Content uploaded by Brad J Schoenfeld
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Brad J Schoenfeld on Feb 01, 2016
Content may be subject to copyright.
Evidence-Based Personal Training
The Evidence-Based Personal Training column provides
practical application of research topics specific to
enhancing results for personal trainers.
COLUMN EDITOR: Brad Schoenfeld, PhD, CSCS,
CSPS, NSCA-CPT
Attentional Focus for
Maximizing Muscle
Development: The
Mind-Muscle Connection
Brad J. Schoenfeld, PhD, CSCS, FNSCA
1
and Bret Contreras, MA, CSCS
2
1
Department of Health Sciences, Program of Exercise Science, City University of New York, Lehman College, New
York, New York; and
2
Sport Performance Research Institute, AUT University, Auckland, New Zealand
ABSTRACT
ATTENTIONAL FOCUS IS A WELL-
RECOGNIZED ASPECT OF MOTOR
LEARNING AND ITS USE HAS
IMPORTANT IMPLICATIONS TO THE
FITNESS PROFESSIONAL. THIS
ARTICLE WILL DISCUSS HOW
ATTENTIONAL FOCUS SHOULD BE
DIRECTED TO MAXIMIZE MUSCU-
LAR DEVELOPMENT.
A
ttentional focus is a well-
recognized aspect of motor
learning and its use has impor-
tant implications to the fitness profes-
sional. Simply stated, attentional focus
refers to what an individual thinks about
when performing a given movement or
activity . Two primary types of atten-
tional focuses have been identified:
internal and external. With an internal
focus, the individual thinks about bodily
movements during performance. Alter-
natively, an external focus directs the
exerciser’s attention to the environment.
For example, in the squat an internal
focus could be to “squeeze your glutes
as you ascend” whereas an external
focus could be to “drive the floor away
from your body.” This article will discuss
how attentional focus should be d irected
to maximize muscular development.
ATTENTIONAL FOCUS RESEARCH
A compelling body of research indi-
cates that performance-oriented tasks
are optimized by adopting an external
focus of attention. In a recent review of
literature encompassing over 50 pub-
lished studies on the topic, Wulf (12)
found that more than 90% of these
studies showed superior improvements
in motor learning when subjects used
an external compared with internal
focus. Beneficial effects were seen
across a wide variety of activities and
outcome measures, lending strong sup-
port for the use of an external focus
when the goal is to boost performance.
With respect to resistance training, the
performance-based superiority of an
external focus has been attributed to
an enhanced economy of movement
associated with greater force produc-
tion and reduced muscular activity (5).
However, whereas a more economical
movement pattern facilitates better skill
acquisition, it may not be optimal for
muscle development. Indeed, when
the goal is to maximize hypertrophy,
indirect evidence suggests that an inter-
nal focus may be the best approach.
Bodybuilders have long preached the
importance of developing a “mind-
muscle connection” when training. This
internally focused strategy involves visu-
alizing the target muscle and consciously
directing neural drive to the muscle dur-
ing exercise performance. Theoretically,
Copyright National Strength and Conditioning Association Strength and Conditioning Journal | www.nsca-scj.com
1
Copyright ª National Strength and Conditioning Association. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
such an approach would increase acti-
vation of the target muscle while dimin-
ishing contribution from secondary
muscle movers. Indeed, research seems
to support this contention.
A number of studies have shown
greater activation of a given muscle
when subjects were instructed to adopt
an internal focus of attention. This has
been most prominently displayed in
the abdominal musculature. Karst and
Willett (3) found that subjects were
able to significantly alter mean electro-
myography (EMG) activity to either
the rectus abdominis or obliques by
consciously focusing on the respective
muscles during performance of the curl
up. Before engaging in exercise, sub-
jects in this study were instructed on
how to visualize either the rectus or
obliques and verbal reinforcement of
these instructions were provided dur-
ing performance. A control condition
involved focusing on the movement
itself without regard to any specific
muscles. These results are consistent
with research showing increased acti-
vation of the transversus abdominis
after instruction to tighten the pelvic
floor muscles (2). Similarly, Bressel
et al. (1) demonstrated that mean and
peak EMG amplitude were significantly
increased in both superficial and deep
abdominal musculature during the
squat when subjects were directed to
“brace yourself as if you were going to
be punched in the stomach.”
Findings of heightened EMG activity
from an internal focus have been noted
in other muscles as well. Lewis and
Sahrmann (4) showed that young
women were able to achieve greater
mean EMG activity of the gluteus
maximus and reduced activation of
the hamstrings when cued to contract
the gluteal muscles during perfor-
mance of the prone hip extension
(“Use your gluteal muscles to lift your
leg while keeping your hamstrings
muscles relaxed”). Moreover, the tim-
ing of activation was altered so that
the gluteus maximus was activated sig-
nificantly earlier during movement.
Likewise, research has shown that
intentionally focusing on the target
muscle resulted in higher activation
of the latissimus dorsi, pectoralis major,
biceps brachii, and triceps brachii (5,7–
9). Interestingly, evidence seems to
indicate that the increased activation
does not always coincide with reduc-
tions in the activity of secondary mus-
cle movers (7,8).
Although it remains unclear as to
whether increased muscle activation
translates into greater muscle protein
accretion, emerging research indicates
that this may in fact be the case. In a 2-
part experiment, Wakahara et al. (11)
first investigated acute muscle activation
in 12 untrained men after a single bout
of resistance training for the elbow ex-
tensors through T2-weighted magnetic
resonance imaging. The exercise proto-
col consisted of 5 sets of 8 repetitions of
lying triceps extensions with 90 seconds
rest between sets. Results showed sig-
nificantly greater activation in the prox-
imal and mid-portions of the triceps
brachii compared with the distal aspect.
Another 12 subjects were then recruited
to perform a 3-day-per-week program
consisting of the same routine used in
part 1 of the study. After 12 weeks of
regimented training, increases in muscle
cross-sectional area were found to be
well-correlated to the areas most acti-
vated by the exercise regimen. Follow-
up work by the same laboratory showed
similar results using different triceps bra-
chii exercises (10), which in combina-
tion provide evidence for an association
between activation levels and muscle
growth. It should be noted that these
studies did not attempt to investigate
muscle activation in conjunction with
altered attentional focus, so it is unclear
whether results would translate to the
adoption of an internal focus. Moreover,
the results of these studies are specific to
the triceps brachii and thus cannot nec-
essarily be generalized to other muscles.
Interestingly, the effectiveness of using
an internal focus is reduced when
training at higher loads. Snyder and
Fry (7) found that activation of the pec-
torals was amplified by 22% when
resistance-trained men were provided
with verbal instructions to focus on the
chest muscles during bench press at
50% 1 repetition maximum (1RM).
However, the magnitude of this effect
decreased to 13% when the same in-
structions were provided during per-
formance at 80% 1RM. This may be
a function of needing to exert greater
levels of force when training at heavier
loads, thereby altering one’s ability to
focus on the muscle being worked.
Moreover, in accordance with the size
principle, fewer motor units will be
available for the mind to influence with
heavy loading when compared with
lighter loads. This suggests that adopt-
ing an internal attentional focus with
very heavy loads (above 85–90% of
1RM) is unnecessary because it might
limit force production without enhanc-
ing muscle activation, but more
research is needed in subjects with
varying levels of experience to explore
this hypothesis.
PRACTICAL APPLICATIONS
Attentional f ocus should match the
goal of the task. Competitive sport
athletes should rely heavily on exter-
nal attentional focus in practice and
during games or matches. This in-
cludes powerlifters, weightlifters, or
strongmen seeking to set a 1RM or
to maximize force or tor que produc-
tion; basketball players or track &
field athletes seeking to maximize
jump height or distance; runners or
rowers seeking to improve economy;
and dart throwers, golfers, and pool
players seeking maximum accuracy.
Alternatively, when attempting to
maximize muscle activation, an inter-
nal focus of attention would seem to
be a better choice. Bodybuilders, phy-
sique athletes, and others seeking
maximal hypertrophy will conceiv-
ably benefit by focusing on the target
muscle during an exercise rather than
on the outcome or environment. It is
likely that the molecular signaling for
all 3 primary mechanisms of muscular
hypertrophy, namely mechanical ten-
sion, metabolic stress, and muscle
damage (6), are increased when the
exerciser focuses their attention inter-
nally, which could ultimately result in
greater muscular development for
a given exercise and load. The effects
Evidence-Based Personal Training
VOLUME 0 | NUMBER 0 | MONTH 2016
2
Copyright ª National Strength and Conditioning Association. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
of this strategy seem to be particularly
beneficial when training with rela-
tively light loads.
Conflicts of Interest and Source of Funding:
The authors report no conflicts of interest
and no source of funding.
Brad J. Schoenfeld is an assistant
professor in the exercise science program
at CUNY Lehman College and director
of their human performance laboratory.
Bret Contreras is currently pursuing
his PhD in Sports Science at the Auck-
land University of Technology in Auck-
land, New Zealand.
REFERENCES
1. Bressel E, Willardson JM, Thompson B, and
Fontana FE. Effect of instruction, surface
stability, and load intensity on trunk muscle
activity. J Electromyogr Kinesiol 19: e500–
e504, 2009.
2. Critchley D. Instructing pelvic floor
contraction facilitates transversus abdominis
thickness increase during low-abdominal
hollowing. Physiother Res Int 7: 65–75,
2002.
3. Karst GM and Willett GM. Effects of
specific exercise instructions on abdominal
muscle activity during trunk curl exercises.
J Orthop Sports Phys Ther 34: 4–12,
2004.
4. Lewis CL and Sahrmann SA. Muscle
activation and movement patterns during
prone hip extension exercise in women.
J Athl Train 44: 238–248, 2009.
5. Marchant DC, Greig M, and Scott C.
Attentional focusing instruct ions
influence force production and muscular
activity during isok inetic elbow flexio ns.
J Strength Cond R es 23: 2358–2366,
2009.
6. Schoenfeld BJ. The mechanisms of muscle
hypertrophy and their application to
resistance training. J Strength Cond Res 24:
2857–2872, 2010.
7. Snyder BJ and Fry WR. Effect of verbal
instruction on muscle activity during the
bench press exercise. J Strength Cond Res
26: 2394–2400, 2012.
8. Snyder BJ and Leech JR. Voluntary increase
in latissimus dorsi muscle activity during the
lat pull-down following expert instruction.
J Strength Cond Res 23: 2204–2209,
2009.
9. Vance J, Wulf G, Tollner T, McNevin N, and
Mercer J. EMG activity as a function of the
performer’s focus of attention. J Mot Behav
36: 450–459, 2004.
10. Wakahara T, Fukutani A, Kawakami Y, and
Yanai T. Nonuniform muscle hypertrophy: Its
relation to muscle activation in training session.
Med Sci Sports Exerc 45: 2158–2165, 2013.
11. Wakahara T, Miyamoto N, Sugisaki N,
Murata K, Kanehisa H, Kawakami Y,
Fukunaga T, and Yanai T. Association
between regional differences in muscle
activation in one session of resistance
exercise and in muscle hypertrophy after
resistance training. Eur J Appl Physiol 112:
1569–1576, 2012.
12. Wulf G. Attentional focus and motor
learning: A review of 15 years. Int Rev
Sport Exerc Psychol 6: 77–104, 2013.
Strength and Conditioning Journal | www.nsca-scj.com
3
Copyright ª National Strength and Conditioning Association. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.