (spanning ages
3– 96 years), and continents (North America, South America,
Europe, and Asia)(14).
Additional evidence for the distinctiveness of SCT from
ADHD comes from Barkley’s two nationally representative
studies examining SCT in children (16) and adults (15). As in
other studies, Barkley found evidence for an SCT factor that did
not load with ADHD. Further, Barkley classied participants as
meeting research criteria for SCT and/ or ADHD and found only
moderate overlap between the SCT and ADHD classications. In
both studies, approximately half of the participants with ADHD
were not classied with SCT, and vice versa. ese ndings led
Barkley (15) to conclude that the relation between SCT and ADHD
‘is similar to comorbidity between two disorders, such as between
anxiety and depression, than one of subtyping within a single dis-
order or in which subgroups share the same disorder of attention’
(p. 987). In line with this conclusion, two recent studies using
bifactor modelling found SCT to fall outside of a general ADHD or
disruptive behaviour factor (22, 23). SCT and ADHD also appear to
have dierent developmental trajectories, with ADHD hyperactive-
impulsive symptoms declining, ADHD inattentive symptoms
remaining relatively stable, and SCT symptoms showing a slight
increase across childhood and adolescence(24).
SECTION4 148
Little is known regarding the aetiology of SCT, but again, there
is emerging evidence for the separateness of SCT from ADHD. e
only published twin study to date found SCT to be substantially
heritable, although SCT symptoms were less heritable than ADHD
and more strongly inuenced by shared and non- shared environ-
mental factors (25). e one neuroimaging study of SCT found an
association between SCT symptom severity and hypoactivity in the
le superior parietal lobe during a cued anker task, suggestive of
decits in reorientating or shiing of attention (26) that is dierent
from the attention networks implicated in ADHD (27). Although
these ndings are intriguing, much more research examining the
aetiology of SCT is clearly needed. Nevertheless, essentially all
available research points to SCT as a set of attentional symptoms
distinct from those captured byADHD.
Sluggish cognitive tempo inrelation
tofunctioning and impairment
In comparison to the number of studies examining the structure of
SCT, especially as related to ADHD, far fewer studies have examined
SCT in relation to functioning and impairment. Nevertheless,
a growing body of research indicates that SCT is associated with
poorer adjustment across a range of domains of major life activities,
with many associations remaining even aer controlling for ADHD
(for additional reviews, see [5,14]).
Mental health functioning
Multiple studies have documented a signicant association
between SCT and internalizing symptoms such as anxiety and
depression (20, 28– 30), although it should be noted that SCT is dis-
tinct from both anxiety and depression (18, 31, 32). In considering
specic internalizing comorbidities, SCT seems to be more strongly
related to depression than to anxiety (16, 31, 33, 34). However,
this may be more true in children than in adults (14) and could
therefore simply reect the increased rates of depression as chil-
dren transition to adolescence and adulthood. In any event, SCT
is clearly more strongly related to internalizing problems than
with externalizing problems such as hyperactivity- impulsivity and
oppositionality (14). In fact, although SCT and ADHD inattention
are themselves strongly correlated, these two attention domains
evince dierential associations with hyperactive- impulsive and
externalizing symptoms when controlling for each other. When
controlling for ADHD inattention, SCT is unassociated with, or
is negatively associated with, externalizing behaviours, whereas
ADHD inattention remains strongly positively associated with
externalizing behaviours (18– 20, 31, 32). Crucially, recent
longitudinal studies have found the same pattern whereby SCT
predicted greater internalizing behaviours and fewer hyperactive-
impulsive and oppositional behaviours up to 2years later (35– 37).
Further, children with ADHD who also have high levels of SCT
show lower rates of externalizing behaviours such as aggression
than other children with ADHD (38,39).
ese ndings provide additional compelling evidence for the
separation of SCT and ADHD. It would be even more so if add-
itional research supports the hypothesis that SCT falls under the
internalizing umbrella of psychopathology whereas ADHD falls
under the externalizing domain (40). As Barkley has noted (41), it
remains important to identify functioning domains on which SCT
may have a negative impact, as well as domains that are unlikely to
be impacted by SCT or for which SCT may actually play a buering
role. Future studies should therefore continue to examine SCT
in relation to internalizing- salient domains while also testing the
hypothesis that SCT would be unrelated to externalizing- salient
domains characterized by impulsivity and risk- taking, including
substance use, delinquency and antisocial behaviours, speedy and
reckless driving, and accidental injuries.
Academic functioning
Somewhat discrepant ndings have been reported in studies
examining SCT in relation to academic functioning. Several studies
did not nd the presence of SCT to predict academic impairment
or negatively impact academic achievement aer accounting
for ADHD (29, 38, 39, 42, 43). is may be due to these studies
examining the relation between SCT and academics in samples of
children diagnosed with ADHD, which may confound ndings
specic to SCT. It could also result from using very short measures
of SCT comprised of just a few items. In line with this possibility,
the most recent studies that have used longer, validated measures
of SCT have repeatedly found SCT symptoms to be uniquely
associated with poorer academic functioning, even in ADHD
samples (30, 33, 44) or aer controlling for ADHD symptom
severity (17, 18, 32, 34, 45– 47). In particular, it appears that SCT
is associated with problems in organization and homework com-
pletion (16, 39, 44). In terms of academic achievement, SCT may
be uniquely associated with lower achievement in mathematics,
word reading, and written language (28, 32). Nevertheless, there
is some indication even from these recent studies that ADHD is
a stronger contributor than SCT to academic problems (16, 18,
19, 48), although more research needs to be done using real world
measures of academic functioning and success. Of note, the three
studies that have examined overall grade point average (GPA) each
found SCT to signicantly predict lower GPA aer accounting for
ADHD symptoms (30, 32, 44). Indeed, two of these studies found
ADHD symptoms to no longer remain associated with GPA when
SCT was added to the model (30, 44). In sum, additional research
is needed to determine the precise relation between SCT and
Box 15.1 Sluggish cognitive tempoitems
1. Apathetic/ unmotivated
2. Daydreams
3. Easily confused
4. Inafog
5. Loses train of thought/ cognitiveset
6. Lost in thoughts
7. Sluggish
8. Sleepy/ drowsy
9. Slow thinking/ processing
10. Spacey
11. Stares blankly
12. Tired/ lethargic
13. Underactive/ slowmoving
Note: ese 13 items were identied in a meta- analysis as having a mean
factor loading >.70 on a sluggish cognitive tempo factor; see Becker etal.(14).
Data from Becker SP, Leopold DR, Burns GL, Jarrett MA, Langberg JM,
Marshall SA, et al. e internal, external, and diagnostic validity of sluggish
cognitive tempo: A meta-analysis and critical review. J Am Acad Child
Adolesc Psychiatry. 2015. Advance online publication. DOI: http://dx.doi.
org/10.1016/j.jaac.2015.12.006
CHAPTER15 149
academic functioning, as well as the mechanisms and develop-
mental progression of this association.
Socioemotional functioning
Studies have repeatedly found a link between SCT and problems/
impairment in social functioning (17, 18, 29, 31, 45, 47). One recent
study found SCT symptoms to predict increased peer problems
across the duration of a school year even aer controlling for other
psychopathology symptoms and initial levels of peer functioning
(49). In terms of specic domains of social functioning, both
Bauermeister etal. (28) and McBurnett etal. (19) found SCT to
predict poorer social skills in children even aer controlling for
ADHD severity, though only when teacher ratings were used and
not parent ratings. is is perhaps because teachers have ample op-
portunity to observe children’s interactions with peers and the spe-
cic types of peer diculties related to SCT. In a novel computer
chat room experiment, SCT symptoms were found to be associated
with a poorer perception of subtle social cues and less memory for
the chat room conversation (50). Further, whereas children with
ADHD are oen disliked or actively rejected by peers owing to nox-
ious and annoying behaviours (51), children with SCT are likely to
be socially withdrawn and isolated (32, 38, 39, 52). Whether or not
this is primarily due to shyness or social disinterest has not yet been
specically examined. It seems likely due to shyness as opposed to
lack of interest, since SCT is also associated with parent reports
of children’s fear/ shyness as part of the ght– ight– freeze motiv-
ational system (40) and children’s own ratings of loneliness (17).
Additional studies supporting this conclusion would have clear
implications for intervention (e.g. social skills training).
Since SCT is associated with both social problems and depres-
sive symptoms, it is not surprising that it has also been linked to
problems in emotion regulation, although ndings are not fully
consistent across studies. In his nationally representative sample
of youth, Barkley (16) found SCT to be signicantly associated
with children’s self- regulation of emotion decits, but SCT was
a much smaller contributor than either ADHD inattention or
hyperactivity- impulsivity to these decits. Conversely, SCT was the
strongest predictor of self- regulation of emotion decits in Barkley’s
representative sample of adults (15). Does this mean the association
between SCT and emotion regulation diculties grows stronger
across development? is is unknown, and no longitudinal studies
examining the association between SCT and emotion regulation
exist. However, there is some evidence that SCT is in fact associated
with poorer emotion regulation in youth. Specically, Becker etal.
(17) found both child- and teacher- rated SCT symptoms to predict
poorer child- rated emotion regulation coping (ability to cope with
and control emotional experiences appropriately) and lower self-
esteem even aer controlling for ADHD, anxiety, and depressive
symptoms. Likewise, Araujo Jiménez etal. (53) found SCT to be
signicantly associated with poorer parent- rated emotional con-
trol in a sample of children and adolescents with ADHD, even
aer controlling for both ADHD inattention and hyperactivity-
impulsivity. ese ndings converge with other studies of college
students that consistently document a link between SCT and
emotion dysregulation (54– 56). Interestingly, Willcutt etal. (32)
hypothesized that regulation diculties may account for the asso-
ciation between SCT and social isolation, whereby ‘individuals
with SCT may become overwhelmed by the rapid ow of complex
information that must be processed continuously to successfully
navigate social interactions, which may then lead to avoidance of
social situations and subsequent isolation’ (pp.32– 3). In line with
this possibility, Flannery etal. (54) directly tested and found support
for emotion dysregulation mediating the association between SCT
and social problems, albeit with a cross- sectional design making
longitudinal studies that are better suited for testing indirect and
developmental pathways a clear research priority. Also important
to distinguish in future research is whether such dysregulation
actually reects greater emotional distress, more emotional lability,
or poor self- regulation of elicited emotions.
Neuropsychological and executive functioning
Only a handful of studies have examined neuropsychological
decits associated with SCT, and most of these studies have used
samples (and, thus, neuropsychological test batteries) specic to,
or oversampled for, ADHD (28, 32, 43, 57, 58). In general, these
studies demonstrate that SCT is unassociated with pervasive
neuropsychological decits, whereas a large literature links
ADHD to wideranging neuropsychological impairments (59,
60). In particular, ADHD is clearly associated with decits in
response inhibition, working memory, and response variability.
In contrast, SCT does not seem to be associated with any of these
decits when accounting for the contribution of ADHD symptoms
(28, 32, 43). However, there is some indication that SCT may be
uniquely related to problems with early information processing
or selective attention (58). Furthermore, two of the larger studies
examining SCT in relation to neuropsychological test performance
both found SCT to be related to poorer sustained attention aer
controlling for ADHD inattention (32, 43). ere is mixed evi-
dence in terms of whether or not SCT remains associated with
slower motor or processing speed aer controlling for ADHD (28,
32, 61). One study found SCT to be associated with variability of
spatial memory performance specically (57). Replication will
be needed to draw conclusions regarding any of these ndings. It
will be especially important for future studies examining SCT in
relation to neuropsychological functioning to do so in non- ADHD
samples, as well as to use neuropsychological and cognitive tests
more specic to the nature of SCT that may not be frequently used
in studies of ADHD. In addition, only one study has examined SCT
in relation to neuropsychological test performance in adults. is
study found neither SCT nor ADHD to predict test performance,
which is likely due to this study’s use of a college student sample
(55). In sum, although more studies are needed, it seems clear
that SCT is not associated with substantial or pervasive executive
functioning (EF) decits. is is yet one more indication that SCT
is best conceptualized as distinct fromADHD.
When ratings of EF in daily life are used, as opposed to
neuropsychological tests of EF, there appear to be dierent
conclusions between studies of youth or adults. is can be seen
most clearly in Barkley’s national studies (15, 16), which did not
suer from possible ascertainment bias by using previously ADHD
dened samples. In the study of children and adolescents, SCT
accounted for very little variance across all EF dimensions when
controlling for ADHD symptoms in youth, though SCT did contrib-
ute somewhat to decits in self- organization and problem- solving.
ADHD inattention was far and away the strongest predictor of EF
decits across all EF dimensions (16). In contrast, in the national
study of adults, ADHD inattention remained the strongest predictor
of self- management to time and self- motivation EF dimensions,
but SCT accounted for the most variance in the self- organization
SECTION4 150
and problem- solving, self- restraint, and self- regulation of emotion
dimensions (15). In line with these studies, Araujo Jiménez (53) found
ADHD symptoms to be a stronger predictor than SCT symptoms of
most EF dimensions in a sample of youth aged 6– 17years, though
SCT did contribute meaningful variance in predicting emotional
control, working memory, and planning/ organization. Similarly,
Becker and Langberg (62) found SCT to be signicantly associated
with metacognitive EF decits in a sample of young adolescents
diagnosed with ADHD. However, this was only true when parent
ratings of SCT were used and not teacher ratings. As hypothesized,
SCT was not signicantly associated with behavioural regulation
EF decits that are more strongly linked to poor inhibition (62).
In line with ndings from Barkley’s nationally representative study
of adults (15), two studies conducted with college students suggest
SCT to be more clearly related to EF ratings in adults, with the
strongest associations apparent for the organization and problem-
solving, self- motivation, and self- regulation of emotion dimensions,
with signicant, albeit weaker, associations also found for the time
management and self- restraint dimensions (55, 56). Once again,
longitudinal and developmentally informed studies are needed
to evaluate the developmental pathways and mechanisms of the
linkage between SCT and daily life EF decits.
Functional impairment
In addition to studies examining SCT in relation to aspects of ad-
justment, several studies have examined SCT in relation to global
and specic domains of functional impairment. SCT is related to
global impairment (14), though likely to a lesser degree and less
pervasively so than ADHD (15, 16). Nevertheless, it is clear that
SCT is associated with functional impairment not explained by any
overlap with ADHD (15, 16, 19, 32, 56). It is thus not surprising
that SCT is also linked to increased stress (63) and a poorer
quality of life (64), as well as lower educational attainment and
socioeconomic status (14– 16). As such, SCT is clearly not benign
and so is deserving of both research inquiry and clinical attention.
Key directions forfuture research
roughout this chapter we have pointed out key directions for fu-
ture research, but it is important to mention a few additional areas
(see also 3, 14). First, there is a clear need for additional longitu-
dinal research. Such studies would allow for a better understanding
of the developmental course of SCT, longitudinal correlates, and
pathways by which SCT is associated with developmental outcomes
and functional impairments. Second, and relatedly, the vast ma-
jority of extant SCT studies have focused on school- aged children
and college students, making research in other developmental
periods, as well as in other samples of young adults, a necessity.
ird, very little is known regarding the aetiology of SCT, including
either biological and/ or environmental contributions to the SCT
phenotype. In particular, social adversities may be more relevant
for the aetiology of SCT than for ADHD (3, 14). Pathological mind
wandering is another intriguing possibility for the aetiology of
SCT (3, 65). As we have discussed elsewhere (5, 6, 17), it is also
possible that dierent attentional networks are implicated in SCT
versus ADHD. Moreover, SCT is only modestly associated with
nighttime sleep problems (66– 68), but is moderately related to both
sleep quality and daytime sleepiness (66, 69). It would be fruitful
for studies to evaluate whether similar mechanisms contribute to
SCT and hypersomnia, including use of methodologies such as the
multiple sleep latency test and default mode network connectivity.
Finally, it is important to examine whether the presence of SCT
symptoms predicts or moderates treatment response to currently
established evidence- based interventions for ADHD as well as
other psychiatric disorders (e.g. anxiety, mood, sleep disorders). It
is likewise important to evaluate interventions that might be e-
cacious for treating SCT, including both pharmacological and psy-
chosocial interventions. Initial evidence indicates that atomoxetine
may eectively reduce SCT symptoms (70), and selective serotonin
reuptake inhibitors may also be eective given the strong asso-
ciation between SCT and internalizing problems such as anxiety
and depression (5). In terms of psychosocial interventions, Pner
and colleagues (71) found evidence- based parent- and teacher-
mediated behavioural interventions to reduce SCT symptoms in
children with ADHD predominantly inattentive type. Given SCT’s
associations with internalizing problems (especially depression)
and social withdrawal, we have also hypothesized that cognitive–
behavioural treatment (CBT), social skills training, and behav-
ioural activation may be eective for treating SCT (3, 5, 14, 72).
However, each of these possibilities has yet to be examined empir-
ically. Of course, it will be important to do so in samples that are
not restricted to individuals diagnosed with ADHD but instead use
a broader sampling strategy (e.g. including individuals with anx-
iety, depression, and/ or sleep disorders) or are selected based on
SCT specically.
A comment onterminology
Before concluding, we believe one additional point regarding
terminology is warranted. In coauthoring this chapter, we hope
it is clear that we (SPB and RAB) agree on the vast majority of
conclusions that can be drawn regarding the current state of SCT, as
well as the very clear need for additional research that can ultimately
shed light on the aetiology, course, and clinical implications of SCT.
Nevertheless, we also acknowledge one area of professional dis-
agreement; namely, the best term to be used to describe this con-
stellation of symptoms.
Barkley has advocated moving away from the SCT terminology
and suggests the term concentration decit disorder (CDD) (3, 5,
41, 73)for various reasons, ‘not the least of which is that SCT can be
viewed by the public as pejorative, derogatory, or frankly oensive’
(5)(p.435). It is important to note that Becker agrees that the SCT
term is far from optimal and ‘shares with Barkley the overarching
concerns he has for the SCT label’ (17) (p.1038). In fact, this is why
Becker avoided the SCT term when naming his youth self- report
scale, calling it the Child Concentration Inventory (17). However,
Becker has also expressed concerns that it may be ‘premature to use
terminology suggesting that the SCT construct is a diagnostic entity
(e.g. Concentration Decit Disorder)’ (14). Just as the SCT label
may be perceived as pejorative and oensive, premature use of the
term ‘disorder’ may create confusion for families and professionals
while also drawing unfavourable media attention that detracts from
the important value of SCT research (74, 75). Further, terminology
suggestive of a ‘disorder’ casts SCT in a dichotomous light, and
Becker believes it is important to consider other options, espe-
cially the possibility that SCT is best viewed as a transdiagnostic
construct as opposed to a categorical diagnosis (4, 76). is latter
possibility aligns with the broader shi in psychiatry as outlined
CHAPTER15 151
by the United States National Institute of Mental Health research
domain criteria (RDoC) initiative (77, 78). us, Becker would cur-
rently favour a term that focuses more on ‘symptoms’ as opposed
to ‘diagnosis’ and is actively considering possible alternatives as
research continues to emerge, even though he has so far continued
to use the SCT terminology.
However, as Barkley has appropriately noted, ‘the mere fact that
SCT has existed for 35years and so should continue to be used
out of mere historical tradition is not a convincing reason to retain
a term that research participants and other consumers likely will
nd oensive’ (41). Becker agrees fully with this point, but for him
the issue is one of timing:a new term is needed but much more
remains to be learned before we can settle on an ideal term. ere
is no easy solution to this dilemma, as the introduction of a new
term earlier may likely spur the very research needed to determine
an even better term. Barkley has clearly acknowledged that he is
not wedded to his proposed CDD terminology (41). He suggests
here that perhaps the term ‘syndrome’ might be a better modier
of ‘concentration decit’ so as not to convey ocial sanctioning of
it as a recognized clinical disorder. Where does this leave us? We
both agree that we need a new term that:1) accurately describes the
constellation of symptoms and state of the science; and 2)is not o-
putting to either the scientic community or general population.
We hope we can nd consensus around such a term in the near
future. In the meantime, we are certainly in agreement that there is
a crucial need for much more research on SCT/ CDD, and we hope
our colleagues will join us in pursuing this theoretically and clinic-
ally important area ofstudy.
References
1. Diamond A. Attention- decit disorder (attention- decit/ hyperactivity
disorder without hyperactivity):a neurobiologically and behaviorally
distinct disorder from attention- decit/ hyperactivity disorder (with
hyperactivity). Development and Psychopathology. 2005;17(3):807– 25.
2. Milich R, Balentine AC, Lynam DR. ADHD combined type and
ADHD predominantly inattentive type are distinct and unrelated
disorders. Clinical Psychology:Science and Practice. 2001;8(4):463– 88.
3. Barkley RA. Sluggish cognitive tempo (concentration decit
disorder?):current status, future directions, and a plea to change the
name. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2014;42(1):117– 25.
4. Becker SP, Marshall SA, McBurnett K. Sluggish cognitive tempo in
abnormal child psychology:an historical overview and introduction
to the special section. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology.
2014;42(1):1– 6.
5. Barkley RA. Concentration decit disorder (sluggish cognitive tempo).
In:Barkley RA, editor. Attention- decit/ hyperactivity disorder:A
handbook for diagnosis and treatment. 4th edn. NewYork:Guilford,
2015. pp. 435– 52.
6. Becker SP. Topical review:sluggish cognitive tempo:research ndings
and relevance for pediatric psychology. Journal of Pediatric Psychology.
2013;38(10):1051– 7.
7. Crichton A. An inquiry into the nature and origin of mental
derangement:comprehending a concise system of the physiology and
pathology of the human mind and a history of the passions and their
eects. London:Cadell & Davies, reprinted by AMS Press, NewYork,
1976;1798.
8. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual
of mental disorders, 3rd edn. Washington, D.C.:American Psychiatric
Association, 1980. 494pp.
9. Carlson CL, Lahey BB, Neeper R. Direct assessment of the cognitive
correlates of attention- decit disorders with and without hyperactivity.
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 1986;8(1):69– 86.
10. Lahey BB, Schaughency EA, Frame CL, Strauss CC. Teacher ratings of
attention problems in children experimentally classied as exhibiting
attention decit disorder with and without hyperactivity. Journal of the
American Academy of Child Psychiatry. 1985;24(5):613– 16.
11. Neeper R, Lahey BB. e Children’s Behavior Rating Scale:a
factor analytic developmental study. School Psychology Review.
1986;15(2):277– 88.
12. Frick PJ, Lahey BB, Applegate B, Kerdyck L, Ollendick T, Hynd
GW, etal. DSM- IV eld trials for the disruptive behavior
disorders:symptom utility estimates. Journal of the American Academy
of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1994;33(4):529– 39.
13. McBurnett K, Pner LJ, Frick PJ. Symptom properties as a function
of ADHD type:an argument for continued study of sluggish cognitive
tempo. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2001;29(3):207– 13.
14. Becker SP, Leopold DR, Burns GL, Jarrett MA, Langberg JM, Marshall SA,
etal. e internal, external, and diagnostic validity of sluggish cognitive
tempo:a meta- analysis and critical review. Journal of the American
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2016;55(3):163– 78.
15. Barkley RA. Distinguishing sluggish cognitive tempo from attention-
decit/ hyperactivity disorder in adults. Journal of Abnormal Psychology.
2012;121(4):978– 90.
16. Barkley RA. Distinguishing sluggish cognitive tempo from ADHD
in children and adolescents:executive functioning, impairment,
and comorbidity. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology.
2013;42(2):161– 73.
17. Becker SP, Luebbe AM, Joyce AM. e Child Concentration Inventory
(CCI):initial validation of a child self- report measure of sluggish
cognitive tempo. Psychological Assessment. 2015;27(3):1037– 52.
18. Lee S, Burns GL, Snell J, McBurnett K. Validity of the sluggish cognitive
tempo symptom dimension in children:sluggish cognitive tempo
and ADHD- inattention as distinct symptom dimensions. Journal of
Abnormal Child Psychology. 2014;42(1):7– 19.
19. McBurnett K, Villodas M, Burns GL, Hinshaw SP, Beaulieu A, Pner
LJ. Structure and validity of sluggish cognitive tempo using an
expanded item pool in children with attention- decit/ hyperactivity
disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2014;42(1):37– 48.
20. Penny AM, Waschbusch DA, Klein RM, Corkum P, Eskes G.
Developing a measure of sluggish cognitive tempo for children:content
validity, factor structure, and reliability. Psychological Assessment.
2009;21(3):380– 9.
21. McBurnett K. Kiddie- Sluggish Cognitive Tempo Diagnostic Interview
Module for Children and Adolescents. San Francisco:McBurnett,2010.
22. Garner AA, Peugh J, Becker SP, Kingery KM, Tamm L, Vaughn AJ,
etal. Does sluggish cognitive tempo t within a bi- factor model of
ADHD? Journal of Attention Disorders. 2017;21(8):642– 54.
23. Lee S, Burns GL, Beauchaine TP, Becker SP. Bifactor latent structure
of attention- decit/ hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)/ oppositional
deant disorder (ODD) symptoms and rst- order latent structure
of sluggish cognitive tempo symptoms. Psychological Assessment.
2016;28(8):917– 28.
24. Leopold DR, Christopher ME, Burns GL, Becker SP, Olson RK,
Willcutt EG. Attention- decit/ hyperactivity disorder and sluggish
cognitive tempo throughout childhood:temporal invariance and
stability from preschool through ninth grade. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry. 2016;57(9):1066– 74.
25. Moruzzi S, Rijsdijk F, Battaglia M. A twin study of the relationships
among inattention, hyperactivity/ impulsivity and sluggish
cognitive tempo problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology.
2014;42(1):63– 75.
26. Fassbender C, Kra CE, Schweitzer JB. Dierentiating SCT and
inattentive symptoms in ADHD using fMRI measures of cognitive
control. Neuroimage Clinic. 2015;8:390– 7.
27. Posner MI, Rothbart MK. Research on attention networks as a
model for the integration of psychological science. Annual Review of
Psychology. 2007;58:1– 23.
28. Bauermeister JJ, Barkley RA, Bauermeister JA, Martinez JV,
McBurnett K. Validity of the sluggish cognitive tempo, inattention,
SECTION4 152
and hyperactivity symptom dimensions:neuropsychological and
psychosocial correlates. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology.
2012;40(5):683– 97.
29. Becker SP, Langberg JM. Sluggish cognitive tempo among young
adolescents with ADHD:relations to mental health, academic, and
social functioning. Journal of Attention Disorders. 2013;17(8):681– 9.
30. Becker SP, Langberg JM, Luebbe AM, Dvorsky MR, Flannery AJ.
Sluggish cognitive tempo is associated with academic functioning
and internalizing symptoms in college students with and without
attention- decit/ hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychology.
2014;70(4):388– 403.
31. Becker SP, Luebbe AM, Fite PJ, Stoppelbein L, Greening L. Sluggish
cognitive tempo in psychiatrically hospitalized children:factor
structure and relations to internalizing symptoms, social problems,
and observed behavioral dysregulation. Journal of Abnormal Child
Psychology. 2014;42(1):49– 62.
32. Willcutt EG, Chhabildas N, Kinnear M, DeFries JC, Olson RK, Leopold
DR, etal. e internal and external validity of sluggish cognitive tempo
and its relation with DSM- IV ADHD. Journal of Abnormal Child
Psychology. 2014;42(1):21– 35.
33. Fenollar Cortés J, Servera M, Becker SP, Burns GL. External validity
of ADHD inattention and sluggish cognitive tempo dimensions
in Spanish children with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders.
2017;21(8):656– 66.
34. Jacobson LA, Murphy- Bowman SC, Pritchard AE, Tart- Zelvin A, Zabel
TA, Mahone EM. Factor structure of a sluggish cognitive tempo scale
in clinically- referred children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology.
2012;40(8):1327– 37.
35. Bernad MD, Servera M, Becker SP, Burns GL. Sluggish cognitive tempo
and ADHD inattention as predictors of externalizing, internalizing,
and impairment domains:a 2- year longitudinal study. Journal of
Abnormal Child Psychology. 2016;44(4):771– 85.
36. Bernad MD, Servera M, Grases G, Collado S, Burns GL. A
cross- sectional and longitudinal investigation of the external
correlates of sluggish cognitive tempo and ADHD- inattention
symptom dimensions. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology.
2014;42(7):1225– 36.
37. Servera M, Bernad MD, Carrillo JM, Collado S, Burns GL.
Longitudinal correlates of sluggish cognitive tempo and ADHD-
inattention symptom dimensions with Spanish children. Journal of
Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology. 2016;45(5):632– 41.
38. Carlson CL, Mann M. Sluggish cognitive tempo predicts a dierent
pattern of impairment in the attention decit hyperactivity disorder,
predominantly inattentive type. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent
Psychology. 2002;31(1):123– 9.
39. Marshall SA, Evans SW, Eiraldi RB, Becker SP, Power TJ. Social and
academic impairment in youth with ADHD, predominately inattentive
type and sluggish cognitive tempo. Journal of Abnormal Child
Psychology. 2014;42(1):77– 90.
40. Becker SP, Fite PJ, Garner AA, Greening L, Stoppelbein L, Luebbe AM.
Reward and punishment sensitivity are dierentially associated with
ADHD and sluggish cognitive tempo symptoms in children. Journal of
Research in Personality. 2013;47(6):719– 27.
41. Barkley RA. Commentary on the internal, external, and diagnostic
validity of sluggish cognitive tempo:a meta- analysis and critical review.
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.
2016;55(3):157– 8.
42. Watabe Y, Owens JS, Evans SW, Brandt NE. e relationship between
sluggish cognitive tempo and impairment in children with and without
ADHD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2014;42(1):105– 15.
43. Wåhlstedt C, Bohlin G. DSM- IV- dened inattention and sluggish
cognitive tempo:independent and interactive relations to
neuropsychological factors and comorbidity. Child Neuropsychology.
2010;16(4):350– 65.
44. Langberg JM, Becker SP, Dvorsky MR. e association between
sluggish cognitive tempo and academic functioning in youth with
attention- decit/ hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Journal of Abnormal
Child Psychology. 2014;42(1):91– 103.
45. Burns GL, Servera M, Carrillo JM, Cardo E. Distinctions between
sluggish cognitive tempo, ADHD- IN, and depression symptom
dimensions in Spanish rst- grade children. Journal of Clinical Child &
Adolescent Psychology. 2013;42(6):796– 808.
46. Khadka G, Burns GL, Becker SP. Internal and external validity of
sluggish cognitive tempo and ADHD inattention dimensions with
teacher ratings of Nepali children. Journal of Psychopathology and
Behavioral Assessment. 2016;38(3):433– 42.
47. Lee S, Burns GL, Becker SP. Towards establishing the transcultural
validity of sluggish cognitive tempo:evidence from a sample of South
Korean children. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology.
Advance online publication. doi:10.1080/ 15374416.2016.1144192.
48. Belmar M, Servera M, Becker SP, Burns GL. Validity of sluggish
cognitive tempo in South America:an initial examination using
mother and teacher ratings of Chilean children. Journal of Attention
Disorders. 2017;21(8):667– 72.
49. Becker SP. Sluggish cognitive tempo and peer functioning in school-
aged children:a six- month longitudinal study. Psychiatry Research.
2014;217(1– 2):72– 8.
50. Mikami AY, Huang- Pollock CL, Pner LJ, McBurnett K, Hangai D.
Social skills dierences among attention- decit/ hyperactivity disorder
types in a chat room assessment task. Journal of Abnormal Child
Psychology. 2007;35(4):509– 21.
51. Gardner DM, Gerdes AC. A review of peer relationships and
friendships in youth with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders.
2015;19(10):844– 55.
52. Capdevila- Brophy C, Artigas- Pallarés J, Navarro- Pastor JB, García-
Nonell K, Rigau- Ratera E, Obiols JE. ADHD predominantly inattentive
subtype with high sluggish cognitive tempo:a new clinical entity?
Journal of Attention Disorders. 2014;18(7):607– 16.
53. Araujo Jiménez EA, Jané Ballabriga MC, Bonillo Martin A, Arrufat FJ,
Serra Giacobo R. Executive functioning in children and adolescents
with symptoms of sluggish cognitive tempo and ADHD. Journal of
Attention Disorders. 2015;19(6):507– 14.
54. Flannery AJ, Becker SP, Luebbe AM. Does emotion dysregulation
mediate the association between sluggish cognitive tempo and
college students’ social impairment? Journal of Attention Disorders.
2016;20(9):802– 12.
55. Jarrett MA, Rapport HF, Rondon AT, Becker SP. ADHD dimensions
and sluggish cognitive tempo symptoms in relation to self- report and
laboratory measures of neuropsychological functioning in college
students. Journal of Attention Disorders. 2017;21(8):673– 83.
56. Wood WL, Lewandowski LJ, Lovett BJ, Antshel KM. Executive
dysfunction and functional impairment associated with sluggish
cognitive tempo in emerging adulthood. Journal of Attention Disorders.
2017;21(8):691– 700.
57. Skirbekk B, Hansen BH, Oerbeck B, Kristensen H. e relationship
between sluggish cognitive tempo, subtypes of attention- decit/
hyperactivity disorder, and anxiety disorders. Journal of Abnormal
Child Psychology. 2011;39(4):513– 25.
58. Huang- Pollock CL, Nigg JT, Carr TH. Decient attention is hard
to nd:applying the perceptual load model of selective attention to
attention decit hyperactivity disorder subtypes. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 2005;46(11):1211– 18.
59. Willcutt EG, Doyle AE, Nigg JT, Faraone SV, Pennington BF.
Validity of the executive function theory of attention- decit/
hyperactivity disorder:a meta- analytic review. Biological Psychiatry.
2005;57(11):1336– 46.
60. Hervey AS, Epstein JN, Curry JF. Neuropsychology of adults with
attention- decit/ hyperactivity disorder:a meta- analytic review.
Neuropsychology. 2004;18(3):485– 503.
61. Hinshaw SP, Carte ET, Sami N, Treuting JJ, Zupan BA.
Preadolescent girls with attention- decit/ hyperactivity disorder:II.
Neuropsychological performance in relation to subtypes and
CHAPTER15 153
individual classication. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology.
2002;70(5):1099– 111.
62. Becker SP, Langberg JM. Attention- decit/ hyperactivity disorder
and sluggish cognitive tempo dimensions in relation to executive
functioning in adolescents with ADHD. Child Psychiatry and Human
Development. 2014;45(1):1– 11.
63. Combs MA, Canu WH, Broman- Fulks JJ, Rocheleau CA, Nieman DC.
Perceived stress and ADHD symptoms in adults. Journal of Attention
Disorders. 2015;19(5):425– 34.
64. Combs MA, Canu WH, Broman- Fulks JJ, Nieman DC. Impact of
sluggish cognitive tempo and attention- decit/ hyperactivity disorder
symptoms on adults’ quality of life. Applied Research in Quality of Life.
2014;9(4):981– 95.
65. Adams ZW, Milich R, Fillmore MT. A case for the return
of attention- decit disorder in DSM- 5. e ADHD Report.
2010;18(3):1– 6.
66. Becker SP, Luebbe AM, Langberg JM. Attention- decit/ hyperactivity
disorder dimensions and sluggish cognitive tempo symptoms in
relation to college students’ sleep functioning. Child Psychiatry and
Human Development. 2014;45(6):675– 85.
67. Koriakin TA, Mahone EM, Jacobson LA. Sleep diculties are
associated with parent report of sluggish cognitive tempo. Journal of
Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics. 2015;36(9):717– 23.
68. Becker SP, Pner L, Stein MA, Burns GL, McBurnett K. Sleep habits
in children with attention- decit/ hyperactivity disorder predominantly
inattentive type and associations with comorbid psychopathology
symptoms. Sleep Medicine. 2016;21:151– 9.
69. Langberg JM, Becker SP, Dvorsky MR, Luebbe AM. Are sluggish
cognitive tempo and daytime sleepiness distinct constructs?
Psychological Assessment. 2014;26(2):586– 97.
70. Wietecha L, Williams D, Shaywitz S, Shaywitz B, Hooper SR, Wigal
SB, etal. Atomoxetine improved attention in children and adolescents
with attention- decit/ hyperactivity disorder and dyslexia in a 16 week,
acute, randomized, double- blind trial. Journal of Child and Adolescent
Psychopharmacology. 2013;23(9):605– 13.
71. Pner LJ, Yee Mikami A, Huang- Pollock C, Easterlin B, Zalecki C,
McBurnett K. A randomized, controlled trial of integrated home-
school behavioral treatment for ADHD, predominantly inattentive
type. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Psychiatry. 2007;46(8):1041– 50.
72. Becker SP, Ciesielski HA, Rood JE, Froehlich TE, Garner AA, Tamm L,
etal. Uncovering a clinical portrait of sluggish cognitive tempo within
an evaluation for attention- decit/ hyperactivity disorder:a case study.
Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2016;21(1):81– 94.
73. Saxbe C, Barkley RA. e second attention disorder? Sluggish cognitive
tempo vs. attention- decit/ hyperactivity disorder:update for clinicians.
Journal of Psychiatric Practice . 2014;20(1):38– 49.
74. Schwarz A. Idea of new attention disorder spurs research, and debate.
e NewYork Times. April 11,2014.
75. Aldhous P. e daydream disorder:is sluggish cognitive tempo a
disease or disease mongering? Slate. September 29,2014.
76. Becker SP, Willcutt EG. Advancing the study of sluggish cognitive
tempo via DSM, RDoC, and hierarchical models of psychopathology.
(Under review).
77. Cuthbert BN, Insel TR. Toward the future of psychiatric diagnosis:the
seven pillars of RDoC. BioMed Central Medicine. 2013;11:126.
78. Insel T, Cuthbert B, Garvey M, Heinssen R, Pine DS, Quinn K,
etal. Research domain criteria (RDoC):toward a new classication
framework for research on mental disorders. American Journal of
Psychiatry. 2010;167(7):748– 51.
Oxford Textbook of
Attention Deficit
Hyperactivity
Disorder
Oxford Textbooks inPsychiatry
Oxford Textbook of Correctional Psychiatry
Edited by Robert Trestman, Kenneth Appelbaum, and Jeffrey Metzner
Oxford Textbook of Old Age Psychiatry
Edited by Tom Dening and Alanomas
Oxford Textbook of Community MentalHealth
Edited by Graham ornicroft, George Szmukler, Kim T. Mueser, and Robert E.Drake
Oxford Textbook of Suicidology and Suicide Prevention
Edited by Danuta Wasserman and Camilla Wasserman
Oxford Textbook of Women and MentalHealth
Edited by DoraKohen
1
Oxford Textbook of
Attention Deficit
Hyperactivity
Disorder
Editedby
Tobias Banaschewski
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy of Childhood and Adolescence,
The Central Institute of Mental Health,
Mannheim, Germany
David Coghill
Department of Paediatrics and Department of Psychiatry,
The University of Melbourne, Australia;
Murdoch Children’s Research Institute, Melbourne, Australia;
The Royal Children’s Hospital, Melbourne, Australia
and
AlessandroZuddas
Child & Adolescent Neuropsychiatry, Section of Neuroscience & Clinical Pharmacology,
Department of Biomedical Science, University of Cagliari; ‘G.Brotzu’ Hospital Trust,
Cagliari,Italy
1
Great Clarendon Street, Oxford, OX26DP,
United Kingdom
Oxford University Press is a department of the University of Oxford.
It furthers the University’s objective of excellence in research, scholarship,
and education by publishing worldwide. Oxford is a registered trade markof
Oxford University Press in the UK and in certain other countries
© Oxford University Press2019
Chapter8 is © National Institutes of Health 2018
e moral rights of the authors have been asserted
First Edition published in 2018
Impression:1
All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, storedin
a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, withoutthe
prior permission in writing of Oxford University Press, or as expressly permitted
by law, by licence or under terms agreed with the appropriate reprographics
rights organization. Enquiries concerning reproduction outside the scopeofthe
above should be sent to the Rights Department, Oxford University Press,atthe
addressabove
You must not circulate this work in any otherform
and you must impose this same condition on any acquirer
Published in the United States of America by Oxford UniversityPress
198 Madison Avenue, NewYork, NY 10016, United States of America
British Library Cataloguing in PublicationData
Data available
Library of Congress Control Number:2017959059
ISBN 978– 0– 19– 873925– 8
Printed and boundby
CPI Group (UK) Ltd, Croydon, CR04YY
Oxford University Press makes no representation, express or implied, thatthe
drug dosages in this book are correct. Readers must therefore alwayscheck
the product information and clinical procedures with the most up- to- date
published product information and data sheets provided by the manufacturers
and the most recent codes of conduct and safety regulations. e authorsand
the publishers do not accept responsibility or legal liability for any errorsinthe
text or for the misuse or misapplication of material in this work. Exceptwhere
otherwise stated, drug dosages and recommendations are for the non- pregnant
adult who is not breast- feeding
Links to third party websites are provided by Oxford in good faithand
for information only. Oxford disclaims any responsibility for the materials
contained in any third party website referenced in thiswork.
Foreword
TO COME
Contents
Abbreviations xv
Contributors xi
SECTION 1
Introduction
1 Development of the concept 3
EricTaylor
2 ADHD in the twenty- first century:biology,
context, policy, and the need for
integrative perspective 9
Stephen P. Hinshaw and Richard M. Scheffler
SECTION 2
Aetiology and pathophysiology
3 ADHD genetics 19
Kate Langley
4 Conceptualizing and investigating the role of
the environment in ADHD:correlate, cause,
consequence, context, and treatment 25
Edmund Sonuga- Barke and GordonHarold
5 Gene– environment interactions 35
Barbara Franke and Jan K. Buitelaar
6 Brain volumes and intrinsic brain
connectivity in ADHD 57
Kerstin Konrad, Adriana Di Martino, and YutaAoki
7 ADHD brain function 64
KatyaRubia
8 Insights from neuroanatomical imaging
into ADHD throughout the lifespan 73
Philip Shaw and Eszter Szekely
9 Neurophysiology 82
Daniel Brandeis, Sandra K. Loo, Grainne McLoughlin,
Hartmut Heinrich, and Tobias Banaschewski
10 Cognitive functioning in ADHD:inhibition,
memory, temporal discounting, decision-
making, timing, and reaction time variability 94
David Coghill, Maggie Toplak, Sinead Rhodes,
and NicolettaAdamo
11 Emotional dysregulation and ADHD 103
Celine Ryckaert, Jonna Kuntsi, and Philip Asherson
12 Neuropsychological functioning and
ADHD:a developmental perspective 118
Sarah O’Neill, Jeffrey M. Halperin, and David Coghill
SECTION 3
Epidemiology
13 Epidemiology 131
Guilherme V. Polanczyk
SECTION 4
Clinical presentation
14 Current diagnostic criteria:DSM,
ICD, and future perspectives 139
Luis Augusto Rohde, Christian Kieling,
and Giovanni AbrahãoSalum
15 Sluggish cognitive tempo 147
Stephen P. Becker and Russell A. Barkley
16 Sex differences in ADHD 154
Corina U. Greven, Jennifer S. Richards, and Jan K. Buitelaar
17 Quality of life and impairment in ADHD 161
Melissa Mulraney and David Coghill
18 Adult ADHD and employment 170
MariosAdamou
19 Adult ADHD:clinical presentation
and assessment 178
Philip Asherson, Josep Antoni Ramos- Quiroga,
and SusanYoung
viii
SECTION 5
Comorbidity
20 Conduct disorder in ADHD 193
Anita apar and Stephanie vanGoozen
21 Irritability, disruptive mood, and ADHD 200
Melissa Mulraney, Argyris Stringaris, and EricTaylor
22 Comorbidity:depression and anxiety 206
Cristal Oxley and Argyris Stringaris
23 ADHD and substance misuse 215
Timothy Wilens, Nicholas Carrellas,
and Joseph Biederman
24 Autism spectrum disorder 227
Sven Bölte, Luise Poustka, and HildeGeurts
25 Intellectual impairment and
neurogenetic disorders 235
Emily Simonoff
26 Influence of tics and/ or obsessive- compulsive
behaviour on the phenomenology
of coexisting ADHD 247
Aribert Rothenberger, Andreas Becker,
Lillian- Geza Rothenberger, and Veit Roessner
27 Developmental coordination disorder 254
Christopher Gillberg, Elisabeth Fernell, I.
Carina Gillberg, and Björn Kadesjö
28 ADHD and communication disorders 261
Rosemary Tannock
29 ADHD and reading disorder 273
Erik G. Willcutt
30 ADHD and sleep 280
Melissa Mulraney, Emma Sciberras,
and Michel Lecendreux
31 e relationship of ADHD to
obesity and asthma 289
Samuele Cortese and Marcel Romanos
SECTION 6
Clinical assessment 295
32 Children and adolescents:assessment
in everyday clinical practice 297
Marina Danckaerts and David Coghill
33 ADHD in adults; assessment issues 307
Sandra Kooij, Philip Asherson, and MichaelRösler
SECTION 7
Interventions
34 Long- term outcomes in the Multimodal
Treatment study of Children with ADHD 315
James M. Swanson, L. Eugene Arnold, Peter S. Jensen,
Stephen P. Hinshaw, Lily T. Hechtman, William E. Pelham,
Laurence L. Greenhill, C. Keith Conners, Helena C.
Kraemer, Timothy Wigal, Benedetto Vitiello, Glen R.
Elliott, Howard B. Abikoff, Betsy Hoza, Jeffrey H. Newcorn,
Karen Wells, Marc Lerner, Brooke S.G. Molina, Jeffery N.
Epstein, Elizabeth B. Owens, James Waxmonsky, Desiree
W. Murray, Margaret H. Sibley, John T. Mitchell, Arunima
Roy, Annamaire Stehli for the MTA CooperativeGroup
35 Behavioural interventions for
preschool ADHD 333
David Daley and Saskia Van derOord
36 Cognitive– behavioural treatment in
childhood and adolescence 340
Manfred Döpfner and Saskia van derOord
37 Behavioural therapy (adolescent/ adult) 348
Alexandra Philomena Lam and Alexandra Philipsen
38 Cognitive training approaches for ADHD:can
they be made more effective? 358
Edmund Sonuga- Barke and Samuele Cortese
39 Neurofeedback 366
Martin Holtmann, Björn Albrecht, and Daniel Brandeis
40 Nutritional intervention for ADHD 373
Jan K. Buitelaar, Nanda Rommelse,
Verena Ly, and Julia J. Rucklidge
41 ADHD treatment:psychostimulants 379
Alessandro Zuddas, Tobias Banaschewski,
David Coghill, and Mark A.Stein
42 Non- stimulants in the treatment of ADHD 393
Ralf W. Dittmann, Alexander Häge, Juan
D. Pedraza, and Jeffrey H. Newcorn
43 ADHD and transitions to adult
mental health services 402
ChrisHollis
44 ADHD and school 408
Christine Merrell and KapilSayal
ix
SECTION 8
Clinical management
45 Organizing and delivering
treatment for ADHD 417
David Coghill and Marina Danckaerts
46 Treatment in adult ADHD 426
Philip Asherson and Josep Antoni Ramos- Quiroga
47 e next steps:future clinical and
research developments in ADHD 437
David Coghill, Alessandro Zuddas, Luis
Augusto Rohde, and Tobias Banaschewski
Index 445
Contributors
Howard B. Abikoff, NewYork University, NewYork,USA
Nicoletta Adamo, MRC Social, Genetic and Developmental
Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and
Neuroscience, King’s College London,UK
Marios Adamou, South West Yorkshire NHS Partnership
Foundation Trust, Manygates Clinic, Wakeeld, UK; Visiting
Professor, School of Human and Health Sciences, University of
Hudderseld,UK
Björn Albrecht, Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, University
Medical Center Göttingen, Germany
Yuta Aoki, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
NewYork University, NewYork, NY,USA
L. Eugene Arnold, Ohio State University University, Columbus,
OH,USA
Philip Asherson, Social Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry,
King’s College London, London,UK
Tobias Banaschewski, Department of Psychiatry and
Psychotherapy of Childhood and Adolescence, e Central
Institute of Mental Health, Mannheim, Germany
Russell A. Barkley, Virginia Treatment Center for Children and
Virginia Commonwealth University Medical Center, Richmond,
VA,USA
Andreas Becker, Department of Childhood and Adolescent
Psychotherapy, University Medical Centre Gottingeny, Germany
Stephen P. Becker, Division of Behavioral Medicine and Clinical
Psychology, Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center,
Cincinnati, OH,USA
Joseph Biederman, Clinical and Research Programs in Pediatric
Psychopharmacology and Adult ADHD, Massachusetts General
Hospital, Boston, MA,USA
Sven Bölte, Center of Neurodevelopmental Disorders (KIND),
Department of Women’s and Children’s Health, Karolinska
Institute, Center for Psychiatry Research, Stockholm County
Council, Stockholm,Sweden
Daniel Brandeis, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry
and Psychotherapy, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical
Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim,
Germany
Jan K. Buitelaar, Department of Cognitive Neurosciences, Donders
Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud
University Medical Center, Nijmegen, e Netherlands
Nicholas Carrellas, Clinical and Research Programs in Pediatric
Psychopharmacology and Adult ADHD, Massachusetts General
Hospital, Boston, MA,USA
David Coghill, Department of Paediatrics and Department of
Psychiatry, e University of Melbourne, Australia; Murdoch
Children’s Research Institute, Melbourne, Australia; e Royal
Children’s Hospital, Melbourne, Australia
C. Keith Conners, Duke University, Durham, NC,USA
Samuele Cortese, Academic Unit of Psychology, Developmental
Brain- Behaviour Laboratory, Southampton, UK; Clinical
and Experimental Sciences (CNS and Psychiatry), Faculty of
Medicine, University of Southampton, UK; e Child Study
Center, Hassenfeld Children’s Hospital of NewYork, NYU
Langone Medical Center, NewYork, NY,USA
David Daley, Professor of Psychological Intervention and
Behaviour Change, Division of Psychiatry and Applied
Psychology, School of Medicine, University of Nottingham,UK
Marina Danckaerts, Department of Child and Adolescent
Psychiatry, UPC KU Leuven, Belgium and Department of
Neurosciences, KU Leuven, Belgium
Adriana di Martino, NewYork University Child Study Center,
NewYork, NY,USA
Ralf W. Dittmann, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry
and Psychotherapy, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical
Faculty Mannheim, University of Heidelberg, Germany
Manfred Döpfner, Department of Child and Adolescent
Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, Medical Faculty
of the University of Cologne, Germany
Glen R. Elliott, Children’s Health Council, Palo Alto, CA,USA
xii
Jeffery N. Epstein, Cincinnati Children’s Medical Center,
Cincinnati, OH,USA
Elisabeth Fernell, Gillberg Neuropsychiatry Centre, University of
Gothenburg,Sweden
Barbara Franke, Departments of Human Genetics and Psychiatry,
Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud
University Medical Center, Nijmegen, e Netherlands
Hilde Geurts, University of Amsterdam, Department of
Psychology, Dutch Autism and ADHD Research Center,
Amsterdam, e Netherlands
Christopher Gillberg, Institute of Health and Wellbeing, University
of Glasgow, UK; Gillberg Neuropsychiatry Centre, University of
Gothenburg,Sweden
I. Carina Gillberg, Gillberg Neuropsychiatry Centre, University of
Gothenburg, Gothenburg,Sweden
Laurence L. Greenhill, Columbia University, NewYork, NY,USA
Corina U. Greven, Radboud University Medical Centre, Donders
Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Department of
Cognitive Neuroscience, Nijmegen, e Netherlands
Alexander Häge, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry
and Psychotherapy, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical
Faculty Mannheim, University of Heidelberg, Germany
Jeffrey M. Halperin, Department of Psychology, Queens College
and e Graduate Center, City University of NewYork,
NY,USA
Gordon Harold, Andrew and Virginia Rudd Professor of Child and
Adolescent Mental Health, School of Psychology, University of
Sussex, UK; School of Psychology, University of Dublin, Ireland
Lily T. Hechtman, McGill University, Montreal Children’s Hospital,
Montreal, QC,Canada
Hartmut Heinrich, Department of Child and Adolescent Mental
Health, University Hospital Erlangen, Erlangen, Germany; kbo-
Heckscher- Klinikum, Munich, Germany
Stephen P. Hinshaw, University of California, Berkeley, CA,USA
Chris Hollis, University of Nottingham and Institute of Mental
Health, Developmental Psychiatry, Queen’s Medical Centre,
Nottingham,UK
Martin Holtmann, LWL- University- Hospital for Child and
Adolescent Psychiatry of the Ruhr- University Bochum, Hamm,
Germany
Betsy Hoza, University of Vermont, Burlington, VT,USA
Peter S. Jensen, University of Arkansas for Medical Sciences, Little
Rock, AR,USA
Björn Kadesjö, Gillberg Neuropsychiatry Centre, University of
Gothenburg,Sweden
Christian Kieling, Department of Psychiatry, Universidade Federal
do Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre,Brazil
Kerstin Konrad, Child Neuropsychology Section, Department of
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry of RTWH Aachen, Germany;
JARA- Brain Institute (INM 11), Research Centre Juelich,
Germany
Sandra Kooij, Associate Professor of Psychiatry, VUMc
Amsterdam, PsyQ Psycho- Medical Programs, Expertise Center
Adult ADHD, e Hague, e Netherlands
Helena C. Kraemer, Stanford University, Palo Alto, CA,USA
Jonna Kuntsi, Social Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry, King’s
College London,UK
Alexandra Philomena Lam, Medical Campus University of
Oldenburg, School of Medicine and Health Sciences, Psychiatry
and Psychotherapy— University Hospital, Karl- Jaspers- Klinik,
Bad Zwischenahn, Germany
Kate Langley, School of Psychology, Cardi University, UK and
MRC Centre for Psychiatric Genetics and Genomics, School of
Medicine, Cardi University,UK
Michel Lecendreux, AP- HP, Pediatric Sleep Center, Hospital
Robert- Debré, Paris, France; National Reference Centre for
Orphan Diseases, Narcolepsy, Idiopathic Hypersomnia and
Kleine- Levin Syndrome (CNR Narcolepsie- Hypersomnie),
Paris,France
Marc Lerner, University of California, Irvine, CA,USA
Sandra K. Loo, Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral
Sciences, David Geen School of Medicine, University of
California, Los Angeles, CA,USA
Verena Ly, Department of Clinical Psychology, Leiden University,
the Netherlands
Gráinne McLoughlin, MRC Social Genetic & Developmental
Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and
Neuroscience, King’s College London,UK
Christine Merrell, Durham University,UK
John T. Mitchell, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral
Sciences, Duke University Medical Center, NC,USA
Brooke S.G. Molina, Departments of Psychiatry, Psychology, and
Pediatrics, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA,USA
Melissa Mulraney, Department of Paediatrics, e University of
Melbourne, Australia; Murdoch Children’s Research Institute,
Melbourne, Australia
Desiree W. Murray, Frank Porter Graham Child Development
Institute, University of North Carolina, NC,USA
Jeffrey H. Newcorn, Associate Professor of Psychiatry and
Pediatrics, Director, Division of ADHD and Learning
Disorders, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, Director,
Pediatric Psychopharmacology, Mount Sinai Health System,
NewYork, NY,USA
Sarah O’Neill, Department of Psychology, e City College and
e Graduate Center, City University of NewYork, NY,USA
xiii
Elizabeth B. Owens, Institute of Human Development, University
of California, Berkeley, CA,USA
Cristal Oxley, National and Specialist CAMHS Bipolar and
Depression Clinic, Maudsley Hospital, London,UK
Juan D. Pedraza, Child and Adolescent Psychiatry— Icahn
School of Medicine at Mount Sinai Medical Center, NewYork,
NY,USA
William E. Pelham, Florida International University, Miami,
FL,USA
Alexandra Philipsen, Medical Campus University of Oldenburg,
School of Medicine and Health Sciences, Psychiatry and
Psychotherapy— University Hospital, Karl- Jaspers- Klinik,
Bad Zwischenahn, Germany
Guilherme V. Polanczyk, Division of Child and Adolescent
Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, University of São Paulo
Medical School, São Paulo,Brazil
Luise Poustka, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry/
Psychotherapy, University Medical Center Göttingen,
Göttingen, Germany
Josep Antoni Ramos- Quiroga, Department of Psychiatry,
OBERSAM, Hospital Universitari Vall d’Hebron,
Barcelona,Spain
Sinead Rhodes, Salvesen Mindroom Centre, University of
Edinburgh,UK
Jennifer S. Richards, Radboud University Medical Centre, Donders
Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Department of
Cognitive Neuroscience, Nijmegen, e Netherlands; Karakter
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. University Centre Nijmegen,
e Netherlands; University of Groningen, University Medical
Center Groningen, Department of Psychiatry, Groningen,
e Netherlands
Veit Roessner, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry
and Psychotherapy, University Hospital Carl Gustav Carus at
the Technical University Dresden, Dresden, Germany
Luis Augusto Rohde, ADHD Program, Child and Adolescent
Psychiatric Division, Hospital de Clinicas de Porto Alegre,
Porto Alegre,Brazil
Marcel Romanos, University Hospital of Würzburg, Department
of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and
Psychotherapy, Würzburg, Germany
Nanda Rommelse, Radboud University, Nijmegen, the
Netherlands
Michael Rösler, Neurozentrum, Universitätsklinikum des
Saarlandes, Hamburg, Germany
Aribert Rothenberger, Department of Childhood and Adolescent
Psychotherapy, University Medical Centre Gottingeny, Germany
Lillian- Geza Rothenberger, Institute of Ethics and History in
Medicine, Center for Medicine, Society and Prevention,
University of Tuebingen, Germany
Arunima Roy, Divison of Molecular Psychiatry, University
Hospital Würzburg, Germany
Katya Rubia, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience,
King’s College London,UK
Julia J. Rucklidge, Department of Psychology, University of
Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand
Celine Ryckaert, Social Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry,
King’s College London,UK
Giovanni Abrahão Salum, Department of Psychiatry, Universidade
Federal do Rio Grande do Sul,Brazil
Kapil Sayal, Division of Psychiatry and Applied Psychology, School
of Medicine, University of Nottingham, UK; Centre for ADHD
and Neuro- developmental Disorders across the Lifespan
(CANDAL), Institute of Mental Health, Nottingham,UK
Richard M. Scheffler, Goldman School of Public Policy, School of
Public Health, University of California, Berkeley, CA,USA
Emma Sciberras, School of Psychology, Faculty of Health, Deakin
University, Geelong, Australia
Philip Shaw, Child Psychiatry Branch, National Institute of Mental
Health, Bethesda, MD,USA
Margaret H. Sibley, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral
Health, Florida Internatiuonal University, FL,USA
Emily Simonoff, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
King’s College London, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and
Neuroscience, London,UK
Edmund Sonuga- Barke, Department of Child and Adolescent
Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and
Neuroscience, King’s College London, Department of
Experimental Clinical and Health Psychology, Ghent University,
Belgium
Annamaire Stehli, Department of Pediatrics, University of
California, Irvine, CA,USA
Mark A. Stein, Seattle Children’s Hospital, Seattle, WA,USA
Argyris Stringaris, Chief Mood Brain and Development Unit,
National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD,USA
James M. Swanson, University of California, Irvine, CA,USA
Eszter Szekely, Department of Psychiatry, McGill University
Faculty of Medicine, Montreal,Canada
Rosemary Tannock, Neuroscience and Mental Health Research
Program, Research Institute of the Hospital for Sick Children,
Toronto, ON,Canada
Eric Taylor, King’s College London, Institute of Psychiatry,
Psychology and Neuroscience, London,UK
Anita apar, Institute of Psychological Medicine and Clinical
Neurosciences, Cardi University School of Medicine,
Cardi,UK
xiv
Maggie Toplak, Clinical Developmental Program, Core Member,
LaMarsh Centre for Child and Youth Research, Department of
Psychology, York University, York,UK
Saskia Van der Oord, Associate Professor, Research Group Clinical
Psychology, Department of Psychology and Educational
Sciences, KU Leuven, Belgium
Stephanie van Goozen, School of Psychology, Cardi
University,UK
Benedetto Vitiello, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda,
MD,USA
James Waxmonsky, Department of Psychiatry, Penn State College
of Medicine, PA,USA
Karen Wells, Duke University, Durham, NC,USA
Timothy Wigal, Avida, Inc., Newport Beach, CA,USA
Timothy Wilens, Clinical and Research Programs in Pediatric
Psychopharmacology and Adult ADHD, Massachusetts General
Hospital, Boston, MA,USA
Erik G. Willcutt, Department of Psychology and Neuroscience,
University of Colorado Boulder, CO,USA
Susan Young, Centre for Psychiatry, Imperial College, London,UK
Alessandro Zuddas, Child and Adolescent Neuropsychiatry,
Sect Neuroscience and Clinical Pharmacology, Department
of Biomedical Science, University of Cagliari, and ‘G.Brotzu’
Hospital Trust, Cagliari,Italy
Abbreviations
ACC anterior cingulatecortex
ACG anterior cingulategyrus
ADD attention decit disorder
ADHD- RS ADHD ratingscale
AERS Adverse Event ReportingSystem
AHI apnea– hypopneaindex
AIM ADHD plus impairments inmood
AMHS adult mental health services
APA American Psychiatric Association
ARI aective reactivityindex
ASD autism spectrum disorder
ATX atomoxetine
BADDS Brown Attention Decit DisorderScale
BDNF brain- derived neurotropicfactor
BMI body massindex
BOLD blood oxygen level- dependent
BPD borderline personality disorder
BPT behavioural parent training
CAADID Conners Adult ADHD Diagnostic Interview
CAARS Conners Adult ADHD RatingScale
CAI computer- assisted instruction
CAMHS child and adolescent mental health services
CAPA Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Assessment
CBCL childhood behaviour checklist
CBT cognitive– behavioural therapy/ treatment
CD conduct disorder
CDD concentration decit disorder
CDI child- directed intervention
CELF Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals
CEN central executive network
CGAS Child Global AssessmentScale
CGH comparative genome hybridization
CGI Clinical Global Impressions
CHMP Committee for Medicinal Products for HumanUse
CHP Challenging Horizons Programme
CI condence interval
CLAS Child Life and AttentionSkills
CNV contingent negative variation
CNV copy number variant
CoT children- of- twins
CSD current source density
CWPT classwide peer tutoring
CXR clonidine extended- release
DA dopamine
DAMP decits in attention, motor control + perception
DAWBA development and wellbeing assessment
DCD developmental coordination disorder
DESR decient emotional self- regulation
DIVA 2.0 Diagnostic Interview for ADHD foradults
DLMO dim light melatoninonset
DMDD disruptive mood dysregulation disorder
DMN default mode network
DN default network
DPFC dorsolateral prefrontalcortex
DSM Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders
DTI diusion tensor imaging
DZ dizygotic
EAGG European ADHD GuidelinesGroup
EC eortful control
EDS excessive daytime sleepiness
EEG electroencephalogram/ electroencephalography
EF executive functioning
EIS emotional impulsivenessscale
EMA European MedicinesAgency
EMG electromyogram/ electromyography
EMR electronic medicalrecord
EPSC excitatory postsynaptic current
ERA English and Romanian Adopteesstudy
ERN error- related negativity
ERP event- related potential
ES eectsize
ESM experience samplingmethod
ESSENCE early symptomatic syndromes eliciting
neurodevelopmental clinical examinations
FAS fetal alcohol syndrome
FDA Food and Drug Administration
fMRI functional magnetic resonance imaging
FR familial riskindex
GABA gamma- aminobutyricacid
GCTA genome- wide complex trait analysis
GREML genomic- relationship- matrix restricted maximum
likelihood
GWAS genome- wide association studies
GXR guanfacine extended- release
HKD hyperkinetic disorder
HRQoL health- related quality oflife
ICA independent component analysis
ICD International Classication of Diseases
xvi
ID intellectual disability
IFC inferior prefrontalcortex
IFG inferior frontalgyrus
IPC inferior parietalcortex
iPSC induced pluripotent stemcells
ITT intention totreat
K- SADS Kiddie- Schedule of Aective Disorders
LDX lisdexamphetamine
LI language impairment
LMIC lower- and middle- income countries
LNCG local normative comparisongroup
MAP mindful awareness practice
MAS mixed amfetaminesalts
MBCT mindfulness- based cognitive therapy
MBD minimal brain dysfunction
MDD major depressive disorder
MEG magnetoencephalography
MEP motor- evoked potential
MET motivational enhancement therapy
MI motivational interviewing
MMN mismatch negativity
MND minor neurological dysfunction
MPFC medial prefrontalcortex
MPH methylphenidate
MRI magnetic resonance imaging
MRR mortality rateratio
MSLT multiple sleep latencytest
MTA Multimodal Treatment sudy of children withADHD
MZ monozygotic
NCLB No Child LeBehind
NE negative emotionality
NE norepinephrine
NIDA National Institute of DrugAbuse
NIMH National Institute of MentalHealth
NIRS near- infrared spectroscopy
NMDA N- methyl- D- aspartate
NREM non- rapid eye movement
NSCH National Survey for Children’sHealth
OCB obsessive- compulsive behaviour
OCD obsessive- compulsive disorder
ODD oppositional deant disorder
OEST other early standard therapy
OFC orbitofrontalcortex
OR oddsratio
OSA obstructive sleepapnea
PAS- ADD Psychiatric Assessment Schedule for Adults with
Developmental Disabilities
PCC posterior cingulatecortex
PCIT parent– child interaction therapy
PD pharmacodynamic
PDD pervasive development disorder
PDI parent- directed intervention
PET positron emission tomography
PFC prefrontalcortex
PK pharmacokinetic
PLMD periodic limb movement disorder
PSG polysomnography
PUFA polyunsaturated fattyacids
QoL quality oflife
RAI resource allocationindex
RCT randomized controlledtrial
RD reading disorder
RDoC research domain criteria
REM rapid eye movement
rGE gene– environment interaction
RLS restless leg syndrome
ROI region of interest
RR relative riskratio
RTV response time variability
SCID- 5 Structured Clinical Interview forDSM- 5
SCN suprachiasmatic nucleus
SCP shared care protocol
SCP slow cortical potential
SCT sluggish cognitivetempo
SD standard deviation
SDB sleep- disordered breathing
SDQ Strengths and Diculties Questionnaire
SES socioeconomicstatus
SFC superior frontalcortex
SFT solution- focused treatment
SICI short- interval intracortical inhibition
SMA supplementary motorarea
SMD severe mood dysregulation
SMD standardized mean dierence
SMR sensorimotorrhythm
SNAP Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham ratingscale
SNP single nucleotide polymorphism
SNRI serotonin- norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors
SPCD social pragmatic communication disorder
SPECT single- photon emission computed tomography
SSD speech sound disorder
SSRI selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor
SSRT stop signal reactiontime
SST social skills training
SUD substance use disorder
SWA slow- wave activity
tACS transcranial alternating current stimulation
TAU treatment asusual
TBR theta to beta powerratio
TCA tricyclic antidepressant
TD tic disorders
TDC typically developing control
tDCS transcranial direct current stimulation
TMS transcranial magnetic stimulation
ToM theory ofmind
VAN ventral attention network
VBM voxel- based morphometry
VNTR variable number of tandem repeats
WCC weak central coherence
WHO World Health Organization