ArticlePDF Available

Denying Rape but Endorsing Forceful Intercourse: Exploring Differences Among Responders

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Sexual assault is a problem on many college campuses, and many researchers have conducted studies assessing the prevalence of sexual assault perpetration and intentions to be coercive. Behaviorally descriptive survey items (i.e., “Have you ever coerced somebody to intercourse by holding them down?”) versus labeling survey items (i.e., “Have you ever raped somebody?”) will yield different responses, in that more men will admit to sexually coercive behaviors and more women will self-report victimization when behavioral descriptions are used (Koss 1998) instead of labels. Indeed, some men will endorse items asking whether they have used force to obtain intercourse, but will deny having raped a woman. There has been little research on differences between individuals to endorse a behaviorally descriptive item versus a labeling item. The present study uses discriminant function analysis to separate men who do not report intentions to be sexually coercive, those who endorse behaviorally descriptive intentions but deny it when the word rape is used, and those who endorse intentions to rape outright. Results indicated that participants can be differentiated into three groups based on scores from scales on hypermasculinity and hostility toward women. High hostility toward woman and callous sexual attitudes separated the no intentions group from those who endorsed either intentions to rape or those who endorses only the behavioral description of rape. The two types of offender groups were distinguishable mostly by varying levels of hostility, suggesting that men who endorse using force to obtain intercourse on survey items but deny rape on the same may not experience hostile affect in response to women, but might have dispositions more in line with benevolent sexism.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Denying Rape but Endorsing Forceful Intercourse:
Exploring Differences Among Responders
Sarah R. Edwards, PhD,
1
Kathryn A. Bradshaw, MA,
1
and Verlin B. Hinsz, PhD
2
Abstract
Sexual assault is a problem on many college campuses, and many researchers have conducted studies assessing the
prevalence of sexual assault perpetration and intentions to be coercive. Behaviorally descriptive survey items (i.e., ‘Have
you ever coerced somebody to intercourse by holding them down?’’) versus labeling survey items (i.e., ‘‘Have you ever
raped somebody?’’) will yield different responses, in that more men will admit to sexually coercive behaviors and more
women will self-report victimization when behavioral descriptions are used (Koss 1998) instead of labels. Indeed, some
men will endorse items asking whether they have used force to obtain intercourse, but will deny having raped a woman.
There has been little research on differences between individuals to endorse a behaviorally descriptive item versus a labeling
item. The present study uses discriminant function analysis to separate men who do not report intentions to be sexually
coercive, those who endorse behaviorally descriptive intentions but deny it when the word rape is used, and those who
endorse intentions to rape outright. Results indicated that participants can be differentiated into three groups based on scores
from scales on hypermasculinity and hostility toward women. High hostility toward woman and callous sexual attitudes
separated the no intentions group from those who endorsed either intentions to rape or those who endorses only the
behavioral description of rape. The two types of offender groups were distinguishable mostly by varying levels of hostility,
suggesting that men who endorse using force to obtain intercourse on survey items but deny rape on the same may not
experience hostile affect in response to women, but might have dispositions more in line with benevolent sexism.
Introduction
F
ederal data estimate that about one in five women
becomes the victim of sexual assault while in college,
most of which is committed by assailants known to the
victim (National Center for Injury Prevention and Control
2012). These staggering numbers, as well as the govern-
ment’s response of creating a task force addressing college
sexual assault, have generated an increase in news coverage
with some mainstream media and social network users
questioning whether many of these cases constitute rape.
For example, commentators have suggested that rape only
occurs if a woman labels it as such, while experiencing an
unwanted sexual experience under the influence does not
constitute rape. This perspective demonstrates stereotypical
assumptions many people still hold about rape, considering
it to be an act of extreme violence perpetrated by an un-
known assailant upon an unsuspecting woman who is
willing to defend her sexual purity with her life. Such
sentiments contribute to a culture that continues to put at
least partial responsibility for sexual assault on the victim
and normalizes sexual aggression as part of the male gender
role. Such thinking highlights a well-known phenomenon in
research on sexual aggression, in which individuals struggle
with accurately identifying the range of different circum-
stances that all might constitute sexual assault (Bachman
and Paternoster 1993; Littleton and Axsom 2003; Littleton
et al. 2009).
Specifically, when survey items describe behaviors (i.e.,
‘Have you ever coerced somebody to intercourse by
holding them down?’’) instead of simply label them (i.e.,
‘Have you ever raped somebody?’’), more men will admit
to sexually coercive behaviors in the past and more women
will self-report past victimization (Koss 1998). Little re-
search has examined what dispositional factors might prompt
an individual to admit to rape when asked through behav-
iorally worded questions as opposed to labeling themselves a
rapist. Given that rape is defined as intercourse by use of
force or threat of force against a victim’s wishes, this dis-
crepancy suggests that at least some men who rape do not
seem to classify their behaviors as such. Hence, we would
like to explore the dispositional motivations and reasons that
1
Counseling Psychology and Community Services, University of North Dakota, Grand Forks, North Dakota.
2
Department of Psychology, North Dakota State University, Fargo, North Dakota.
VIOLENCE AND GENDER
Volume 1, Number 4, 2014
ª Mary Ann Liebert, Inc.
DOI: 10.1089/vio.2014.0022
188
some individuals endorse behaviorally descriptive intentions
to use force to engage in sexual aggression, but deny in-
tentions to rape, and other individuals endorse intentions to
rape. The present study is an exploratory attempt to distin-
guish between these different types of rapists via the dis-
positional constructs of hostility toward women, and callous
sexual attitudes.
Callous Sexual Attitudes
Callous sexual attitudes are commonly defined as atti-
tudes that objectify women and expect men to exhibit sexual
dominance (Donaldson 1993). Callous sexual attitudes are a
part of the larger group of constructs that make up hyper-
masculinity. Hypermasculinity reflects an exaggeration of
traditional masculine messages, such as dictating that real
men should enjoy danger, aggressively pursue sexual op-
portunities, and stand ground in a fight. While previous
studies have related hypermasculine attitudes to sexual ag-
gression (Koralewski and Conger 1992; Good et al. 1995;
Suarez-Al-Adam et al. 2000; Tatum and Foubert 2009),
treating hypermasculinity as a global construct creates diffi-
culty in determining which particular dimensions of hyper-
masculinity predict sexual aggression. Understanding specific
dimensions that contribute to sexual aggression may help re-
fine sexual assault prevention programming or rehabilitation
programming for sexual offenders.
Most research specifically links one dimension of hy-
permasculinity, callous sexual attitudes, with sexual ag-
gression (Koralewski and Conger 1992; Parrot and Zeichner
2003). Vokey et al. (2013) noted that the dominant nature of
callous sexual attitudes prompts the hypermasculine sexual
exchange to become a depersonalized act rather than an
expression of intimacy. This is supported by research
demonstrating that men who exhibit higher sexual coercion
exhibit more callous sexual attitudes when compared to less
sexually coercive men (Koralewski and Conger 1992).
Further, research indicates that callous sexual attitudes are
linked with laboratory aggression (Parrot and Zeichner
2003) as well as cultural attitudes that justify rape or rape
myth acceptance (e.g., endorsing the belief that wearing a
short skirt invites rape) (Good et al. 1995; Tatum and
Foubert 2009). Ryan (2004) proposed that the relationship
between rape myth acceptance and sexual aggression might
be due to the extent to which callous sexual attitudes support
rape beliefs. This indicates that callous sexual attitudes may
be a unique contributor to sexual aggression.
Given that callous sexual attitudes permit violence and
consider women as passive sexual objects, it follows that for
men who endorse these, sexual aggression becomes an ap-
propriate and accepted expression of masculinity. In this sense,
using force to obtain intercourse does not become an act of
rape, but rather an expression of hypermasculinity, which may
be thought of as a desirable disposition in certain subcultures.
Taken together, these research findings suggest that an ex-
pression of hypermasculinity through callous sexual attitudes
may relate to an inclination to endorse a behavioral description
(i.e., using force to hold an individual down) versus referring to
a sexually aggressive act as rape. Hence, we hypothesize that
the construct of callous sexual attitudes will be found at the
highest levels in those men who endorse intentions to force a
woman to sexual acts but deny intentions to rape.
Hostility Toward Women
Hostility toward women is a tendency to be focused on
negative interactions with women (i.e., attacks, rejections),
resulting in an angry and unfriendly attitude toward them
(Check 1985). Hostility toward women reflects a disposition
in which men view women’s motives as manipulative or
deceitful. By extension, hostility toward women can feature
an unwillingness to trust women, as well as a tendency to
blame women for one’s problems.
Generally, sexually aggressive men, in both offending
populations and college populations, exhibit higher levels of
hostility toward women (Bernat et al. 1997; Rando et al.
1998). In a correlational study examining various attitudes
related to sexual aggression, Forbes et al. (2004) concluded
that hostility towards women closely related to college men’s
levels of sexual coercion and sexual aggression. Rando et al.
(1998) found that sexually aggressive men ranked higher
on hostility toward women when compared to nonsexually
aggressive men. Perpetrators of sexual violence scored
higher on hostility toward women compared to nonsexually
violent criminal offenders (Gudjonsson et al. 1989; Mar-
shall and Moulden 2001). However, research investigating
the relationship of hostility toward women and sexual of-
fending is not conclusive (Beech et al. 2006). Overall, while
some research points to a relationship between hostility
toward women and sexual aggression, most investigations
do not differentiate between sexual coercion (i.e., utilizing
nonphysical tactics to obtain a nonconsensual sexual en-
counter), and rape (i.e., utilizing force to obtain noncon-
sensual sexual encounter). Combining the constructs of
sexual coercion and rape may limit the predictive power of
attitudinal measures, and potentially reflects an assumption
that all manifestations of sexually aggressive behavior re-
flect the same dispositional pathway. Hence, examining the
extent to which hostility toward women predicts male’s
perceptions of future intentions as constituting rape or force
could help differentiate such mixed results.
Prior research differentiated coercive men (i.e., utilizing
nonphysical means to obtain a nonconsensual sexual en-
counter) from sexually aggressive men (i.e., utilizing
physical force to obtain a nonconsensual sexual encounter).
DeGue et al. (2010) found that only sexually aggressive
men exhibited hostility toward women when compared to
men that utilized consent, and sexually coercive men. How-
ever, DeGue et al. also noted that a substantial proportion of
the sample indicated that they engaged in both sexual coer-
cion and sexual aggression, which may have resulted in an
overall lack of prominent differences between sexually co-
ercive men and sexually aggressive men. Such results support
the exploration of delineating between perceiving one’s use
of forceful sexual behaviors as constituting rape or not. We
believe that men exhibiting higher levels of hostility toward
women will exhibit awareness that their behaviors constitute
rape, and still endorse use of force given that the motivation
of the encounter is to punish women.
The Present Study
We investigated whether the constructs of hostility to-
ward women and callous sexual attitudes differed among
men who self-identify as denying any likelihood to rape or
use force to obtain intercourse from women in the future,
DENYING RAPE BUT ADMITTING USE OF FORCE 189
self-identify as using force to obtain intercourse but denying
any intentions to rape ever, and men who respond affirma-
tively to both having intentions to rape and using force in
the future. This allows us to test whether there are differ-
ences in men who do not identify with the ‘rape’ label on
sexual aggression surveys, although they have committed
acts that would be defined as rape. Men who admit inten-
tions to force women to have sexual intercourse only, but do
not believe that this act constitutes rape, might not be pri-
marily motivated by a desire to retaliate and overpower
women. Their behavior could be guided by other factors in
line with stereotypically masculine gender roles such as
having a high desire for sexual activity, viewing sexuality as
a competition and a way to gain respect among peers, and
lacking consideration for women or viewing them as sexual
objects. Therefore, we hypothesize that men do not endorse
any intentions for sexual aggression will differ from the
other two groups of men primarily on a dimension charac-
terized by hostility toward women as the strongest loading
factor. Men who openly endorse intentions to rape women
versus those who only endorse intentions to use force but
deny rape are hypothesized to differ along a second di-
mension on which callous sexual attitudes play a more
important role. In other words, we expect a pattern of results
showing two significant functions along which we can dif-
ferentiate the three groups of men.
Materials and Methods
Participants and measures
Eighty-six male college students received extra credit for
their participation. All participants were over 18 (M = 21,
SD = 3.6) and most were juniors in college. The over-
whelming majority of participants ( > 90%) identified as
Caucasian, consistent with the general student make up at
this university, and all identified as heterosexual, with prior
sexual experiences.
Hostility toward women. The hostility toward women
scale (Check 1985) assesses hostile, negative, and resentful
feelings participants might hold against women. This scale
consists of 22 items, specifically addressing male hostility
against females, such as: ‘I feel that many women flirt with
men just to tease them and hurt them’ or ‘I am easily
angered by women.’’ Judgments were made on a 0 (strongly
agree)to6(strongly disagree) response scale, with higher
scores indicating more hostility. The scale demonstrated
good reliability (a = .87) in the current study.
Hypermasculinity scale. The survey also included the
callous sexual attitudes subscale from the hypermasculinity
scale (Mosher and Sirkin 1984). This scale consists of three
subscales: viewing danger as exciting, regarding violence as
manly, and endorsement of callous sexual attitudes. Only the
callous sexual attitudes subscale was used in this study,
because we were interested specifically in the contribution of
callous sexual attitudes to likelihood to commit sexual as-
sault. The subscale consisted of 10 forced choice items. The
scale demonstrated good reliability in this study (a = 0.86).
Attraction to sexual aggression. The survey concluded
with part of the attraction to sexual aggression scale (Ma-
lamuth 1989a,b). This scale measures self-reported likeli-
hood to engage in a variety of sexual behaviors ‘if nobody
would ever know and there wouldn’t be any consequences’
for the participants. The behaviors that were included were
heterosexual intercourse, forcing a female to do something
sexual she does not want to, and rape. We chose to use a
hypothetical scale like this because we believed that it
would lead to less social desirability bias in this research, as
well as lessen any concerns participants might have about
divulging information about past crimes. In addition, for the
purpose of this study, the key points of having labels (rape)
versus behavioral descriptions of sexual aggression is
completely addressed with this scale.
Marlow–Crowne Social Desirability Scale. Participants
also completed the Marlow–Crowne Social Desirability
Scale in order to account for biases in reporting (Crowne
and Marlow 1960). Participants agreed with 10 dichoto-
mous items designed to assess the respondent’s propensity
to wanting to paint themselves in a favorable light.
Procedure
A male research assistant collected informed consent and
administered the survey in a private location. After finishing
the survey, participants dropped the survey into a mailbox.
Participants were then debriefed by the experimenter. The
debriefing was extensive and provided hotline information,
addressed rape myths, social norms campaign, university
policy on sexual assault, and a full presentation aimed at
preventing sexual aggression. This debriefing also included
resources to obtain free counseling services. Providing an
educational debriefing is a common practice in research
examining sexual aggression or utilizing sexually aggressive
materials (Allen et al. 1996), and our debriefing followed all
appropriate guidelines.
Results
The Marlow–Crown scale was not correlated with any of
the measures of interest, indicating that participants did not
exhibit any significant bias in reporting. Hence, we were able
to proceed with our analyses as planned. Data of the partici-
pants’ intentions to commit sexual assault are displayed in
Table 1. Table 2 shows the correlations of dispositional var-
iables and self-reported intentions to commit sexual assault.
To investigate whether hostility toward women and cal-
lous sexual attitudes differentiated among participants who
endorsed forced sex but not rape versus those who endorsed
both or neither, a descriptive discriminant function analysis
was conducted. We chose this analysis because it is well
suited to describe differences between preexisting groups
along multiple constructs at the same time. Discriminant
function analysis attempts to predict group membership
Table 1. Frequency of Intentions by Item Wording
Yes No
Intentions to force a woman
to sexual intercourse
31.7% (n = 26) 68.3% (n = 56)
Any intentions to rape a woman 13.6% (n = 11) 86.4% (n = 70)
190 EDWARDS ET AL.
based on canonical variables created by a given set of pre-
dictors. This allows the researcher to understand the nature
of the differences in groups more clearly by revealing which
variables are important and in which manner they should be
combined to optimally describe the distinction between groups.
Each of the discriminant functions represents a canonical var-
iable that is made up of a unique linear combination of the
constructs used (Duarte Silva and Stam 1995). In this study, we
attempted to predict membership in the three groups (reports no
intentions to be sexually coercive, reports intentions to force a
woman to have sex but denies intentions to rape, and reports
intentions to rape a women) based on hostility toward women
scores and callous sexual attitudes. In a sense, discriminant
function analysis can be viewed as a reverse multivariate
analysis of variance (MANOVA).
There was one participant who indicated that he would rape a
woman, but denied any likelihood to use force to obtain in-
tercourse. Because we did not know how to make sense of this
answer and could not exclude a random error (e.g., careless
marking), this case was dropped from the analysis. Cases with
missing data for the dispositional measures or intentions were
also dropped. This left us 73 cases for analysis, which all fell
into one of these groups: endorsing no intentions of sexual
assault (n = 49), endorsing intentions to use force but denying
intentions to rape a woman (n = 13), and endorsing both
(n = 10). Statistical guidelines for our chosen analysis suggests
that the minimum number of cases per group should exceed the
number of variables to be workable, and 5 observations per
variable entered into the analysis, although 20 is preferred. For
our 3 groups and 2 variables, we have 73 cases that surpass
these requirements (Tabachnick and Fidell 1996).
In discriminant function analysis, each function consists
of a unique linear combination of the predictor variables
used (here: hostility toward women and callous sexual atti-
tudes). Function 1 significantly discriminated among groups:
k = 0.65, v
2
(4) = 32.76, p < 0.001. Observing the standardized
canonical function coefficients and structure matrix suggests
that the first function is very strongly related to hostility toward
women (r
s
= 0.87), and moderately strongly related to callous
sexual attitudes (r
s
= 0.75). Function 2 was also significant:
k = 0.91, v
2
(1) = 7.20, p < 0.01. Function 2 appears to represent
moderately strong callous sexual attitudes (r
s
= 0.66), and a
moderate inverse of hostility (r
s
=-0.50). Plotting the group
centroids along the 2 significant dimensions represented by the
functions suggests that function 1 differentiates between re-
spondents who deny any intentions to rape or use force and the
other participants, as seen by the large distance between group
1 and groups 2 and 3 along the x axis in Figure 1. Given that
hostility is the attribute most strongly represented on the first
canonical variable, with moderately high levels of callous
sexual attitudes also represented, we interpret this as confir-
mation of previous research that suggests men who do not act
sexually coercive to have lower levels of both hostility toward
women and callous sexual attitudes compared to sexually co-
ercive men.
Inspection of function 2 in Figure 1 suggests that this di-
mension separated all three groups, although most strongly the
‘force’ group from the ‘rape’ group, as seen by the distance
between groups on the y axis in figure. Moderately high levels
of callous sexual attitudes coupled with an inverse of hostility
suggest that this function represents a unique set of charac-
teristics where the predominant disposition is not related to
being angry or violent toward women. Table 3 lists the
structure matrix and group centroids.
Discussion
The present study serves as initial investigation to exam-
ine how respondents who endorse behavioral descriptions of
rape but deny rape when labeled as such outright differ from
those who self-report intentions to rape and those who do
not endorse any sexual coercion. A descriptive discriminant
function analysis was used to reveal which variables should
be combined in what manner to best distinguish between the
groups. As hypothesized, a sizable number of participants
indicated that they might use force to obtain intercourse, but
would not rape a woman. Men who indicate intentions to use
force but deny intentions to rape exhibit a unique disposition
featuring an inverse construct of hostility toward women but
high levels of callous sexual attitudes (Check 1985). Given
that hostility toward women involves resentment, bitterness,
rejection sensitivity, and paranoia about women’s motives,
we consider the inverse of hostility toward women in men
Table 2. Correlations of Dispositional Measures
and Intentions to Commit Sexual Assault
Hostility
Callous sexual
attitudes
Intentions
to rape
Intentions
to use force
Hostility 0.43** 0.32** 0.35**
Callous sexual
attitudes
0.09 0.24*
Intentions to rape 0.81**
*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01.
FIG. 1. Functions at group centroids.
Table 3. Structure Matrix and Functions at Group
Centroids by Function and Dispositional Measure
Structure matrix Function 1 Function 2
Hostility 0.84 - 0.55
Callous sex 0.81 0.58
Functions at group centroids
No intentions - 0.37 0.05
Force 1.06 0.34
Rape 0.52 - 0.64
DENYING RAPE BUT ADMITTING USE OF FORCE 191
that intend to use force to be indicative of an affable, trust-
ing, and nonreactive affect toward women. When combined
with callous sexual attitudes, we interpret this function as
representing personality characteristics that might lend
themselves to allowing men to not perceive his actions as
rape and may even view the forced intercourse as an
achievement. The primary motivation in this case could be
sexual gratification, accomplishment, and/or perceived
compliance with stereotypical masculine gender norms. The
use of force in these cases might be seen as an acceptable
mean to reach one’s goal, or the woman’s ‘no’’ is perceived
as a token resistance consistent with stereotypical gender
norms. While the ultimate outcome of either act constitutes
rape, this pattern of results suggests that there might be
different types of offenders with potential differences in
underlying motivation, cognition, and/or personality traits.
These results are informative because knowing ahead of
time that a particular subgrouping of men exhibit higher
levels of callous sexual attitudes may suggest that males in
this group are more likely to commit rape without labeling it
as such. Therefore, programming for these men needs to
include a strong educational component focused on clari-
fying different behaviors that all constitute sexual assault,
but do not follow the stereotypically imagined scenarios
related to rape. Furthermore, such programming would also
have to prompt men to be engaged and open to thinking
about their own behaviors to avoid having participants in-
ternally distance themselves (i.e., exhibiting an ‘I am not
that kind of guy who rapes women, this programming is not
for me’ attitude) that would preclude men from fully par-
ticipating and benefitting from the program.
Males who might assault women without conceptualizing
their actions as rape might benefit more from psychoedu-
cation and immersion in cultural messages that do not con-
done the use of force, but might actually not take in rape
prevention messages directly due to not identifying with the
role of rapist. If improper beliefs and attitudes were sup-
planted with acceptable ones, the potential for sexual assaults
might be reduced, and the men could develop mutually
satisfying sexual relationships with women. If hostility, on
the other hand, is a prominent motivator, successful inter-
ventions likely need to include techniques to manage affect,
manage and/or process anger toward women, and cognitive
restructuring to disconnect negative feelings and women.
These men might need more individual services to allow for
successful processing of their affective bases of sexually
violent behaviors toward women, and would likely not
benefit from rape awareness and prevention programming
that focuses on norms and definitions of consent.
Limitations and Future Directions
There is likely more than hostility toward women and
callous sexual attitudes that distinguishes men who endorse
forced intercourse but not rape from those who admit to rape
on psychological studies. Our study was only a first ex-
ploration of this topic. Future studies could help account for
this by examining more dispositional measures. Given the
high perpetration of sexual assaults on college campuses
(Abbey et al. 1996), coupled with the fact that many studies
on sexual assault use college men, we felt that a conve-
nience sample of college males was a good place to start.
Understanding the motives and meaning associated with
such rapes is beyond this scope of the present research;
however, future studies could better clarify such motivations
by examining the extent to which other dimensions of
sexual aggression predict intentions to use force versus rape.
Further, it is important to note that sexual aggression is a
multidimensional construct, and both hostility toward wo-
men and hypermasculinity relate to rape myth acceptance
(Forbs et al. 2004; Ryan 2004). Given that our discrimi-
nant function analysis points to a construct that objectifies
women but exhibits positive affect toward women, future
research should incorporate other measures such as rape
myth acceptance, the full hypermasculinity scale, or benev-
olent sexism to achieve a greater dimension on the disposi-
tions contributing to different behavioral manifestations of
sexual aggression.
Conclusions
Our results suggest that there is no one-size-fits-all ap-
proach to sexual assault prevention. If men who sexually
assault harbor different motivations to varying degrees,
identifying them ahead of time and targeting interventions
toward groups of men with similar motivations, or better
even, providing personalized prevention programs, may
be more successful. Men who are primarily motivated by
negative, hostile affect toward women and who conceptu-
alize their own intentions and behaviors as rape are unlikely
to benefit from the large group primary prevention efforts
done as part of college outreach efforts. However, program-
ming using a group and norm-based approach appears to be
appropriate for men who endorse force but deny rape, as long
as the programming can establish rapport and credibility with
participants. Because these men do not view their sexually
aggressive intentions as rape, failing to attend to issues around
beliefs about the stereotypical rapist and not identifying with
them could weaken the effectiveness of the programming due
to not receiving buy in from participants. This would ulti-
mately likely leave the men who could benefit most from
these prevention efforts disengaged.
Author Disclosure Statement
In the present study, no competing financial interests
exist.
References
Abbey A, Ross LT, McDuffie D, McAuslan P. (1996). Alcohol and
dating risk factors for sexual assault among college women. Psychol
Women Q. 20, 147–169.
Allen M, D’Alessio D, Emmers T, Gebhart L. (1996). The role of
educational briefing in mitigating effects of experimental exposure
to violent sexually explicit media: A meta-analysis. J Sex Res. 33,
135–141.
Bachman R, Paternoster R. (1993). A contemporary look at the effects
of rape law reform: How far have we really come? J Criminal Law
Criminol. 84, 554–574.
Beech AR, Ward T, Fisher D. (2006). The identification of sexual and
violent motivations in men who assault women: Implication for
treatment. J Interpers Violence. 21, 1635–1653.
Bernat JA, Stolp S, Calhoun KS, et al. (1997). Construct validity and
test-retest reliability of a date rape decision-latency measure. J
Psychopathol Behav Assess. 19, 315–330.
192 EDWARDS ET AL.
Check JV. (1985). The Hostility Toward Women Scale. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, University of Manitoba, Manitoba, Canada.
Crowne DP, Marlowe D. (1960). A new scale of social desirability
independent of psychopathology. J Consult Psychol. 24, 349–354.
DeGue S, DiLillo D, Scalora M. (2010). Are all perpetrators alike?
Comparing risk factors for sexual coercion and aggression. Sex.
Abuse. 22, 402–426.
Donaldson M. (1993). What is hegemonic masculinity? Theory Soc.
22, 643–657.
Duarte Silva AP, Stam A. (1995). Discriminant analysis. In Reading
and Understanding Multivariate Statistics. LG Grimm, PR Yarnold,
eds. (APA, Washington, DC), pp. 277–319.
Forbes GB, Adams-Curtis LE, White KB. (2004). First-and second-
generation measures of sexism, rape myths and related beliefs, and
hostility toward women: Their interrelationships and association
with college students’ experiences with dating aggression and
sexual coercion. Violence Against Women. 10, 236–261.
Good GE, Hepper MJ, Hillenbrand-Gunn T, et al. (1995). Sexual and
psychological violence: An exploratory study of predictors in col-
lege men. J Mens Stud. 4, 59–71.
Gudjonsson GH, Petursson H, Skulason S. (1989). Psychiatric evidence:
A study of psychological issues. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 80, 165–169.
Koralewski MA, Conger JC. (1992). The assessment of social skills
among sexually coercive college males. J Sex Res. 29, 169–188.
Koss MP. (1998). Hidden rape: Sexual aggression and victimization in
a national sample in higher education. In Rape and Sexual Assault,
Vol. II. AM Burgess, ed. (Garland Press, New York, NY), pp. 3–25.
Littleton HL, Axsom D. (2003). Rape and seduction scripts of uni-
versity students: Implications for rape attributions and unacknowl-
edged rape. Sex Roles 49, 465–475.
Littleton HL, Tabernik H, Canales EJ, et al. (2009). Risky situation or
harmless fun? A qualitative examination of college women’s bad
hook-up and rape scripts. Sex Roles. 60, 793–804.
Malamuth NM. (1989a). The attraction to sexual aggression scale: Part
one. J Sex Res. 26, 26–49.
Malamuth NM. (1989b). The attraction to sexual aggression scale: Part
two. J Sex Res. 26, 324–354.
Marshall WL, Moulden H. (2001). Hostility toward women and victim
empathy in rapists. Sex Abuse. 13, 249–255.
Mosher DL, Sirkin M. (1984). Measuring a macho personality con-
stellation. J Res Pers. 18, 150–163.
National Center for Injury Prevention and Control. (2012). Sexual
violence. Retrieved from www.cdc.gov/violenceprevention/pdf/sv-
datasheet-a.pdf
Parrott DJ, Zeichner A. (2003). Effects of hypermasculinity on phys-
ical aggression against women. Psychol Men Masculinity. 4, 70.
Rando RA, Rogers JR, Brittan-Powell CS. (1998). Gender role conflict
and college men’s sexually aggressive attitudes and behavior. J
Mental Health Couns. 20, 359–369.
Ryan KM. (2004). Further evidence for a cognitive component of rape.
Aggress Violent Behav. 9, 579–604.
Suarez-Al-Adam M, Raffaelli M, O’Leary A. (2000). Influence of
abuse and partner hypermasculinity on the sexual behavior of La-
tinas. AIDS Educ Prev. 12, 263–274.
Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS. (1996). Using Multivariate Statistics
(Harper Collins, New York, NY).
Tatum JL, Foubert JD. (2009). Rape myth acceptance, hypermasculi-
nity, and SAT scores as correlates of moral development: Under-
standing sexually aggressive attitudes in first year college men. J
Coll Stud Dev. 50, 195–209.
Vokey M, Tefft B, Tysiaczny C. (2013). An analysis of hyper-
masculinity in magazine advertisements. Sex Roles. 68, 562–576.
Address correspondence to:
Sarah R. Edwards, PhD
Counseling Psychology and Community Services
University of North Dakota
Education Building, Room 314
231 Centennial Dr. STOP 8255
Grand Forks, ND 58202-8255
E-mail: sarah.edwards@email.und.edu
DENYING RAPE BUT ADMITTING USE OF FORCE 193
... The use of the word rape itself may be a barrier to perpetration acknowledgment research. When surveyed anonymously, 13.6% of college men report an intention to rape if they could (Edwards et al., 2014), and 35% report the same intention when the word rape is not used (Malamuth, 1981). There appears to be meaningful differences between those who endorse a behaviorally specific item that is consistent with rape and the label rape. ...
... Men who endorse willingness to use force to obtain sex but deny "rape" tended to hold generally positive attitudes toward women but callous sexual attitudes. Further this group of men were differentiated from those who would use force and use the rape label who held hostility toward women and callous sexual attitudes (Edwards et al., 2014). Donde et al. (2018) found that the label "sexual assault" was used across a wider range of victimization experiences suggesting this word may be less stigmatized than "rape." ...
Article
Individual acknowledgment of sexual assault and rape perpetration is extraordinarily low in prior research. Only about 1% of individuals report perpetrating rape, in contrast to the 6% perpetrating rape as estimated by using behaviorally specific items that exclude stigmatized words such as rape. The goal of this study was to examine two possible measurement mechanisms for increasing perpetration acknowledgment: label choice and response format. In Sample 1 (N = 291), participants completed two acknowledgment items which varied in label choice. One item used the term rape; one used the term sexual assault. Acknowledgment of perpetration using the label sexual assault was significantly higher than when using the term rape (6.38%-1.71%, p = .01, Cohen's d = 0.44). In Sample 2 (N = 438), participants were presented with a scaled and a dichotomous sexual assault item at different parts of the overall survey. Sexual assault acknowledgment was higher on the scaled item compared to the dichotomous item (15.75% vs. 3.2%, p < .0001, Cohen's d = 0.64). Rates of sexual perpetration as measured behaviorally were higher for ambiguous acknowledgment types ("might or might not," "probably not") than for those reporting "definitely not," (76.81% vs. 29.0%, p < .0001, Cohen's d = 0.59). The two different measurement strategies tested here, using a less stigmatized label such as sexual assault and using a scaled response format, both increased rates of perpetration acknowledgment 3-15x greater than rates documented in prior research.
... Reporting for male victims of sexual assault is even less likely (Barnard, 2018;Sable et al., 2006). Edwards et al. (2014) found that more women self-reported victimization when behavioral descriptions were used rather than labels, like "rape" or "sexual assault." The use of the secondary dataset is restricting, as the question cannot be rephrased using behavioral descriptions, and the nature of the assault(s) and/or the period of time since the assault cannot be considered in the prediction of outcomes. ...
... To address barriers to disclosure, surveys utilizing behavioral descriptions of assault and rape may yield more disclosures than labeling descriptions (i.e., "Have you ever been coerced by somebody to have intercourse while being held down?" vs. "Have you ever been raped?"; Edwards et al., 2014). In light of the findings in this study, investigations should aim at identifying promising therapeutic techniques to restore sense of control. ...
Article
This investigation utilized the Midlife in the United States Survey (N = 3,258) to assess the relationships between sexual assault, sense of control, and financial stability. Age of first sexual assault and sexual assault revictimization were also considered in analyses of sexual assault survivors' data. Results revealed consistent associations between experiences of sexual assault and revictimization with lower financial stability and suggest that sense of control may be an indirect mechanism linking these variables. Findings have policy relevance and practical implications for practitioners. Restoring sexual assault victims' internal loci of control may promote more positive financial outcomes.
... For example, among college students conceptualizations of consent are multifaceted (e.g., distinguishing wanting from consent; Peterson & Muehlenhard, 2007), and whether an event is construed as rape or assault can be related to an endorsement of particular rape myths about resistance (Peterson & Muehlenhard, 2004) and how closely the event matches schemas of "prototypical" sexual violence that usually involves strangers and physical violence (Bondurant, 2001;Littleton et al., 2009;McKimmie et al., 2014), although they are not the most common features of assault (Koss et al., 1988;Planty et al., 2013;Sanday, 1997). Even when sexual violence occurs, it may not be construed that way (Bondurant, 2001;Edwards et al., 2014). ...
Article
Full-text available
What solutions can we find in the research literature for preventing sexual violence, and what psychological theories have guided these efforts? We gather all primary prevention efforts to reduce sexual violence from 1985 to 2018 and provide a bird’s-eye view of the literature. We first review predominant theoretical approaches to sexual-violence perpetration prevention by highlighting three interventions that exemplify the zeitgeist of primary prevention efforts at various points during this time period. We find a throughline in primary prevention interventions: They aim to change attitudes, beliefs, and knowledge (i.e., ideas) to reduce sexual-violence perpetration and victimization. Our meta-analysis of these studies tests the efficacy of this approach directly and finds that although many interventions are successful at changing ideas, behavior change does not follow. There is little to no relationship between changing attitudes, beliefs, and knowledge and reducing victimization or perpetration. We also observe trends over time, including a shift from targeting a reduction in perpetration to targeting an increase in bystander intervention. We conclude by highlighting promising new strategies for measuring victimization and perpetration and calling for interventions that are informed by theories of behavior change and that center sexually violent behavior as the key outcome of interest.
... The phrasing for the sexual violence question (i.e., "Have you ever had sex with the person or people you are currently dating [or if you are not currently dating, the person or people you most recently dated] when you didn't want to because of the following reasons?") was chosen based on past research that has shown that people are less likely to admit to sexual harm if stronger language acknowledging sexual violence is used (S. Edwards et al., 2014). 2SLGBTQI youths are more likely than their heterosexual peers to use substances heavily (Watson et al., 2018), which puts them at increased risk of being targets of violence. ...
Article
Full-text available
2SLGBTQI youth are at disproportionately high risk of experiencing gender-based violence compared to their cis-heterosexual peers, although there is a gap in research explaining why as well as what this violence looks like. Part of the explanation relates to ongoing homophobia and transphobia; however, more research is needed to understand 2SLGBTQI youths’ feelings of safety within their communities, their experiences of violence with partners, and their help-seeking behaviours. Given the limited Canadian research, the Speak Out project was undertaken. Te Speak Out project is a multi-phase project with Phase 1 encompassing a survey of youth across Canada about their experiences of gender-based violence. From across Canada, 292 youths were recruited and asked to complete a survey on gender-based violence. Te survey covered four domains related to violence (physical violence, emotional abuse/control, sexual violence, virtual violence) participants experienced, experiences with help-seeking, and connections to the 2SLGBTQI community. Most youths had connections to the broader 2SLGBTQI community and were open about their identities, but many reported being discriminated against based on their gender expression (50%) and sexual orientation (43%). Of the participants who answered questions related to physical, emotional, and sexual abuse, very few reported any incidents. More youths reported virtual violence via text messages (34%), making them afraid to ignore phone calls or other contact (18%), and surveillance of their social media (18%). These results contribute valuable knowledge on the experiences of Canadian 2SLGBTQI youths and have important implications for both education and frontline service provision.
... The emotionally charged issues of consent and sexual assault resulted in some students engaging in significant victim-blaming behaviors, simultaneously reflecting broader rape culture and contributing to its reproduction (Wade 2017, 220;Edwards, Bradshaw, and Hinsz 2014). We observed three types of victim-blaming: a mistakes on both sides narrative, a transitional rape is always wrong, but . . . ...
Article
Full-text available
Because university campuses are microcosms of broader political and social climates, the increasingly polarized climates outside universities can permeate the classroom, challenging faculty who teach topics characterized by controversy and discomfort. We conducted a lesson study project at a college in the southeastern United States in three first-year courses from different disciplines to examine how the broader tensions of the #MeToo movement emerged and affected a class activity focused on gender. We sought to understand our students’ responses to a moment of discomfort generated by discussions of sexual roles, consent, and assault—issues that are relevant in both this cultural moment and in the lives of many first-year college students. We observed responses ranging from affirmation to resistance in what felt at times like our own failure. Without this collaboration, each of us may have been left with a narrower view of what the students learned and an incomplete sense of our own work. What began as an investigation into students’ transformative learning experiences ended as a transformative experience in our own understanding of the acts of teaching and the complexities of student learning.
... There is some evidence of greater tolerance of domestic violence among AYA (Vézina et al., 2015), young adult women (Rich et al., 2005), youth and adults (Black et al., 2010), adult majority women (Ogland, 2011), and majority men (Zavala et al., 2015) who were exposed to parental violence as children. Findings for men are more congruent and suggest a link between boys' experiences of abuse and their later violence against women in adulthood (Edwards et al., 2014;O'Leary et al., 1994;Rada, 2014;Wareham et al., 2009). If this has been demonstrated for domestic violence in adulthood, we can assume that it is also applicable to violent behaviour online. ...
Article
Prior research has mainly focused on why individuals are engaging in sexting. However, little is known about sexting in intimate relationships, particularly sexting coercion. This study examined sexting coercion using social learning theory in a sample of young adults who had experience with a romantic relationship (N = 315, aged 14–28 years, 67.94% female). Individuals completed the sexting coercion scale and the social learning scale online. The results showed that over 33% of the respondents reported being victims of sexting coercion in an intimate relationship, while about 28% of them had perpetrated sexting coercion. The results suggest that sexting coercion in an intimate relationship is significantly predicted by differential association, differential reinforcement, and imitation. The findings of this study highlight the importance of exposing youth to evidence-based preventive educational interventions on sexting from the earliest age, based on the constructs of social learning theory. Practice Impact Statement The findings of this study of sexting coercion among young adults are relevant to educational programmes given recent evidence of the prevalence of sexting coercion victimisation and sexting coercion perpetration among youth in romantic relationships. The findings suggest that those who engage in sexting coercion use a social learning mechanism that should be considered when developing educational interventions to prevent sexting coercion.
Thesis
Full-text available
Social media is a new form of communication that has thrived in the 2010s; for Twitter alone, 30 million accounts were active in early 2010 and in late 2019 they were 330 million.1 This emerging tool has sometimes been described as a positive weapon for feminism. A social medium such as Twitter has allowed activists and social movements to prosper online and has sparked discussions around important issues of society. Thus, it is relevant to delve into the mutual influence between social media and social movements, and this paper will focus on the influence of Twitter on the issue of rape culture. This research was motivated by a genuine interest in the impact social media could have not only online but also on society offline. As I had social media accounts growing up, I developed an interest in the discussions around rape and the role society plays in it. My first intention was to focus on the #MeToo movement and the discussions it sparked on Twitter. However, my research on the movement led me to an article entitled “#MeToo and the Promise and Pitfalls of Challenging Rape Culture through Digital Feminist Activism,” written by Mendes Kaitlynn, Jessica Ringrose, and Jessalynn Keller in 2018.2 This article encouraged me to explore more extensively the question of rape culture on Twitter and to assess the negative and positive effect such a social medium could have on feminism. It was indisputable from the start that the results would be nuanced and that I would discover both positive and negative aspects of feminism on Twitter. However, I became increasingly aware of how entangled rape culture was with the American society and culture, how some behaviors I had not thought about could encourage sexual assault and harassment, and how virtual life is both a representation of real life, but also an accurate depiction of what the real world would be like if people acted as uncensored and as unafraid of reprisals as they do when they are online.
Article
Gay, bisexual, and other men who have sex with men (GBMSM) have been identified as a population at higher risk of experiencing non-consensual sexual experiences (NSEs). However, previous research studies examining the prevalence of NSEs in this population have been limited by inconsistent terminology and a lack of research on the topic. The main focus of this study was to compare the effectiveness of using self-labels versus behavioural indicators to measure the prevalence of NSEs experienced in adulthood in GBMSM and contribute to more accurate prevalence rates in the Canadian context. A total of 346 participants were recruited from various social media platforms in Canada. The study used a two-part question that asked participants if they had ever been sexually assaulted or raped, followed by a question about other nonconsensual sexual experiences. The study also used a formal behavioural measure, the Sexual Experiences Survey, to assess the prevalence of NSEs. The results indicated that a two-part question and the formal measure reported similar prevalence rates of NSEs—64.5% and 66.8% respectively. Overall prevalence of NSEs was very high in this sample, and people of colour and trans men reported even higher rates. Depending on the context, the parsimonious choice of questioning persons on the NSE history may be valid, however, only if applied in the context of asking for rape and assault as well as other NSEs that may not be captured by these definitions. The study highlights the importance of using consistent terminology and effective measurement methods when studying the prevalence of NSEs in GBMSM. These findings may have important implications for developing interventions and for obtaining more accurate prevalence rates in a variety of settings without having to use a longer, more formalized measure.
Article
Full-text available
Traditional masculinities (TM) are a set of meanings related to men. The power attached to these meanings operates to maintain the inequality of gender relations. Previous studies have reported a positive correlation between traditional masculinities and men’s sexism, limiting women’s personal and professional opportunities. To quantify this relationship, a meta-analysis was conducted using 107 independent samples (mainly from the United States) from 21,078 men, reported in 44 articles between 1984 and 2021. Although we observed that TM positively correlated with men’s sexism, there was wide variability in effect sizes. An examination of potential moderators revealed that the relationship was significantly different between different forms of TM. The relationship between sexism and conformity to masculine norms and traditional masculinity ideology was stronger than the relationship with gender role stress. Further, TM were more strongly related to forms of sexism that reflect overt negative attitudes toward women (old-fashioned and hostile sexisms) than to forms that reflect covert sexist attitudes (modern and benevolent sexism). Moreover, the link between TM and sexism was stronger in the general population than in student samples. However, these findings should be interpreted and taken with caution due to the presence of substantial heterogeneity in effect sizes.
Article
Full-text available
In this second article of a two‐part series, data from three separate studies are presented supporting the reliability, discriminant, and construct validity of the Attraction to Sexual Aggression (ASA) scale. Higher scores on this scale were associated with attitudes supporting aggression against women, affective reactions to media portrayals of forced sex, physiological and self‐reported sexual arousal, hostility toward women, dominance motives, and antisocial personality characteristics. Based on data with low sexual aggressors, higher ASA scores were associated with greater similarity (on relevant dimensions) to high sexual aggressors, suggesting that ASA may help identify potentially sexually aggressive men at “risk” for future sexual coercion. However, caution in the use of such identification is urged. Finally, it is concluded that although the data support the usefulness of earlier work with self‐reported likelihood measures, the use of ASA is an improvement in this line of research. Directions for future development of this scale and research are also discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Conducting experiments on the impact of sexually explicit material (SEM) requires the investigator to expose a person to material that may “harm” the research participant. To maintain ethical conduct for such research, investigators use some type of educational briefing to mitigate any potential impact to consuming SEM. Ten studies specifically measuring the impact of educational briefings in mitigating impacts of SEM were located in a 1,500‐article database of research relating to SEM. All studies demonstrated the effectiveness of some type of educational de‐ or prebriefing in minimizing the impact of experimental exposure to sexually explicit materials (Mr = .29, N = 1002, range .03 to .68, where rs of 0 and up indicate complete mitigation). Educational briefings appear to mitigate the effects of experimental exposure to SEM, although long‐term consequences and the comparative effectiveness of different briefing techniques have yet to be assessed.
Article
Full-text available
Male perpetrated sexual aggression has long been recognized as a serious problem on college campuses. The purpose of this multiple regression correlation study was to assess the relationship between levels of moral development (measured by the Defining Issues Test) and the degree to which first-year college men (N = 161) ascribed to rape supportive attitudes, as measured by the Illinois Rape Myth Acceptance Scale and the Hypermasculinity Inventory. Respondents completed these instruments and a demographic questionnaire prior to the beginning of the fall semester. Pearson correlations indicated that there was a significant (p < .01) relationship between rape myth acceptance and moral development. There was not a significant relationship between hypermasculinity and moral development. Stepwise multiple regression analysis indicated that rape myths and SAT verbal scores accounted for 9% of moral development variance. Additional stepwise analysis suggested that the rape myth subscale, It Wasn’t Really Rape, in combination with SAT verbal scores, accounted for approximately 10% of moral development variance. Implications for practitioners and researchers are provided.
Article
Full-text available
A scale designed to measure attraction to sexual aggression is described, based on earlier work assessing self‐reported likelihood of committing rape. This scale's associations with measures of theoretically relevant attitudes, perceptions, and behavioral inclinations are examined in comparison with briefer measures, and with a number of other scales measuring attraction to various types of sexual interactions. These include conventional sex (e.g., heterosexual intercourse), homosexuality, bondage, unconventional sex (e.g., group sex), and deviant sex (e.g., pedophilia). Data supporting the longer and shorter versions of the attraction to sexual aggression scale are presented, showing internal consistency, test‐retest reliability, and discriminant and construct validity. As well, a number of issues raised by critiques of research in this area are addressed empirically.
Article
The abstract for this document is available on CSA Illumina.To view the Abstract, click the Abstract button above the document title.
Article
Hyper-masculinity is a gender-based ideology of exaggerated beliefs about what it is to be a man. HM consists of four inter-related beliefs, namely toughness as emotional self-control, violence as manly, danger as exciting, and calloused attitudes toward women and sex (Zaitchik & Mosher 1993). Adherence to HM is linked to a host of social and health problems in North America, such as dangerous driving and violence toward women (Parrot & Zeichner 2003). Advertising is believed to play a role in constructing hyper-masculinity (Kilbourne 1999). In this study, eight U.S. men’s magazines published in 2007–2008, differentiated by readership age, education, and household income, were analyzed for hyper-masculine depictions in their advertisements. Using a behavioural checklist with good inter-rater reliability, it was found that 56 % (n = 295) of 527 advertisements depicted one or more hyper-masculine beliefs. Some magazines depicted at least one hyper-masculine belief in 90 % or more of advertisements. In addition, reader age, education, and income were all inversely related to the prevalence of hyper-masculine beliefs, with HM depictions presented more often in advertisements targeting young, less educated, and less affluent men. Implications of these findings for the well-being of men and society are discussed.
Article
This study investigated the relationship between sexual coercion and social competence in a college population. Males who self‐reported low, moderate, and high levels of sexual coercion were administered several performance and paper‐and‐pencil measures designed to assess global social competence and specific skills, such as assertiveness, and social perception, or cue‐reading. In addition, subjects were administered questionnaires designed to assess attitudes towards women, rape myths, and hypermasculinity. While the attitude instruments measuring sexual callousness and acceptance of interpersonal violence discriminated among coercion groups, performance or paper‐and‐pencil measures of social competence and skills did not, despite the large number and array of measures. This research does not support the notion that men engage in coercive sexual behavior because they lack the appropriate social skills. Suggestions for future research include the examination of disinhibitory factors such as the use of alcohol as well as characterological and personality features that may facilitate the transition from aggressive attitudes to aggressive behavior.